Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Cerebral localization of higher functions: Memory-related anatomic structures

LOCALIZAÇÃO CEREBRAL DE FUNÇÕES SUPERIORES: ESTRUTURAS ANATÔMICAS RELACIONADAS COM A MEMÓRIA. ACHADOS INICIAIS

ABSTRACT

The nature of memory and the search for its localization have been a subject of interest since Antiquity. After millennia of theoretical concepts, shifting from the heart to the brain, then from the ventricles to solid parts, the core memory-related structures finally began to be identified through modern scientifically-based methods at the diencephalic and cortical (hippocampal and neocortical) levels, mostly in the late Modern period, culminating in the current state of knowledge on the subject.

Key words:
memory; anatomical structures; diencephalon; hippocampus; neocortex

RESUMO

A natureza da memória e a busca de sua localização tem sido objeto de interesse desde a Antiguidade. Após milênios de conceitos teóricos, mudando do coração para o cérebro e daí dos ventrículos para as partes sólidas, as estruturas centrais relacionadas com a memória finalmente começaram a ser identificadas através de métodos modernos com base científica, nos níveis diencefálico e cortical (hipocampal e neocortical), principalmente no período Moderno tardio, aproximando-se do estado atual do conhecimento sobre o tema.

Palavras-chave:
memória; estruturas anatômicas; diencéfalo; hipocampo; neocórtex

The nature of memory (function and failure), as well as the search for its localization, have been an object of interest since Antiquity.11 Clarke E. and O'Malley CD. The Human Brain and Spinal Cord: A Historical Study. 2ª ed. San Francisco: Norman Publishing, 1996.,22 Julião R, Presti RL, Perler D, van der Eijk P. Mapping Memory. Theories in Ancient, Medieval and Early Modern Philosophy and Medicine. J Ancient Studies 2016;6:678-702. The earlier Western civilizations (e.g., ancient Egyptians) had elected the heart as the central organ, while the Greeks were divided on this matter, cardiocentric vs encephalocentric. Thus, Empedocles, Democritus, Aristotle, Diocles, Praxagoras favoured the heart, whereas Alcmaeon, Pythagoras, Plato, Herophilus, Erasistratus, Rufus, and Galen chose the brain.11 Clarke E. and O'Malley CD. The Human Brain and Spinal Cord: A Historical Study. 2ª ed. San Francisco: Norman Publishing, 1996.

2 Julião R, Presti RL, Perler D, van der Eijk P. Mapping Memory. Theories in Ancient, Medieval and Early Modern Philosophy and Medicine. J Ancient Studies 2016;6:678-702.
-33 Engelhardt E. Cerebral localization of the mind and higher functions The beginnings. Dem Neuropsychol. 2018;12(3):321-5. Finally, the brain prevailed. The description of the ventricles in the human brain by Herophilus and Erasistratus (IV-III century BC) created a place to locate the soul and mind. Much later, Nemesius of Emesa (ca. 390 AD) located the functions of the human mind – senses, reasoning, and memory – in these different ventricles, proposing the “doctrine of ventricular localization of mental function” (possibly around the end of the IV century AD [first translation published in 1538]).22 Julião R, Presti RL, Perler D, van der Eijk P. Mapping Memory. Theories in Ancient, Medieval and Early Modern Philosophy and Medicine. J Ancient Studies 2016;6:678-702.

3 Engelhardt E. Cerebral localization of the mind and higher functions The beginnings. Dem Neuropsychol. 2018;12(3):321-5.
-44 Nemesius. Némésius. De la Nature de l'homme [trad M. J. B. Thibault]. Paris: Hachette, 1844. [Retrieved from: http://services.biusante.parisdescartes.fr/medicapdf/main.php?key=ZnVsbHwxNTAxNzJ8fA]
http://services.biusante.parisdescartes....
This view, in a descriptive manner, prevailed for many centuries, being expanded by Albertus Magnus (ca. 1193-1280), whose book illustrated the cerebral cavities (ventricles) schematically, apparently for the first time (1506, chapter XIII – posthumous release).33 Engelhardt E. Cerebral localization of the mind and higher functions The beginnings. Dem Neuropsychol. 2018;12(3):321-5.,55 Acar F, Naderi S, Guvencer M, Türe U, Arda MN. Herophilus of Chalcedon: a pioneer in neuroscience. Neurosurgery 2005;56(4):861-7.

6 Finger S. Origins of Neuroscience: A History of Explorations into Brain Function. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1994.
-77 Albertus Magnus. Philosophia naturalis. Mich. Furter, 1506. [Retrieved from: https://books.googleusercontent.com]
https://books.googleusercontent.com...
Nemesius, Albertus and other authors of their time who localized the mind and its faculties within these cavities placed memory in the posterior ventricle (cerebellar ventricle) (4th ventricle), where it remained until the XVII century (Renaissance). At this point, the faculties were understood to be in the solid parts of the cerebrum, as proposed by Johannes Jakob Wepfer (1658), and a short time later by Thomas Willis (1664).33 Engelhardt E. Cerebral localization of the mind and higher functions The beginnings. Dem Neuropsychol. 2018;12(3):321-5. During the entire Middle Ages (V-XIV century) the ventricles remained the center of the theories relating mind and brain, with the latter housing the faculty of memory.22 Julião R, Presti RL, Perler D, van der Eijk P. Mapping Memory. Theories in Ancient, Medieval and Early Modern Philosophy and Medicine. J Ancient Studies 2016;6:678-702.,66 Finger S. Origins of Neuroscience: A History of Explorations into Brain Function. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1994.

The long preceding period of these initial localization theories gave way to a phase of modern, more concrete and scientifically-based facts, in a bid to relate memory failure (amnesia) to solid brain structures, which took place mainly in the XIX century, extending into the XX century (late Modern period). At this time. numerous anatomical brain structures were found to be related to this dysfunction (and function), furthering understanding on the matter and culminating in the present state of knowledge.

MEMORY-RELATED ANATOMICAL BRAIN STRUCTURES

The memory-related brain structures known today can be divided into diencephalic and cortical (hippocampal and neocortical), and involve the core components of the declarative memory circuits. The nuclei with modulatory functions, such as the cholinergic Meynert’s basal nucleus, will not be addressed. The development of this knowledge is outlined in the Table. It is meaningful to stress that most of the considered structures have been already described a long time before determining their memory function, a point that will not be focused in the present paper.

Table
Anatomical structures first related to memory (current names in square brackets).

DIENCEPHALIC MEMORY-RELATED STRUCTURES

The modern studies of memory were triggered by Carl Wernicke (1848-1904) and Sergei Sergeievich Korsakow (1854-1900). First, Wernicke described three acute cases (two with alcoholic cause), without memory impairment, which evolved to death. The autopsy revealed punctiform hemorrhages in the floor of the 3rd and 4th ventricles, and in the acqueductal region. For this condition, he proposed the diagnosis of ‘Superior Hemorrhagic Poliencefalitis’ (1881).88 Wernicke C. Lehrbuch der Gehirnkrankheiten für Aerzte und Studirende. Band II. Berlin: Th Fischer, 1881, pp 229-242. [Retrieved from: https://ia600502.us.archive.org/16/items/lehrbuchdergehir02wern/lehrbuchdergehir02wern.pdf]
https://ia600502.us.archive.org/16/items...
Later he described chronic alcoholics with severe loss of retention memory and retroactive amnesia, spared memory storage [remote], besides confabulation and temporal disorientation. No autopsy was reported (1900).99 WernickeC. Grundriss der Psychiatrie in klinischen Vorlesungen. Leipzig: Thieme, 1900, pp 295-304. [Retrieved from: https://ia801403.us.archive.org/20/items/grundrissderpsyc00wern/grundrissderpsyc00wern.pdf]
https://ia801403.us.archive.org/20/items...
Contemporaneously, Korsakow described a disorder related to alcoholism (1887-1891), with symptoms comprising disorientation, amnesia characterized by quickly forgotten recent facts [anterograde amnesia], preserved remote events [long-term memory], loss of memory from onset to a period preceding the disease [retrograde amnesia], and pseudo-reminiscences [confabulation].1010 Korsakow SS. Eine psychische Störung combinirt mit multipler Neuritis (Psychosis polyneuritica seu Cerebropathia psychica toxaemica). Allgem Zschr Psychiatr 1890;46:475-85.,1111 Korsakow SS. Ueber eine besondere Form psychischer Störung, combinirt mit multipler Neuritis. Arch Psychiatr 1890;21:669-704. The condition was named after him as ‘Korsakow’s syndrome’ or ‘Symptom complex of Korsakow’ by Friedrich Jolly (1887).1212 Bonhoeffer KL. Der Korsakowsche Symptomenkomplex in seinen Beziehungen zu den verschiedenen Krankheitsformen. Allg Z Psychiat 1904; 61:744-52. He published one case of this type with autopsy verification, where no macroscopic or microscopic abnormalities of the brain were observed.1313 Korsakow S, Serbski W. Ein Fall von polyneuritischer Psychose mit Autopsie. Archiv f. Psychiatrie 1892;23:112-34.

Their studies were not able to objectively identify the underlying anatomic structures. Wernicke’s pathological findings would only be later understood, retrospectively.88 Wernicke C. Lehrbuch der Gehirnkrankheiten für Aerzte und Studirende. Band II. Berlin: Th Fischer, 1881, pp 229-242. [Retrieved from: https://ia600502.us.archive.org/16/items/lehrbuchdergehir02wern/lehrbuchdergehir02wern.pdf]
https://ia600502.us.archive.org/16/items...
Korsakow believed the memory failure might be explained by the disorganization of association fibers connecting the nervous cells of the cerebral cortex,1414 Korsakow SS. Étude médico-psychologique sur une forme des maladies dela mémoire. Revue philosophique de la France et de l'étranger. 1889; 28:501-530. [gallica.bnf.fr]
gallica.bnf.fr...
but without supporting evidence.

The close relationship between both syndromes was not recognized by their authors. However, Adolf Elzholz and Karl Ludwig Bonhoeffer identified this relationship, recognizing the strong relation between the two syndromes, regarding them as part of the same pathological process.1212 Bonhoeffer KL. Der Korsakowsche Symptomenkomplex in seinen Beziehungen zu den verschiedenen Krankheitsformen. Allg Z Psychiat 1904; 61:744-52.,1515 Arts NJM, Walvoort SJW, Kessels RPC. Korsakoff's syndrome: a critical review. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment 2017;13: 2875-90.,1616 Elzholz A. Ueber Beziehungen der Korsakowsche Psychose zur Polioencephalitis acuta haemorrhagica superior. Wiener klin Wochenschrift 1900;15:337-44.

Two publications that followed Korsakow’s are worthy of note, as they furthered understanding on the subject, namely, the studies of Hans Gudden (1866-1940) and of Eduard Gamper (1887-1938), which provided entirely novel anatomical information. Gudden presented clinical and pathological data on five patients with severe chronic alcoholism who died in the acute or chronic phase of the condition (1896). They presented a clinical picture resembling that of Korsakow’s and pathological examination showed lesions compatible with Hemorrhagic encephalitis, affecting the walls of the 3rd ventricle, superior brainstem, and 4th ventricle, and also lesion of the anterior tubercle of the thalamus, and marked atrophy of the mammillary bodies, while the fornix and Vicq d’Azyr bundle showing abnormal changes in one of the cases.1717 Gudden H. Klinische und anatomische Beiträge zur Kenntniss der multiplen Alkoholneuritis nebst Bemerkungen über die Regenerationsvorgänge im peripheren Nervensystem. Arch f. Psychiatrie u. Nervenkrankheiten 1896;28(3):643-741.,1818 Vann SD, Nelson AJ. The mammillary bodies and memory: more than a hippocampal relay. Progress in Brain Research 2015; 219:163-85.

At a later date (1928), Gamper held a conference about chronic alcoholics who died in the acute, subacute or chronic phase with the clinical manifestation of Korsakow’s psychosis, with or without Polioencephalitis haemorrhagica (Wernicke’s). Neuropathological examination showed lesions of the brainstem, mammillary bodies, and thalamic nuclei (parafascicularis, submedial, reuniens, and medial part of the medial thalamic nucleus). He held that the mammillary bodies constituted an important nodal point of the vegetative mechanism underlying the psychic processes of the amnesic syndrome, considering their strong connections, on one side with the midbrain and the thalamus (and through this certainly to the cingulate gyrus) and on the other side with the Ammon’s formation by way of the fornix.1818 Vann SD, Nelson AJ. The mammillary bodies and memory: more than a hippocampal relay. Progress in Brain Research 2015; 219:163-85.,1919 Gamper E. Zur Frage der polioencephalitis haemorrhagica der chronischen Alkoholiker. Anatomiche Befunde beim alkolischen Korasakow und ihre Beziehungen klinischen bild. Deutsch Ztsch Nervenh 1928;102: 122-9.

Apparently Gudden was the first author to relate the amnesic symptoms of his cases with anatomic structures, such as the mammillary bodies and related tracts (fornix and Vicq d’Azyr bundle), and the anterior tubercle of the thalamus. Gamper endorsed and extended Gudden’s finding, and proposed a circuit formed by the connections of the mammillary bodies, thalamus, and cortical structures (hippocampus and cingulate gyrus), related to memory failure.

The findings of Gudden and those of Gamper, with the suggestion of the above-mentioned circuit, can be viewed as possible precursors of the circuit described later by James Papez (1937), initially proposed as a mechanism of emotion.

The cited studies, particularly those of Gudden and Gamper, have revealed the critical role of diencephalic structures in memory function, prompting further search for distinct brain structures and neural circuits underpinning the memory process.

HIPPOCAMPAL MEMORY-RELATED STRUCTURES

Vladimir Michailovich Bechterew (1857-1927) presented a case with severe memory weakness [amnesia], falsification of memories (pseudo-memories) [confabulation], and apathy (1900). The autopsy revealed softening of the cerebral cortex in the region of the anterior (gyrus uncinatus) [uncus], and the internal part of both temporal lobes (gyrus cornu Ammonis) [hippocampus proper – fascia dentata – subiculum (according to Bechterew, 1899)] and adjacent parts. He commented that the case presented the characteristic symptom complex of Polyneuritic psychosis [Korsakow’s] that could also occasionally manifest in organic lesions of the cerebral cortex.2020 Bechterew W. Demonstration eines Gehirns mit Zerstörung der vorderen und inneren Teile der Hirnrinde beider Schläfenlappen. Neurologisches. Zentralblatt 1900;19:990-1.,2121 Bechterew MW. Les Voies de Conduction du Cerveau et de la Moelle. Trans Bonne C from 2nd German ed. Paris: Octave Doin, 1900, pp 535-536. [Retrieved from: gallica.bnf.fr]
gallica.bnf.fr...

Bechterew’s report was the first relating memory impairment with lesion of the hippocampal region.

About half a century later, this feature was confirmed by William Beecher Scoville (1906-1984). He performed bilateral medial temporal resection for refractory epileptic seizures and other conditions. One of these patients, later known as the HM case, had surgical removal of the medial surface of the temporal lobes, anterior ⅔ of the hippocampus and the [para]hippocampal gyrus bilaterally, as well as the uncus and the amygdala. The patient was assessed by Brenda Milner who detected severe loss of anterograde memory and partial loss of retrograde memory, whereas early memories appeared normal and technical skills also remained intact. These findings indicated the importance of the hippocampal region [hippocampus and related structures] for the normal functioning of anterograde memory.2222 Scoville WB, Milner B. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1957;20(1):11-21. It is important to stress that the removed hippocampal region included the hippocampus proper, dentate gyrus and subiculum, as well as adjacent temporal lobe structures, as revealed later by neuroimaging and autopsy studies.2323 Augustinack JC, van der Kouwe AJ, Salat DH, Benner T, Stevens AA, Annese J, Fischl B, Frosch MP, Corkin, S. HM's contributions to neuroscience: a review and autopsy studies. Hippocampus 2014;24(11):1267-86. Further studies proceeded in the ensuing decades to distinguish the role of the various components of the hippocampal region in memory processing – the hippocampus proper,2424 Zola-Morgan S, Squire LR, Amaral DG. Human amnesia and the medial temporal region: enduring memory impairment following a bilateral lesion limited to field CA1 of the hippocampus. J Neurosci 1986;6:2950-67. dentate gyrus2525 Baker S, Vieweg P, Gao F, Gilboa A, Wolbers T, Black SE, Rosenbaum RS. The human dentate gyrus plays a necessary role in discriminating new memories. Current Biology 2016;26(19):2629-34. and subiculum complex, and the immediately related regions, namely the perirhinal, entorhinal and para-hippocampal cortical areas.2626 Zola-Morgan S, Squire LR. Neuroanatomy of Memory. Annu Rev Neurosci 1993;16:547-63.

NEOCORTICAL MEMORY-RELATED STRUCTURES

The first to suggest the involvement of the cerebral cortex with memory handling was Willis (1664), albeit in a conjectural manner.2727 Engelhardt E. Cerebral localization of higher functions: The period between Thomas Willis and Paul Broca. Dement Neuropsychol 2019; 13(2):238-43.,2828 Willis T. Cerebri anatome, cui accessit nervorum descriptio et usus. Tho. Roycroft, London: Jo. Martyn & Ja. Allestry, 1664. [Retrieved from: http://books.googleusercontent.com]
http://books.googleusercontent.com...
Further speculative studies followed. New approaches established that the cerebral neocortex represented the seat of storage of long-term memory. This was examined experimentally by Karl Lashley, who inferred that widely dispersed neuronal assemblies represented memories or ‘engrams’ (1950) and also proposed theoretically by Friedrich Hayek, who formalized it in large-scale cortical networks (or “maps”) representing all experience acquired through the senses (1952).2929 Fuster JM. Cortical memory. Scholarpaedia 2007;2(4):1644.

The HM case, as seen earlier, in which long term-memory (‘early memories’) was preserved after the surgery,2222 Scoville WB, Milner B. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1957;20(1):11-21. supported the belief that its area of storage lay outside the regions removed (i.e., extra-hippocampal), presumably located in the spared association neocortex.3030 Squire LR, Bayley PJ. The neuroscience of remote memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2007;17(2):185-96.,3131 Winocur G, Moscovitch M, Bontempi B. Memory formation and long-term retention in humans and animals: Convergence towards a transformation account of hippocampal-neocortical interactions. Neuropsychologia 2010;48(8):2339-56.

The first to demonstrate experimentally that the human cerebral cortex was related to memory was Wilder Graves Penfield (1891-1976). He stimulated the cortex of awake patients who underwent surgery for epilepsy treatment and stated (1959): “There is an area of the surface of the human brain where local electrical stimulation can call back a sequence of past experiences”.3232 Penfield W. The interpretive cortex: The stream of consciousness in the human brain can be electrically reactivated. Science 1959;129(3365): 1719-25. The stimulated regions mentioned were the superior temporal gyrus and the temporo-occipital area, mainly on the left side, where auditory and visual past experiences [identified as stored long-term memory] were elicited (1963).3333 Penfield W. Engrams in the human brain: mechanisms of memory. Proc Royal Soc Med 1968; 61:831-40.,3434 Milner B. Wilder Penfield: his legacy to neurology. Memory mechanisms. Can Med Ass Journal 1977;116(12):1374-6. These areas are now acknowledged as associative neocortex.

Today, it is known that the frontal and temporo-parieto-occipital associative neocortical areas constitute the sites where long-term memories are presumed to be stored.3131 Winocur G, Moscovitch M, Bontempi B. Memory formation and long-term retention in humans and animals: Convergence towards a transformation account of hippocampal-neocortical interactions. Neuropsychologia 2010;48(8):2339-56.,3535 Eichenbaum H. A cortical-hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2000;1(1):41-50.

CONCLUSION

The nature of memory function, as well as the search for its localization, has been a subject of interest since Ancient times. After millennia of theoretical concepts, the core memory-related structures began to be identified, mostly in the late Modern period (XIX and XX centuries), through modern scientifically-based methods. First the diencephalic, then the hippocampal structures were found, and finally the neocortical structures, culminating in the current state of knowledge.

  • This study was conducted at the Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology Unit, INDC – CDA-IPUB – UFRJ – Rio de Janeiro RJ, Brazil.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Clarke E. and O'Malley CD. The Human Brain and Spinal Cord: A Historical Study. 2ª ed. San Francisco: Norman Publishing, 1996.
  • 2
    Julião R, Presti RL, Perler D, van der Eijk P. Mapping Memory. Theories in Ancient, Medieval and Early Modern Philosophy and Medicine. J Ancient Studies 2016;6:678-702.
  • 3
    Engelhardt E. Cerebral localization of the mind and higher functions The beginnings. Dem Neuropsychol. 2018;12(3):321-5.
  • 4
    Nemesius. Némésius. De la Nature de l'homme [trad M. J. B. Thibault]. Paris: Hachette, 1844. [Retrieved from: http://services.biusante.parisdescartes.fr/medicapdf/main.php?key=ZnVsbHwxNTAxNzJ8fA]
    » http://services.biusante.parisdescartes.fr/medicapdf/main.php?key=ZnVsbHwxNTAxNzJ8fA
  • 5
    Acar F, Naderi S, Guvencer M, Türe U, Arda MN. Herophilus of Chalcedon: a pioneer in neuroscience. Neurosurgery 2005;56(4):861-7.
  • 6
    Finger S. Origins of Neuroscience: A History of Explorations into Brain Function. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1994.
  • 7
    Albertus Magnus. Philosophia naturalis. Mich. Furter, 1506. [Retrieved from: https://books.googleusercontent.com]
    » https://books.googleusercontent.com
  • 8
    Wernicke C. Lehrbuch der Gehirnkrankheiten für Aerzte und Studirende. Band II. Berlin: Th Fischer, 1881, pp 229-242. [Retrieved from: https://ia600502.us.archive.org/16/items/lehrbuchdergehir02wern/lehrbuchdergehir02wern.pdf]
    » https://ia600502.us.archive.org/16/items/lehrbuchdergehir02wern/lehrbuchdergehir02wern.pdf
  • 9
    WernickeC. Grundriss der Psychiatrie in klinischen Vorlesungen. Leipzig: Thieme, 1900, pp 295-304. [Retrieved from: https://ia801403.us.archive.org/20/items/grundrissderpsyc00wern/grundrissderpsyc00wern.pdf]
    » https://ia801403.us.archive.org/20/items/grundrissderpsyc00wern/grundrissderpsyc00wern.pdf
  • 10
    Korsakow SS. Eine psychische Störung combinirt mit multipler Neuritis (Psychosis polyneuritica seu Cerebropathia psychica toxaemica). Allgem Zschr Psychiatr 1890;46:475-85.
  • 11
    Korsakow SS. Ueber eine besondere Form psychischer Störung, combinirt mit multipler Neuritis. Arch Psychiatr 1890;21:669-704.
  • 12
    Bonhoeffer KL. Der Korsakowsche Symptomenkomplex in seinen Beziehungen zu den verschiedenen Krankheitsformen. Allg Z Psychiat 1904; 61:744-52.
  • 13
    Korsakow S, Serbski W. Ein Fall von polyneuritischer Psychose mit Autopsie. Archiv f. Psychiatrie 1892;23:112-34.
  • 14
    Korsakow SS. Étude médico-psychologique sur une forme des maladies dela mémoire. Revue philosophique de la France et de l'étranger. 1889; 28:501-530. [gallica.bnf.fr]
    » gallica.bnf.fr
  • 15
    Arts NJM, Walvoort SJW, Kessels RPC. Korsakoff's syndrome: a critical review. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment 2017;13: 2875-90.
  • 16
    Elzholz A. Ueber Beziehungen der Korsakowsche Psychose zur Polioencephalitis acuta haemorrhagica superior. Wiener klin Wochenschrift 1900;15:337-44.
  • 17
    Gudden H. Klinische und anatomische Beiträge zur Kenntniss der multiplen Alkoholneuritis nebst Bemerkungen über die Regenerationsvorgänge im peripheren Nervensystem. Arch f. Psychiatrie u. Nervenkrankheiten 1896;28(3):643-741.
  • 18
    Vann SD, Nelson AJ. The mammillary bodies and memory: more than a hippocampal relay. Progress in Brain Research 2015; 219:163-85.
  • 19
    Gamper E. Zur Frage der polioencephalitis haemorrhagica der chronischen Alkoholiker. Anatomiche Befunde beim alkolischen Korasakow und ihre Beziehungen klinischen bild. Deutsch Ztsch Nervenh 1928;102: 122-9.
  • 20
    Bechterew W. Demonstration eines Gehirns mit Zerstörung der vorderen und inneren Teile der Hirnrinde beider Schläfenlappen. Neurologisches. Zentralblatt 1900;19:990-1.
  • 21
    Bechterew MW. Les Voies de Conduction du Cerveau et de la Moelle. Trans Bonne C from 2nd German ed. Paris: Octave Doin, 1900, pp 535-536. [Retrieved from: gallica.bnf.fr]
    » gallica.bnf.fr
  • 22
    Scoville WB, Milner B. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1957;20(1):11-21.
  • 23
    Augustinack JC, van der Kouwe AJ, Salat DH, Benner T, Stevens AA, Annese J, Fischl B, Frosch MP, Corkin, S. HM's contributions to neuroscience: a review and autopsy studies. Hippocampus 2014;24(11):1267-86.
  • 24
    Zola-Morgan S, Squire LR, Amaral DG. Human amnesia and the medial temporal region: enduring memory impairment following a bilateral lesion limited to field CA1 of the hippocampus. J Neurosci 1986;6:2950-67.
  • 25
    Baker S, Vieweg P, Gao F, Gilboa A, Wolbers T, Black SE, Rosenbaum RS. The human dentate gyrus plays a necessary role in discriminating new memories. Current Biology 2016;26(19):2629-34.
  • 26
    Zola-Morgan S, Squire LR. Neuroanatomy of Memory. Annu Rev Neurosci 1993;16:547-63.
  • 27
    Engelhardt E. Cerebral localization of higher functions: The period between Thomas Willis and Paul Broca. Dement Neuropsychol 2019; 13(2):238-43.
  • 28
    Willis T. Cerebri anatome, cui accessit nervorum descriptio et usus. Tho. Roycroft, London: Jo. Martyn & Ja. Allestry, 1664. [Retrieved from: http://books.googleusercontent.com]
    » http://books.googleusercontent.com
  • 29
    Fuster JM. Cortical memory. Scholarpaedia 2007;2(4):1644.
  • 30
    Squire LR, Bayley PJ. The neuroscience of remote memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2007;17(2):185-96.
  • 31
    Winocur G, Moscovitch M, Bontempi B. Memory formation and long-term retention in humans and animals: Convergence towards a transformation account of hippocampal-neocortical interactions. Neuropsychologia 2010;48(8):2339-56.
  • 32
    Penfield W. The interpretive cortex: The stream of consciousness in the human brain can be electrically reactivated. Science 1959;129(3365): 1719-25.
  • 33
    Penfield W. Engrams in the human brain: mechanisms of memory. Proc Royal Soc Med 1968; 61:831-40.
  • 34
    Milner B. Wilder Penfield: his legacy to neurology. Memory mechanisms. Can Med Ass Journal 1977;116(12):1374-6.
  • 35
    Eichenbaum H. A cortical-hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2000;1(1):41-50.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    16 Mar 2020
  • Date of issue
    Jan-Mar 2020

History

  • Received
    20 Sept 2019
  • Accepted
    23 Nov 2019
Academia Brasileira de Neurologia, Departamento de Neurologia Cognitiva e Envelhecimento R. Vergueiro, 1353 sl.1404 - Ed. Top Towers Offices, Torre Norte, São Paulo, SP, Brazil, CEP 04101-000, Tel.: +55 11 5084-9463 | +55 11 5083-3876 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistadementia@abneuro.org.br | demneuropsy@uol.com.br