Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Does previous presentation of verbal fluency tasks affect verb fluency performance?

A apresentação prévia de tarefas de fluência verbal afeta o desempenho da fluência de verbos?

Abstracts

Background

: Performance on the verb fluency (VF) task may be influenced by administration procedures and demographic factors of each population.

Objective

: The aim of this study was to verify whether the previous administration of semantic and phonemic verbal fluency tasks can influence performance on VF; and to analyze the correlation of VF performance with education, age and type of errors in Brazilian healthy elderly.

Methods

: Sixty-two participants were subdivided into experimental (semantic and the phonemic fluency tasks were administered before the VF) and control groups (VF only). The total score and the types of errors on the VF task were determined. Additional information was computed for the correlational analysis.

Results

: VF performance did not differ statistically between experimental and control groups, but correlated positively with education and negatively with intrusions.

Conclusion

: The lack of influence of other verbal fluency tasks on performance of the VF task in elderly individuals allows the use of this order of administration. A strong influence of educational level on VF task performance reinforces the need for further studies in different populations.

verbal fluency; verb fluency; aging; elderly; neuropsychological test; neuropsychology


Introdução

: O desempenho na tarefa de fluência de verbos (FV) pode ser influenciado pelo modo de administração e por fatores sócio-demográficos de cada população.

Objetivo

: Este estudo teve como objetivo verificar se a administração prévia das tarefas de fluência verbal (fluência semântica e fonêmica) poderia influenciar no desempenho da FV, assim como analisar a correlação da FV com escolaridade, idade e tipos de erros em Brasileiros idosos saudáveis.

Métodos

: Sessenta e dois participantes foram divididos em grupo experimental (fluências semântica e fonêmica foram aplicadas antes da FV) e em grupo controle (apenas a FV foi aplicada). O escore total e os tipos de erros da FV foram verificados. Informações adicionais foram consideradas para a análise de correlação.

Resultados

: Não houve diferença estatisticamente significativa entre os grupos experimental e controle quanto ao desempenho da FV. A FV se correlacionou positivamente com a escolaridade e negativamente com as intrusões.

Conclusão

: A falta de influência de outras tarefas de fluência verbal no desempenho da FV em indivíduos idosos permite o uso dessa ordem de administração. Uma forte influência da escolaridade sobre o desempenho da FV reforça a necessidade de estudos em diferentes populações.

fluência verbal; fluência de verbos; envelhecimento; idoso; teste neuropsicológico; neuropsicologia


INTRODUCTION

Verb Fluency (VF) is a task of verbal fluency that requests the subject to say as many verbs (or "things that people do") as they can within one minute. The classic verbal fluency tasks (semantic and phonemic) request the production of nouns or object categories.11. Pekkala S. Verbal Fluency Tasks and the Neuropsychology of Language. In: Miriam Faust (Editor). The Handbook of the Neuropsychology of Language. vol. 1 & 2. Blackwell Publishing Ltd.; 2012. p. 619-34. Patients with different types of frontal brain pathology exhibit worst performance on the VF than on other verbal fluency tasks. This dissociation has been described in patients with nonfluent Primary Progressive Aphasia, behavioral Frontotemporal Dementia, HIV-1 infection, schizophrenia and Parkinson's disease.22. Badcock JC, Dragovic M, Garrett C, Jablensky A. Action (verb) fluency in schizophrenia: Getting a grip on odd speech. Schizophr Res 2011; 126:138-143.

3. Davis C, Heidler-Gary J, Gottesman RF, et al. Action versus animal naming fluency in subcortical dementia, frontal dementias, and Alzheimer's disease. Neurocase 2010;16:259-266.

4. Delbeuck X, Debachy B, Pasquier F, Moroni C. Action and noun fluency testing to distinguish between Alzheimer's disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2013;35:259-268.

5. McDowd J, Hoffman L, Rozek E, et al. Understanding verbal fluency in healthy aging, Alzheimer's disease, and Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychology 2011;25:210-225.

6. Woods SP, Morgan EE, Dawson M, Scott JC, Grant I; HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center (HNRC) Group. Action (verb) fluency predicts dependence in instrumental activities of daily living in persons infected with HIV-1. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2006;28:1030-1042.

7. Piatt AL, Fields JA, Paolo AM, Koller WC, Tröster AI. Lexical, semantic, and action verbal fluency in Parkinson's disease with and without dementia. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1999;21:435-443.

8. Rodrigues IT, Ferreira JJ, Coelho M, Rosa MM, Castro-Caldas A. Action verbal fluency in Parkinson's patients. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2015; 73:520-525.

9. Signorini M, Volpato C. Action fluency in Parkinson's disease: a follow-up study. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc 2006;21:467-472.

10. Woods SP, Iudicello JE, Dawson MS, et al. HIV-associated deficits in action (verb) generation may reflect astrocytosis. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2010;32:522-527.

11. Woods SP, Carey CL, Tröster AI, Grant I, HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center (HNRC) Group. Action (verb) generation in HIV-1 infection. Neuropsychologia 2005;43:1144-11451.
-1212. Woods SP, Weinborn M, Posada C, O'Grady J. Preliminary evidence for impaired rapid verb generation in schizophrenia. Brain Lang 2007; 102:46-51.

Evidence of this dissociation raises the possibility of using the VF task as a marker of frontosubcortical impairment.1313. Beber BC, Chaves MLF. The Basis and Applications of the Action Fluency and Action Naming Tasks. Dement Neuropsychol 2014;8:47-57. Given this clinical applicability, the task has been used in neuropsychological investigations. However, the administration of the VF task, akin to other neuropsychological tasks, might be influenced by administration procedures and/or demographic factors of individual populations. Previous studies using different tasks of verbal fluency, administered the VF after other verbal fluency tasks.66. Woods SP, Morgan EE, Dawson M, Scott JC, Grant I; HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center (HNRC) Group. Action (verb) fluency predicts dependence in instrumental activities of daily living in persons infected with HIV-1. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2006;28:1030-1042.

7. Piatt AL, Fields JA, Paolo AM, Koller WC, Tröster AI. Lexical, semantic, and action verbal fluency in Parkinson's disease with and without dementia. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1999;21:435-443.
-88. Rodrigues IT, Ferreira JJ, Coelho M, Rosa MM, Castro-Caldas A. Action verbal fluency in Parkinson's patients. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2015; 73:520-525.,1111. Woods SP, Carey CL, Tröster AI, Grant I, HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center (HNRC) Group. Action (verb) generation in HIV-1 infection. Neuropsychologia 2005;43:1144-11451.,1414. Ostberg P, Fernaeus S-E, Hellström K, Bogdanovic N, Wahlund L-O. Impaired verb fluency: a sign of mild cognitive impairment. Brain Lang 2005;95:273-279. The previous administration of other verbal fluency tasks might influence VF scores. The application of the California Verbal Learning Test-II (CVLT-II) prior to verbal fluency tasks produced higher verbal fluency scores.1515. Lloyd KP, Higginson CI, Lating JM, Coiro MJ. Test-Order Effects: A Measure of Verbal Memory Influences Performance on a Measure of Verbal Fluency. Appl Neuropsychol 2012;19:299-304. Nonetheless, the effect of verbal fluency tasks presented before the VF task has not yet been studied.

The influence of age and education on the VF task has been previously evaluated.1616. Casals-Coll M, Sánchez-Benavides G, Quintana M, et al. Spanish normative studies in young adults (NEURONORMA young adults project): norms for verbal fluency tests. Neurol Barc Spain 2013;28:33-40.

17. Piatt AL, Fields JA, Paolo AM, Tröster A. Action verbal fluency normative data for the elderly. Brain Lang 2004;89:580-583.
-1818. Stokholm J, Jørgensen K, Vogel A. Performances on five verbal fluency tests in a healthy, elderly Danish sample. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2013;20:22-33. Education showed higher impact on performance than age. However, these studies were carried out in developed countries in populations with higher educational level. Investigations of performance in low-educated populations from underdeveloped or developing areas of the world are still lacking.

The aim of this study was to verify whether the previous administration of semantic and phonemic verbal fluency tasks can influence performance on the VF task in Brazilian healthy elderly. The correlation of education, age and type of errors with the VF task was also analyzed.

METHODS

Translation and cultural adaptation of verb fluency instructions. The instructions of the VF task are pivotal for proper application of the task. The instructions have to be easily understandable in terms of language as well as cultural features of the population in which the VF will be carried out.

In the present investigation, two researchers produced a previous translation of the VF task instructions from the original,1919. Piatt AL, Fields JA, Paolo AM, Tröster AI. Action (verb naming) fluency as an executive function measure: convergent and divergent evidence of validity. Neuropsychologia 1999;37:1499-503. which included all sentences of the original instructions. The original instructions are as follows:

"I'd like you to tell me as many different things as you can think of that people do. I don't want you to use the same word with different endings, like eat, eating, eaten. Also, just give me single words such as eat, or smell, rather than a sentence. Can you give me an example of something that people do?" (Any verb response is acceptable) If the response is acceptable: "That's the idea. Now in one minute, tell me as many different things as you can think of that people do".

A pilot study was carried out for the application of the translated VF instructions in an elderly group of mixed educational level. The aim of this pilot study was to define a Brazilian Portuguese version with easily understandable instructions for all educational levels (from illiterate to higher education). The investigators administered the direct translation of the instruction to a group of participants. Many participants had difficulty understanding the passages "I don't want you to use the same word with different endings" as well as "give me single words...". This difficulty probably occurred because most participants had a lower educational level, leading to greater difficulty in understanding the linguistic aspects of words. Based on these results, we devised a simplified version containing the essential instructions to execute the task. In an application of this adapted version, the participants showed satisfactory comprehension of the task.

The final Brazilian Portuguese instructions were as follows:

"Irei marcar um minuto, e durante este tempo você terá que dizer o máximo de palavras que significam coisas que podemos fazer, como por exemplo, comer e andar. Você entendeu?" Se a resposta for sim: "Dê-me um exemplo, então." Se o exemplo estiver correto: "Agora, iremos começar. Diga-me outras palavras que representam coisas que podemos fazer, além de comer e andar". Se o exemplo dado estiver incorreto, dê a explicação novamente com mais um exemplo. Para pessoas com maior escolaridade podem ser utilizados os termos "verbo" ou "ação" para explicar a tarefa.

The English translation of the Brazilian Portuguese instructions is:

"I will set one minute and during this time you will have to tell me as many words that mean things that people do as you can, for example eat and walk. Do you understand?" If the answer is yes: "Give me an example". If the example is correct: "We are going to start now. Tell me other words that mean things that people do, besides eat and walk." If the given example is incorrect, give the explanation again with one more example. In the case of individuals with high educational level, the expressions "verb" or "action" can be used to explain the task.

Participants. Sixty-two elderly subjects were recruited from social groups near the clinical research center of the Hospital de Clínicas de Porto Alegre (HCPA) or drawn from relatives or companions of patients of the dementia clinic at the same hospital. The inclusion criteria were: Brazilian Portuguese as the first language, at least 60 years of age, and no cognitive complaints. Individuals reporting medical history of any neurological and/or psychiatric diseases, uncorrected visual or hearing problems, and Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) scores below the cut-off for education2020. Kochhann R, Varela JS, Lisboa CS de M, Chaves MLF. The mini mental state examination: Review of cutoff points adjusted for schooling in a large southern Brazilian sample. Dement Neuropsychol 2010;4:35-41. were excluded.

Procedures. Participants were randomly subdivided into experimental (EG) and control (CG) groups. The EG comprised 32 participants who were administered the semantic2121. Brucki SMD, Malheiros SMF, Okamoto IH, Bertolucci PHF. Normative data: category verbal fluency. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 1997;55:56-61.,2222. Brucki S, Rocha M. Category fluency test: effects of age, gender and education on total scores, clustering and switching in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking subjects. Braz J Med Biol Res 2004;37:1771-1777. and phonemic fluency (FAS)2323. Machado TH, Fichman HC, Santos EL, et al. Normative data for healthy elderly on the phonemic verbal fluency task - FAS. Dement Neuropsychol 2009;3:55-60. tasks before administration of the VF task. Thirty participants comprised the CG to whom the VF task was administered alone. Educational level, age, gender and handedness were matched between groups to ensure group homogeneity. Demographic data were similar between groups (Table 1).

Table 1.
Demographic and clinical data of participants.

The final score on each verbal fluency task was the total number of correct items produced during one minute. Perseverations (repetition or inflection of a previously generated response, including the participant's self-generated word as the example or the example used in the instructions) and intrusions (words from other grammatical classes or neologisms) were scored separately to verify the errors produced by the participants.

This study was approved by the HCPA Research Ethics Committee and all participants gave written informed consent.

Data analyses. All analyses were performed using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) version 18. The continuous variables were expressed as mean, standard deviation, range, and median while categorical variables were expressed as absolute and relative frequencies. The one-sample Shapiro-Wilk test was conducted to evaluate normality. The t-test was performed for the continuous variables with normal distribution, and Mann-Whitney or Kruskal-Wallis test for the continuous variables without normal distribution. The categorical variables were analyzed using the Chi-square association test. Education, age, and type of errors were evaluated with correlational analyses (Spearman's correlation). Subsequently, education was classified into subgroups (low, medium and high). A critical alpha of .05 was employed for the analyses.

RESULTS

No significant difference was observed in VF performance between the CG and EG (Mann-Whitney test; p = 0.133) (Table 2). Likewise, intrusions (p = 0.670) and perseverations (p = 0.969) did not differ between groups (Table 2). Since the previous application of the verbal fluency tasks did not affect the VF, we used data from the total sample for subsequent analyses. The distribution of VF total correct score, intrusions and perseverations are given in Table 3.

Table 2.
Distribution and comparison of verbal fluencies in Control and Experimental Groups

Table 3.
Distribution of Verb Fluency results (total score and errors) in the total sample of Brazilian elderly people.

The total score on the VF correlated positively with education (rho = 0.616; p = 0.001) and negatively with intrusions (rho = -0.318; p = 0.05) using Spearman correlation. Age (rho = -0.055; p > 0.05) and perseverations (rho = -0.017; p > 0.05) did not correlate with VF scores.

Participants were subdivided into 3 groups of education, low (0-5 years), medium (6-11 years) and high (≥ 12 years), to compare VF scores among groups. The high education group showed significantly higher scores than the low education group (p < 0.05) (Table 4).

Table 4.
Distribution of Verb Fluency results (total score) in Brazilian elderly people according to education.

DISCUSSION

We conducted this investigation to verify whether previous application of verbal fluency tasks can influence VF performance, and to verify the correlation of education, age, and type of errors with VF scores in a sample of Brazilian elderly. The analysis of f semantic and phonemic verbal fluency performance was not the aim of the study, therefore the reverse order (verbal fluency after VF) was not investigated. The VF scores were similar in both groups, demonstrating no effect of the application of verbal fluency tasks prior to the VF task.

Verbal fluency tasks demand a fast search of words in the lexical-semantic storage. In addition, other cognitive processes are also important, such as working memory and executive functions.2424. Steiner VAG. Semantic, phonologic, and verb fluency in Huntington's disease Tese de Doutorado, Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de São Paulo; São Paulo, 2013. The VF task requires the production of verbs. Verbs are more complex than nouns because they have more inflexions, more syntactic relationships with other words in the sentence, are acquired later in the process of language acquisition, have a less hierarchic semantic network, a greater number of high frequency words and are associated with longer reaction times in naming than nouns.2525. Beber BC. Estudo da natureza do prejuízo na fluência e nomeação de verbos na doença de Alzheimer e na afasia progressiva primária não-fuente. Tese de Doutorado. Programa de Pós-Graduação em Medicina: Ciências Médicas. Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul (UFRGS); Porto Alegre-RS, 2014. All these features together mean the VF task places greater demands on executive functions and involves more activation in the semantic network than other verbal fluency tasks.

Verbs are a grammatical class of words referring to concepts of actions and have a syntactic relationship with nouns. In the event of administration of semantic and phonemic verbal fluency tasks prior to the VF task, the activation caused by nouns (from semantic and phonemic fluency tasks) in the semantic or lexical networks might propagate and activate syntactic networks or concepts of actions possibly associated with the nouns from the previous fluency tasks. The prior application of verbal fluency tasks may therefore facilitate retrieval of a higher number of verbs in the last task as an effect of practice or priming, consistent with the concept of the semantic network theory2626. Miller GA, Fellbaum C. Semantic networks of English. Cognition 1991; 41:197-229.,2727. Nelson DL, Kitto K, Galea D, McEvoy CL, Bruza PD. How activation, entanglement, and searching a semantic network contribute to event memory. Mem Cognit 2013;41:797-819.. On the other hand, the presentation of the verbal fluency tasks before the VF task might hamper performance on the latter task as a consequence of fatigue. However, no effect of the prior administration of the verbal fluency task on the VF task was found in this investigation.

We observed significant correlation of education with VF total scores, as well as a significant difference between high and low education subgroups. The median observed was 7.50 in the low group, 11.00 in the medium, and 15.50 in the high education group. Participants with high educational level produced a greater number of verbs on this task. The effect of education on the VF task has been demonstrated in elderly populations. However, these populations typically had a higher educational level than that of the present sample.55. McDowd J, Hoffman L, Rozek E, et al. Understanding verbal fluency in healthy aging, Alzheimer's disease, and Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychology 2011;25:210-225.,1414. Ostberg P, Fernaeus S-E, Hellström K, Bogdanovic N, Wahlund L-O. Impaired verb fluency: a sign of mild cognitive impairment. Brain Lang 2005;95:273-279.,1616. Casals-Coll M, Sánchez-Benavides G, Quintana M, et al. Spanish normative studies in young adults (NEURONORMA young adults project): norms for verbal fluency tests. Neurol Barc Spain 2013;28:33-40.

17. Piatt AL, Fields JA, Paolo AM, Tröster A. Action verbal fluency normative data for the elderly. Brain Lang 2004;89:580-583.
-1818. Stokholm J, Jørgensen K, Vogel A. Performances on five verbal fluency tests in a healthy, elderly Danish sample. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2013;20:22-33. Individuals with higher educational levels may be better prepared for the task by recruiting the grammatical framework needed to produce action words.1414. Ostberg P, Fernaeus S-E, Hellström K, Bogdanovic N, Wahlund L-O. Impaired verb fluency: a sign of mild cognitive impairment. Brain Lang 2005;95:273-279. Our findings demonstrated the effect of education in this particular cognitive task. Education as an indicator of cognitive reserve has been investigated in different clinical conditions and in normal aging, showing a protective effect.2424. Steiner VAG. Semantic, phonologic, and verb fluency in Huntington's disease Tese de Doutorado, Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de São Paulo; São Paulo, 2013. Furthermore, individuals with higher educational levels have greater cognitive reserve and consequently more neural connections, making it easier to integrate information and access the lexicon.

We also observed a significant negative correlation of intrusions with VF total scores. The more verbs generated, the fewer intrusions registered, i.e., better, faster grammatical class processing is associated with fewer errors. We assume that intrusions may reflect difficulty in understanding the instructions of the task or difficulty accessing the grammatical class required. Higher intrusions may be a sign of disruption of VF processing due to the aging process. However, this relationship needs to be better investigated by studying VF errors in different clinical conditions.

On the other hand, perseverations can represent memory difficulty or attempts to fill time when new words do not come to mind. However, we found no correlation of perseveration with VF performance in this sample of healthy elderly.

The present investigation showed the lack of influence of other verbal fluency tasks on the performance of the VF task in elderly individuals, which allows the use of this order of administration in clinical and research settings. A strong influence of educational level on VF task performance was also observed, reinforcing the need for further studies in different populations. The cultural and demographic profile of North American or European populations is very different from those observed in developing countries such as Brazil. To our knowledge, apart from some studies in Brazilian patients with clinical conditions,2424. Steiner VAG. Semantic, phonologic, and verb fluency in Huntington's disease Tese de Doutorado, Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de São Paulo; São Paulo, 2013.,2929. Azambuja MJ, Haddad MS, Radanovic M, Barbosa ER, Mansur LL. Semantic, phonologic, and verb fluency in Huntington's disease. Dement Neuropsychol 2007;1:381-385.

30. Beber BC, Cruz AN da, Chaves ML. A behavioral study of the nature of verb production deficits in Alzheimer's disease. Brain Lang 2015;149:128-134.
-3131. Mirandez RM. Estudo da fluência verbal em categorias múltiplas no comprometimento cognitivo leve. Dissertação de Mestrado. Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de São Paulo; São Paulo, 2014. this is the first study focusing on the application of the VF task in a healthy elderly Brazilian population. Future studies should explore VF performance in younger population and other neurological disorders.

References

  • 1
    Pekkala S. Verbal Fluency Tasks and the Neuropsychology of Language. In: Miriam Faust (Editor). The Handbook of the Neuropsychology of Language. vol. 1 & 2. Blackwell Publishing Ltd.; 2012. p. 619-34.
  • 2
    Badcock JC, Dragovic M, Garrett C, Jablensky A. Action (verb) fluency in schizophrenia: Getting a grip on odd speech. Schizophr Res 2011; 126:138-143.
  • 3
    Davis C, Heidler-Gary J, Gottesman RF, et al. Action versus animal naming fluency in subcortical dementia, frontal dementias, and Alzheimer's disease. Neurocase 2010;16:259-266.
  • 4
    Delbeuck X, Debachy B, Pasquier F, Moroni C. Action and noun fluency testing to distinguish between Alzheimer's disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2013;35:259-268.
  • 5
    McDowd J, Hoffman L, Rozek E, et al. Understanding verbal fluency in healthy aging, Alzheimer's disease, and Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychology 2011;25:210-225.
  • 6
    Woods SP, Morgan EE, Dawson M, Scott JC, Grant I; HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center (HNRC) Group. Action (verb) fluency predicts dependence in instrumental activities of daily living in persons infected with HIV-1. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2006;28:1030-1042.
  • 7
    Piatt AL, Fields JA, Paolo AM, Koller WC, Tröster AI. Lexical, semantic, and action verbal fluency in Parkinson's disease with and without dementia. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1999;21:435-443.
  • 8
    Rodrigues IT, Ferreira JJ, Coelho M, Rosa MM, Castro-Caldas A. Action verbal fluency in Parkinson's patients. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2015; 73:520-525.
  • 9
    Signorini M, Volpato C. Action fluency in Parkinson's disease: a follow-up study. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc 2006;21:467-472.
  • 10
    Woods SP, Iudicello JE, Dawson MS, et al. HIV-associated deficits in action (verb) generation may reflect astrocytosis. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2010;32:522-527.
  • 11
    Woods SP, Carey CL, Tröster AI, Grant I, HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center (HNRC) Group. Action (verb) generation in HIV-1 infection. Neuropsychologia 2005;43:1144-11451.
  • 12
    Woods SP, Weinborn M, Posada C, O'Grady J. Preliminary evidence for impaired rapid verb generation in schizophrenia. Brain Lang 2007; 102:46-51.
  • 13
    Beber BC, Chaves MLF. The Basis and Applications of the Action Fluency and Action Naming Tasks. Dement Neuropsychol 2014;8:47-57.
  • 14
    Ostberg P, Fernaeus S-E, Hellström K, Bogdanovic N, Wahlund L-O. Impaired verb fluency: a sign of mild cognitive impairment. Brain Lang 2005;95:273-279.
  • 15
    Lloyd KP, Higginson CI, Lating JM, Coiro MJ. Test-Order Effects: A Measure of Verbal Memory Influences Performance on a Measure of Verbal Fluency. Appl Neuropsychol 2012;19:299-304.
  • 16
    Casals-Coll M, Sánchez-Benavides G, Quintana M, et al. Spanish normative studies in young adults (NEURONORMA young adults project): norms for verbal fluency tests. Neurol Barc Spain 2013;28:33-40.
  • 17
    Piatt AL, Fields JA, Paolo AM, Tröster A. Action verbal fluency normative data for the elderly. Brain Lang 2004;89:580-583.
  • 18
    Stokholm J, Jørgensen K, Vogel A. Performances on five verbal fluency tests in a healthy, elderly Danish sample. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2013;20:22-33.
  • 19
    Piatt AL, Fields JA, Paolo AM, Tröster AI. Action (verb naming) fluency as an executive function measure: convergent and divergent evidence of validity. Neuropsychologia 1999;37:1499-503.
  • 20
    Kochhann R, Varela JS, Lisboa CS de M, Chaves MLF. The mini mental state examination: Review of cutoff points adjusted for schooling in a large southern Brazilian sample. Dement Neuropsychol 2010;4:35-41.
  • 21
    Brucki SMD, Malheiros SMF, Okamoto IH, Bertolucci PHF. Normative data: category verbal fluency. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 1997;55:56-61.
  • 22
    Brucki S, Rocha M. Category fluency test: effects of age, gender and education on total scores, clustering and switching in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking subjects. Braz J Med Biol Res 2004;37:1771-1777.
  • 23
    Machado TH, Fichman HC, Santos EL, et al. Normative data for healthy elderly on the phonemic verbal fluency task - FAS. Dement Neuropsychol 2009;3:55-60.
  • 24
    Steiner VAG. Semantic, phonologic, and verb fluency in Huntington's disease Tese de Doutorado, Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de São Paulo; São Paulo, 2013.
  • 25
    Beber BC. Estudo da natureza do prejuízo na fluência e nomeação de verbos na doença de Alzheimer e na afasia progressiva primária não-fuente. Tese de Doutorado. Programa de Pós-Graduação em Medicina: Ciências Médicas. Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul (UFRGS); Porto Alegre-RS, 2014.
  • 26
    Miller GA, Fellbaum C. Semantic networks of English. Cognition 1991; 41:197-229.
  • 27
    Nelson DL, Kitto K, Galea D, McEvoy CL, Bruza PD. How activation, entanglement, and searching a semantic network contribute to event memory. Mem Cognit 2013;41:797-819.
  • 28
    Stern Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2002;8:448-460.
  • 29
    Azambuja MJ, Haddad MS, Radanovic M, Barbosa ER, Mansur LL. Semantic, phonologic, and verb fluency in Huntington's disease. Dement Neuropsychol 2007;1:381-385.
  • 30
    Beber BC, Cruz AN da, Chaves ML. A behavioral study of the nature of verb production deficits in Alzheimer's disease. Brain Lang 2015;149:128-134.
  • 31
    Mirandez RM. Estudo da fluência verbal em categorias múltiplas no comprometimento cognitivo leve. Dissertação de Mestrado. Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de São Paulo; São Paulo, 2014.
  • Funding. This study was supported by the Fundo de Incentivo à Pesquisa e Eventos (FIPE) of the Hospital de Clínicas de Porto Alegre (HCPA) and by a Doctoral scholarship from the Coordenação de Desenvolvimento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES).
  • This study was conducted at the Hospital de Clínicas de Porto Alegre (HCPA), Porto Alegre RS, Brazil

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Jan-Mar 2016

History

  • Received
    03 Nov 2015
  • Accepted
    05 Jan 2016
Academia Brasileira de Neurologia, Departamento de Neurologia Cognitiva e Envelhecimento R. Vergueiro, 1353 sl.1404 - Ed. Top Towers Offices, Torre Norte, São Paulo, SP, Brazil, CEP 04101-000, Tel.: +55 11 5084-9463 | +55 11 5083-3876 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistadementia@abneuro.org.br | demneuropsy@uol.com.br