Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Brazilian preliminary norms and investigation of age and education effects on the Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Stroop Color and Word test and Digit Span test in adults

Normas brasileiras preliminares e a investigação dos efeitos de idade e escolaridade no desempenho dos testes Wisconsin de Classificação de Cartas modificado, Stroop de Cores e Palavras e Dígitos em adultos

Abstracts

Executive functions are involved in a series of human neurological and psychiatric disorders. For this reason, appropriate assessment tools with age and education adjusted norms for symptom diagnosis are necessary.

OBJECTIVE:

To present normative data for adults (19-75 year-olds; with five years of education or more) on the Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (MWCST), Stroop color and word test and Digit Span test. Age and education effects were investigated.

METHODS:

Three samples were formed after inclusion criteria and data analysis: MWCST (n=124); Digit Span (n=123), and Stroop test (n=158). Groups were divided into young (19-39), middle-aged (40-59) and older (60-75) participants with five to eight years of education and nine years of education or more. Two-way ANOVA and ANCOVA analyses were used.

RESULTS:

Education effects were found in most variables of the three tasks. An age effect was only found on color naming and color-word naming speed from the Stroop test. No interactions were detected.

CONCLUSION:

In countries with heterogeneous educational backgrounds, the use of stratified norms by education to assess at least some components of executive functions is essential for an ethical and accurate cognitive diagnosis.

cognition; neuropsychological tests; age; schooling; executive function


As funções executivas estão envolvidas em uma série de distúrbios neurológicos e psiquiátricos humanos. Por esta razão, instrumentos de avaliação adequados com normas estratificadas por idade e escolaridade são necessárias para diagnóstico dos sintomas.

OBJETIVO:

Apresentar dados normativos para adultos (19 a 75 anos de idade; a partir de cinco anos de educação) no Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Modificado (MWCST), teste Stroop de cores e palavras e no teste de Dígitos. Os efeitos de idade e escolaridade foram investigados.

MÉTODOS:

Três amostras foram formadas após a análise dos critérios de inclusão e dos dados: MWCST (n=124); Dígitos (n=123) e teste Stroop (n=158). Os grupos foram divididos em jovens (19-39), de meia-idade (40-59) e idosos (60-75) de cinco a oito anos de ensino e nove anos de escolaridade ou mais. Foram utilizados os testes Two-way ANOVA e ANCOVA.

RESULTADOS:

Efeitos de educação foram encontrados na maioria das variáveis das três tarefas. O efeito idade foi encontrado somente na velocidade de nomeação de cores e nomeação de cor-palavra do teste Stroop. Não foram detectadas interações.

CONCLUSÃO:

Em países com cenários educacionais heterogêneos, o uso de normas estratificadas por educação para avaliar, pelo menos alguns componentes, das funções executivas é essencial para um diagnóstico cognitivo ético e preciso.

cognição; testes neuropsicológicos; idade; escolaridade; função executiva


INTRODUCTION

Executive functions (EF) are a series of mental processes recruited when automatic responses to problems or environmental demands are no longer sufficient. These processes are basically recognized as inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility, that comprise subcomponents and also lead to higher order cognitive processes, such as metacognition and problem-solving.11. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "Frontal Lobe" tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol 2000;41:49-100. , 22. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol 2013;64:135-168. Many factors are known to affect the course of EF in human life, such as social environment, emotional status, and physical health.33. Jiang H, Xu B. Reward enhances backward inhibition in task switching. J Cogn Psychol 2014;26:178-186. Consequently, EF impairments are highly associated with dysfunction in different life domains, e.g., employment and personal relationships.44. Ardila A. There are two different dysexecutive syndromes. J Neurol Disord 2013;01:1-4. Therefore, efforts in the neuropsychology area are necessary to provide assessment tools and performance parameters for assessments.55. Rognoni T, Casals-Coll M, Sánchez-Benavides G, et al. Spanish normative studies in young adults (NEURONORMA young adults project): norms for Stroop Color-Word Interference and Tower of London-Drexel University tests. Neurologia 2013;28:73-80.

In spite of the popularity of standardized EF tests in the international literature, few or limited norms exist for the Brazilian population. This topic is especially relevant because normative data from foreign countries cannot be used, since education and culture have an impact on overall cognitive performance. In a study, Ardila, Ostrosky-Solís, Rosseli, and Goméz66. Ardila A, Ostrosky-Solis F, Rosselli M, Gómez C. Age-related cognitive decline during normal aging: the complex effect of education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2000;15:495-513. described the complex effect of education during aging. The complex effect consisted of a different role of education across the lifespan, which means education does not have the same effect on cognitive performance in different age ranges. However, absence of interaction between these variables was also found.77. Buriel Y, Gramunt-Fombuena N, Böhm P, Rodés E, Peña-Casanova J. Fluencia verbal: Estudio normativo piloto en una muestra española de adultos jóvenes (20 a 49 años). Neurología 2004;19:153-159. , 88. Van der Elst W, Van Boxtel MPJ, Van Breukelen GJP, Jolles J. The Stroop color-word test: influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment 2006;13:62-79. Rosseli and Ardila99. Rosselli M, Ardila A. The impact of culture and education on non-verbal neuropsychological measurements: A critical review. Brain Cogn 2003; 52:326-333. presented evidence of a cultural effect even on non-verbal task performance. In view of this evidence, the use of foreign normative data may lead to erroneous diagnosis in clinical and research practice.1010. Fonseca RP, Oliveira CR De, Ortiz KZ, Parente MA de MP, Scherer LC. Adaptação de Instrumentos Neuropsicológicos Verbais: Um Fluxograma de Procedimentos para Além da Tradução. Interação em Psicol. 2011;15:59-69.

Some studies have been published in Brazil, mainly over the last decade. Duncan1111. Duncan MT. Assessment of normative data of Stroop test performance in a group of elementary school students in Niterói. J Bras Psiquiatr 2006;55:42-48. published norms for the Stroop color and word test for teenagers in the 12-14 year age bracket, while Klein, Adda, Miotto, Lucia, and Scaff1212. Klein M, Adda CC, Miotto EC, Lucia MCS de, Scaff M. O paradigma stroop em uma amostra de idosos brasileiros. Psicol Hosp [Internet]. Centro de Estudos Psicologia da Saúde da Divisão de Psicologia ICHCFMUSP; 2010;8:93-112. published norms for elderly in the 60-85 year age range with 2 to 18 years of education. Campanholo et al.1313. Campanholo KR, Romão MA, Machado MDAR, et al. Performance of an adult Brazilian sample on the Trail Making Test and Stroop Test. Dement Neuropsychol. 2014;8:26-31. reported normative data for the Stroop test and found age and education effects on a large sample of individuals with ages ranging from 18 to 70 years or older and education from zero to 13 years or more. Ávila1414. Ávila KN. Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (MCST): desempenho de idosos com diferentes níveis de escolaridade. Psicol Hosp 2007; 5:21-35. presented normative data for elderly aged 70-73 years for the Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (MWCST). Figueiredo and Nascimento1515. Figueiredo VLM, Nascimento E. Desempenhos nas duas tarefas do subteste Dígitos do WISC-III e do WAIS-III. Psicol Teor Pesq 2007;23: 313-318. published normative data for children and adolescents (age range 6-16 years) and adolescents and adults with aged 16-89 years on the Digit Span. In conclusion, the normative studies available for the Brazilian population presented above involve samples with restricted age groups and/or not stratified by years of formal education. For this reason, the aim of the present study was to present normative data for adults aged 19-75 years on the MWCST, Stroop color and word test and Digit Span tests with different educational levels (five to eight years and nine years or more). In addition, age and education effects and interactions between these factors were investigated.

METHODS

Participants. The participants included in this study were community-dwelling volunteers recruited by convenience and contacted at local business companies and community centers. None of the participants received payment for participating in the research. The Institutional Review Board of the university approved the project and all individuals signed an informed consent document. Inclusion criteria consisted of being a native speaker of Brazilian Portuguese, aged 19-75 years and having 5 years or more of formal education. Groups were divided into young (19-39), middle-aged (40-59) and older (60-75) participants with five to eight years of education (low education) and nine years of education or more (high education). After fulfillment of all inclusion criteria, a total of 124 individuals performed the MWCST; 180 the Stroop color and word test; and 160 the Digit Span test. Following data analysis, some participants were excluded for demonstrating lack of comprehension of test instructions (e.g., 35/42 perseverative errors on the MWCST, no correct answers on Digit Span backwards), 42 participants were excluded from the final sample for the MWCST; 38 from the Stroop color and word test; and 15 from the Digit Span task. For this reason, the normative studies involved different sample sizes, as follows: MWCST (n=124); Digit Span (n=123); and Stroop color and word test (n=158).

Individuals self-reporting psychiatric or neurological problems, previous or current abuse of alcohol, cigarettes, illegal drugs or psychoactive medications were excluded. In order to exclude abnormal aging-related cognitive decline, the Mini-Mental State Examination1616. Chaves ML, Izquierdo I. Differential diagnosis between dementia and depression: a study of efficiency increment. Acta Neurol Scand 1992; 11:412-429. was administered to individuals aged 40 years and older. All study participants reported no complaints of vision and hearing in order to undergo the tasks. Clinical symptoms of depression were assessed by the Geriatric Depression Scale1717. Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res 1983;17:37-42. , 1818. Almeida OP, Almeida SA. Confiabilidade da versão brasileira da Escala de Depressão em Geriatria (GDS) versão reduzida. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 1999;57:421-426., a scale with yes/no answers. See Table 1, 2, and 3 for sociodemographic, cultural and clinical data of each group.

Table 1.
Sociodemographic, cultural and clinical characteristics of preliminary normative groups for Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.
Table 2.
Sociodemographic, cultural and clinical characteristics of preliminary normative groups for Digit span test.
Table 3.
Sociodemographic, cultural and clinical characteristics of preliminary normative groups for Stroop color and word test.

Instruments. The three tests presented below were administered as part of a larger cognitive test battery of a normative project. The tests were administered and scored by trained research assistants. A double-check method was applied in scoring and data base typing in order to improve reliability of the data presented. All instruments were adapted to the Brazilian Portuguese language following the steps proposed by Fonseca et al.1010. Fonseca RP, Oliveira CR De, Ortiz KZ, Parente MA de MP, Scherer LC. Adaptação de Instrumentos Neuropsicológicos Verbais: Um Fluxograma de Procedimentos para Além da Tradução. Interação em Psicol. 2011;15:59-69.

Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (MWCST). The MWCST first proposed by Nelson (1976) consists of 48 cards containing geometric designs that vary in color, form or number. The original rapport and scoring system were adapted to the Brazilian Portuguese language and culture with help from the author of the adaptation of the traditional WCST for use in Brazil.1919. Trentini CM, Argimon IIDL, Silva M, Werlang BG. O desenvolvimento de normas para o Teste Wisconsin de Classificação de Cartas. Aval Psicol 2006;5:247-250. Four reference cards are presented to the individuals who must sort the desk cards in color, form or number. However, the sorting rules are not given to participants, such that they must infer by means of examiner feedback whether their strategy is right or wrong. No order of category sorting is required and the participant is told when the rule changes after six correct trials. The deck does not contain ambiguous cards. The scores considered for the task were: categories completed, perseverative errors, non-perseverative errors, and failure to maintain set.

Digit span.2020. Wechsler D. The measurement of adult intelligence scale. Baltimore, MD: Williams and Wilkins; 1944.,2121. Nascimento E do. Adaptação, validação e normatização do WAIS-III para uma amostra brasileira. In: Wechsler D, editor. WAIS III:manual para administração e avaliação. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2004. On this task version the rapport was modified and the number order was pseudo-randomized in order to avoid repetitions. This task consists of two parts. Digits forward is administered first and requires the repetition of digits in the same order presented, while in digits backwards participants must repeat digits in an inverse or backwards order. Part B is administered even if the participant fails in Part A.1515. Figueiredo VLM, Nascimento E. Desempenhos nas duas tarefas do subteste Dígitos do WISC-III e do WAIS-III. Psicol Teor Pesq 2007;23: 313-318. Also, Part A must be applied before Part B to enable the assessment of cognitive flexibility components. Correct answers, span scores and discrepancy analysis between parts A and B for accuracy were included in the analysis.

Stroop Color and Word Test.2222. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 1935;18:643-662. On this task, adaptations were made to rapport, frequency and length of colors, and their respective syllables. The version used consists of a word page (black printed words "pink", "blue" and "green"), color page ("X" letter printed in pink, blue and green) and color-word page with the words presented on the first page with the colors printed on the second page, but colors and words do not match. Participants were asked to read 100 words distributed under five columns equally on a white sheet of paper. Each page should be read in 45 seconds maximum. Errors were considered for scoring, but not discounted from the correct answers, since examiner asked for errors to be corrected when they occurred. Variables considered for analysis were the correct answers for the word, color and color-word pages.

Data analysis. Normality of data was analyzed with the Shapiro-Wilk test by stratified groups of age and education. Some variables, although not all, were considered non-parametric after testing. For this reason, parametric and non-parametric tests were conducted and results revealed equivalent directions. Normative groups were compared for years of age, education, reading and writing habits, MMSE score, GDS-30 and GDS-15 by means of a univariate Analysis of Variance (ANOVA); Chi-square analysis was employed to compare groups for sex. Age and education effects were investigated by a two-way ANOVA (MWCST and Digit span) or ANCOVA (Stroop color and word test; with sex as covariate) using post-hoc Bonferroni correction. Effect sizes were calculated for groups with different sample sizes (Cohen's d). Significance level was considered if p≤0.05.

RESULTS

Descriptive data from normative samples. In the MWCST preliminary standardization sample (Table 1), age comparison among groups revealed all groups with high education were younger than the same age-range groups with low education. All groups with the same age-range differed on years of education, and groups of with same education-range did not differ in this regard. The oldest group with high education had lower GDS scores than the younger group with low education (GDS-30 and 15) and the 40-59 years group with high education (GDS-15). Frequency of reading and writing habits was significantly higher in the oldest group with high education than the younger group with low education and the 40-59 years old group with low education.

Table 2 (Digit span data) indicates that the young group with low education was significantly older than the same age-range group with high education. The groups with age range of 40-59 years old with high and low education had equivalent mean age; the same occurred with the 60-75 years group. As expected, the groups with the same age range had significantly different mean years of education, . With regards to GDS-30 score, only the youngest group with low education showed higher scores compared to the oldest group with high education. GDS-15 score of the young group with low education was higher than the same age range group with high education and the two oldest groups. Also, the 40-59 years group with high education showed higher GDS-15 scores than the 60-75 years old group with high education. The frequency of reading and writing habits indicated that the youngest group with low education read less frequently than the 40-59 and 60-75 years groups with high education; whereas the 40-59 years group with low education read less frequently than the oldest group with high education.

Post-hoc analysis of Table 3 with the Stroop test data revealed that young with low education were significantly older than individuals from the same age group with high education. This result was expected since low education has progressively decreased in Brazil with time. Years of education were different among all contrasting education groups. In addition, GDS scores were significantly lower in young with low education compared to 60-75 year-olds with high education. Finally, the oldest group with high education had higher scores for reading and writing frequency compared to low education groups of all ages.

Age and education effects. In Table 4, normative data of the three tasks are presented. Age and education effects and effect sizes are available in Table 5.

Table 4.
Means and standard deviation data for performance on MWCST, Digit span and Stroop color and word test.

Table 5.
Age and education effects on performance of MWCST, Digit span, and Stroop color and word test.

DISCUSSION

The aim of the present study was to investigate the role of age and education in the performance of three executive functions tasks (MWCST, Digit Span, and Stroop color and word test) among adults in the age range of 19-75 years with five years or more of education in a southern Brazilian sample. In addition, preliminary normative data were presented for these tasks. Our data revealed an education effect on almost all variables analyzed. Age effects were observed only for two variables from the Stroop test. No interactions between age and education were found. Implications for clinical and research use of these instruments are discussed below.

Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. In the present study, education had a single and main effect on MWCST variables of categories completed (CC), perseverative errors (PE), non-perseverative errors (NPE), and failure to maintain set (FMS). This indicates that age groups had similar performance on this task. This finding differs to the results of previous studies using the modified version and the original Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). In a meta-analysis review, Rhodes2323. Rhodes MG. Age-related differences in performance on the Wisconsin card sorting test: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Aging 2004;19:482-494. reported that age had a strong effect on the original WCST and MWCST versions. A study investigating flexibility and working memory contributions to WCST performance found that age-related errors were in fact due to decreased working memory performance in older individuals, not flexibility.2424. Hartman M, Bolton E, Fehnel SE. Accounting for age differences on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: Decreased working memory, not inflexibility. Psychol Aging 2001;16:385-399. Another study with structural neuroimaging, proposed that age-related increase in PE on the WCST could be explained by a decrease in processing speed, temporal processing, and working memory, which are mediated by the volume of the prefrontal cortex.2525. Head D, Kennedy KM, Rodrigue KM, Raz N. Age differences in perseveration: cognitive and neuroanatomical mediators of performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Neuropsychologia 2009;47:1200-1203. However, the role of education in performance on the MWCST has been previously reported. Chan et al. (2003), when using the MWCST, found significant effects of education on groups with two years or less, three to nine years and ten years or more of formal education. The three groups had different performance to each other in CC and total errors. The less educated group had a significantly different performance to the other two groups in NPE and finally for PE, the most educated group had a significantly different performance compared to the two less educated groups. These results suggest that CC may be the variable which best discriminates groups with different educational levels, followed by the NPE and PE. Taken together, these findings may indicate that the cognitive demands required for the MWCST differ slightly to those of the original WCST and that age and education effects may also differ.

Digit Span test. Education also showed a single and main effect on Digit Span performance in both forward and backwards forms. A previous study of Hispanic adults in the 16-75 year age range also found no effect of age in the presence of an education effect on the forwards and backwards Digit Span.2626. Pontón MO, Satz P, Herrera L, Ortiz F, et al. Normative data stratified by age and education for the Neuropsychological Screening Battery for Hispanics (NeSBHIS): Initial report. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1996;2:96-104. Ostrosky-Solís and Lozano2727. Ostrosky-Solís F, Lozano A. Digit Span: Effect of education and culture. Int J Psychol 2006;41:333-341. also found education was the stronger predictor of performance on the forward and backward Digit Span in a Spanish-speaking sample. However, the authors also found a cultural effect on these results; another study based on the original North American sample of WAIS-III found independent effects for age and education. An explanation for these findings may be related to the education ranges of each study, as it is known that education level in developing countries are lower than in North America.2727. Ostrosky-Solís F, Lozano A. Digit Span: Effect of education and culture. Int J Psychol 2006;41:333-341. , 2828. Hester RL, Kinsella GJ, Ong B. Effect of age on forward and backward span tasks. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2004;10:475-481.

Stroop Color and Word test. Stroop color and word test data demonstrated independent age and education effects on Color and Color-word pages. For both variables, high education individuals performed better than low educated peers, while the younger group performed better than the older group. The results from the Stroop color and word test indicate that the automatic ability of reading is affected only by education, while less automatic abilities, such as naming colors, and inhibition and flexibility required in Color-word page, are affected by both age and education separately. In previous studies, separate age and education effects were found in an elderly sample (60-90 years old)2929. Seo EH, Lee DY, Choo H, et al. Normative study of the Stroop Color and Word Test in an educationally diverse elderly population. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2008;23:1020-1027. and in a middle-age and elderly sample (50-90 years old),3030. Peña-Casanova J, Quiñones-Ubeda S, Gramunt-Fombuena N, et al. Spanish Multicenter Normative Studies (NEURONORMA Project): norms for the Stroop color-word interference test and the Tower of London-Drexel. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2009;24:413-429. and an education effect (with no age effect) was found in a younger sample (18-49 years old).55. Rognoni T, Casals-Coll M, Sánchez-Benavides G, et al. Spanish normative studies in young adults (NEURONORMA young adults project): norms for Stroop Color-Word Interference and Tower of London-Drexel University tests. Neurologia 2013;28:73-80.

Conclusion. Our findings have some important theoretical implications for the study of sociodemographic variables in cognition, especially with regards to executive functions. As shown by the previous studies described above, education seemed to be more critical than age when comparing group performances, especially for individuals aged up to 75 years old. Moreover, results presented here reinforce the importance for clinicians and researchers to consider age and education variables when discussing neuropsychological performance in order to avoid false positive or false negative errors.

Some limitations of this study are the inclusion of broad age and education ranges, which may have introduced bias, especially to the few age-related findings in the samples. Secondly, the sample size might be considered small, and consequently these results are preliminary. Considering the importance of ecological validity for executive function tests, we suggest that future studies include larger samples with broader age and education variances together with cognitive functional scales or ecological tests. More specifically, oldest-old samples and groups stratified by the quality of education years are relevant demands to be attended in the public Brazilian health and education systems.

Acknowledgement.

We thank Gigiane Gindri, Camila Rosa de Oliveira, Francéia Veiga Lietdke, Murilo Zibetti and Jaqueline Rodrigues for their work on data collection and for organizing the initial database.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "Frontal Lobe" tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol 2000;41:49-100.
  • 2
    Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol 2013;64:135-168.
  • 3
    Jiang H, Xu B. Reward enhances backward inhibition in task switching. J Cogn Psychol 2014;26:178-186.
  • 4
    Ardila A. There are two different dysexecutive syndromes. J Neurol Disord 2013;01:1-4.
  • 5
    Rognoni T, Casals-Coll M, Sánchez-Benavides G, et al. Spanish normative studies in young adults (NEURONORMA young adults project): norms for Stroop Color-Word Interference and Tower of London-Drexel University tests. Neurologia 2013;28:73-80.
  • 6
    Ardila A, Ostrosky-Solis F, Rosselli M, Gómez C. Age-related cognitive decline during normal aging: the complex effect of education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2000;15:495-513.
  • 7
    Buriel Y, Gramunt-Fombuena N, Böhm P, Rodés E, Peña-Casanova J. Fluencia verbal: Estudio normativo piloto en una muestra española de adultos jóvenes (20 a 49 años). Neurología 2004;19:153-159.
  • 8
    Van der Elst W, Van Boxtel MPJ, Van Breukelen GJP, Jolles J. The Stroop color-word test: influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment 2006;13:62-79.
  • 9
    Rosselli M, Ardila A. The impact of culture and education on non-verbal neuropsychological measurements: A critical review. Brain Cogn 2003; 52:326-333.
  • 10
    Fonseca RP, Oliveira CR De, Ortiz KZ, Parente MA de MP, Scherer LC. Adaptação de Instrumentos Neuropsicológicos Verbais: Um Fluxograma de Procedimentos para Além da Tradução. Interação em Psicol. 2011;15:59-69.
  • 11
    Duncan MT. Assessment of normative data of Stroop test performance in a group of elementary school students in Niterói. J Bras Psiquiatr 2006;55:42-48.
  • 12
    Klein M, Adda CC, Miotto EC, Lucia MCS de, Scaff M. O paradigma stroop em uma amostra de idosos brasileiros. Psicol Hosp [Internet]. Centro de Estudos Psicologia da Saúde da Divisão de Psicologia ICHCFMUSP; 2010;8:93-112.
  • 13
    Campanholo KR, Romão MA, Machado MDAR, et al. Performance of an adult Brazilian sample on the Trail Making Test and Stroop Test. Dement Neuropsychol. 2014;8:26-31.
  • 14
    Ávila KN. Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (MCST): desempenho de idosos com diferentes níveis de escolaridade. Psicol Hosp 2007; 5:21-35.
  • 15
    Figueiredo VLM, Nascimento E. Desempenhos nas duas tarefas do subteste Dígitos do WISC-III e do WAIS-III. Psicol Teor Pesq 2007;23: 313-318.
  • 16
    Chaves ML, Izquierdo I. Differential diagnosis between dementia and depression: a study of efficiency increment. Acta Neurol Scand 1992; 11:412-429.
  • 17
    Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res 1983;17:37-42.
  • 18
    Almeida OP, Almeida SA. Confiabilidade da versão brasileira da Escala de Depressão em Geriatria (GDS) versão reduzida. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 1999;57:421-426.
  • 19
    Trentini CM, Argimon IIDL, Silva M, Werlang BG. O desenvolvimento de normas para o Teste Wisconsin de Classificação de Cartas. Aval Psicol 2006;5:247-250.
  • 20
    Wechsler D. The measurement of adult intelligence scale. Baltimore, MD: Williams and Wilkins; 1944.
  • 21
    Nascimento E do. Adaptação, validação e normatização do WAIS-III para uma amostra brasileira. In: Wechsler D, editor. WAIS III:manual para administração e avaliação. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2004.
  • 22
    Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 1935;18:643-662.
  • 23
    Rhodes MG. Age-related differences in performance on the Wisconsin card sorting test: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Aging 2004;19:482-494.
  • 24
    Hartman M, Bolton E, Fehnel SE. Accounting for age differences on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: Decreased working memory, not inflexibility. Psychol Aging 2001;16:385-399.
  • 25
    Head D, Kennedy KM, Rodrigue KM, Raz N. Age differences in perseveration: cognitive and neuroanatomical mediators of performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Neuropsychologia 2009;47:1200-1203.
  • 26
    Pontón MO, Satz P, Herrera L, Ortiz F, et al. Normative data stratified by age and education for the Neuropsychological Screening Battery for Hispanics (NeSBHIS): Initial report. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1996;2:96-104.
  • 27
    Ostrosky-Solís F, Lozano A. Digit Span: Effect of education and culture. Int J Psychol 2006;41:333-341.
  • 28
    Hester RL, Kinsella GJ, Ong B. Effect of age on forward and backward span tasks. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2004;10:475-481.
  • 29
    Seo EH, Lee DY, Choo H, et al. Normative study of the Stroop Color and Word Test in an educationally diverse elderly population. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2008;23:1020-1027.
  • 30
    Peña-Casanova J, Quiñones-Ubeda S, Gramunt-Fombuena N, et al. Spanish Multicenter Normative Studies (NEURONORMA Project): norms for the Stroop color-word interference test and the Tower of London-Drexel. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2009;24:413-429.
  • Funding. Fundação de Amparo a Pesquisa do Rio Grande do Sul (Brazil) and Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (Brazil).
  • This study was conducted at the Psychology Faculty, Pontifical Catholic University of Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Brazil

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Apr-Jun 2015

History

  • Received
    27 Feb 2015
  • Accepted
    30 Apr 2015
Academia Brasileira de Neurologia, Departamento de Neurologia Cognitiva e Envelhecimento R. Vergueiro, 1353 sl.1404 - Ed. Top Towers Offices, Torre Norte, São Paulo, SP, Brazil, CEP 04101-000, Tel.: +55 11 5084-9463 | +55 11 5083-3876 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistadementia@abneuro.org.br | demneuropsy@uol.com.br