Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Are “cool” executive function impairments more salient in ADHD symptoms than in reading disability?

DÉFICITS EM FUNÇÕES EXECUTIVAS “FRIAS” SÃO MAIS SALIENTES NOS SINTOMAS DE TDAH QUE NA DIFICULDADE DE LEITURA?

ABSTRACT

Introduction:

Reading disability (RD) and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) symptoms often co-occur in school-age children.

Methods:

The present study evaluated the performance of 216 Brazilian children from 3rd and 4th grades on “cool” executive function (EF) abilities and phonological processing. The children were divided into three groups: those with ADHD symptoms only, those with RD only, and controls.

Results:

MANOVA analyses, controlling for age and nonverbal intelligence, showed worse performance for the RD group, compared to the ADHD symptoms group, on measures of phonological processing (phonemic awareness, phonological short-term memory, and lexical access) and “cool” EF components (orthographic verbal fluency and processing speed). The ADHD symptoms group did not differ from the control group on the majority of the “cool” EF tasks. Compared to the control group, the ADHD symptoms group and the RD group both showed significantly more errors in rapid automatized naming of figures, which evaluates the inhibition component of EF; performance on this task was similar for these groups.

Conclusion:

We conclude that children with RD have greater impairment in phonological processing and “cool” EF compared to those with ADHD symptoms. Furthermore, deficits in inhibitory control may be shared among children with both conditions.

Key words:
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; reading disability; cognition; child; neuropsychology

RESUMO

Introdução:

Dificuldades de leitura (DL) e sintomas do Transtorno de Déficit de Atenção e Hiperatividade (TDAH) frequentemente coocorrem em crianças escolares.

Métodos:

O presente estudo comparou o desempenho em FE “frias” e processamento fonológico de 216 crianças brasileiras de 3ª e 4ª anos, que foram divididas em três grupos: apenas com sintomas de TDAH, apenas em DL e controles.

Resultados:

As análises de MANOVA, controlando para idade e inteligência não-verbal, indicaram que o grupo com DL apresentou desempenho significativamente inferior ao grupo com sintomas de TDAH nas medidas de processamento fonológico (consciência fonológica, memória verbal de curto prazo e acesso lexical) e em componentes das FE “frias” (fluência verbal ortográfica e velocidade de processamento). O grupo com sintomas de TDAH não se diferiram do grupo controle na maior parte das tarefas de FE “frias”. Ambos os grupos com sintomas de TDAH e DL apresentaram desempenhos significativamente menores (mais erros) em comparação às crianças de desenvolvimento típico na tarefa de Nomeação Seriada Rápida de figuras que avalia o componente de controle inibitório, e o desempenho foi semelhante entre os grupos.

Conclusão:

Conclui-se que crianças com DL apresentam maior comprometimento em processamento fonológico e FE “frias” em comparação àquelas com sintomas de TDAH e que déficits no controle inibitório podem ser compartilhados entre crianças com ambas as condições.

Palavras-chave:
transtorno do déficit de atenção com hiperatividade; dificuldade de leitura; cognição; criança; neuropsicologia

Reading Disability (RD) and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) are two of the most common neurodevelopmental disorders in children.11 McGrath L, Pennington B, Shanahan M, Santerre-Lemmon L, Barnard H, Willcutt E, et al. A multiple deficit model of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Searching for shared cognitive deficits. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52(5):547-57. It is estimated that between 6% and 17% of school-age children are diagnosed with RD,22 Fletcher JM. Dyslexia: The evolution of a scientific concept. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15(4):501-8. and around 5% exhibit ADHD.33 Polanczyk G, Lima MS, Horta BL, Biederman J, Rohde LA. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: A systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(6):942. Furthermore, the rate of comorbidity for these two conditions is high, at between 15% and 30%. Both conditions are persistent and constitute risk factors for difficulties in academic and social skills.44 Peterson R, Boada R, McGrath L, Willcutt E, Olson R, Pennington B. Cognitive prediction of reading, math, and attention: Shared and unique influences. J Learn Disabil. J Learn Disabil. 2017;50(4):408-21. The multiple deficit model of neurodevelopmental disorders55 Pennington BF. From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition. 2006;101(2):385-413. proposes that ADHD and RD have a multifactorial etiology and that a single disorder is produced by a combination of specific and shared deficits, with the shared deficits accounting for the comorbidity between disorders.11 McGrath L, Pennington B, Shanahan M, Santerre-Lemmon L, Barnard H, Willcutt E, et al. A multiple deficit model of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Searching for shared cognitive deficits. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52(5):547-57.,44 Peterson R, Boada R, McGrath L, Willcutt E, Olson R, Pennington B. Cognitive prediction of reading, math, and attention: Shared and unique influences. J Learn Disabil. J Learn Disabil. 2017;50(4):408-21.

In line with this assumption, most authors view RD as a developmental impairment that is best studied based on cognitive causal models.66 Miciak J, Cirino PT, Ahmed Y, Reid E, Vaughn S. Executive Functions and Response to Intervention: Identification of Students Struggling With Reading Comprehension. Learn Disabil Q. 2019;42(1):17-31.,77 Walda SAE, van Weerdenburg M, Wijnants ML, Bosman AMT. Progress in reading and spelling of dyslexic children is not affected by executive functioning. Res Dev Disabil. 2014;35(12):3431-54. RD is a well-known disorder that profoundly disrupts reading development, at the basis of which are phonological processing deficits.88 Vellutino FR, Fletcher JM, Snowling MJ, Scanlon DM. Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45(1):2-40. Studies have shown that deficits in other neurocognitive domains, such as Executive Functions (EF), might also be present in children with RD.99 Moura O, Simões MR, Pereira M. Executive functioning in children with developmental dyslexia. Clin Neuropsychol. 2015;28(Suppl 1):S20-41. A meta-analysis has found an effect size of .67 for differences between children with and without RD in EF.66 Miciak J, Cirino PT, Ahmed Y, Reid E, Vaughn S. Executive Functions and Response to Intervention: Identification of Students Struggling With Reading Comprehension. Learn Disabil Q. 2019;42(1):17-31. However, this does not imply that EF are causally related to word reading ability.1010 Sesma HW, Mahone EM, Levine T, Eason SH, Cutting LE. The Contribution of Executive Skills to Reading Comprehension. Child Neuropsychol. 2009;15(3):232-46. Conversely, ADHD is regarded as a more heterogeneous condition that is associated with distinct patterns of neuropsychological impairments.1111 Coghill D, Seth S, Matthews K. A comprehensive assessment of memory, delay version, timing, inhibition, decision making and variability in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Advancing beyond the three-pathway models. Psychol Med. 2014;44(09):1989-2001.

One important cognitive model of EF distinguishes between “hot” and “cool” EF components.1212 Zelazo PD, Carlson SM. Hot and Cool Executive Function in Childhood and dolescence: Development and Plasticity. Child Dev Perspect. 2012;6(4):354-60.,1313 Zelazo PD, Muller U. Executive Function in Typical and Atypical Development. In: Usha Goswami (Ed). Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development. Australia: Blackwell Publishing; 2002;445-69. Both refer to an individual’s ability to function adequately, regulating thought and behavior according to a means-ends logic.1313 Zelazo PD, Muller U. Executive Function in Typical and Atypical Development. In: Usha Goswami (Ed). Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development. Australia: Blackwell Publishing; 2002;445-69. The term “hot” EF denotes the more emotionally loaded aspects of self-regulation, for example, those pertaining to decision making, reward, and motivation, and coordinated by ventral areas of cortico-subcortical prefrontal circuits.1414 Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H. Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain. 2000;123(11):2189-202. On the other hand, “cool” EF components are less emotionally loaded and are associated with decontextualized thought processes, coordinated by dorsal cortico-subcortical prefrontal networks. This aspect of EF is mostly associated with the parieto-frontal circuit of general intelligence.1515 Haier RJ, White NS, Alkire MT. Individual differences in general intelligence correlate with brain function during nonreasoning tasks. Intelligence. 2003;31(5):429-41.

The literature consistently suggests an association between “cool” EF impairments and ADHD symptoms.1616 Rubia K. Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Its Clinical Translation. Front Hum Neurosci. 2018; 12:100. A meta-analytic1717 Willcutt EG, Sonuga-Barke EJS, Nigg JT, Sergeant JA. Recent developments in Neuropsychological models of childhood psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;24:195-226. study has shown that EF impairments are associated with ADHD in general, with larger effect sizes for inhibitory control. However, the cited study also reported that individual differences in “cool” EF did not fully explain the variance in ADHD symptoms.

Following the assumptions of the multiple deficit model, neuropsychological studies have sought to identify specific deficits and shared deficits between RD and ADHD. These studies compared groups with RD-only, ADHD-only, comorbid profiles, and typically developing children.1818 Gooch D, Snowling M, Hulme C. Time perception, phonological skills and executive function in children with dyslexia and/or ADHD symptoms. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;52(2):195-203.

19 Purvis KL, Tannock R. Phonological Processing, Not Inhibitory Control, Differentiates ADHD and Reading Disability. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39(4):485-94.
-2020 Willcutt EG, Pennington BF, Olson RK, Chhabildas N, Hulslander J. Neuropsychological Analyses of Comorbidity Between Reading Disability and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: In Search of the Common Deficit. Dev Neuropsychol. 2005; 27(1):35-78. Even when ADHD symptoms were measured with a questionnaire answered by parents and teachers, distinct cognitive profiles were found between children and adolescents with ADHD only and those with RD only.2121 Gooch D, Snowling M, Hulme C. Time perception, phonological skills and executive function in children with dyslexia and/or ADHD symptoms. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;52(2):195-203.

Purvis and Tannock1919 Purvis KL, Tannock R. Phonological Processing, Not Inhibitory Control, Differentiates ADHD and Reading Disability. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39(4):485-94. compared the performance of children aged from 7 to 11 years (age range similar to that investigated in the current study) in phonological processing and inhibitory control. They observed significant impairments in all phonological processing measures in the RD-only group, compared to the control group and the ADHD-only group. Additionally, the ADHD-only and RD-only groups performed poorly in inhibitory control. Another study2222 de Jong C, Van De Voorde S, Roeyers H, Raymaekers R, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant J. How Distinctive are ADHD and RD? Results of a Double Dissociation Study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37(7):1007-17. showed that both ADHD and RD are associated with difficulties in inhibitory control, and these difficulties were found to be more severe in RD than in ADHD. Inhibition deficits were marginally present in children (8-12 years old) with ADHD, whereas inhibition deficits were clear in RD, compared to controls without these developmental disorders.2222 de Jong C, Van De Voorde S, Roeyers H, Raymaekers R, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant J. How Distinctive are ADHD and RD? Results of a Double Dissociation Study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37(7):1007-17.

Since ADHD and RD are two of the most common neurodevelopmental disorders, and considering their high comorbidity and negative impact on children’s lives, it is important to analyze and specify the neuropsychological profiles associated with those conditions, as suggested by the multiple deficit model of neurodevelopmental disorders.55 Pennington BF. From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition. 2006;101(2):385-413.

Thus, this study aimed to compare performance in “cool” EF and phonological processing between ADHD-only children, RD-only children, and typically developing children. We expected to find the following results: 1) higher impairment in general “cool” EF and phonological processing among RD children compared to children with ADHD symptoms and to controls and, specifically99 Moura O, Simões MR, Pereira M. Executive functioning in children with developmental dyslexia. Clin Neuropsychol. 2015;28(Suppl 1):S20-41.,2323 Kudo MF, Lussier CM, Swanson HL. Reading disabilities in children: A selective meta-analysis of the cognitive literature. Res Dev Disabil. 2015;40:51-62.

24 Doyle C, Smeaton AF, Roche RAP, Boran L. Inhibition and updating, but not switching, predict Developmental Dyslexia and individual variation in reading ability. Front Psychol. 2018;9(795):1-19.
-2525 Willcutt E, Doyle A, Nigg J, Faraone S, Pennington B. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Adv Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(11):1336-46.; 2) a shared deficit in inhibitory control by children with ADHD symptoms and RD relative to controls.1919 Purvis KL, Tannock R. Phonological Processing, Not Inhibitory Control, Differentiates ADHD and Reading Disability. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39(4):485-94.,2222 de Jong C, Van De Voorde S, Roeyers H, Raymaekers R, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant J. How Distinctive are ADHD and RD? Results of a Double Dissociation Study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37(7):1007-17.

METHODS

Participants

The sample consisted of 216 children, 55.1% girls, aged between 8 and 11 years (M = 8.94, SD = .71), from the 3rd (n = 62) and 4th grades (n = 154). All children attended public elementary schools in Porto Alegre (Rio Grande do Sul; n = 66) and Belo Horizonte (Minas Gerais; n = 150) (Table 1). The exclusion criteria were: uncorrected auditory or visual impairments (reported by parents/guardians); nonverbal reasoning score below the 15th percentile, measured by the Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices (RCPM);2626 Angelini A, Alves I, Custódio E, Duarte W, Duarte J. Manual Matrizes Progressivas Coloridas de Raven: Escala especial. São Paulo: Centro Editor de Testes e Pesquisas em Psicologia; 1999. and comorbidity between ADHD symptoms and RD (n = 6).

Table 1
Characteristics of the three groups according to sociodemographic variables, percentile in the RCPM, MTA-SNAP-IV questionnaire score, and LPI percentile (N = 216).

Study design and procedures

This study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Federal University of Minas Gerais (protocol number 939.562) and the Ethics Committee of the Institute of Psychology at the Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul (protocol number 1.023.371). Two evaluation sessions (a group session and an individual session) were conducted with the children whose parents/guardians signed the Informed Consent Form. The research aims and procedures were explained to the children and parents. Assessments were conducted only after the child’s written and oral informed consent and the parent’s written informed consent were obtained.

The RCPM2626 Angelini A, Alves I, Custódio E, Duarte W, Duarte J. Manual Matrizes Progressivas Coloridas de Raven: Escala especial. São Paulo: Centro Editor de Testes e Pesquisas em Psicologia; 1999. was administered in a group session, while the remaining instruments were administered individually (approximately 90 minutes). Parents/guardians answered a questionnaire about sociodemographic/health conditions for each child, as well as the MTA-SNAP-IV.2727 Mattos P, Serra-Pinheiro MA, Rohde L, Pinto D. Apresentação de uma versão em português para uso no Brasil do instrumento MTA-SNAP-IV de avaliação de sintomas de transtorno do déficit de atenção/hiperatividade e sintomas de transtorno desafiador e de oposição. Rev Psiquiatr Rio Gd Sul. 2006;28(3):290-7. All instruments were administered by trained researchers, and the same procedures were followed in both Porto Alegre and Belo Horizonte.

This study has a quasi-experimental design of contrasting groups.2828 Nachmias C, Nachmias D. Research methods in the social sciences. London: Arnold; 1996. The following three groups were formed according to the children’s’ scores on the MTA-SNAP-IV questionnaire,2727 Mattos P, Serra-Pinheiro MA, Rohde L, Pinto D. Apresentação de uma versão em português para uso no Brasil do instrumento MTA-SNAP-IV de avaliação de sintomas de transtorno do déficit de atenção/hiperatividade e sintomas de transtorno desafiador e de oposição. Rev Psiquiatr Rio Gd Sul. 2006;28(3):290-7. answered by parents/guardians, and on the Oral Single-Word and Pseudoword Reading Task (LPI)2929 Salles, JF, Piccolo LR, Miná CS. Manual do Instrumento de avaliação de leitura de palavras e pseudopalavras (LPI). São Paulo: Vetor Editora; 2017. (Table 1):

  • a) Inattention/hyperactivity symptoms group (ADHD symptoms group; n = 20): comprising children who scored at least 6 items as either “pretty much” or “very much” in questions 1 to 9 (symptoms of inattention) of the MTA-SNAP-IV questionnaire2727 Mattos P, Serra-Pinheiro MA, Rohde L, Pinto D. Apresentação de uma versão em português para uso no Brasil do instrumento MTA-SNAP-IV de avaliação de sintomas de transtorno do déficit de atenção/hiperatividade e sintomas de transtorno desafiador e de oposição. Rev Psiquiatr Rio Gd Sul. 2006;28(3):290-7. and/or at least 6 items as either “pretty much” or “very much” in questions 10 to 18 (hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms), and whose performance was above the 16th percentile on the LPI2929 Salles, JF, Piccolo LR, Miná CS. Manual do Instrumento de avaliação de leitura de palavras e pseudopalavras (LPI). São Paulo: Vetor Editora; 2017. (see the Instruments section).

  • b) RD without ADHD symptoms group (RD group; n = 37): comprising children who scored below the cut-off point described above on the MTA-SNAP-IV questionnaire,2727 Mattos P, Serra-Pinheiro MA, Rohde L, Pinto D. Apresentação de uma versão em português para uso no Brasil do instrumento MTA-SNAP-IV de avaliação de sintomas de transtorno do déficit de atenção/hiperatividade e sintomas de transtorno desafiador e de oposição. Rev Psiquiatr Rio Gd Sul. 2006;28(3):290-7. and whose performance were below the 16th percentile on the LPI.2929 Salles, JF, Piccolo LR, Miná CS. Manual do Instrumento de avaliação de leitura de palavras e pseudopalavras (LPI). São Paulo: Vetor Editora; 2017.

  • c) Control group (without ADHD symptoms or RD; n = 159): children who scored below the cut-off point described in section “a” of the MTA-SNAP-IV questionnaire,2727 Mattos P, Serra-Pinheiro MA, Rohde L, Pinto D. Apresentação de uma versão em português para uso no Brasil do instrumento MTA-SNAP-IV de avaliação de sintomas de transtorno do déficit de atenção/hiperatividade e sintomas de transtorno desafiador e de oposição. Rev Psiquiatr Rio Gd Sul. 2006;28(3):290-7. and with performance above the 16th percentile on the LPI.2929 Salles, JF, Piccolo LR, Miná CS. Manual do Instrumento de avaliação de leitura de palavras e pseudopalavras (LPI). São Paulo: Vetor Editora; 2017.

Instruments

The following instruments were employed:

  • a) Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices (RCPM):2626 Angelini A, Alves I, Custódio E, Duarte W, Duarte J. Manual Matrizes Progressivas Coloridas de Raven: Escala especial. São Paulo: Centro Editor de Testes e Pesquisas em Psicologia; 1999. The age-appropriate Brazilian-validated version of Raven’s Colored Matrices, which assesses nonverbal reasoning.

  • b) MTA-SNAP-IV:2727 Mattos P, Serra-Pinheiro MA, Rohde L, Pinto D. Apresentação de uma versão em português para uso no Brasil do instrumento MTA-SNAP-IV de avaliação de sintomas de transtorno do déficit de atenção/hiperatividade e sintomas de transtorno desafiador e de oposição. Rev Psiquiatr Rio Gd Sul. 2006;28(3):290-7. Abbreviated version of the Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham questionnaire,3030 Swanson JM. School-based assessments and interventions for ADD students. Irvine, CA: KC Publishing;1992.,3131 Swanson JM, Kraemer HC, Hinshaw SP, Arnold LE, Conners CK, Abikoff HB, et al. Clinical relevance of the primary findings of the MTA: Success rates based on severity of ADHD and ODD symptoms at the end of treatment. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40(2):168-79. which assesses symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, and oppositional defiant behaviors, answered by parents/guardians or teachers. This instrument was adapted to the Brazilian context.2727 Mattos P, Serra-Pinheiro MA, Rohde L, Pinto D. Apresentação de uma versão em português para uso no Brasil do instrumento MTA-SNAP-IV de avaliação de sintomas de transtorno do déficit de atenção/hiperatividade e sintomas de transtorno desafiador e de oposição. Rev Psiquiatr Rio Gd Sul. 2006;28(3):290-7. The questionnaire is composed of 26 items corresponding to the list of symptoms for ADHD and Opposition Defiant Disorder described in the diagnostic criterion A of the DSM-IV, which are virtually the same in the DSM-5. Respondents rated their children’s inattentive (items 1-9), hyperactive-impulsive (items 10-18), and defiant (items 19-26) behaviors using a 4-point Likert scale: 0 (not at all), 1 (just a bit), 2 (pretty much) and 3 (very much).2727 Mattos P, Serra-Pinheiro MA, Rohde L, Pinto D. Apresentação de uma versão em português para uso no Brasil do instrumento MTA-SNAP-IV de avaliação de sintomas de transtorno do déficit de atenção/hiperatividade e sintomas de transtorno desafiador e de oposição. Rev Psiquiatr Rio Gd Sul. 2006;28(3):290-7. Moderate to strong correlations were found between the MTA-SNAP-IV and the Brazilian version of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children – Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL). All MTA-SNAP-IV scales showed very high internal consistency coefficients (all above .91).3232 Costa DS, Paula, JJ de, Malloy-Diniz LF, Romano-Silva MA, Miranda DM. Parent SNAP-IV rating of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: accuracy in a clinical sample of ADHD, validity, and reliability in a Brazilian sample. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2019;95(6):736-43.

  • c) Oral Single-Word and Pseudoword Reading Task (LPI):2929 Salles, JF, Piccolo LR, Miná CS. Manual do Instrumento de avaliação de leitura de palavras e pseudopalavras (LPI). São Paulo: Vetor Editora; 2017.,3333 Salles JF, Piccolo LR, Zamo RS, Toazza R. Normas de desempenho em tarefa de leitura de palavras/pseudopalavras isoladas (LPI) para crianças de 1º ano a 7º ano. Estud Pesq Psicol. 2013;13(2):397-419. This Brazilian oral reading task comprises a set of 60 stimuli selected according to regularity, length, frequency, and lexicality. Stimuli are divided into three categories (20 regular, 20 irregular, and 20 pseudowords) and matched for frequency and length. The score in this task consists of the number of items read correctly. Norms for Brazilian children aged from 6 to 12 years and evidence of validity are available for this instrument.2929 Salles, JF, Piccolo LR, Miná CS. Manual do Instrumento de avaliação de leitura de palavras e pseudopalavras (LPI). São Paulo: Vetor Editora; 2017.,3333 Salles JF, Piccolo LR, Zamo RS, Toazza R. Normas de desempenho em tarefa de leitura de palavras/pseudopalavras isoladas (LPI) para crianças de 1º ano a 7º ano. Estud Pesq Psicol. 2013;13(2):397-419.

  • d) Phoneme Elision Task:3434 Lopes-Silva JB, Moura R, Júlio-Costa A, Haase VG, Wood G. Phonemic awareness as a pathway to number transcoding. Front Psychol. 2014; 5:1-9. This instrument provides a measure of phonemic awareness. The child is requested to delete a specific phoneme from a word and state what the resulting word is. Performance is scored based on the number of correct answers. The internal consistency of this instrument is .92 (KR – 20 formula).3434 Lopes-Silva JB, Moura R, Júlio-Costa A, Haase VG, Wood G. Phonemic awareness as a pathway to number transcoding. Front Psychol. 2014; 5:1-9.

  • e) Digit Span (forward and backward):3535 Figueiredo VLM. WISC-III: Escala de Inteligência Wechsler para Crianças. Manual Adaptação e Padronização Brasileira. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2002. Brazilian version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Third Edition.3535 Figueiredo VLM. WISC-III: Escala de Inteligência Wechsler para Crianças. Manual Adaptação e Padronização Brasileira. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2002. The Digits Forward task primarily measures short-term phonological memory, while Digits Backward measures children’s ability to manipulate verbal information while holding it in a temporary storage (working memory). We employed the span measure in the present study.

  • f) Letter span: This instrument is analogous to the digit span task and measures short-term phonological memory and working memory. The task contains both forward and backward letter span conditions, each of which is scored on a scale from 2 to 9. The instrument was developed by the authors and showed internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) of .66.3636 Lima M, Piccolo LR, Basso FP, Júlio-Costa A, Lopes-Silva JB, Haase VG, Salles JF. Neuropsychological and environmental predictors of reading performance in Brazilian children. Appl Neuropsychol Child. 2019;18:1-12.

  • g) Corsi Block-Tapping Task:3737 Kessels RPC, van Zandvoort MJE, Postma A, Kappelle LJ, de Haan EHF. The Corsi Block-Tapping Task: Standardization and normative data. Appl Neuropsychol. 2000;7(4):252-8. This task assesses visuospatial short-term and working memory. Total scores were calculated by summing the number of sequences reproduced correctly in the forward and backward trials.

  • h) Five Digit Test:3838 Sedó M, de Paula JJ, Malloy-Diniz LF. O Teste dos Cinco Dígitos. São Paulo: Hogrefe; 2015. A multilingual instrument for assessing EF and attention. The first two parts measure automatic attention and speed processing. Participants are instructed to read Arabic digits (up to five) and count randomly presented star sets (up to five). The last two subtests measure controlled attention and executive attention with inhibition and set-shifting tasks. In the inhibition task (choosing), the child counts the number of Arabic digits instead of reading them. In the set-shifting condition (switching), the child counts the number of Arabic digits in most trials, switching to reading the digits when a frame surrounds the stimulus set. Accuracy and response time are registered. Evidence of validity and reliability for Brazilian adults is available.3939 Campos MC, Silva ML, Florêncio NC, de Paula JJ. Confiabilidade do Teste dos Cinco Dígitos em adultos brasileiros. J Bras Psiquiatria. 2016;65(2):135-9.

  • i) Phonemic and Semantic Verbal Fluency:4040 Salles JF, Fonseca RP, Cruz-Rodrigues C, Mello CBD, Barbosa T, MC. Desenvolvimento do Instrumento de Avaliação Neuropsicológica Breve Infantil NEUPSILIN-INF. Psico USF. 2011;16(3):297-305.,4141 Salles, JF, Sbicigo JB, Machado WDL, Miranda MC, Fonseca RP. Análise fatorial confirmatória do instrumento de Avaliação Neuropsicológica Breve Infantil-NEUPSILIN-Inf. Psico USF. 2014;19(1):119-30. This task is part of the Brief Neuropsychological Battery for Brazilian Children (NEUPSILIN-INF). In the phonemic fluency task, children must generate as many words as possible beginning with the letter M within 60 seconds. On the semantic fluency task, they must generate as many animal names as possible, also within 60 seconds. Each task is scored based on the number of correct words produced within the time limit. Evidence of validity and reliability is available for this task for Brazilian children.4141 Salles, JF, Sbicigo JB, Machado WDL, Miranda MC, Fonseca RP. Análise fatorial confirmatória do instrumento de Avaliação Neuropsicológica Breve Infantil-NEUPSILIN-Inf. Psico USF. 2014;19(1):119-30.

  • j) Contingency Naming Task (CNT):4242 Van der sluis S, de Jong PF, Leij A van der. Inhibition and shifting in children with learning deficits in arithmetic and reading. J Exp Child Psychol. 2004;87(3):239-66. This is an experimental task adapted from a study by Van der Sluis et al.,4242 Van der sluis S, de Jong PF, Leij A van der. Inhibition and shifting in children with learning deficits in arithmetic and reading. J Exp Child Psychol. 2004;87(3):239-66. which measures rapid automatized naming (RAN)4343 Denckla M, Rudel R. Rapid automatized naming (RAN): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychology. 1976;14:471-9. and EF. In the first three subtests, participants are instructed to name as fast as possible a set of letters (D, A, O, S), numbers (1, 2, 3, 4), and geometric figures (square, circle, triangle, and diamond). The last two subtests assess inhibitory control and set-shifting. The inhibition subtest uses the same geometric figures as the RAN subtest, but a smaller additional figure is placed in the center, and children are instructed to name the smaller figure (e.g., a triangle inside a square); that is, they have to inhibit the larger, more prepotent figures in favor of the smaller, less noticeable figure. In the set-shifting task, smaller figures are presented in the center of larger figures, and the stimulus color determines which figure has to be named: when the stimulus is black, the figure placed in the center has to be named; when the stimulus is red, the surrounding figure has to be named. Accuracy and response time are registered.4242 Van der sluis S, de Jong PF, Leij A van der. Inhibition and shifting in children with learning deficits in arithmetic and reading. J Exp Child Psychol. 2004;87(3):239-66.

Data analysis

We performed descriptive analyses and examined the dependent variables to detect group differences to control for in further analyses. We conducted Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA), controlling for age and nonverbal reasoning, to compare performances on the neuropsychological tasks between the groups, employing the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. Analyses were run in the SPSS software v. 20.0 at a significance level of 5%.

RESULTS

A t-test for independent samples showed differences between cities (Porto Alegre vs. Belo Horizonte) in both reading and phonological processing. These differences may be related to the schooling process.4444 Casarin FS, Wong CEI, Parente MAMP, Salles JF, Fonseca RP. Comparison of neuropsychological performance between students from public and private Brazilian schools. Span J Psychol. 2012;15:942-51. Children from Porto Alegre had better performance on phoneme elision, t(204) = 4.37, p < .001, whereas children from Belo Horizonte were faster in RAN of letters and figures, t(213) = 3.58, p < .001 and t(213) = 3.39, p = .001, respectively. Also, in RAN of figures, children from Belo Horizonte made fewer errors than those from Porto Alegre, t(213) = 2.28, p = .05. There were no significant differences between cities in phonological short-term memory or working memory, nor in reading of regular words, irregular words, or pseudowords.

The main objective of the present study was to compare the neuropsychological performances of the ADHD symptoms group and the RD group. The RD group had significantly lower scores on the digit span forward compared to the ADHD symptoms group (phonological short-term memory) [F(2,168) = 3.608, p = .029, Wilk’s Λ = .865, η2 = .03]. They performed worse than the other groups (controls and ADHD symptoms) in phonemic elision (phonological awareness) [F(2,168) = 11.309, p < .001, Wilk’s Λ = .865, η2 = .11]; orthographic verbal fluency (lexical access, verbal memory, oral and written language, and “cool” EF such as inhibitory control) [F(2,210) = 9.088, p < .001, Wilk’s Λ = .752 , η2 = .08]; figure RAN (more errors) (speed of lexical access and phonological processing skills) [F(2,210) = 5.940, p = .003, Wilk’s Λ = 0.752, η2 = .05]; and response time – inhibitory component (processing speed and inhibitory control) [F(2,210) = 5.518, p = .005, Wilk’s Λ = .792, η2 = .10] (Table 2).

Table 2
MANOVA results for the neuropsychological variables, controlling for age and nonverbal IQ.

Regarding “cool” EF (shifting, processing speed, and verbal fluency), verbal and visuospatial working memory, and phonological processing, the performance of the ADHD symptoms group did not differ from that of the control group (p > .05). The only measure in which the ADHD symptoms group exhibited worse performance (larger mean number of errors) than the control group was figure RAN – inhibitory component (inhibitory control) [F(2,210) = 11.594, p < .001, Wilk’s Λ =.752, η2 = .12]. Nevertheless, their performance did not differ significantly from that of the RD group, which, as described previously, also made significantly more errors than the controls (Table 2).

DISCUSSION

This study aimed to compare performance in “cool” EF and phonological processing between children with ADHD symptoms only, children with RD only, and their typically developing peers, after controlling for age and nonverbal reasoning. As expected, the RD group showed greater impairment in “cool” EF components (orthographic verbal fluency and processing speed) and phonological processing (phoneme awareness, short-term phonological memory, and lexical access) compared to both the ADHD symptoms and control groups.99 Moura O, Simões MR, Pereira M. Executive functioning in children with developmental dyslexia. Clin Neuropsychol. 2015;28(Suppl 1):S20-41.,4545 Kudo MF, Lussier CM, Swanson HL. Reading disabilities in children: A selective meta-analysis of the cognitive literature. Res Dev Disabil. 2015;40:51-62.,2525 Willcutt E, Doyle A, Nigg J, Faraone S, Pennington B. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Adv Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(11):1336-46.

There is currently a consensus in the literature showing consistent associations between deficits in phonological processing and RD.66 Miciak J, Cirino PT, Ahmed Y, Reid E, Vaughn S. Executive Functions and Response to Intervention: Identification of Students Struggling With Reading Comprehension. Learn Disabil Q. 2019;42(1):17-31.,77 Walda SAE, van Weerdenburg M, Wijnants ML, Bosman AMT. Progress in reading and spelling of dyslexic children is not affected by executive functioning. Res Dev Disabil. 2014;35(12):3431-54. Concerning difficulties in “cool” EF, it is crucial to consider limitations of study designs when drawing causal inferences. Relationships between EF deficits and reading performance/difficulties do not imply that EF are causal factors for reading performance. Such relationships may be reciprocal, i.e., executive abilities may develop with higher reading proficiency. Furthermore, the relationship between EF and reading tends to be stronger for more complex reading skills, such as reading comprehension.4646 Salles JF, Corso HV. Preditores Neuropsicológicos da Leitura. In: Jerusa Fumagalli de Salles; Vitor Geraldi Haase; Leandro F. Malloy-Diniz. (Orgs.). Neuropsicologia do Desenvolvimento: infância e adolescência. 1ed. Porto Alegre: Artmed; 2015. For example, after controlling for commonly accepted contributors to reading comprehension (i.e., attention, decoding skills, fluency, and vocabulary), EF continued to make a significant contribution to reading comprehension, but not to word identification skills.1010 Sesma HW, Mahone EM, Levine T, Eason SH, Cutting LE. The Contribution of Executive Skills to Reading Comprehension. Child Neuropsychol. 2009;15(3):232-46.

It is noteworthy that the tasks we employed to measure EF (orthographic verbal fluency and figure RAN), in which children with RD performed worse than children with ADHD symptoms only and control children, also require language skills. The children with RD in this study differed from the other groups in verbal memory and oral and written language abilities, such as phonological awareness and lexical-semantic processing.4747 Areces D, García T, González-Castro P, Alvarez-García D, Rodríguez C. Naming speed as a predictive diagnostic measure in reading and attentional problems. Child Neuropsychol. 2018;24(8):1115-28.,4848 Becker N, Piccolo LR, Salles JF. Verbal Fluency Development Across Childhood: Normative Data from Brazilian-Portuguese Speakers and Underlying Cognitive Processes. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2019;34(7): 1217-31. Therefore, children with RD may perform poorly on these tasks due to difficulties in verbal aspects rather than executive aspects, since verbal fluency is also related to the number of words known/learned. Indeed, vocabulary is a significant predictor of reading performance (words and text) in school-age children.4949 Piccolo LR, Salles JF. Vocabulário e memória de trabalho predizem desempenho em leitura de crianças. Psicol Teor Prat. 2013;15(2):180-91. Hence, the difficulties in orthographic verbal fluency observed in this sample may also be associated with linguistic variables, such as vocabulary.

Additionally, as our results showed, EF deficits are not always present in children with ADHD symptoms.1717 Willcutt EG, Sonuga-Barke EJS, Nigg JT, Sergeant JA. Recent developments in Neuropsychological models of childhood psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;24:195-226.,2525 Willcutt E, Doyle A, Nigg J, Faraone S, Pennington B. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Adv Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(11):1336-46. The moderate effect sizes found in other studies and the lack of universality of EF deficits among individuals with ADHD suggest that difficulties in EF are neither necessary nor sufficient to cause ADHD.2525 Willcutt E, Doyle A, Nigg J, Faraone S, Pennington B. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Adv Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(11):1336-46. This has consequences for controversies over the validity and utility of employing EF measures in the diagnosis of ADHD, especially considering that EF measures exhibit a lack of specificity in discriminating ADHD from other clinical disorders.5050 Duff CT, Sulla EM. Measuring Executive Function in the Differential Diagnosis of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Does It Really Tell Us Anything? Appl Neuropsychol Child. 2014;4(3):188-96. Nonetheless, EF tests could still be useful to characterize the cognitive profile of specific individuals.5151 Fuermaier ABM, Fricke JA, de Vries SM, Tucha L, Tucha O. Neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD: A Delphi consensus study. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2018; 26(4):340-54.

Despite the fact that children with RD displayed a more prominent profile of executive deficits in the present study, we also found a shared inhibitory control deficit between RD and ADHD children on one of the tasks, even after controlling for age and nonverbal reasoning. Inhibitory control deficits are described as an important endophenotype for the study of ADHD. This important EF component may help explain difficulties in both attention regulation and hyperactive/impulsive behaviors in children.5252 Crosbie J, Arnold P, Paterson A, Swanson J, Dupuis A, Li X, et al. Response inhibition and ADHD traits: Correlates and heritability in a community sample. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2013;41(3):497-507.

53 Kamradt JM, Nigg JT, Friderici KH, Nikolas MA. Neuropsychological performance measures as intermediate phenotypes for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A multiple mediation analysis. Dev Psychopathol. 2016;29(01):259-72.
-5454 Pauli-Potti U, Becker K. Time windows matter in ADHD-related developing neuropsychological basic deficits: A comprehensive review and meta-regression analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;55:165-72. However, inhibitory control deficit is not unique to ADHD. Moreover, individuals with RD showed small-to-average deficits, suggesting that impaired inhibition is not likely to be at the core of RD.5555 Lipszyc J, Schachar R. Inhibitory control and psychopathology: A meta-analysis of studies using the stop signal task. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16(06):1064-76. Furthermore, the present study also suggests that deficits in inhibitory control observed in children with RD are probably not explained by the frequent association with high levels of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity, as hypothesized previously,5656 Schoot van der M, Licht R, Horsley TM, Sergeant JA. Inhibitory Deficits in Reading Disability Depend on Subtype: Guessers but not Spellers. Child Neuropsychol. 2000;6(4):297-312. and may be present even when no comorbidity between these conditions is present.1919 Purvis KL, Tannock R. Phonological Processing, Not Inhibitory Control, Differentiates ADHD and Reading Disability. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39(4):485-94.

There has been speculation regarding the distinct mechanisms underlying inhibitory control deficits in ADHD and RD. It has been suggested that children with ADHD may have a more pervasive inhibitory deficit compared to typically developing children, which leads to behavioral impulsivity, whereas children with RD may have difficulties in inhibition due to impairments in rapid information processing.1919 Purvis KL, Tannock R. Phonological Processing, Not Inhibitory Control, Differentiates ADHD and Reading Disability. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39(4):485-94.

In conclusion, the current results corroborate the hypothesis that RD is fundamentally characterized by neuropsychological impairments, where these are less prominent in children with ADHD symptoms.11 McGrath L, Pennington B, Shanahan M, Santerre-Lemmon L, Barnard H, Willcutt E, et al. A multiple deficit model of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Searching for shared cognitive deficits. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52(5):547-57. This might be explained by differences in the assessment of the two groups. Children with RD were evaluated using a standardized word reading test, i.e., a direct measure of performance, even though some did not meet all the criteria for diagnosis of dyslexia. On the other hand, inattention/hyperactivity symptoms in the ADHD symptoms group were assessed through parent/guardian reports; thus, this group did not undergo a detailed diagnostic procedure. Moreover, this type of assessment may introduce some evaluation biases, as discussed below in the Limitations section. Children with ADHD and concurrent neuropsychological deficits exhibit greater functional impairment than cases without significant neuropsychological impairments, and hence are more likely to be referred for treatment.5757 Van Lieshout M, Luman M, Schweren LJS, Twisk JWR, Faraone SV, Heslenfeld DJ, et al. The Course of Neurocognitive Functioning and Prediction of Behavioral Outcome of ADHD Affected and Unaffected Siblings. J Abnormal Child Psych. 2018;47(3):405-19. Nevertheless, children with ADHD may not display any deficits in neuropsychological functions.1111 Coghill D, Seth S, Matthews K. A comprehensive assessment of memory, delay version, timing, inhibition, decision making and variability in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Advancing beyond the three-pathway models. Psychol Med. 2014;44(09):1989-2001. The evidence for a shared deficit in inhibitory control is also in line with other studies1919 Purvis KL, Tannock R. Phonological Processing, Not Inhibitory Control, Differentiates ADHD and Reading Disability. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39(4):485-94.,2222 de Jong C, Van De Voorde S, Roeyers H, Raymaekers R, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant J. How Distinctive are ADHD and RD? Results of a Double Dissociation Study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37(7):1007-17. and suggests a potential explanatory deficit that underlies the co-occurrence of learning disorders and ADHD, as held by the multiple deficit cognitive model.55 Pennington BF. From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition. 2006;101(2):385-413.

Limitations

The results presented here must be interpreted with caution since the study has some limitations. These limitations include small sample sizes, especially for the ADHD symptoms group, and a difference in sample sizes between the two cities. It is important to observe that the proportion of girls in the RD group is twice that of boys, which contradicts the literature showing equal or greater proportion of boys with RD,22 Fletcher JM. Dyslexia: The evolution of a scientific concept. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15(4):501-8. indicating a sample bias. Additionally, the single source of information (parents/guardians) about children’s inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms also constitutes a bias in this study. Measuring these indicators through teachers’ reports may contribute to greater reliability in the evaluation. The MTA-SNAP-IV questionnaire also has limitations: ADHD symptoms are nonspecific and may refer to other clinical conditions, such as anxiety. Another criticism of the use of the MTA-SNAP-IV is the limited investigation of its psychometric properties. In Brazil, the accuracy of the MTA-SNAP-IV for ADHD screening in non-clinical samples has not yet been examined.3232 Costa DS, Paula, JJ de, Malloy-Diniz LF, Romano-Silva MA, Miranda DM. Parent SNAP-IV rating of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: accuracy in a clinical sample of ADHD, validity, and reliability in a Brazilian sample. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2019;95(6):736-43. Therefore, this instrument does not have robust evidence of validity in the Brazilian context and needs further psychometric investigation.3232 Costa DS, Paula, JJ de, Malloy-Diniz LF, Romano-Silva MA, Miranda DM. Parent SNAP-IV rating of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: accuracy in a clinical sample of ADHD, validity, and reliability in a Brazilian sample. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2019;95(6):736-43.

We also emphasize the need to include fine measures for other EF components, especially “hot” components. Furthermore, measures of other modalities of inhibitory control, such as motor inhibition, are needed, since the inhibitory control task employed here specifically involves inhibition of verbal responses to competing visual stimuli. Additionally, the groups in our sample were formed based solely on observed reading difficulties and ADHD symptoms, not proper diagnosis of dyslexia and ADHD. This may limit comparisons with studies that included samples selected following standardized diagnostic procedures for both conditions. We also did not split the ADHD symptoms group by subtype (inattentive, hyperactive/impulsive, and combined) due to the small sample size. However, grouping children with unspecified ADHD symptoms is consistent with the current version of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5),5858 American Psychological Association (2013). DSM-5 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th Edition). Washington: DC. which no longer considers the subtypes as stable categories throughout development.

In conclusion, the present study indicates that phonological measures may aid the diagnosis and intervention of RD.88 Vellutino FR, Fletcher JM, Snowling MJ, Scanlon DM. Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45(1):2-40.,2222 de Jong C, Van De Voorde S, Roeyers H, Raymaekers R, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant J. How Distinctive are ADHD and RD? Results of a Double Dissociation Study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37(7):1007-17. On the other hand, EF tasks cannot be used as neuropsychological markers for ADHD and RD, and impairments in verbal tasks of inhibitory control with visual stimuli may not differentiate clinical groups with these conditions.2222 de Jong C, Van De Voorde S, Roeyers H, Raymaekers R, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant J. How Distinctive are ADHD and RD? Results of a Double Dissociation Study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37(7):1007-17. However, we would like to highlight the importance of assessing executive difficulties, as well as difficulties in other neuropsychological domains, in children with RD or those with inattention/hyperactivity symptoms above those expected for their age and development. Early neuropsychological assessment and interventions for EF domains and phonological processing may help promote the adequate development of other cognitive functions, functional adaptation, and academic performance of such children.

  • This study was conducted at the Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (UFMG). Psychology Department, Belo Horizonte, MG, Brazil and Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul (UFRGS), Psychology Institute, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil.
  • Financial support and acknowledgments. We thank the Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq): PhD grant (Gabriella Koltermann), research productivity grant (Jerusa Salles), call CNPq/ MCTI Nº 25/2015.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    McGrath L, Pennington B, Shanahan M, Santerre-Lemmon L, Barnard H, Willcutt E, et al. A multiple deficit model of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Searching for shared cognitive deficits. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52(5):547-57.
  • 2
    Fletcher JM. Dyslexia: The evolution of a scientific concept. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15(4):501-8.
  • 3
    Polanczyk G, Lima MS, Horta BL, Biederman J, Rohde LA. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: A systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(6):942.
  • 4
    Peterson R, Boada R, McGrath L, Willcutt E, Olson R, Pennington B. Cognitive prediction of reading, math, and attention: Shared and unique influences. J Learn Disabil. J Learn Disabil. 2017;50(4):408-21.
  • 5
    Pennington BF. From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition. 2006;101(2):385-413.
  • 6
    Miciak J, Cirino PT, Ahmed Y, Reid E, Vaughn S. Executive Functions and Response to Intervention: Identification of Students Struggling With Reading Comprehension. Learn Disabil Q. 2019;42(1):17-31.
  • 7
    Walda SAE, van Weerdenburg M, Wijnants ML, Bosman AMT. Progress in reading and spelling of dyslexic children is not affected by executive functioning. Res Dev Disabil. 2014;35(12):3431-54.
  • 8
    Vellutino FR, Fletcher JM, Snowling MJ, Scanlon DM. Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45(1):2-40.
  • 9
    Moura O, Simões MR, Pereira M. Executive functioning in children with developmental dyslexia. Clin Neuropsychol. 2015;28(Suppl 1):S20-41.
  • 10
    Sesma HW, Mahone EM, Levine T, Eason SH, Cutting LE. The Contribution of Executive Skills to Reading Comprehension. Child Neuropsychol. 2009;15(3):232-46.
  • 11
    Coghill D, Seth S, Matthews K. A comprehensive assessment of memory, delay version, timing, inhibition, decision making and variability in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Advancing beyond the three-pathway models. Psychol Med. 2014;44(09):1989-2001.
  • 12
    Zelazo PD, Carlson SM. Hot and Cool Executive Function in Childhood and dolescence: Development and Plasticity. Child Dev Perspect. 2012;6(4):354-60.
  • 13
    Zelazo PD, Muller U. Executive Function in Typical and Atypical Development. In: Usha Goswami (Ed). Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development. Australia: Blackwell Publishing; 2002;445-69.
  • 14
    Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H. Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain. 2000;123(11):2189-202.
  • 15
    Haier RJ, White NS, Alkire MT. Individual differences in general intelligence correlate with brain function during nonreasoning tasks. Intelligence. 2003;31(5):429-41.
  • 16
    Rubia K. Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Its Clinical Translation. Front Hum Neurosci. 2018; 12:100.
  • 17
    Willcutt EG, Sonuga-Barke EJS, Nigg JT, Sergeant JA. Recent developments in Neuropsychological models of childhood psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;24:195-226.
  • 18
    Gooch D, Snowling M, Hulme C. Time perception, phonological skills and executive function in children with dyslexia and/or ADHD symptoms. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;52(2):195-203.
  • 19
    Purvis KL, Tannock R. Phonological Processing, Not Inhibitory Control, Differentiates ADHD and Reading Disability. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39(4):485-94.
  • 20
    Willcutt EG, Pennington BF, Olson RK, Chhabildas N, Hulslander J. Neuropsychological Analyses of Comorbidity Between Reading Disability and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: In Search of the Common Deficit. Dev Neuropsychol. 2005; 27(1):35-78.
  • 21
    Gooch D, Snowling M, Hulme C. Time perception, phonological skills and executive function in children with dyslexia and/or ADHD symptoms. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;52(2):195-203.
  • 22
    de Jong C, Van De Voorde S, Roeyers H, Raymaekers R, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant J. How Distinctive are ADHD and RD? Results of a Double Dissociation Study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37(7):1007-17.
  • 23
    Kudo MF, Lussier CM, Swanson HL. Reading disabilities in children: A selective meta-analysis of the cognitive literature. Res Dev Disabil. 2015;40:51-62.
  • 24
    Doyle C, Smeaton AF, Roche RAP, Boran L. Inhibition and updating, but not switching, predict Developmental Dyslexia and individual variation in reading ability. Front Psychol. 2018;9(795):1-19.
  • 25
    Willcutt E, Doyle A, Nigg J, Faraone S, Pennington B. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Adv Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(11):1336-46.
  • 26
    Angelini A, Alves I, Custódio E, Duarte W, Duarte J. Manual Matrizes Progressivas Coloridas de Raven: Escala especial. São Paulo: Centro Editor de Testes e Pesquisas em Psicologia; 1999.
  • 27
    Mattos P, Serra-Pinheiro MA, Rohde L, Pinto D. Apresentação de uma versão em português para uso no Brasil do instrumento MTA-SNAP-IV de avaliação de sintomas de transtorno do déficit de atenção/hiperatividade e sintomas de transtorno desafiador e de oposição. Rev Psiquiatr Rio Gd Sul. 2006;28(3):290-7.
  • 28
    Nachmias C, Nachmias D. Research methods in the social sciences. London: Arnold; 1996.
  • 29
    Salles, JF, Piccolo LR, Miná CS. Manual do Instrumento de avaliação de leitura de palavras e pseudopalavras (LPI). São Paulo: Vetor Editora; 2017.
  • 30
    Swanson JM. School-based assessments and interventions for ADD students. Irvine, CA: KC Publishing;1992.
  • 31
    Swanson JM, Kraemer HC, Hinshaw SP, Arnold LE, Conners CK, Abikoff HB, et al. Clinical relevance of the primary findings of the MTA: Success rates based on severity of ADHD and ODD symptoms at the end of treatment. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40(2):168-79.
  • 32
    Costa DS, Paula, JJ de, Malloy-Diniz LF, Romano-Silva MA, Miranda DM. Parent SNAP-IV rating of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: accuracy in a clinical sample of ADHD, validity, and reliability in a Brazilian sample. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2019;95(6):736-43.
  • 33
    Salles JF, Piccolo LR, Zamo RS, Toazza R. Normas de desempenho em tarefa de leitura de palavras/pseudopalavras isoladas (LPI) para crianças de 1º ano a 7º ano. Estud Pesq Psicol. 2013;13(2):397-419.
  • 34
    Lopes-Silva JB, Moura R, Júlio-Costa A, Haase VG, Wood G. Phonemic awareness as a pathway to number transcoding. Front Psychol. 2014; 5:1-9.
  • 35
    Figueiredo VLM. WISC-III: Escala de Inteligência Wechsler para Crianças. Manual Adaptação e Padronização Brasileira. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2002.
  • 36
    Lima M, Piccolo LR, Basso FP, Júlio-Costa A, Lopes-Silva JB, Haase VG, Salles JF. Neuropsychological and environmental predictors of reading performance in Brazilian children. Appl Neuropsychol Child. 2019;18:1-12.
  • 37
    Kessels RPC, van Zandvoort MJE, Postma A, Kappelle LJ, de Haan EHF. The Corsi Block-Tapping Task: Standardization and normative data. Appl Neuropsychol. 2000;7(4):252-8.
  • 38
    Sedó M, de Paula JJ, Malloy-Diniz LF. O Teste dos Cinco Dígitos. São Paulo: Hogrefe; 2015.
  • 39
    Campos MC, Silva ML, Florêncio NC, de Paula JJ. Confiabilidade do Teste dos Cinco Dígitos em adultos brasileiros. J Bras Psiquiatria. 2016;65(2):135-9.
  • 40
    Salles JF, Fonseca RP, Cruz-Rodrigues C, Mello CBD, Barbosa T, MC. Desenvolvimento do Instrumento de Avaliação Neuropsicológica Breve Infantil NEUPSILIN-INF. Psico USF. 2011;16(3):297-305.
  • 41
    Salles, JF, Sbicigo JB, Machado WDL, Miranda MC, Fonseca RP. Análise fatorial confirmatória do instrumento de Avaliação Neuropsicológica Breve Infantil-NEUPSILIN-Inf. Psico USF. 2014;19(1):119-30.
  • 42
    Van der sluis S, de Jong PF, Leij A van der. Inhibition and shifting in children with learning deficits in arithmetic and reading. J Exp Child Psychol. 2004;87(3):239-66.
  • 43
    Denckla M, Rudel R. Rapid automatized naming (RAN): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychology. 1976;14:471-9.
  • 44
    Casarin FS, Wong CEI, Parente MAMP, Salles JF, Fonseca RP. Comparison of neuropsychological performance between students from public and private Brazilian schools. Span J Psychol. 2012;15:942-51.
  • 45
    Kudo MF, Lussier CM, Swanson HL. Reading disabilities in children: A selective meta-analysis of the cognitive literature. Res Dev Disabil. 2015;40:51-62.
  • 46
    Salles JF, Corso HV. Preditores Neuropsicológicos da Leitura. In: Jerusa Fumagalli de Salles; Vitor Geraldi Haase; Leandro F. Malloy-Diniz. (Orgs.). Neuropsicologia do Desenvolvimento: infância e adolescência. 1ed. Porto Alegre: Artmed; 2015.
  • 47
    Areces D, García T, González-Castro P, Alvarez-García D, Rodríguez C. Naming speed as a predictive diagnostic measure in reading and attentional problems. Child Neuropsychol. 2018;24(8):1115-28.
  • 48
    Becker N, Piccolo LR, Salles JF. Verbal Fluency Development Across Childhood: Normative Data from Brazilian-Portuguese Speakers and Underlying Cognitive Processes. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2019;34(7): 1217-31.
  • 49
    Piccolo LR, Salles JF. Vocabulário e memória de trabalho predizem desempenho em leitura de crianças. Psicol Teor Prat. 2013;15(2):180-91.
  • 50
    Duff CT, Sulla EM. Measuring Executive Function in the Differential Diagnosis of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Does It Really Tell Us Anything? Appl Neuropsychol Child. 2014;4(3):188-96.
  • 51
    Fuermaier ABM, Fricke JA, de Vries SM, Tucha L, Tucha O. Neuropsychological assessment of adults with ADHD: A Delphi consensus study. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2018; 26(4):340-54.
  • 52
    Crosbie J, Arnold P, Paterson A, Swanson J, Dupuis A, Li X, et al. Response inhibition and ADHD traits: Correlates and heritability in a community sample. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2013;41(3):497-507.
  • 53
    Kamradt JM, Nigg JT, Friderici KH, Nikolas MA. Neuropsychological performance measures as intermediate phenotypes for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A multiple mediation analysis. Dev Psychopathol. 2016;29(01):259-72.
  • 54
    Pauli-Potti U, Becker K. Time windows matter in ADHD-related developing neuropsychological basic deficits: A comprehensive review and meta-regression analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;55:165-72.
  • 55
    Lipszyc J, Schachar R. Inhibitory control and psychopathology: A meta-analysis of studies using the stop signal task. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16(06):1064-76.
  • 56
    Schoot van der M, Licht R, Horsley TM, Sergeant JA. Inhibitory Deficits in Reading Disability Depend on Subtype: Guessers but not Spellers. Child Neuropsychol. 2000;6(4):297-312.
  • 57
    Van Lieshout M, Luman M, Schweren LJS, Twisk JWR, Faraone SV, Heslenfeld DJ, et al. The Course of Neurocognitive Functioning and Prediction of Behavioral Outcome of ADHD Affected and Unaffected Siblings. J Abnormal Child Psych. 2018;47(3):405-19.
  • 58
    American Psychological Association (2013). DSM-5 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th Edition). Washington: DC.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    16 Mar 2020
  • Date of issue
    Jan-Mar 2020

History

  • Received
    30 Oct 2018
  • Accepted
    23 Nov 2019
Academia Brasileira de Neurologia, Departamento de Neurologia Cognitiva e Envelhecimento R. Vergueiro, 1353 sl.1404 - Ed. Top Towers Offices, Torre Norte, São Paulo, SP, Brazil, CEP 04101-000, Tel.: +55 11 5084-9463 | +55 11 5083-3876 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistadementia@abneuro.org.br | demneuropsy@uol.com.br