Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Cognitive performance and engagement in physical, social and intellectual activities in older adults: The FIBRA study

DESEMPENHO COGNITIVO E ENVOLVIMENTO EM ATIVIDADES FÍSICAS, SOCIAIS E INTELECTUAIS EM IDOSOS: ESTUDO FIBRA

Abstracts

Cognitive decline in aging can negatively impact quality of life in the elderly. However, studies have shown that elderly engaged in advanced activities of daily living (AADLs) can maintain or enhance global cognitive function or specific domains.

Objective:

To investigate the relationship between engagement in AADLs and domains of cognition in elderly from seven different locations in Brazil.

Methods:

A cross-sectional study involving 2,549 elderly without cognitive deficits suggestive of dementia was conducted. Data were collected on sociodemographic characteristics, health status, the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) by subdomain (orientation, memory, attention/calculus, language and constructional praxis), and engagement in AADL grouped under physical, social and intellectual activities.

Results:

Multivariate linear regression analysis revealed an association, albeit modest, between intellectual AADLs and the domains orientation, attention/calculus, language and constructional praxis (R2=0.005, 0.008, 0.021, and 0.021 respectively). Social AADLs were correlated with memory (R2=0.002) and language (R2=0.004) domains. No association was found between physical AADLs and MMSE domains. Schooling and family income were the sociodemographic variables exhibiting the strongest relationship with cognitive domains.

Conclusion:

The study found associations between intellectual and social AADLs with higher cognitive performance, suggesting that active aging can provide opportunities to attenuate cognitive decline in aging.

activities of daily living; social participation; motor activity; cognition; elderly


O declínio cognitivo no envelhecimento pode interferir na qualidade de vida do idoso. No entanto, estudos tem documentado que idosos engajados em atividades avançadas de vida diária (AAVD) podem manter ou melhorar a função cognitiva global ou domínios específicos.

Objetivo:

Investigar a relação entre o engajamento em AAVD e domínios da cognição em idosos residentes em sete localidades brasileiras.

Métodos:

Participaram de estudo transversal 2.549 idosos sem déficits cognitivos sugestivos de demência. Foram coletados dados referentes às características sociodemográficas, de saúde, Mini Exame do Estado Mental (MEEM) subdividido em subdomínios (orientação, memória, atenção/ cálculo, linguagem e praxia construtiva) e o engajamento em AAVD agrupadas em físicas, sociais e intelectuais.

Resultados:

Embora modesta a análise de regressão linear multivariada mostrou associação entre as AAVD intelectuais e os domínios de orientação, atenção/ cálculo, linguagem e praxia construtiva (R2=0.005, 0.008, 0.021, 0.021 respectivamente). As AAVD sociais se associaram aos domínios de memória (R2=0.002) e linguagem (R2=0.004). Não houve associação entre a as AAVD físicas e os domínios do MEEM. Escolaridade e renda familiar foram as variáveis sociodemográficas que apresentaram relação mais robusta com os domínios cognitivos.

Conclusão:

O estudo mostrou associação entre as AAVD intelectuais e sociais e o melhor desempenho cognitivo, sugerindo que o envelhecimento ativo pode oferecer oportunidades para amenizar o declínio cognitivo no envelhecimento.

atividades cotidianas; participação social; atividade motora; cognição; idoso


INTRODUCTION

Among the morphophysiological and functional changes brought on by aging, cognitive perfor-

mance is affected and some cognitive abilities decline.11 Salthouse TA. Selective review of cognitive aging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010;16:754-760. These changes are considered normal and generally do not affect global functioning.11 Salthouse TA. Selective review of cognitive aging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010;16:754-760. Changes are often seen in attention, information processing speed, performance of working and episodic memory, and in executive functions.11 Salthouse TA. Selective review of cognitive aging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010;16:754-760.,21 Salthouse TA. Localizing age-related individual differences in a hierarchical structure. Intelligence 2004;32:541-561. However, individual differences, life-style and sociodemographic variables can lead to more marked cognitive decline33 López Perez-Dias AGL, Calero MD, Navarro-González E. Prediction of cognitive impairment in the elderly by analyzing their performance in verbal fluency and in sustained attention. Rev Neurol 2013;56:1-7.,44 Foss MP, Formigheri P, Speciali JG. Heterogeneity of cognitive aging in Brazilian normal elderly. Dement Neuropsychol 2009;3:344-351. and increase the risk of dementia which negatively impact autonomy, independence and quality of life of the elderly.55 Marventano S, Prietro-Flores ME, Sanz-Barbero B, et al. Quality of life in older people with dementia: a multilevel study of individual attributes and residential care center characteristics. Geriatr Gerontol Int 2015; 15:104-110.,66 Argimon IIL. Aspectos cognitivos em idosos. Avaliação Psicológica 2006;5:243-245.

In aging, it is necessary to differentiate normal from pathological cognitive decline, as early diagnosis of significant changes in cognitive performance allows better treatment, management and therapeutic planning for the elderly patient and their family members, as well as enabling interventions for prevention and for promotion of mental health in the elderly.77 Aprahamian I, Martinelli JE, Neri AL, et al. The accuracy of the Clock Drawing Test compared to that of standard screening tests for Alzheimer's disease: results from a study of Brazilian elderly with heteregeneous educational backgrounds. Int Psychogeriatr 2010;22:64-71.,88 de Oliveira GM, Yokomizo JE, E Silva LD, et al. The applicability of the cognitive abilities screening instrument-short (CASI-S) in primary in Brazil. Int Psychogeriatr 2015;29:1-7. Recent studies have explored the relationship between engagement in advanced activities of daily living (AADLs) and cognitive performance, as a means of favoring successful healthy aging.99 Wang HX, Xu W, Pei JJ. Leisure activities, cognition and dementia. Biochim Biophys Acta 2012;1822:482-491.,1010 Dias EG, Duarte YAO, Morgani MH, et al. As atividades de vida diária como componente da avaliação funcional do idoso. Rev Ter Ocup Univ São Paulo 2014;25:225-232. AADLs are part of the competencies related to daily functioning, but considered more complex as they depend on motivation and preservation of a group of physical and cognitive competencies enabling independent functioning and social participation in broader settings.1010 Dias EG, Duarte YAO, Morgani MH, et al. As atividades de vida diária como componente da avaliação funcional do idoso. Rev Ter Ocup Univ São Paulo 2014;25:225-232.

11 Reuben DB, Laliberte L, Hiris, et al. A hierarchical exercise scale to measure function at the Advanced Activities of Daily Living (AADL) level. J Am Geriatr Soc 1990;38:855-861.
-1212 Rubenstein LV, Calkins DR, Greenfield S, et al. Health status assessment for elderly patients. Report of the Society of General Internal Medicine Task Force on Health Assessment. J Am Geriatr Soc 1989;37:562-569. Discontinuation of these activities can be an early marker of dependence for carrying out instrumental activities of daily living.1111 Reuben DB, Laliberte L, Hiris, et al. A hierarchical exercise scale to measure function at the Advanced Activities of Daily Living (AADL) level. J Am Geriatr Soc 1990;38:855-861.

12 Rubenstein LV, Calkins DR, Greenfield S, et al. Health status assessment for elderly patients. Report of the Society of General Internal Medicine Task Force on Health Assessment. J Am Geriatr Soc 1989;37:562-569.
-1313 Minhat HS, Mohd Amin RM. Sociodemographic determinants of leisure participation among elderly in Malaysia. J Community Health 2012; 37:840-847. Engagement in AADLs benefits health, autonomy, functioning and well-being of the elderly population.1414 Rizzuto D, Orsini N, Qiu C, et al. Lifestyle, social factors, and survival after age 75: population based study. BMJ 2012;345:1-10. Such activities are known to play a protective role against cognitive decline and in the prevention and progress of dementia.99 Wang HX, Xu W, Pei JJ. Leisure activities, cognition and dementia. Biochim Biophys Acta 2012;1822:482-491.

Involvement in different types of complex activities favors and reflects good functioning of various cognitive domains. Intellectual and physical AADLs have been linked to superior results on tests of episodic memory, executive function and language.1515 Wang HX, Jin Y, Hendrie HC, et al. Late life leisure activities and risk of cognitive decline. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2013;68:205-213.

16 Wilson RS, et al. Cognitive activity and incident AD in a populationbased sample of older persons. Neurology 2002;59:1910-1914.
-1717 Barnes DE, Yaffe K, Satariano WA, et al. A Longitudinal Study of cardiorespiratory fitness and cognitive functional in healthy older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 2003;51:459-465. Engagement in social AADLs is associated with less decline in episodic, semantic and working memory, perception speed, visuospatial ability and global cognition in the aging process.1717 Barnes DE, Yaffe K, Satariano WA, et al. A Longitudinal Study of cardiorespiratory fitness and cognitive functional in healthy older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 2003;51:459-465.

Despite the fact that there is evidence of beneficial effects of performing AADLs on various cognitive functions, no systematic assessments investigating the influence of the frequency, intensity and duration of AADLs on cognition are currently available. In addition, there is a lack of consensus over which activities make up this construct, and also on their classification into specific domains.99 Wang HX, Xu W, Pei JJ. Leisure activities, cognition and dementia. Biochim Biophys Acta 2012;1822:482-491.,1919 Ghisletta P, McArdle JJ, Lindenberger U. Longitudinal cognition-survival relations in old and very old age. 13-year data from the Berlin Aging Study. European Psychologist 2006;11:204-223.

20 Wang JY, Zhou DH, Li J, et al. Leisure activity and risk of cognitive impairment: the Chongqing Aging Study. Neurology 2006;66:911-913.
-2121 Aartsen MJ, Smits CH, van Tilburg T, et al. Activity in older adults: cause or consequence of cognitive functioning? A longitudinal study on everyday activities and cognitive performance in older adults J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 57:153-162. Information on which cognitive domains benefit most from the practice of AADLs by elderly remains scarce1515 Wang HX, Jin Y, Hendrie HC, et al. Late life leisure activities and risk of cognitive decline. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2013;68:205-213. and also which modality or modalities (physical, social or intellectual) of AADL most influence cognition.

Therefore, the objective of the present study was to examine the relationship between engagement in physical, social and intellectual AADLs, and performance on the subdomains of the MMSE (orientation, memory, attention and calculus, language and praxis), controlling for the effect of sociodemographic (age, gender, schooling and family income) and health (number of diseases and depressive symptoms) variables in communitydwelling elderly.

METHODS

This investigation drew on data from the FIBRA study (Study Network on Frailty in Brazilian Elderly - Unicamp). FIBRA is a multicenter, cross-sectional cohort study whose purpose is to investigate frailty profiles and relationships between this condition and sociodemographic, biological and psychosocial variables in urban community-dwelling elderly aged 65 years or older. The FIBRA study involved representative samples of elderly from seven different locations in Brazil. In each location, simple random sampling of urban census sectors was carried out where the sample size was calculated based on the ratio between the number of elderly envisaged and the universe of urban census sectors.2222 Neri AL, Yassuda MS, Araujo LF, et al. Metodologia e perfil sociodemográfico, cognitivo e fragilidade de idosos comunitários de sete cidades brasileiras: Estudo FIBRA. Cad Saúde Pública 2013;29:778-792. Data were collected between June 2008 and September 2009.

The eligibility criteria used for recruitment, performed at households by trained personnel based on a pre-established script within each randomly selected census sector, were as follows: age 65 years or older, permanent residence at the domicile within the census sector, and no severe cognitive, communication, sensory or mobility deficits. The exclusion criteria adopted were:2323 Ferrucci L, Guralnik JM, Studenski S, et al. Designing randomized, controlled trials aimed at preventing or delaying functional decline and disability in frail, older persons: a consensus report. J Am Geritr Soc 2004;52:625-634. [a] problems affecting memory, attention, spatial or temporal orientation, and communication suggestive of dementia, or mention of this diagnosis reached by a physician, as reported by a family member; [b] temporarily or permanently bedridden; [c] stroke sequela, such as localized weakness or aphasia; [d] Parkinson's Disease with severely impaired motricity, speech or affectivity; [e] severe visual or auditory loss, hampering communication; [f] terminal stage.

Data were collected during a single session of between 40 and 120 minutes conducted in a public, easily accessible place at an address, time and day arranged with the elder at the time of recruitment. During the session, measurements were taken for sociodemographic, anthropometric, clinical and frailty variables and dementia screening were carried out. The Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE)2424 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive status of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189-198. was applied to detect cognitive impairment, using the mean cut-off scores for levels of schooling suggested by Brucki et al. (2003)2525 Brucki SMD, Nitrini R, Caramelli P, et al. Sugestões para o uso do mini-exame do estado mental no Brasil. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2003;61: 777-781. minus one standard-deviation (17 for uneducated elderly; 22 for elderly with between 1 and 4 years of schooling; 24 for 5 to 8 years; and 26 for elderly with 9 or more years of schooling). Elderly scoring over the MMSE cut-off proceeded to additional self-report measures of physical health status, functioning, social engagement, depressive symptoms and life satisfaction. Only elderly who scored over the MMSE cut-off subsequently answered the questions on practice of AADLs, where these participants constituted the sample of the present analysis.

All participants signed a free and informed consent form for the study approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the School of Medical Sciences of Unicamp (report 208/2007, CAAE 39547014.0.10001.5404).

Participants. A total of 2,549 elderly with no cognitive deficits suggestive of dementia, selected from the universe of participants of the FIBRA Unicamp Study (n=3.478) based on the above-mentioned cognitive criteria took part in the present study. Of the 2,549 participants, 568 (22.28%) were from Belém (PA), 294 (11.53%) Parnaíba (PI), 239 (9.38%) Campina Grande (PB), 316 (12.40%) Poços de Caldas (MG), 673 (26.40%) Campinas (SP), 300 (11.77%) from the sub-district of Ermelino Matarazzo, in São Paulo (SP) and 159 (6.24%) from Ivoti (RS),

Instruments and measures

  1. Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE).2424 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive status of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189-198.,2525 Brucki SMD, Nitrini R, Caramelli P, et al. Sugestões para o uso do mini-exame do estado mental no Brasil. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2003;61: 777-781. This is a screening instrument for dementia in elderly comprising 30 questions. For this study, the MMSE was analyzed according to the domains: orientation (10 points), memory (6 points), attention/ calculus (5 points), language (8 points) and constructional praxis (1).2626 Macuco CRM, Batistoni SST, Lopes A, et al. Mini-Mental State Examination performance in frail, pre-frail, and non-frail community dwelling older adults in Ermelino Matarazzo, São Paulo, Brazil. Int Psychogeriatr 2012;24:1725-1731.

  2. Sociodemographic variables. Gender; age group (6569 years, 70-74 years, 75-79 years and ≥ 80 years); years of schooling grouped under the classes never been to school, 1-4 years of schooling, 5-8 years and 9 years or more, and monthly family income in number of minimum salaries in the ranges <1.0; 1.1-3.0; 3.1-5.0; 5.1-10 and >10.

  3. Number of chronic diseases. Data was obtained by applying a questionnaire with nine dichotic items on the existence of a medical diagnosis for the following conditions in the 12 months leading up to the interview: heart disease; stroke, infarction or ischemia; hypertension; diabetes; osteoporosis; arthritis or arthrosis/ lung diseases; cancer and depression. Affirmative answers were counted and categorized into the groups 0; 1-2; ≥3.

  4. Depressive symptoms. Symptoms were assessed using the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS),2727 Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression Screening Scale - a Preliminary-report. J Psychiatr Res 1982-1983;17:37-49. a depression screening instrument for the elderly containing 15 dichotomous items inquiring about emotional state in recent weeks. The cut-off score adopted for suspected presence of significant depressive symptoms was >6.

  5. Social AADL. Practice of this type of AADLs was assessed by 8 closed-type questions with the possible answers. "never did", "stopped doing" and "still do", using a list devised from the literature.1111 Reuben DB, Laliberte L, Hiris, et al. A hierarchical exercise scale to measure function at the Advanced Activities of Daily Living (AADL) level. J Am Geriatr Soc 1990;38:855-861. Activities included were: pay visits, receive visitors, frequent churches, temples or practice activities linked to religion, take part in meetings, parties and dances, participate in cultural events, drive a car, take day trips outside the city and undertake longer trips outside the city or country. The number of activities with the "still do" answer was calculated for each participant.

  6. Physical AADLs. The questions were taken from the Brazilian version of the Minnesota Leisure Time Activity Questionnaire (MLTAQ)2828 Lustosa LP, Pereira DS, Dias RC, et al. Tradução e adaptação transcultural do Minnesota Leisure Time Activities Questionnaire em idosos. Rev Bras Geriatria e Gerontologia 2011;5:57-65.,2929 Taylor HL, Jacobs Jr DR, Schucker B, et al. A questionnaire for the assessment of leisure time physical activities. J Chronic Dis 1978;31: 741-755. included in the FIBRA protocol. Physical AADLs encompassed practices involving physical activities, sport and house work, such as: light or vigorous walks; light or vigorous runs; use stairs by choice; cycling/ ballroom dancing; gymnastics in the home, gym or community center; hydrogymnastics; swimming in pool, lake or sea; weight training; volleyball; table tennis, and participants were also asked whether they engaged in any other physical exercise or sport not mentioned. Men were also asked whether they had played football or been a football referee. The following activities were also investigated: doing domestic chores. preparing food, cutting grass with electric or manual mower, removing weeds and keeping a previously established garden or vegetable patch, hoeing, turning the soil, composting, digging, planting or seeding to grow a garden or vegetable patch, making or repairing furniture or domestic utensils using tools, besides asking whether the individual performed any other domestic activities not listed. The activities practiced by each participant were summed.

  7. Intelectual AADLs. The number of intellectual AADLs practiced was calculated based on the questions for leisure and relaxation activities taken from the Brazilian version of the Minnesota Leisure Time Activity Questionnaire (MLTAQ),2828 Lustosa LP, Pereira DS, Dias RC, et al. Tradução e adaptação transcultural do Minnesota Leisure Time Activities Questionnaire em idosos. Rev Bras Geriatria e Gerontologia 2011;5:57-65.,2929 Taylor HL, Jacobs Jr DR, Schucker B, et al. A questionnaire for the assessment of leisure time physical activities. J Chronic Dis 1978;31: 741-755. such as: watching television, reading newspapers or magazines, playing table games, and practicing other leisure or relaxation activity not mentioned. Women were asked whether they did knitting, crochet, lacework, painting, handicrafts or collecting at home, and men were asked whether they did handicrafts, painting or collecting at home. The activities practiced by each participant were summed together.

Statistical analysis. Descriptive analyses of the continuous variables was performed by calculating means and standard deviations. The categorical variables were described as percentages and tested using the Chisquared for different groups. The Mann-Whitney test was used to compare interval variables between the two groups. The Kruskal-Wallis test was used for comparison of values of the distributions of three or more groups.

Due to the sample size and the fact that the AADLs did not have a grading indicating the number of activities necessary to influence cognitive performance, the frequency distributions of the physical, social and intellectual AADLs were divided into quartiles (less active, not very active, very active and extremely active).3030 Yaffe K, Barnes D, Nevitt M, et al. A prospective study of physical activity and cognitive decline in elderly women: women who walk. Ach Intern Med 2001;161:1703-1708.

On multivariate regression analysis (applying Stepwise Forward criteria), the scoring of the five domains of the MMSE was used as dependent variables while the remaining sociodemographic variables (gender, age, schooling, family income), health conditions (number of diseases and depressive symptoms), and AADLs (physical, social and intellectual) served as independent variables. The variables male gender, GDS<6 and lower quartile (less active) of the AADLs were used as a reference. The interval variables were rank transformed owing to the absence of a normal distribution.3131 Conover WJ, Ronald LI. Rank transformations as a bridge between parametric and nonparametric statistic. The American Statistician 1981;35:124-129.

In order to avoid the multicollinearity often found among independent variables in regression analyses, the variance inflation factors (VIF) were analysed. In the present study, the VIF was found to lie between 1.01 and 1.77, well below 10, the cut-off point for potential multicolinearity.3232 Hair Jr JF; et al. Multivariate Data Analysis. 3rd ed. New York: Macmillan; 1995. A level of significance of 5% (p<0.05) was adopted for all statistical tests.

RESULTS

Of the 2,549 elderly that took part in the study, 65.71% were women and mean age was 72.32 (±5.55) years. Mean years of schooling was 4.37 (±3.99) and mean family income was 3.97 (±4.92) minimum wages (MW). The mean number of diseases reported was 2.02 (±1.33) and 79.54% had a score of < 6 on the GDS. Mean level of engagement in total AADLs (physical, social, intellectual) was 11.67 (±4.03) activities practiced. Performance on total MMSE and individual domain scores are given in Table 1 together with characteristics of the sample.

Table 2 depicts a comparison among the mean scores of the five domains of the MMSE according to sociodemographic variables, number of reported diseases and depressive symptoms. The means for the five domains of the MMSE were significantly higher in educated elderly. Participants with higher family income and absence of depressive symptoms (GDS<6) had higher mean scores for all domains except memory. There was no difference in the domain attention and calculus for age group.

Table 1
Means and standard deviations for sociodemographic variables, number of diseases reported, depressive symptoms, AADLs and cognitive performance. FIBRA study, Unicamp, 2008-2009.
Table 2
Comparison of mean scores by elderly on MMSE domains, according to sociodemographic variables, number of reported diseases and depressive symptoms (n=2,549). FIBRA Study, Unicamp, elderly, 2008-2009.
Table 3
Multivariate linear regression analysis with sociodemographic variables, number of diseases reported, depressive symptoms and AADLs (social and intellectual) as independent variables and score on the 5 MMSE domains as dependent variables (n=2193). FIBRA network, Unicamp, 2008-2009.

On the multivariate linear regression analysis, a significant association was observed between intellectual AADLs and the domains orientation, attention & calculus, language and praxis (with the exception of memory domain), whereas social AADLs were associated with memory and language domains. No association was found between physical activities and MMSE domains. Schooling was the variable most associated with the domains analyzed (Table 3).

DISCUSSION

The objective of the present study was to assess the relationship between engagement in AADLs (subdivided into physical, social and intellectual) and cognitive performance, as assessed on the five domains of the MMSE (orientation, memory, attention & calculus, language and praxis) in a sample of community-dwelling elderly, controlling for the effect of sociodemographic (age, gender, schooling and family income) and health (number of diseases and depressive symptoms) variables.

Besides benefits for physical health and psychological well-being,3333 Colcombe SJ, Erickson KL, Scalf PE, et al. Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2006;61:1166-1170. recent studies have shown that engagement in physical activity can play a key role in relation to cognitive decline and dementia.88 de Oliveira GM, Yokomizo JE, E Silva LD, et al. The applicability of the cognitive abilities screening instrument-short (CASI-S) in primary in Brazil. Int Psychogeriatr 2015;29:1-7.,3434 Hertzog C, Kramer AF, Wilson RS, et al. Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: Can the functional capacity of older be preserved and enhanced? Psychol Sci Public Interest 2008;9:1-69. Wang et al.1515 Wang HX, Jin Y, Hendrie HC, et al. Late life leisure activities and risk of cognitive decline. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2013;68:205-213. assessed engagement in AADLs and cognitive decline in 1,463 elderly without cognitive or physical impairment. The sample was followed for a mean of 2.4 years, and a significant relationship between engagement in physical AADLs and lower decline in episodic memory and language was noted. In a similar study, Barnes et al.1717 Barnes DE, Yaffe K, Satariano WA, et al. A Longitudinal Study of cardiorespiratory fitness and cognitive functional in healthy older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 2003;51:459-465. analyzed the cognitive performance of a cohort of 349 healthy individuals aged 55 years or older in relation to the practice of physical activity over a five-year period. The authors found that individuals who practiced physical activities and attained a high level of cardiovascular fitness had better results on tests assessing executive functions, attention and global cognitive function compared to those with poorer cardiovascular fitness. However, no association was found in the present study between engagement in physical AADLs and the cognitive domains assessed by the MMSE. Possible explanation for this disparity may stem from the different methods used for assessing adherence to exercise programs (duration, intensity, frequency or metabolic consumption of the physical activities practiced) and the different cognitive domains analyzed.99 Wang HX, Xu W, Pei JJ. Leisure activities, cognition and dementia. Biochim Biophys Acta 2012;1822:482-491. Moreover, in the present study, domestic activities were grouped under the domain of physical AADLs and these activities may not demand sufficient physical effort to attain metabolic spend or may not be performed consistently.

Engagement in social AADLs during aging tends to increase self-esteem and in theory can reduce social isolation and depression.3535 Dias EG, Duarte YAO, Lebrão ML. Efeitos longitudinais das atividades avançadas de vida diária em idosos: implicações para a reabilitação gerontologia. O Mundo da Saúde, São Paulo; 2010;34:258-267. A study by de Frias and Dixon3636 de Frias CM, Dixon RA. Lifestyle engagement affects cognitive status differences and trajectories on executive functions in older adults. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2014;29:16-25. examined the effect of engagement in AADLs (physical, social and intellectual) on cognitive performance among 570 older adults between 53 and 90 years of age followed for 4.5 years. The results showed that at base line those individuals engaged in social activities had better performance on tests of executive functions compared to individuals not engaging in these activities. James et al.1818 James BD, Wilson RS, Barnes LL, et al. Late-fife social activity and cognitive decline in old age. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2011;17:998-1005. examined the association of engagement in social activities with cognitive decline among 1,398 older adults without cognitive deficits at base line followed for up to 12 years through annual reassessments. Engagement in social activities was associated with less cognitive decline during follow-up. A one-point increase in social activity score was associated with a 47% reduction in the level of global cognitive decline. The rate of global decline was 70% lower in elderly frequently engaged in social activities compared to elderly with low frequency of this activity. A similar association was found in all five assessed domains (episodic memory, semantic memory, working memory, perception speed and visuospatial ability).

The cross-sectional cohort study of Krueger et al.. (2009)3737 Krueger K, Wilson RS, Kamenetsky JM, et al. Social engagement and cognitive function in old age. Exp Aging Res 2009;35:45-60. found an association between engagement of elderly in social activities and domains of memory, information processing speed and visuospatial abilities. The present study evidenced a significant, albeit modest, association between engagement in social AADLs and scores on the MMSE memory and language domains. This finding suggests that social activities might produce an enriched and stimulating environment which leads to greater engagement in complex social actions that can benefit cognitive performance.88 de Oliveira GM, Yokomizo JE, E Silva LD, et al. The applicability of the cognitive abilities screening instrument-short (CASI-S) in primary in Brazil. Int Psychogeriatr 2015;29:1-7.,3838 Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurol 2012;11:1006-1012.,3939 Fratiglioni L, Paillard-Borg S, Winblad B. An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect dementia. Lancet Neurol 2004;3:343-353. More complex social activities, such as engagement in political activities may involve greater cognitive demand to perform the task.99 Wang HX, Xu W, Pei JJ. Leisure activities, cognition and dementia. Biochim Biophys Acta 2012;1822:482-491. Less complex social activities, such as paying family visits, may not be sufficiently stimulating to benefit cognitive performance in aging.1919 Ghisletta P, McArdle JJ, Lindenberger U. Longitudinal cognition-survival relations in old and very old age. 13-year data from the Berlin Aging Study. European Psychologist 2006;11:204-223.

20 Wang JY, Zhou DH, Li J, et al. Leisure activity and risk of cognitive impairment: the Chongqing Aging Study. Neurology 2006;66:911-913.
-2121 Aartsen MJ, Smits CH, van Tilburg T, et al. Activity in older adults: cause or consequence of cognitive functioning? A longitudinal study on everyday activities and cognitive performance in older adults J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 57:153-162. Yet, exploring the complexity of social activities was beyond the scope of the present study.

The practice of intellectual AADLs was associated with scores on the domains of orientation, attention/calculus, language and praxis, but not with memory score.

Although these associations were modest, recent studies have shown that intellectual AADLs are key for good cognitive performance in aging, and that such activities may have a protective role against cognitive decline and dementia progression.99 Wang HX, Xu W, Pei JJ. Leisure activities, cognition and dementia. Biochim Biophys Acta 2012;1822:482-491. The longitudinal study of Wang et al.1515 Wang HX, Jin Y, Hendrie HC, et al. Late life leisure activities and risk of cognitive decline. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2013;68:205-213. cited previously, showed that elderly engaged in intellectual activities had less decline on the domains of language, executive function and in global cognition. Wilson et al.1616 Wilson RS, et al. Cognitive activity and incident AD in a populationbased sample of older persons. Neurology 2002;59:1910-1914. analyzed engagement in intellectual activities of 801 older persons who were members of a catholic clergy and their association with lower risk of Alzheimer's disease (AD) during an average follow-up of 4.5 years. In a proportional hazards model controlled for age, gender and education, a one-point increase in engagement in intellectual activities was associated with a 33% lower risk of AD. Engagement in intellectual activities was also associated with less decline in global cognition (47%), working memory (60%) and information processing speed (30%). These results suggest that frequent participation in intellectual activities is associated with a lower risk of developing AD.

In the literature, the cognitive reserve model is the most widely accepted to explain the positive relationships between practice of AADLs and cognitive performance. According to the model, preservation of cognitive performance in aging takes places as a result of exposure to complex activities during the life cycle, such as education, complex occupational activities and engagement in activities (physical, social and intellectual).3838 Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurol 2012;11:1006-1012. The concept of cognitive reserve suggests that previous experiences can influence neural processes and synaptic organization, rendering them more effective, adaptive and plastic, thereby delaying the onset of neural losses associated with aging.3838 Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurol 2012;11:1006-1012. The cognitive reserve model has also been used to clarify the association between low schooling and greater cognitive decline, as well as the higher risk of developing dementia among low educated individuals.3838 Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurol 2012;11:1006-1012.

The results of the present study, and those of previous studies, could be interpreted differently, i.e. engagement in AADLs may also depend on the opportunities afforded by the environment and modulated by socioeconomic aspects. In other words, better opportunities in the environment result in healthier and more active elderly with superior cognitive functioning,2121 Aartsen MJ, Smits CH, van Tilburg T, et al. Activity in older adults: cause or consequence of cognitive functioning? A longitudinal study on everyday activities and cognitive performance in older adults J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 57:153-162.,4040 Hall CB, Lipton RB, Sliwinski M, Derby CA, Verguese J. Cognitive activities delay onset memory decline in persons who develop dementia. Neurology 2009;73:356-361. therefore reverse causality cannot be ruled out.

The present study has several limitations. Although AADLs were grouped into categories, some activities were not purely social activities for example, some may include physical and mental components or vice a versa.99 Wang HX, Xu W, Pei JJ. Leisure activities, cognition and dementia. Biochim Biophys Acta 2012;1822:482-491.,1515 Wang HX, Jin Y, Hendrie HC, et al. Late life leisure activities and risk of cognitive decline. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2013;68:205-213. Further studies investigating a clearer definition for this construct are warranted, along with descriptions of activities to be included in this context. Also, the frequency, intensity and duration of the AADLs practiced by the elderly were not measured, factors which may further influence cognitive performance.99 Wang HX, Xu W, Pei JJ. Leisure activities, cognition and dementia. Biochim Biophys Acta 2012;1822:482-491. Additionally, the use of other cognitive instruments could allow these relationships to be explored in more depth in domains not addressed by the MMSE, such as executive functions and other memory subsystems. MMSE is an instrument for assessing global cognition, and exploring its domains is a limited resource and may have impacted the conclusion of the present study. Moreover, the use of the MMSE for cognitive screening may have allowed the inclusion of elderly with some degree of cognitive impairment.

In summary, the results of the present study suggest that active aging (based on social and intellectual activities) may be associated with cognitive performance. These findings can help guide public policies by ensuring that programs and services targeting the elderly promote engagement in such activities.

Author contributions. Neri, AL planned and coordinated the FIBRA network study - Unicamp Center, Yassuda, MS planned this study and assisted Sposito, G on the analysis and interpretation of the data and also in writing up the article.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Salthouse TA. Selective review of cognitive aging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010;16:754-760.
  • 1
    Salthouse TA. Localizing age-related individual differences in a hierarchical structure. Intelligence 2004;32:541-561.
  • 3
    López Perez-Dias AGL, Calero MD, Navarro-González E. Prediction of cognitive impairment in the elderly by analyzing their performance in verbal fluency and in sustained attention. Rev Neurol 2013;56:1-7.
  • 4
    Foss MP, Formigheri P, Speciali JG. Heterogeneity of cognitive aging in Brazilian normal elderly. Dement Neuropsychol 2009;3:344-351.
  • 5
    Marventano S, Prietro-Flores ME, Sanz-Barbero B, et al. Quality of life in older people with dementia: a multilevel study of individual attributes and residential care center characteristics. Geriatr Gerontol Int 2015; 15:104-110.
  • 6
    Argimon IIL. Aspectos cognitivos em idosos. Avaliação Psicológica 2006;5:243-245.
  • 7
    Aprahamian I, Martinelli JE, Neri AL, et al. The accuracy of the Clock Drawing Test compared to that of standard screening tests for Alzheimer's disease: results from a study of Brazilian elderly with heteregeneous educational backgrounds. Int Psychogeriatr 2010;22:64-71.
  • 8
    de Oliveira GM, Yokomizo JE, E Silva LD, et al. The applicability of the cognitive abilities screening instrument-short (CASI-S) in primary in Brazil. Int Psychogeriatr 2015;29:1-7.
  • 9
    Wang HX, Xu W, Pei JJ. Leisure activities, cognition and dementia. Biochim Biophys Acta 2012;1822:482-491.
  • 10
    Dias EG, Duarte YAO, Morgani MH, et al. As atividades de vida diária como componente da avaliação funcional do idoso. Rev Ter Ocup Univ São Paulo 2014;25:225-232.
  • 11
    Reuben DB, Laliberte L, Hiris, et al. A hierarchical exercise scale to measure function at the Advanced Activities of Daily Living (AADL) level. J Am Geriatr Soc 1990;38:855-861.
  • 12
    Rubenstein LV, Calkins DR, Greenfield S, et al. Health status assessment for elderly patients. Report of the Society of General Internal Medicine Task Force on Health Assessment. J Am Geriatr Soc 1989;37:562-569.
  • 13
    Minhat HS, Mohd Amin RM. Sociodemographic determinants of leisure participation among elderly in Malaysia. J Community Health 2012; 37:840-847.
  • 14
    Rizzuto D, Orsini N, Qiu C, et al. Lifestyle, social factors, and survival after age 75: population based study. BMJ 2012;345:1-10.
  • 15
    Wang HX, Jin Y, Hendrie HC, et al. Late life leisure activities and risk of cognitive decline. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2013;68:205-213.
  • 16
    Wilson RS, et al. Cognitive activity and incident AD in a populationbased sample of older persons. Neurology 2002;59:1910-1914.
  • 17
    Barnes DE, Yaffe K, Satariano WA, et al. A Longitudinal Study of cardiorespiratory fitness and cognitive functional in healthy older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 2003;51:459-465.
  • 18
    James BD, Wilson RS, Barnes LL, et al. Late-fife social activity and cognitive decline in old age. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2011;17:998-1005.
  • 19
    Ghisletta P, McArdle JJ, Lindenberger U. Longitudinal cognition-survival relations in old and very old age. 13-year data from the Berlin Aging Study. European Psychologist 2006;11:204-223.
  • 20
    Wang JY, Zhou DH, Li J, et al. Leisure activity and risk of cognitive impairment: the Chongqing Aging Study. Neurology 2006;66:911-913.
  • 21
    Aartsen MJ, Smits CH, van Tilburg T, et al. Activity in older adults: cause or consequence of cognitive functioning? A longitudinal study on everyday activities and cognitive performance in older adults J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 57:153-162.
  • 22
    Neri AL, Yassuda MS, Araujo LF, et al. Metodologia e perfil sociodemográfico, cognitivo e fragilidade de idosos comunitários de sete cidades brasileiras: Estudo FIBRA. Cad Saúde Pública 2013;29:778-792.
  • 23
    Ferrucci L, Guralnik JM, Studenski S, et al. Designing randomized, controlled trials aimed at preventing or delaying functional decline and disability in frail, older persons: a consensus report. J Am Geritr Soc 2004;52:625-634.
  • 24
    Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive status of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189-198.
  • 25
    Brucki SMD, Nitrini R, Caramelli P, et al. Sugestões para o uso do mini-exame do estado mental no Brasil. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2003;61: 777-781.
  • 26
    Macuco CRM, Batistoni SST, Lopes A, et al. Mini-Mental State Examination performance in frail, pre-frail, and non-frail community dwelling older adults in Ermelino Matarazzo, São Paulo, Brazil. Int Psychogeriatr 2012;24:1725-1731.
  • 27
    Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression Screening Scale - a Preliminary-report. J Psychiatr Res 1982-1983;17:37-49.
  • 28
    Lustosa LP, Pereira DS, Dias RC, et al. Tradução e adaptação transcultural do Minnesota Leisure Time Activities Questionnaire em idosos. Rev Bras Geriatria e Gerontologia 2011;5:57-65.
  • 29
    Taylor HL, Jacobs Jr DR, Schucker B, et al. A questionnaire for the assessment of leisure time physical activities. J Chronic Dis 1978;31: 741-755.
  • 30
    Yaffe K, Barnes D, Nevitt M, et al. A prospective study of physical activity and cognitive decline in elderly women: women who walk. Ach Intern Med 2001;161:1703-1708.
  • 31
    Conover WJ, Ronald LI. Rank transformations as a bridge between parametric and nonparametric statistic. The American Statistician 1981;35:124-129.
  • 32
    Hair Jr JF; et al. Multivariate Data Analysis. 3rd ed. New York: Macmillan; 1995.
  • 33
    Colcombe SJ, Erickson KL, Scalf PE, et al. Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2006;61:1166-1170.
  • 34
    Hertzog C, Kramer AF, Wilson RS, et al. Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: Can the functional capacity of older be preserved and enhanced? Psychol Sci Public Interest 2008;9:1-69.
  • 35
    Dias EG, Duarte YAO, Lebrão ML. Efeitos longitudinais das atividades avançadas de vida diária em idosos: implicações para a reabilitação gerontologia. O Mundo da Saúde, São Paulo; 2010;34:258-267.
  • 36
    de Frias CM, Dixon RA. Lifestyle engagement affects cognitive status differences and trajectories on executive functions in older adults. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2014;29:16-25.
  • 37
    Krueger K, Wilson RS, Kamenetsky JM, et al. Social engagement and cognitive function in old age. Exp Aging Res 2009;35:45-60.
  • 38
    Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurol 2012;11:1006-1012.
  • 39
    Fratiglioni L, Paillard-Borg S, Winblad B. An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect dementia. Lancet Neurol 2004;3:343-353.
  • 40
    Hall CB, Lipton RB, Sliwinski M, Derby CA, Verguese J. Cognitive activities delay onset memory decline in persons who develop dementia. Neurology 2009;73:356-361.
  • Financing. This study was funded by the CNPq grant number 555082/2006-7. The first author received financial support from CAPES.
  • 2
    This study was conducted at the School of Medical Sciences of the State University of Campinas, SP, Brazil.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Jul-Sep 2015

History

  • Received
    11 Feb 2015
  • Accepted
    16 Apr 2015
Academia Brasileira de Neurologia, Departamento de Neurologia Cognitiva e Envelhecimento R. Vergueiro, 1353 sl.1404 - Ed. Top Towers Offices, Torre Norte, São Paulo, SP, Brazil, CEP 04101-000, Tel.: +55 11 5084-9463 | +55 11 5083-3876 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistadementia@abneuro.org.br | demneuropsy@uol.com.br