Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Relationships between episodic memory performance prediction and sociodemographic variables among healthy older adults

Relações entre previsão de desempenho de memória episódica e variáveis sociodemográficas entre idosos saudáveis

Abstracts

Previous studies have suggested that performance prediction, an aspect of metamemory, may be associated with objective performance on memory tasks.

OBJECTIVE:

The objective of the study was to describe memory prediction before performing an episodic memory task, in community-dwelling older adults, stratified by sex, age group and educational level. Additionally, the association between predicted and objective performance on a memory task was investigated.

METHODS:

The study was based on data from 359 participants in the FIBRA study carried out at Ermelino Matarazzo, São Paulo. Memory prediction was assessed by posing the question: "If someone showed you a sheet with drawings of 10 pictures to observe for 30 seconds, how many pictures do you think you could remember without seeing the sheet?". Memory performance was assessed by the memorization of 10 black and white pictures from the Brief Cognitive Screening Battery (BCSB).

RESULTS:

No differences were found between men and women, nor for age group and educational level, in memory performance prediction before carrying out the memory task. There was a modest association (rho=0.11, p=0.041) between memory prediction and performance in immediate memory. On multivariate linear regression analyses, memory performance prediction was moderately significantly associated with immediate memory (p=0.061).

CONCLUSION:

In this study, sociodemographic variables did not influence memory prediction, which was only modestly associated with immediate memory on the Brief Cognitive Screening Battery (BCSB).

memory; memory prediction; metamemory; older adults


Estudos prévios sugerem que a previsão de desempenho, um dos aspectos da metamemória, pode estar associada ao desempenho em tarefas de memória.

OBJETIVO:

Objetivou-se descrever a previsão de desempenho antes da realização de uma tarefa de memória episódica em idosos residentes na comunidade, divididos por sexo, faixas de idade e escolaridade. Adicionalmente, objetivou-se estudar a associação entre a previsão de desempenho com o desempenho objetivo em tarefa de memória.

MÉTODOS:

Foram utilizados os dados de 359 idosos participantes do estudo FIBRA realizado em Ermelino Matarazzo, São Paulo. Foi utilizada uma questão referente à previsão de desempenho: "Se alguém lhe mostrasse uma folha com desenhos de 10 figuras para observar por 30 segundos, quantas figuras o/a senhor/a acha que conseguiria se lembrar em seguida sem ver a folha?". O desempenho de memória foi avaliado pela memorização de 10 figuras em preto e branco da Bateria Breve de Rastreio Cognitivo (BBRC).

RESULTADOS:

Não foram encontradas diferenças entre homens e mulheres, entre faixas etárias ou de escolaridade para a previsão de desempenho antes da realização de tarefa de memória. Houve associação modesta (rho=0,11, p=0,041) entre a previsão de desempenho e o desempenho em memória imediata. Na análise de regressão linear multivariada a previsão de desempenho associou-se de forma moderatemente significante com a memória imediata (p=0,061).

CONCLUSÃO:

Neste estudo observou-se que as variáveis sociodemográficas não influenciaram a previsão de desempenho que esteve associada de forma modesta com a memória imediata na Bateria Breve de Rastreio Cognitivo (BBRC).

memória; previsão de desempenho; metamemória; idosos


INTRODUCTION

Some cognitive functions tend to decline with age, such as episodic and working memory, executive functions and attention.1Salthouse TA. Influence of age on practice effects in longitudinal neurocognitive change. Neuropsychology 2010;24:563-572. However, some aspects of cognition, such as metamemory, remain less well studied in aging.

The term "metamemory" originally referred to a broad array of knowledge which people held about memory.2Flavell JH. First discussant's comments: What's memory development the development of? Hum Development 1971;14:272-278. The concept now also encompasses beliefs, such as self-efficacy, performance prediction and emotions in memory. Metamemory is especially relevant in Gerontology, since it is held that beliefs about memory can affect performance of older adults on memory tasks.3Cavanaugh JC, Feldman JM, Hertzog C. Memory beliefs as social cognition: A reconceptualization of what memory questionnaires assess. Rev Gen Psychol 1998;2:48-65.

Cavanaugh JC, Green EE. I believe, therefore I can: Self-efficacy beliefs in memory aging. In: E. A. Lovelace (Eds.), Aging and cognition: Mental processes, self awareness, and interventions. Amsterdam: Elsevier 1990:189-230.
- 5Woo E, Schmitter-Edgecombe M, Fancher, JB. Memory Prediction Accuracy in younger and older adults: A Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Analysis. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2008;15:68-94. Negative beliefs about memory are thought to have a deleterious impact on the use of strategies, on effort dedicated to the task and goal-setting, all of which can be regarded as moderating variables of the relationship between beliefs and memory performance.6Berry JM, West RL, Dennehey DM. Reliability and validity of the self-efficacy questionnaire. Dev Psychol 1989;25:701-713.

Metamemory can be studied by means of performance prediction. Under this paradigm, participants are asked to make predictions about their performance before performing memory task. Performance prediction is thought to involve an assessment of the difficulty level of the proposed task, together with an assessment of one's own ability to perform that task.5Woo E, Schmitter-Edgecombe M, Fancher, JB. Memory Prediction Accuracy in younger and older adults: A Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Analysis. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2008;15:68-94.

Studies on performance prediction in memory tasks have produced mixed results, but tend to indicate that elderly overestimate their performance - not as a result of over confidence, but because they tend to under estimate the difficulty level of the task.5Woo E, Schmitter-Edgecombe M, Fancher, JB. Memory Prediction Accuracy in younger and older adults: A Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Analysis. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2008;15:68-94. , 7Mantyla T, Ronnlund M, Kliegel M. Components of executive functioning in metamemory. Appl Neuropsychol 2010;17:289-98.

In Brazil, Yassuda et al.8Yassuda MS, Lasca VB, Neri AL. Metamemória e auto-eficácia: Um estudo de validação de Instrumento de pesquisa sobre memória e envelhecimento. Psicol Reflex Crít 2005;18:78-90. assessed the validity of the Portuguese version of a metamemory questionnaire, the Metamemory in Adulthood Questionnaire (MIA) and also of a self-efficacy questionnaire, the Memory Self-Efficacy Questionnaire (MSEQ), in 33 younger and 27 older healthy Brazilian adults. The results of the analyses suggested that the Portuguese versions of these instruments exhibit good psychometric characteristics and are promising for research use in Brazil.

The relationship between performance prediction and objective performance on memory tasks may be influenced by the low level of schooling often found among elderly in the Brazilian milieu. Therefore, the objective of the present study was to describe memory prediction before performing a visual episodic memory task, in a sample of older adults from Ermelino Matarazzo, participants of the FIBRA study, stratified by sex, age group and educational level. Additionally, the association between predicted and objective performance on an episodic memory task was investigated.

METHODS

Participants. This study was based on data from 359 participants of the population-based study "Frailty profiles in Brazilian elderly", conducted by the Fibra Network, UNICAMP, in response to Public call MCT-CNPq/MS-SCTIE-DECIT - no. 17/2006. A total of 384 elderly residents of Ermelino Matarazzo were interviewed between July 2008 and June 2009. The present study included all participants with complete data for the variables of interest (N=359). Participants scoring below the cut-off score on the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) were not excluded.9Brucki SMD, Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Bertolucci PHF, Okamoto IH. Sugestões para o uso do mini-exame do estado mental no Brasil. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2003;61: 77-781. Further information on the methods employed in the FIBRA study is available in Neri et al.1010 Neri AL, Yassuda MS, de Araújo LF, et al. Methodology and social, demographic, cognitive, and frailty profiles of community-dwelling elderly from seven Brazilian cities: the FIBRA Study. Cad Saúde Pública 2013; 29:778-792.

Instruments. This study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the School of Medical Sciences of the State University of Campinas, under report number 208/2007. All participants completed an extensive protocol, during a single session, which included sociodemographic variables, health-related variables, anthropometric measures, psychosocial variables and variables on frailty criteria.

Participants completed the MMSE and answered the following question about memory performance prediction: "If someone showed you a sheet with drawings of 10 pictures to observe for 30 seconds, how many pictures do you think you could remember without seeing the sheet?". The participants then carried out a memory task from the Brief Cognitive Screening Battery.1111 Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Herrera JR, et al. Performance of illiterate and literate nondemented elderly subjects in two tests of long-term memory. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2004;10:634-638.

The task consisted of naming and memorization of 10 common black and white drawings. The figures were named by the subject (Naming), who was then asked to recall each drawing immediately, without having been told the figures had to be memorized (Incidental Memory). Subsequently, the figures were displayed again and the subject asked to memorize them for 30 seconds for further recall (Immediate Memory). The procedure was then repeated (Learning). After performing other tasks for around 5 minutes, the subject was asked to evoke the figures shown previously (Delayed Memory). Finally, the 10 figures, mixed with another 10 distractor figures, were redisplayed and the participant asked to recognize those figures displayed originally (Recognition). Scoring on these tests ranges from 0 to 10 points.

For the present study, data related to performance on memory and performance prediction tests were analyzed. Sociodemographic data were used as independent variables and to characterize the sample.

Data analyses. The Chi-square test was employed to compare the categorical variables between groups. The absence of a normal distribution for the continuous variables dictated the use of non-parametric tests, namely, the Mann-Whitney and Kruskall-Wallis U-tests. When a p-value <0.05 was determined on the Kruskall-Wallis test, the Multiple Comparisons z values test was applied. Spearman Correlation analyses were also carried out.

For analysis of the relationship between predicted and objective performance on memory tests in the presence of sociodemographic variables, linear regression analysis was carried out with the multivariate model using Stepwise Forward variable selection criteria, i.e. from the most simple to most complex model. The variables yielding p<0.10 on univariate regression analyses were included in the final multiple models. The variables sex, age, schooling, family income in minimum wages and performance prediction were included in the models as independent variables whereas the cognitive variables from the BCSB were used as dependent variables in separate models (MMSE, Naming, Incidental Memory, Immediate Memory, Learning, Delayed Memory and Recognition).

The data were keyed into Version 3.1 of the Epidata Program. All statistical analyses were performed using the SPSS v.17.0 and Statistica v. 7.0 software packages. The level of significance adopted for the statistical tests was 5%, i.e. a p-value<0.05.

RESULTS

The sample comprised predominantly female participants, aged 65-75 years, married or in common law union and educated to primary school level (Table 1).

Table 1.
Data characterizing the sample (n=359).

Participants predicted recall of an average of five figures (SD=2.3) if asked to memorize 10 figures (Table 2). Participants scored an average of 23.90 (SD=3.56) on the MMSE and 7.49 (SD=1.97) on the Delayed Memory task. No significant difference in performance prediction was found between men and women. Table 2 reveals that men performed better than women on the MMSE (24.75(M) vs. 23.48 (W) p<0.001).

Table 2.
Mean and standard deviations for metamemory and cognitive performance variables for total sample and for men and women.

No significant difference in performance prediction was found for age or schooling (Table 3). Younger and more educated participants had better cognitive performance.

Table 3.
Mean and standard deviations for metamemory and cognitive performance variables among elderly from different age and schooling groups.

Table 4 depicts correlations, revealing a significant association among the cognitive variables. The performance prediction variable was moderately but significantly associated with Immediate Memory.

Table 4.
Correlation matrix between performance prediction and cognitive variables.

The multivariate regression analysis (Table 5) revealed that the cognitive variables were influenced by schooling, age and sex. Performance prediction had no significant influence on cognitive variables but was moderately associated with Immediate Memory.

Table 5.
Multiple linear regression analyses for sex, age, schooling, income and performance prediction as independent variables. Variable selection criteria: Stepwise Forward Modeling (p<0.10).

DISCUSSION

In the present study, no influence of sex, age or schooling on performance prediction was found. Performance prediction was moderately associated with Immediate Memory.

In contrast with the present findings, a previous study detected an association between performance prediction and sex. Hertzog, Dixon and Hultsch1212 Hertzog C, Dixon, RA, Hultsch DF. Relationships between metamemory, memory predictions, and memory task performance in adults. Psychol Aging 1990;5:215-227. reported that women showed a significant increase in their sequential performance predictions, i.e. they were able to monitor their performance and revise predictions more effectively than men.

In the present study, older participants did not predict lower performance, as might be expected given age-related decline in episodic memory. Previous studies have reported the influence of age on performance prediction but included younger adults, children and older adults in their samples.1212 Hertzog C, Dixon, RA, Hultsch DF. Relationships between metamemory, memory predictions, and memory task performance in adults. Psychol Aging 1990;5:215-227.

13 Cavanaugh JC, Poon LW. Metamemorial Predictors of Memory Performance in young and Older Adults. Psychol Aging 1989;4:365-368.
- 1414 Jopp, D, Hertzog C. Activities, Self-Referent Memory Beliefs, and Cognitive Performance: Evidence for Direct and Mediated Relations. Psychol Aging 2007;22:811-227.

With regard to schooling, elderly with higher educational level, who generally perform better on memory tasks, can be expected to have superior performance prediction. However, this expectation was not confirmed by the present data. Additionally, no association was found between predicted and objective performance on memory tasks, in contrast with previous international studies. In the present study, analysis of the distribution of scores for the performance prediction variable showed that a significant number of participants gave an intermediate prediction value (5). It is possible that faced with the difficulty of providing an estimate on future performance, participants chose a mid-point on the scale, which may partially explain the disparities with previous studies. The present study should encourage other researchers to investigate this aspect of metamemory and its relationship with performance on episodic memory tasks.

Myths and stereotypes associated with aging can potentially exert an influence on cognitive performance of elderly individuals. Future studies should further investigate the variables associated with the metamemory construct, which has been little investigated in Brazil's elderly population.

To conclude, sociodemographic variables did not influence predicted memory performance, which was only modestly associated with immediate memory on the BCSB.

Acknowledgements. We thank all those involved in the FIBRA Project for their assistance. This study is based on data collected in Ermelino Matarazzo. Financial support was provided by the National Research Council (CNPq), under process no.1467755/2011-1.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Salthouse TA. Influence of age on practice effects in longitudinal neurocognitive change. Neuropsychology 2010;24:563-572.
  • 2
    Flavell JH. First discussant's comments: What's memory development the development of? Hum Development 1971;14:272-278.
  • 3
    Cavanaugh JC, Feldman JM, Hertzog C. Memory beliefs as social cognition: A reconceptualization of what memory questionnaires assess. Rev Gen Psychol 1998;2:48-65.
  • 4
    Cavanaugh JC, Green EE. I believe, therefore I can: Self-efficacy beliefs in memory aging. In: E. A. Lovelace (Eds.), Aging and cognition: Mental processes, self awareness, and interventions. Amsterdam: Elsevier 1990:189-230.
  • 5
    Woo E, Schmitter-Edgecombe M, Fancher, JB. Memory Prediction Accuracy in younger and older adults: A Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Analysis. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2008;15:68-94.
  • 6
    Berry JM, West RL, Dennehey DM. Reliability and validity of the self-efficacy questionnaire. Dev Psychol 1989;25:701-713.
  • 7
    Mantyla T, Ronnlund M, Kliegel M. Components of executive functioning in metamemory. Appl Neuropsychol 2010;17:289-98.
  • 8
    Yassuda MS, Lasca VB, Neri AL. Metamemória e auto-eficácia: Um estudo de validação de Instrumento de pesquisa sobre memória e envelhecimento. Psicol Reflex Crít 2005;18:78-90.
  • 9
    Brucki SMD, Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Bertolucci PHF, Okamoto IH. Sugestões para o uso do mini-exame do estado mental no Brasil. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2003;61: 77-781.
  • 10
    Neri AL, Yassuda MS, de Araújo LF, et al. Methodology and social, demographic, cognitive, and frailty profiles of community-dwelling elderly from seven Brazilian cities: the FIBRA Study. Cad Saúde Pública 2013; 29:778-792.
  • 11
    Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Herrera JR, et al. Performance of illiterate and literate nondemented elderly subjects in two tests of long-term memory. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2004;10:634-638.
  • 12
    Hertzog C, Dixon, RA, Hultsch DF. Relationships between metamemory, memory predictions, and memory task performance in adults. Psychol Aging 1990;5:215-227.
  • 13
    Cavanaugh JC, Poon LW. Metamemorial Predictors of Memory Performance in young and Older Adults. Psychol Aging 1989;4:365-368.
  • 14
    Jopp, D, Hertzog C. Activities, Self-Referent Memory Beliefs, and Cognitive Performance: Evidence for Direct and Mediated Relations. Psychol Aging 2007;22:811-227.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Jan-Mar 2015

History

  • Received
    14 May 2014
  • Accepted
    03 Sept 2014
Academia Brasileira de Neurologia, Departamento de Neurologia Cognitiva e Envelhecimento R. Vergueiro, 1353 sl.1404 - Ed. Top Towers Offices, Torre Norte, São Paulo, SP, Brazil, CEP 04101-000, Tel.: +55 11 5084-9463 | +55 11 5083-3876 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistadementia@abneuro.org.br | demneuropsy@uol.com.br