SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.9 issue3Analysis of prototypical narratives produced by aphasic individuals and cognitively healthy subjectsEvaluation of quality of life and psychological aspects of Parkinson's disease patients who participate in a support group author indexsubject indexarticles search
Home Pagealphabetic serial listing  

Services on Demand

Journal

Article

Indicators

Related links

Share


Dementia & Neuropsychologia

Print version ISSN 1980-5764

Dement. neuropsychol. vol.9 no.3 São Paulo July/Sept. 2015

http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642015dn93000012 

Original Article

Executive functions as a potential neurocognitive endophenotype in anxiety disorders: A systematic review considering DSM-IV and DSM-5 diagnostic criteria classification

FUNÇÕES EXECUTIVAS COMO UM POTENCIAL ENDOFENÓTIPO NEUROCOGNITIVO NOS TRANSTORNOS DE ANSIEDADE: UMA REVISÃO SISTEMÁTICA DA LITERATURA CONSIDERANDO OS CRITÉRIOS DIAGNÓSTICOS DO DSM-IV E DO DSM-5

Juliana de Lima Muller1 

Kamilla Irigaray Torquato2 

Gisele Gus Manfro3 

Clarissa Marceli Trentini4 

1Psychologist. Doctoral student at the Institute of Psychology, Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre RS, Brazil.

2Student of Psychology at the Federal University of Health Sciences of Porto Alegre, Porto Alegre RS, Brazil.

3PhD, Psychiatrist, Professor at the Department of Psychiatry and on the Post-graduate Program in Medical Sciences: Psychiatry, Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre RS, Brazil. Coordinator of the Anxiety Disorders Outpatient unit Program (PROTAN) of the Hospital de Clínicas de Porto Alegre and the Anxiety Disorders Program in Childhood and Adolescence (PROTAIA) of the Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul and Hospital de Clínicas de Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil.

4PhD, Psychologist, Professor at the Institute of Psychology, Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre RS, Brazil. Coordinator of the Núcleo de Estudos em Avaliação Psicológica e Psicopatologia (NEAPP).

ABSTRACT

Evidence in the literature indicates that neurocognitive impairments may represent endophenotypes in psychiatric disorders.

Objective:

This study aimed to conduct a systematic review on executive functions as a potential neurocognitive endophenotype in anxiety disorder diagnosis according to the DSM-IV and DSM-5 classifications.

Methods:

A literature search of the LILACS, Cochrane Library, Index Psi Periódicos Técnico-Científicos, PubMed and PsycInfo databases was conducted, with no time limits. Of the 259 studies found, 14 were included in this review.

Results:

Only studies on obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) were found. The executive function components of decision-making, planning, response inhibition, behavioral reversal/alternation, reversal learning and set-shifting/cognitive flexibility were considered to be a neurocognitive endophenotypes in OCD.

Conclusion:

Further studies on executive functions as a neurocognitive endophenotype in other anxiety disorders are needed since these may have different neurocognitive endophenotypes and require other prevention and treatment approaches.

Key words: endophenotypes; executive function; anxiety disorders; neuropsychology

RESUMO

Evidências na literatura indicam que déficits neurocognitivos podem representar endofenótipos nos transtornos psiquiátricos.

Objetivo:

Esse estudo teve como objetivo realizar uma revisão sistemática das funções executivas como um potencial endofenótipo neurocognitivo nos transtornos de ansiedade de acordo com as classificações diagnósticas do DSM-IV e do DSM-5.

Métodos:

Uma pesquisa na literatura nas bases de dados LILACS, Cochrane Library, Index Psi Periódicos Técnico-Científicos, PubMed and PsycInfo foi conduzida, sem limite de tempo. Dos 259 estudos encontrados, 14 foram incluídos nessa revisão.

Resultados:

Somente foram encontrados estudos sobre o transtorno obsessivocompulsivo (TOC). Os componentes das funções executivas como a tomada de decisão, planejamento, inibição de resposta, inversão comportamental/alternância, aprendizagem reversa e mudança de foco/flexibilidade cognitiva foram considerados endofenótipos neurocognitivos no TOC.

Conclusão:

É necessário o desenvolvimento de estudos sobre funções executivas como um endofenótipo neurocognitivo em outros transtornos de ansiedade, pois eles podem apresentar diferentes endofenótipos neurocognitivos e podem exigir abordagens de prevenção e tratamento distintas.

Palavras-Chave: endofenótipos; função executiva; transtornos de ansiedade; neuropsicologia

INTRODUCTION

Endophenotypes have been considered an important concept in the study of neuropsychiatric diseases. It is known that there are different types of endophenotypes: neurophysiological, biochemical, endocrinologic, neuroanatomical, cognitive, and neuropsychological (including configured self-report data). Endophenotypes are intermediate measures of diseases between phenotype and genotype, and may represent simpler clues to genetic underpinnings than the disease syndrome itself, providing the decomposition or deconstruction of psychiatric diagnosis. They are associated with a candidate gene or gene region, as well as to the heritability that is inferred from relative risk for the disorder in relatives, and disease association parameters.1 According to this view, some criteria must be fulfilled in order to be considered an endophenotype: [a] be associated with the disease in the population; [b] be stateindependent (manifests in an individual whether or not the illness is active); [c] be heritable; [d] be co-segregated with the disease; e) be identified in unaffected first-degree relatives (UFDR) of patients at a higher rate than in the general population.1,2

From this perspective, there is an ongoing search in psychiatry for candidate endophenotypes that may represent vulnerability markers for disease development and lie closer to the genetic origins of the disorder.1 Research on this topic has focused attention on diseases such as autism,3 schizophrenia,4,5 bipolar disorder,6 major depressive disorder7 and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.8 However, to date, there are few studies exploring neuropsychological endophenotypes in anxiety disorders. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5), considers the following as anxiety disorders: separation anxiety disorder, selective mutism, specific phobia, social anxiety disorder (social phobia), panic disorder, agoraphobia and generalized anxiety disorder.9 Although the obsessivecompulsive disorder originally belonged to this group in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition,10,11 it is currently classified into the obsessive-compulsive and related disorders group representing a specific new group of disorders.9 Moreover, posttraumatic stress disorder and acute stress disorder, both originally part of the anxiety disorders group under the DSM-IV, now belong to trauma- and stressorrelated disorders, another a new group. Although obsessive-compulsive disorder, posttraumatic stress disorder and acute stress disorder are not considered anxiety disorders under the DSM-5, there is a close relationship between them and anxiety disorders.9

Anxiety disorders are among the most prevalent psychiatric disorders12 and the most frequent in Brazil.13 Besides presenting a high prevalence, anxiety disorders are associated to impairment in social, academic and health aspects, as well as increased suicide rates.14 One of the deleterious effects that may be observed in these patients is deficit in cognitive abilities, as well as in executive functions.15,16 The executive functions are a complex and comprehensive construct.17 They allow a person to guide their own behavior according to specific objectives, evaluate their efficiency and adequacy, discard ineffective strategies and maintain the most adapted ones, aiming at problem-solving in everyday functioning.18 This construct encompasses specific cognitive processes, for example, controlled attention, fluency, abstract thinking, self-regulation, planning, inhibitory control and cognitive shifting.19

Neurocognitive dysfunctions are potential endophenotype markers in different psychiatric disorders1,20 and are regarded to be among the most promising candidate endophenotypes.21 The fact that neurocognitive functions can be reliable and stable over time makes them valuable endophenotypes.22

With regard to research involving the evaluation of executive functions in anxiety disorders according to the DSM-IV, there are many studies evaluating patients with OCD. It has been suggested that individuals with OCD experience difficulties in planning ability,23-25 cognitive and motor inhibition,20,25,26 shifting attention,27,28 decision making23,29 and verbal fluency.25,30 A meta-analysis indicated that patients with OCD were significantly impaired on tasks measuring executive functions. The researchers found a relatively large effect size for planning and a moderate effect size for set-shifting ability, cognitive inhibition, verbal fluency and processing speed.31

On the other hand, deficits have been found in working memory,32 sustained attention,33 processing speed,34,35 inhibition34,36,37 and attentional switching34 in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Furthermore, findings of a meta-analysis indicated that PTSD is associated with neurocognitive deficits of a medium magnitude in attention/working memory, and processing speed, but with smaller deficits in other components of executive functions.38

Studies involving the evaluation of executive functions in anxiety disorders other than OCD and PTSD suggest impairments to executive functions, as well as to set-shifting abilities,39 verbal fluency15 and working memory40,41, in social anxiety disorder (SAD). Conversely, a systematic review indicated sparse evidence that patients with SAD have executive dysfunction, where only one out of five neuropsychological studies found significant differences between clinical and control groups.42

Research investigating panic disorder (PD) has found some deficits in affected individuals on divided attention,15 psychomotor speed,15,16 initiation, inhibition,16 working memory,16,43 verbal fluency and category formation.43 Nevertheless, research on cognitive functions in patients with PD is limited and some studies found no impairment in executive functions.44 Research indicates that individuals with generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) have inhibition and cognitive flexibility difficulties.45-47 By contrast, other researchers have failed to find deficits in GAD or in specific phobia.15

Some studies have described deficits in working memory,48 attentional components and processing speed49 in selective mutism.49 Nevertheless, there is a lack of studies on executive functions in selective mutism, as well as separation anxiety disorder.

Therefore, it can be concluded that results are inconsistent, with little clarification as to which components of executive functions may be impaired in anxiety disorders. Research results on executive functions as an endophenotype may help elucidate this issue, clarifying whether deficits in executive functions are secondary to the presence of the disorder or whether they can serve as vulnerability markers for disease development and lie closer to the genetic origins of the disorder. Furthermore, identifying those components of executive functions that can be considered vulnerability markers for the development of an anxiety disorder may assist toward prevention in at risk populations and also emphasize the importance of a better understanding of potential neurocognitive endophenotypes in anxiety disorders.

Thus, the objective of this study was to conduct a systematic review on executive functions as a potential neurocognitive endophenotype in anxiety disorders classified according to the DSM-IV and DSM-5 diagnostic criteria. Until recently, studies involving anxiety disorder samples have assessed anxiety disorders as defined by the DSM-IV. Therefore, both DSM-IV and DSM-5 anxiety disorders were included in this system-

atic review.9,10

METHODS

The research question that directed this study was as follows: are executive functions a neurocognitive endophenotype in anxiety disorders, classified according to the DSM-IV or DSM-5 diagnostic criteria? In order to answer this question based on a systematic review, the Assessment of Multiple Systematic Reviews (AMSTAR) protocol was followed. The following search engines were consulted to conduct this review: LILACS, The Cochrane Library, Index Psi Periódicos Técnico-Científicos, PubMed and PsycInfo, at or around January 2015 (all research published up to this date). The descriptors were taken from DeCS (Portuguese), DeCS (English), Terminologia em Psicologia, MeSH and the Thesaurus of Psychological Index Terms, respectively. A search of the best descriptors to be used in each database was performed, as these differed across the databases. The descriptors used on each database can be seen in Figure 1 under the results section. For each database, the criteria "any field" was adopted, not using selection by title, author, etc.

The search and study selection were systematically and independently conducted by two investigators. Inclusion criteria were as follows: [a] empirical studies of clinical or subclinical samples with Separation Anxiety Disorder, Selective Mutism, Specific Phobia, Social Anxiety Disorder, Panic Disorder, Agoraphobia, Generalized Anxiety Disorder, Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder, Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and/or Acute Stress Disorder, and/or with unaffected relatives; [b] studies answering the research question, considering investigations on executive functions as a neurocognitive endophenotype in anxiety disorders; studies using at least one neuropsychological instrument to assess executive functions and/or its components; [c] studies published in Portuguese, English or Spanish. Studies were separately examined by the investigators and excluded if they did not meet the inclusion criteria or if they were repeated. Any discordance between the investigators was discussed to reach a consensus conclusion.

RESULTS

Based on the intersection of descriptors in the databases, the search retrieved 259 papers. The final search resulted in 13 studies analyzing components of executive functions as a possible neurocognitive endophenotype of anxiety disorders. Besides the studies selected from the databases consulted, one further paper was added from the author's personal records.20 Therefore, 14 studies were included in total. The flowchart is shown in Figure 1.

The 14 selected studies evaluated participants with OCD and/or their UFDR, or subclinical obsessive-compulsive participants. Table 1 shows information on the studies included in the review by country, sample, age, instruments and results. Only instruments assessing components of executive functions were included in Table 1. Also, only results that indicated deficits/impairments in components of executive functions as a neurocognitive endophenotype are shown. Instruments and results regarding other neurocognitive functions were not given in Table 1, as this was beyond the scope of the paper.

The deficits/impairments most frequently found in OCD, were related to the following abilities: behavioral reversal/alternation,50,51 set-shifting/cognitive flexibility,50,52,53 decision making,23,51,54-56 response inhibition,20,50,52,53,57,58 planning,23,55,59 reversal learning,60 spatial working memory59,61 and sustained attention.57 For a deeper analysis of the results found in the systematic review, the neuropsychological instruments were grouped in the Discussion section according to the components of executive function evaluated.

DISCUSSION

The purpose of this systematic review was to verify whether executive functions are a potential neurocognitive endophenotype in anxiety disorders, as diagnosed according to DSM-IV andz DSM-5 classification. Only studies on OCD were found in the systematic review, although descriptors of all anxiety disorders were used. As noted regarding research on anxiety disorders, most studies have investigated neurocognitive aspects of OCD, with few studies focusing on other anxiety disorders.62 This same pattern was found with regard to studies investigating executive functions as an endophenotype in anxiety disorders.

Figure 1 Flowchart with studies selection steps forming this systematic review. 

Table 1 Studies included in the review by country, sample, age (years), instruments and results. 

Study Country Sample Age (M) Instruments Results (deficits/impairments)*
Abramovitch et al., 201557 United States 27 SOC 27 LOC 24.2 24.1 Expanded Go/No-Go Task Response inhibition and sustained attention (SOC)
Cavedini et al., 201023 Italy 35 OCD 35 UFDR 31 HC 31 HCR 35.6 45 34.7 43.2 Iowa Gambling Task, Tower of Hanoi, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Decision making and planning (OCD and UFDR)
Chamberlain et al., 200752 United Kingdom 20 OCD 20 UFDR 20 HC 32.1 34.2 33.1 Intradimensional/Extradimensional Shift Task, Stop Signal Task, Cambridge Gamble Task Set-shifting/cognitive flexibility and response inhibition (OCD and UFDR)
Chamberlain et al., 200860 United Kingdom 14 OCD 12 UFDR 15 HC 31.7 39.5 34.8 A functional magnetic resonance imaging task capable of fractionating different components of cognitive flexibility Reversal learning (OCD and UFDR)
Delorme et al., 200759 France 64 UFDR** 47 HC 42.3 38 Tower of London Test, Trail Making Test, Design Fluency Task, Verbal Fluency Test (letter), Association Fluency task Planning (UFDR**)
da Rocha et al., 200854 Brazil 32 OCD S/Lg 17 OCD La/La 29.4 31.2 Iowa Gambling Task, CPT-II, Trail Making Test Decision making (OCD S/Lg)
de Vries et al., 201461 Netherlands 43 OCD 17 UFDR 37 HC 38.1 36.4 39.2 N-back Task Spatial working memory (OCD and UFDR)
Lennertz et al., 201258 Germany 30 OCD 30 UFDR 30 HC 40.6 42.1 42.7 Verbal Fluency Test (letter), Tower of London, Trail Making Test, Saccadic tasks Response inhibition (OCD and UFDR)
Menzies et al., 200720 United Kingdom 31 OCD 31 UFDR 31 HC 32.5 36.7 33.4 Stop Signal Task Response inhibition (OCD and UFDR)
Rajender et al., 201153 India 30 OCD 30 UFDR 30 HC 25.6 26.4 26.9 Colour Trails Test, Digit Vigilance Test, The Triads Test, Tower of London Test, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Stroop Test- NIMHANS version Set-shifting/cognitive flexibility and response inhibition (OCD and UFDR)
Rao et al., 200850 India 30 ROCD 30 HC 27.8 27.9 Digit span test, Continuous Performance Test, Trail Making Test, Stroop Color Word Interference Test, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Delayed Alternation Test, Tower of London, Controlled Oral Word Association Test, Letter Number Sequencing and Spatial Span Set-shifting/cognitive flexibility, behavioral reversal/alternation and response inhibition (ROCD)
Viswanath et al., 2009 51 India 25 UFDR** 25 HC 27.5 27.4 Continuous Performance Test, Trail Making Test, Stroop Colour Word Interference Test, Delayed Alternation Test, Tower of London, Controlled Oral Word Association Test, Iowa Gambling Task, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Letter number sequencing, Spatial span Decision making and behavioral reversal/alternation (UFDR**)
Zhang et al., 201555 China 55 OCD 55 UFDR 55 HC 26.5 28.4 27.9 Stroop Color Word Test, Trail Making Test, Digit Span Test, Verbal Fluency Tests (letter and category), Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Tower of London, Iowa Gambling Task, Game of Dice Task Decision making and planning (OCD and UFDR)
Zhu et al., 201456 China 14 SOC 14 LOC 19.9 19.6 Iowa Gambling Task Decision-making (SOC)

OC: obsessive-compulsive; OCD: obsessive-compulsive disorder patients; UFDR: unaffected first-degree relatives of patients; SOC: subclinical obsessive- compulsive participants; LOC: low obsessive-compulsive symptoms control participants; HC: healthy controls; HCR: healthy controls relatives; ROCD: recovered obsessive-compulsive disorder patients; S/Lg: patients with S- and/or Lg-carriers; La/La: patients with the La/La genotype. * Participants with deficits/impairments given in parentheses). **Relatives of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder.

Although the 14 studies found considered OCD, they employed different methodologies and samples for the investigation of executive functions as an endophenotype of the disorder. Most of the investigations (eight studies) comprised pairs of UFDR and OCD patients

compared to healthy controls,20,23,52,53,55,58,60,61 a design which has been used since the first studies of the endophenotype concept in psychiatry.1 Two studies had a similar methodology, comparing UFDR of OCD patients against healthy controls.51,59 One study compared OCD patients in remission versus healthy controls, investigating whether neuropsychological deficits would be present in the recovered phase.50 Another study evaluated OCD patients, but explored the link between decision-making and the serotonin system (serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism) in the sample,54 another approach to better understand the endophenotype concept.1 Two studies used a sampling type that has been used more recently in the study of endophenotypes. The participants of these studies were a subclinical obsessive-compulsive sample and a low obsessivecompulsive symptoms control sample.56,57 It has been hypothesized that the current understanding of endophenotypes in psychiatric research is that these markers lie along a continuum in the population. Concerning this hypothesis, complementary investigations in the general population are needed.63

It has been suggested that components of executive functions can be considered neurocognitive endophenotypes in OCD, as all the 14 studies retrieved in the systematic review indicated deficits/impairments in at least one such component. On the other hand, some components of executive functions are not linked to neurocognitive endophenotype in this disorder, such as verbal fluency, processing speed, working memory and sustained attention. Verbal fluency did not represent a vulnerability marker for development of the disease in all studies in which this component was evaluated.50,51,55,58,59 Some research evaluated the orthographic component (e.g. Lennertz et al., 2012)58 while others investigated the semantic one (e.g. Zhang et al., 2015),55 but in all studies verbal fluency was not suggested to be a vulnerability marker for the development of the disorder.

Processing speed was evaluated using different tests, such as the Trail Making Test - reaction time part A (e. g. Lennertz et al., 2012; Rao et al., 2008),50,58 Trail Making Test - reaction time part B minus reaction time part A (e.g. da Rocha et al., 2008)54 and Continuous Performance Test - reaction time (e.g. Rao et al., 2008).50 However, none of the studies that investigated this component found that it could be an endophenotype of the disorder.50,51,54,55,58

One study, using a visuospatial n-back task during functional magnetic resonance imaging, suggested that the working memory could be a neurocognitive endophenotype in OCD.61 The authors found that OCD patients and their UFDR showed task-related hyperactivity in the frontoparietal network as compared to healthy participants, providing evidence that increased recruitment of the frontoparietal network constitutes an endophenotype of the disorder.61 Other studies investigating working memory using the Digit Span Test, the Letter Number Sequencing and the Spatial Span presented negative findings.50,51,55 Thus, the majority of studies indicate that working memory is not an endophenotype in OCD.

Abramovitch et al. (2015)57 studied sustained attention using the Expanded Go No-Go Task (response time) to compare a subclinical obsessive-compulsive sample and a low obsessive-compulsive symptoms control sample and found that the former group had deficient sustained attention. Nevertheless, the study used a non-clinical sample and no structured clinical interview, making it difficult to extrapolate the results. Besides this study, others have assessed sustained attention with the Continuous Performance Test - omission errors (e.g. Viswanath et al., 2009),51 the Colour Trails Test - part 153 and the Digit Vigilance Test.53 With the exception of Abramovitch et al. (2015),57 all other studies indicated that sustained attention deficits are not associated with OCD.50,51,53,54

On the other hand, according to this systematic review, some components of executive functions are considered neurocognitive endophenotypes in OCD. These components include the following: decision-making, planning, response inhibition, behavioral reversal/ alternation, reversal learning and set-shifting/cognitive flexibility.

The most used task for the assessment of decisionmaking was the Iowa Gambling Task and all studies that used this test suggested that decision-making might qualify as an endophenotype for OCD.23,51,54-56 An interesting issue is that, in the study of da Rocha et al. (2008),54 this neuropsychological function was also associated with the presence of the polymorphism of the serotonin transporter gene and verified that those with the short allele (s/Lg), i.e. low expression function, performed significantly worse on the test.

As outcomes and probabilities are implicit in the Iowa Gambling Task, the participant has to initially find some effective information and figure out the options' qualities by himself by means of processing feedback of previous choices. This task assesses decision-making under ambiguity, in which the possible choices are highly ambiguous and the participant must learn to avoid the disadvantageous card decks through feedback from previous trials.55,64

Only two studies found intact decision-making in OCD patients and their relatives compared to healthy controls.52,55 One study used The Cambridge Gamble Task52 and the other the Game of Dice Task.55 The Game of Dice Task consists of a task that evaluates decisionmaking under risk, because explicit information about the potential consequences of different choices and their probabilities are provided in some decision situations.65 The study of Zhang et al. (2015)55 went a step further to simultaneously evaluate decision-making under ambiguity (Iowa Gambling Task) and decision making under risk situations (Game of Dice Task), and showed that dissociation of decision making under ambiguity and decision making under risk is a more appropriate potential neurocognitive endophenotype for the disorder. However, more studies involving neuropsychological instruments that assess decision making under ambiguity and decision making under risk are needed to confirm this hypothesis.

Two studies that used the Tower of London Test55,59 and one study that used the Tower of Hanoi Test,23 demonstrated that deficits in planning might represent a neurocognitive endophenotype for OCD. These findings however, are not consistent, since other studies50,51,53,58 also using the Tower of London Test did not indicate impairments in the groups of unaffected relatives of OCD patients or in recovered OCD patients. These studies had smaller sample sizes as compared to others,23,55,59 suggesting that smaller sample size may not have the power to detect differences between groups.

Considering response inhibition, Chamberlain et al. (2007)52 and Menzies et al. (2007)20 found lower performance on the Stop Signal Task (reaction times) in UFDR and OCD patients. Lennertz et al. (2012)58 also indicated impaired response inhibition in UFDR and OCD patients, evaluated using the anti-saccade task. Moreover, Abramovitch et al. (2015)57 found that a subclinical obsessive-compulsive sample committed more errors on the Expanded go/no-go task (commission errors) compared to a low obsessive-compulsive symptoms control sample. These results suggested that poor response inhibition appears to be a familial marker of OCD across the mentioned tasks.

On the other hand, the findings of two studies using the Continuous Performance Test - commission errors50,54 and of two studies employing the Stroop Colour Word Interference Test51,55 were contradictory in as far as the results did not indicate that response inhibition could be a potential neurocognitive endophenotype for the disorder. Rao et al. (2008)50 showed that patients in the recovered phase of the illness had significant deficits in response inhibition on the Stroop Colour Word Interference Test, but the instrument had not been validated for use in their population and language, compromising the findings observed.

Thus, it can be hypothesized that the Stop Signal Task (reaction times), the Expanded go/no-go task (commission errors) and the anti-saccade task used by Lennertz et al. (2012)58 appear to be more sensitive than the Continuous Performance Test (commission errors) and the Stroop Colour Word Interference Test for evaluating response inhibition as an endophenotype in OCD. Although the present systematic review showed that response inhibition represents a vulnerability marker for OCD development, impairments in this component had a relatively small effect size among patients with OCD in a recent meta-analysis.31 Further exploration to compare different response inhibition tests among OCD samples are needed, enabling a better understanding of the role of this component as a candidate endophenotype marker.

Behavioral reversal/alternation and reversal learning abilities were evaluated by few studies. Only two assessed behavioral reversal/alternation and used the Delayed Alternation Test.50,51 In this test, a rule is learnt and then subsequently needs to be inhibited and reversed in order to maintain good performance.66 Viswanath et al. (2009)51 found that unaffected relatives of OCD probands showed significant deficits on the test as compared to healthy controls whereas Rao et al. (2008)50 showed that patients in the recovered phase of the disorder performed poorly when compared to healthy controls i.e., deficits in behavioral reversal/ alternation could be a potential endophenotype in OCD.

Reversal learning, an ability associated to behavioral flexibility after negative feedback, was evaluated in only one of the studies found in this systematic review.60 The authors used a functional magnetic resonance imaging task to fractionate different components of behavioral flexibility, including reversal of responses, and identified abnormally reduced activation of several cortical regions, including the lateral orbitofrontal cortex, during reversal learning in OCD patients and their unaffected relatives. The authors concluded that reversallearning is related to hypofunction and this appeared to be a vulnerability marker for OCD. Thus, there is evidence that behavioral reversal/alternation as well as reversal learning could be considered endophenotype candidates for OCD. However, more research is needed to corroborate these findings.

Regarding set-shifting/cognitive flexibility, different instruments were used to measure these components of executive functions. Studies using the Trail Making Test (response time part B), the Design Fluency Test and the Colour Trails Test (part 2) suggested that set-shifting/

cognitive flexibility are not deficient in OCD.50,51,53,55,58,59

Nevertheless, other studies showed contradictory results. Chamberlain et al. (2007)52 assessed set-shifting/cognitive flexibility with an Intradimensional/ Extradimensional Shift Task and demonstrated that OCD patients and their relatives had impaired performance on these abilities. Similarly, three studies used the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and found that deficits in set-shifting/cognitive flexibility were observed in OCD patients and their relatives23,53 or among patients in the recovered phase of the disease.50 On the other hand, two other studies used the same instrument and indicated that OCD patients and their relatives performed as well as healthy controls.51,55

It should be noted, however, that different versions of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test were used by the different studies, for example, Viswanath et al. (2009)51 assessed their sample with a computerized version, while Zhang et al. (2015)55 and Rao et al. (2008)50 used a non-computerized version. A meta-analysis previously revealed that the use of different forms of this test might explain a significant proportion of the heterogeneity in the estimated effects for the test and that the computerized version appears to be more sensitive than the classical method in identifying deficits in patients with OCD.31 Thus, according to the results of this review, there is evidence that set-shifting/cognitive flexibility could be considered endophenotype candidate markers in OCD and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test appears to be the most sensitive test for investigating these abilities. However, according to the findings of Shin et al. (2014),31 further studies with the computerized version could further understanding on the role of this ability as an endophenotype of OCD.

An important issue to be noted is that there was a fair degree of heterogeneity in certain variables employed by the evaluated studies. Age at disease onset was a variable indicated in only five of the studies20,23,50,51,55 while disease duration was also described in only five studies.20,50,51,53,55 These variables have previously been considered as possible moderators affecting cognitive functioning in OCD67 and should be better investigated in future studies.

Furthermore, the medication status and presence of comorbidities in the samples of patients differed among studies. In three studies, patients were free of medication, 53,55,61 however, in five studies the majority or all patients were on medication.20,50,54,58,60 The studies of Cavedini et al. (2010)23 and Chamberlain et al. (2007)52 evaluated patients with OCD and provided no information about the use of medications. Regarding comorbidities, three studies did not exclude psychiatric comorbidities in their sample,54,58,61 while in seven studies the OCD patients had no comorbidities.20,23,50,52,53,55,60 It is possible that discrepant findings in this systematic review are attributable to confounding variables including medication status68 and the presence of comorbidities.27

Other aspects that can be attributed to the inconsistent pattern of results for some components of executive functions are the heterogeneous nature of OCD.69 Moreover, sample size and different test forms and methods of testing most likely influenced performance of the samples. Future studies are needed to carefully select the form of each test and the methods of testing to better investigate whether executive functions can be considered a neurocognitive endophenotype in OCD.

The investigation of endophenotypes in psychiatry is very recent1 and research evaluating executive functions as a neurocognitive endophenotype in OCD started even later, with the first study published in 2007.52 Thus, research assessing executive functions in patients and relatives with anxiety disorders, such as PD, GAD and SAD, could provide a better understanding of these disorders, contributing to more appropriate diagnosis and treatment of patients.

In conclusion, there are indications that decisionmaking, planning, response inhibition, behavioral reversal/alternation, reversal learning and set-shifting/ cognitive flexibility are inherent traits of OCD. However, additional research should be conducted before definitive conclusions are reached, since few related studies have been carried out to date. Finally, through this systematic review, studies evaluating neurocognitive functions in other anxiety disorder patients besides individuals with OCD are warranted. Anxiety disorders, including OCD, have been shown to share genetic and environmental risk factors.70 Nevertheless, although these disorders exhibit similar features, they can have different neurocognitive endophenotypes and may require different prevention and treatment approaches. Identifying neurocognitive vulnerability markers might prove to be an important avenue toward better understanding and treatment of anxiety disorders.

Author contributions. Juliana de Lima Muller: literature search, study design, data acquisition, studies selection, preparation and writing of the manuscript, approval of the final version to be sent to the journal. Kamilla Irigaray Torquato: literature search, study design; data acquisition, studies selection, preparation and writing of the manuscript; approval of the final version to be sent to the journal. Gisele Gus Manfro: study design; scientific contribution and critical content review; approval of the final version to be sent to the journal. Clarissa Marceli Trentini: study design; scientific contribution and critical content review; approval of the final version to be sent to the journal.

Acknowledgments.

This work was supported by the Fundo de Incentivo à Pesquisa e Eventos(FIPE). Juliana de Lima Muller received a CAPES scholarship.

REFERENCES

1 Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry 2003;160:636645. [ Links ]

2 Leboyer M. Searching for alternative phenotypes in psychiatric genetics. Methods Mol Med 2003;77:145-161. [ Links ]

3 Segovia F, Holt R, Spencer M, et al. Identifying endophenotypes of autism: a multivariate approach. Front Comput Neurosci 2014;6:60. [ Links ]

4 Docherty AR, Coleman MJ, Tu X, Deutsch CK, Mendell NR, Levy DL. Comparison of putative intermediate phenotypes in schizophrenia patients with and without obsessive-compulsive disorder: examining evidence for the schizo-obsessive subtype. Schizophr Res 2012; 140:83-86. [ Links ]

5 Leppänen JM, Niehaus DJ, Koen L, Du Toit E, Schoeman R, Emsley R. Deficits in facial affect recognition in unaffected siblings of Xhosa schizophrenia patients: evidence for a neurocognitive endophenotype. Schizophr Res 2008;99:270-273. [ Links ]

6 Glahn DC, Almasy L, Barguil M, et al. Neurocognitive endophenotypes for bipolar disorder identified in multiplex multigenerational families. Arch Gen psychiatry 2010;67:168-177. [ Links ]

7 Peterson BS, Wang Z, Horga G, et al. Discriminating risk and resilience endophenotypes from lifetime illness effects in familial major depressive disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 2014;71:136-148. [ Links ]

8 Nikolas MA, Nigg JT. Moderators of neuropsychological mechanism in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2015; 43:271-281. [ Links ]

9 American Psychiatric Association. DSM-5. Manual Diagnóstico e Estatístico de Transtornos Mentais.5. ed. Porto Alegre: Artmed; 2014: 992. [ Links ]

10 American Psychiatric Association. DSM-IV-TR: Manual Diagnóstico e Estatístico de Transtornos Mentais. 4a ed., texto revisado. Porto Alegre: Artmed; 2003:880. [ Links ]

11 American Psychiatric Association. 2002. DSM-IV: Manual Diagnóstico e Estatístico de Transtornos Mentais. Referência Rápida 4a ed. Porto Alegre: Artmed; 1995. p.321. [ Links ]

12 Kadri N, Agoub M, El Gnaoui S, Berrada S, Moussaoui D. Prevalence of anxiety disorders: a population-based epidemiological study in metropolitan area of Casablanca, Morocco. Ann Gen Psychiatry 2007;6:6. [ Links ]

13 Andrade LH, Wang YP, Andreoni S, et al. Mental disorders in megacities: findings from the Sao Paulo megacity mental health survey, Brazil. PloS One 2012;7:e31879. [ Links ]

14 Weiller E, Bisserbe JC, Maier W, Lecrubier Y. Prevalence and recognition of anxiety syndromes in five European primary care settings. A report from the WHO study on Psychological Problems in General Health Care. Brit J Psychiatry Suppl 1998:18-23. [ Links ]

15 Airaksinen E, Larsson M, Forsell Y. Neuropsychological functions in anxiety disorders in population-based samples: evidence of episodic memory dysfunction. J Psychiatric Res 2005;39:207-214. [ Links ]

16 Bolshaw M, Greca DV, Nardi AE, Cheniaux Júnior E, Fonseca RPF, Fernandez JL. Funções cognitivas no transtorno do pânico: um estudo comparativo com controles saudáveis. PSICO 2011;42:87-97. [ Links ]

17 Gilbert SJ, Burgess PW. Executive function. Curr Biol 2008;18:110114. [ Links ]

18 Malloy-Diniz LF, Sedo M, Fuentes D, Leite, WB. Neuropsicologia das Funções Executivas. In Fuentes D, Malloy-Diniz LF, Carmargo CHP, Cosenza R (Eds), Neuropsicologia, Teoria e Prática. Porto Alegre: Artmed; 2008:187-206. [ Links ]

19 Chan RC, Shum D, Toulopoulou T, Chen EY. Assessment of executive functions: review of instruments and identification of critical issues. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2008;23:201-216. [ Links ]

20 Menzies L, Achard S, Chamberlain SR, et al. Neurocognitive endophenotypes of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain 2007;130:3223-3236. [ Links ]

21 Cornblatt BA, Malhotra AK. Impaired attention as an endophenotype for molecular genetic studies of schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet 2001;105:11-15. [ Links ]

22 Rund BR. A review of longitudinal studies of cognitive functions in schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Bull 1998;24:425-435. [ Links ]

23 Cavedini P, Zorzi C, Piccinni M, Cavallini MC, Bellodi L. Executive dysfunctions in obsessive-compulsive patients and unaffected relatives: searching for a new intermediate phenotype. Biol Psychiatry 2010;67:1178-1184. [ Links ]

24 Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Blackwell AD, Clark L, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. A neuropsychological comparison of obsessive-compulsive disorder and trichotillomania. Neuropsychologia 2007;45:654-662. [ Links ]

25 Tukel R, Gurvit H, Ertekin BA, et al. Neuropsychological function in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Compr Psychiatry 2012;53:167-175. [ Links ]

26 Abramovitch A, Dar R, Schweiger A, Hermesh H. Neuropsychological impairments and their association with obsessive-compulsive symptom severity in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2011;26:364-376. [ Links ]

27 Aycicegi A, Dinn WM, Harris CL, Erkmen H. Neuropsychological function in obsessive-compulsive disorder: effects of comorbid conditions on task performance. Europ Psychiatry 2003;18:241-248. [ Links ]

28 Fenger MM, Gade A, Adams KH, Hansen ES, Bolwig TG, Knudsen GM. Cognitive deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder on tests of frontal lobe functions. Nordic J Psychiatry 2005;59:39-44. [ Links ]

29 Starcke K, Tuschen-Caffier B, Markowitsch HJ, Brand M. Dissociation of decisions in ambiguous and risky situations in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res 2010;175:114-120. [ Links ]

30 Rampacher F, Lennertz L, Vogeley A, et al. Evidence for specific cognitive deficits in visual information processing in patients with OCD compared to patients with unipolar depression. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2010;34:984-991. [ Links ]

31 Shin NY, Lee TY, Kim E, Kwon JS. Cognitive functioning in obsessivecompulsive disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychol Med 2014;44:1121-1130. [ Links ]

32 Kozaric-Kovacic D, Mestrovic AH, Rak D, Muzinic L, Marinic I. Cognitive status of Croatian combat veterans and their compensation-seeking. J Forensic Psychiatry Psychol 2013;24:532-548. [ Links ]

33 Vasterling JJ, Duke LM, Brailey K, Constans JI, Allain AN, Jr., Sutker PB. Attention, learning, and memory performances and intellectual resources in Vietnam veterans: PTSD and no disorder comparisons. Neuropsychology 2002;16:5-14. [ Links ]

34 Aupperle RL, Allard CB, Grimes EM, et al. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activation during emotional anticipation and neuropsychological performance in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2012; 69:360-371. [ Links ]

35 Cohen BE, Neylan TC, Yaffe K, Samuelson KW, Li Y, Barnes DE. Posttraumatic stress disorder and cognitive function: findings from the mind your heart study. J Clin Psychiatry 2013;74:1063-1070. [ Links ]

36 Leskin LP, White PM. Attentional networks reveal executive function deficits in posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychology 2007;21: 275-284. [ Links ]

37 Shucard JL, McCabe DC, Szymanski H. An event-related potential study of attention deficits in posttraumatic stress disorder during auditory and visual Go/NoGo continuous performance tasks. Biol Psychol 2008;79:223-233. [ Links ]

38 Scott JC, Matt GE, Wrocklage KM, et al. A quantitative meta-analysis of neurocognitive functioning in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychol Bull 2015;141:105-140. [ Links ]

39 Cohen LJ, Hollander E, DeCaria CM, et al. Specificity of neuropsychological impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comparison with social phobic and normal control subjects. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1996;8:82-85. [ Links ]

40 Amir N, Bomyea J. Working memory capacity in generalized social phobia. J abnorm Psychol 2011;120:504-509. [ Links ]

41 Topçuoglu V, Fistikci N, Ekinci O, Gimzal GA, Comert AB. Assessment of executive functions in social phobia patients using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Turk Psikiyatri Derg 2009;20:322-331. [ Links ]

42 O'Toole, MS, Pedrersen, AD. A systematic review of neuropsychological performance in social anxiety disorder. Nordic J Psychiatry 2011; 65:147-161. [ Links ]

43 Castillo EP, Coy PEC, Shejet FO, Duran ET, Cabrera DM. Evaluación de funciones cognitivas: atención y memoria en pacientes con transtorno de pánico. Salud Mental 2010;33:481-488. [ Links ]

44 Alves MRP, Pereira VM, Machado S, Nardi AE, Silva ACO. Cognitive functions in patients with panic disorder: a literature review. Rev Bras Psiquiatr 2013;35:193-200. [ Links ]

45 Hazlett-Stevens H. Cognitive flexibility deficits in generalized anxiety disorder. In Paper presented at the annual convention of the American psychological association. San Francisco, CA; 2001. [ Links ]

46 Mathews A, MacLeod C. Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states. Behav Res Ther 1985;23:563-569. [ Links ]

47 Salters-Pedneault K, Suvak M, Roemer L. An experimental investigation of the effect of worry on responses to a discrimination learning task. Behav Ther 2008;39:251-261. [ Links ]

48 Manassis K, Tannock R, Garland EJ, Minde K, McInnes A, Clark S. The sounds of silence: language, cognition, and anxiety in selective mutism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2007;46:1187-1195. [ Links ]

49 Gray RM, Jordan CM, Ziegler RS, Livingston RB. Two sets of twins with selective mutism: neuropsychological findings. Child Neuropsychol 2002;8:41-51. [ Links ]

50 Rao NP, Reddy YC, Kumar KJ, Kandavel T, Chandrashekar CR. Are neuropsychological deficits trait markers in OCD? Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2008;32:1574-1579. [ Links ]

Viswanath B, Reddy YCJ, Kumar K J, Kandavel T, Chandrashekar CR. Cognitive endophenotypes in OCD: A study of unaffected siblings of probands with familial OCD. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatr 2009;15;33:610-615. [ Links ]

52 Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Menzies LA, et al. Impaired cognitive flexibility and motor inhibition in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:335-338. [ Links ]

53 Rajender G, Bhatia MS, Kanwal K, Malhotra S, Singh TB, Chaudhary D. Study of neurocognitive endophenotypes in drug-naive obsessivecompulsive disorder patients, their first-degree relatives and healthy controls. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2011;124:152-161. [ Links ]

54 da Rocha FF, Malloy-Diniz L, Lage NV, Romano-Silva MA, de Marco LA, Correa H. Decision-making impairment is related to serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism in a sample of patients with obsessivecompulsive disorder. Behav Brain Res 2008;195:159-163. [ Links ]

55 Zhang L, Dong Y, Ji Y, et al. Dissociation of decision making under ambiguity and decision making under risk: a neurocognitive endophenotype candidate for obsessive-compulsive disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2015;57:60-68. [ Links ]

56 Zhu C, Yu F, Ye R, et al. External error monitoring in subclinical obsessive-compulsive subjects: electrophysiological evidence from a Gambling Task. PloS One 2014;9:e90874. [ Links ]

57 Abramovitch A, Shaham N, Levin L, Bar-Hen M, Schweiger A. Response inhibition in a subclinical obsessive-compulsive sample. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2015;46:66-71. [ Links ]

58 Lennertz L, Rampacher F, Vogeley A, et al. Antisaccade performance in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and unaffected relatives: further evidence for impaired response inhibition as a candidate endophenotype. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2012;262:625-634. [ Links ]

59 Delorme R, Gousse V, Roy I, et al. Shared executive dysfunctions in unaffected relatives of patients with autism and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Eur Psychiatry 2007;22:32-38. [ Links ]

60 Chamberlain SR, Menzies L, Hampshire A, et al. Orbitofrontal dysfunction in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and their unaffected relatives. Science 2008;321:421-422. [ Links ]

61 de Vries FE, de Wit SJ, Cath DC, et al. Compensatory frontoparietal activity during working memory: an endophenotype of obsessivecompulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2014;76:878-887. [ Links ]

62 Ferreri F, Lapp LK, Peretti CS. Current research on cognitive aspects of anxiety disorders. Curr Opin Psychiatry 2011;24:49-54. [ Links ]

63 Cannon TD, Keller MC. Endophenotypes in the genetic analysis of mental disorders. Ann Rev Clin Psychol 2006;2:267-290. [ Links ]

64 Bechara A, Damasio, AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW. Insensivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 1994;50:7-15. [ Links ]

65 Brand M, Fujiwara E, Borsutzky S, Kalbe E, Kessler J, Markowitsch HJ. Decision-making deficits of korsakoff patients in a new gambling task with explicit rules: associations with executive functions. Neuropsychology 2005;19(3):267-277. [ Links ]

66 Chamberlain SR, Blackwell AD, Fineberg NA, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. The neuropsychology of obsessive compulsive disorder: the importance of failures in cognitive and behavioural inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2005;29:399-419. [ Links ]

67 Kuelz AK, Hohagen F, Voderholzer U. Neuropsychological performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a critical review. Biol Psychol 2004; 65:185-236. [ Links ]

68 Segalas C, Alonso P, Real E, et al. Memory and strategic processing in first-degree relatives of obsessive-compulsive patients. Psychol Med 2010;40:2001-2011. [ Links ]

69 Hashimoto N, Nakaaki S, Omori IM, et al. Distinct neuropsychological profiles of three major symptom dimensions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res 2011;187:166-173. [ Links ]

70 Tambs K, Czajkowsky N, Roysamb E, et al. Structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for dimensional representations of DSM-IV anxiety disorders. Brit J Psychiatry 2009;195:301-307. [ Links ]

2The study was conducted at the Institute of Psychology, Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Brazil.

Received: May 19, 2015; Accepted: July 20, 2015

Juliana de Lima Muller. Rua Ramiro Barcelos 2600 / room 119 / first floor - 90035-003 Porto Alegre RS - Brazil. E-mail: julianalm@hotmail.com

Disclosure: The authors report no conflits of interest.

Creative Commons License This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License