Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Phonetic-phonological performance of typical younger and aged adults from Brazil’s capital city

Desempenho fonético-fonológico de adultos jovens e idosos típicos da capital do Brasil

ABSTRACT.

Given the sociodemographic diversity in Brazil, it is fundamental to understand the speech performance of a sample from the Brazilian capital. The repetition task can assess phonological and motor-phonetic planning. Previous studies found phonological-phonetic performance of speakers to be associated with education, age, and other demographic factors. Objectives: To compare the phonetic-phonological performance for speech of younger and aged adults in the capital of Brazil, Federal District (FD); to compare FD performance against national normative means based on São Paulo; to determine the association of phonetic-phonological agility with sociodemographic, cognitive, and neuropsychiatric variables for the sample. Methods: Cross-sectional study. A total of 60 volunteers from the FD, comprising 30 older adults and 30 younger ones, were stratified by education into two subgroups: 2‒7 years and ≥8 years of education. Data on age, educational level, and socioeconomic status were collected. The Verbal Agility subtest of the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination was applied to assess phonetic-phonological performance. Results: No statistically significant difference in performance for verbal agility was found between aged and younger adults from the FD. There was a statistically significant difference in the phonetic-phonological performance of the FD sample compared with the Brazilian normative mean values. Cognitive and socioeconomic variables were associated with verbal agility. Conclusions: In the capital of Brazil, economic status, age, education, and cognitive variables were associated with verbal agility performance, despite there being no difference in phonetic-phonological performance between younger and aged adult groups. Regional differences in phonetic-phonological performance were also evident.

Keywords:
aging; language; speech; phonetics; aged; health of the elderly

RESUMO.

Diante da diversidade sociodemográfica do Brasil, é fundamental compreender o desempenho de fala de uma amostra da capital do país. O teste de repetição pode avaliar o planejamento fonológico e fonético-motor. Estudos prévios mostram que o desempenho fonético-fonológico de falantes foi associado à escolaridade, à idade e a outras variáveis demográficas. Objetivos: Comparar o desempenho fonético-fonológico da fala de idosos e adultos jovens da capital do Brasil, Distrito Federal (DF); comparar o desempenho da amostra do DF com as médias normativas nacionais, obtidas em São Paulo; e verificar a associação da agilidade fonético-fonológica da nossa amostra com as variáveis sociodemográficas, cognitivas e neuropsiquiátricas. Métodos: Estudo transversal. Foram selecionados 60 voluntários do DF, 30 idosos e 30 adultos jovens, subdivididos em dois grupos conforme a escolaridade, de 2 a 7 anos e de 8 anos ou mais de estudo. As características etárias, de escolaridade e de classe econômica foram coletadas. O subteste de agilidade verbal do Teste de Boston para Diagnóstico das Afasias foi aplicado para avaliação do desempenho fonético-fonológico. Resultados: O desempenho de agilidade verbal dos idosos não apresentou diferença estatisticamente significante em relação aos adultos jovens do DF. Houve diferença estatisticamente significante ao se comparar o desempenho fonético-fonológico da amostra do DF com as médias normativas brasileiras. As variáveis cognitivas e socioeconômicas estiveram associadas à agilidade verbal. Conclusões: Na capital do Brasil, as variáveis econômicas, etárias, de escolaridade e cognitivas estiveram associadas à agilidade verbal, apesar da ausência de diferença do desempenho fonético-fonológico entre adultos jovens e idosos. Houve diferenças de desempenho fonético-fonológico conforme a região do país.

Palavras-chave:
envelhecimento; idioma; fala; fonética; idoso; saúde do idoso

INTRODUCTION

According to population projections by the Brazilian Institute of Geography and Statistics (IBGE), the proportion of elderly in the country’s capital city, the Federal District (FD), is set to rise from 11% in 2019 to 33% by 2060.11. Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística - IBGE. 2018. Ministério do Planejamento Orçamento e Gestão. Projeção da população do Brasil por sexo e idade para o período 2010/2060: projeção da população das unidades da federação por sexo e idade para o período 2010/2060, Brasília, Brazil [accessed on Feb 16, 2020]. Available at: Available at: https://www.ibge.gov.br/estatisticas/sociais/populacao/9109-projecao-da-populacao.html?=&t=resultados
https://www.ibge.gov.br/estatisticas/soc...
In view of the growing elderly population and consequent prioritizing of promotion and prevention of elderly health, enshrined in Law 8.842 of 1994,22. Brasil, 1994, Lei nº 8.842, de 4 de janeiro de 1994. Dispõe sobre a política nacional do idoso, cria o Conselho Nacional do Idoso e dá outras providências. Diário Oficial da República Federativa do Brasil, Brasília, Brazil [accessed on Feb 16, 2020]. Available at: Available at: http://www.planalto.gov.br/ccivil_03/leis/L8842.htm
http://www.planalto.gov.br/ccivil_03/lei...
understanding the changes associated with the aging process is paramount.

Phonological working memory abilities decline with age.33. Grivol M, Hage S. Phonological working memory: a comparative study between different age groups. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;23(3):245-51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2179-64912011000300010
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
However, high-educated Brazilian elderly have shown better cognitive performance44. Bertolucci PHF, Brucki SMD, Campacci SR, Juliano Y. The Mini-Mental State Examination in an outpatient population: influence of literacy. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 1994;52(1):1-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X1994000100001
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
and motor speed has predicted deficits in the cognition of individuals with neuropsychiatric disorders,55. Salazar-Fraile J, Balanzá-Martínez V, Selva-Vera G, Martínez-Aran A, Sánchez-Moreno J, Rubio C, et al. Motor speed predicts stability of cognitive deficits in both schizophrenic and bipolar I patients at one-year follow-up. Eur J Psychiat. 2009;23(3):184-97. suggesting a relationship between cognitive-communicative performance and sociodemographic and neuropsychiatric characteristics.

Phonological planning is a linguistic-symbolic function responsible for the sequential selection and combination of phonemes, while phonetic-motor planning is responsible for transforming phonemes into action, a strategy for the motor target.66. van der Merwe A. A theoretical framework for the characterization of pathological speech sensorimotor control. In: McNeil MR, editor. Clinical management of sensorimotor speech disorders. New York: Thieme; 2009. p.3-18. The repetition task can assess both phonological and motor-phonetic speech planning. These communication processing stages can be assessed by the verbal agility subtest from the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE).77. Goodglass H, Kaplan E. The assessment of aphasia and related disorders. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger; 1983. In this subtest, the performance of speakers of Brazilian Portuguese and English was found to be associated with education.88. Borod JC, Goodglass H, Kaplan E. Normative data on the Boston diagnostic aphasia examination, parietal lobe Battery, and the Boston naming test. J Clin Neuropsychol. 1980;2(3):209-15. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688638008403793
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...
,99. Radanovic M, Mansur LL, Scaff M. Normative data for the Brazilian population in the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination: influence of schooling. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(11):1731-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001100019
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
However, in contrast to a North American study,88. Borod JC, Goodglass H, Kaplan E. Normative data on the Boston diagnostic aphasia examination, parietal lobe Battery, and the Boston naming test. J Clin Neuropsychol. 1980;2(3):209-15. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688638008403793
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...
no statistically significant difference in verbal agility was found between different age groups in a Brazilian study.99. Radanovic M, Mansur LL, Scaff M. Normative data for the Brazilian population in the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination: influence of schooling. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(11):1731-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001100019
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...

It is noteworthy that the normative data for the verbal agility subtest used throughout Brazil were collected in São Paulo (SP), southeastern region of the country.99. Radanovic M, Mansur LL, Scaff M. Normative data for the Brazilian population in the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination: influence of schooling. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(11):1731-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001100019
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
As previously reported, sociodemographic factors influence cognitive performance.44. Bertolucci PHF, Brucki SMD, Campacci SR, Juliano Y. The Mini-Mental State Examination in an outpatient population: influence of literacy. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 1994;52(1):1-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X1994000100001
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
,99. Radanovic M, Mansur LL, Scaff M. Normative data for the Brazilian population in the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination: influence of schooling. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(11):1731-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001100019
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
Given the sociodemographic diversity in Brazil, it is essential to understand the phonetic-phonological speech performance of a sample from the Brazilian capital, and its relationship with the participants’ sociodemographic characteristics.

Therefore, the objective of this study was to compare the verbal agility performance of younger and aged adults from the FD, capital of Brazil. The study hypothesis is that the verbal agility performance of younger adults is superior to that of aged ones. The study also compared the verbal agility performance of the FD sample with that of normative means for the Brazilian population and observed that it is different from those obtained in other regions of the country. The study also aimed to determine the association between phonetic-phonological performance and sociodemographic, cognitive, and neuropsychiatric variables. The hypothesis is that these variables are associated with verbal agility.

METHODS

The study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the University, under CAAE 56190716.0.0000.0030 and permit No. 1.657.122. All participants signed the Informed Consent Form, drafted in accordance with Resolution No. 466/2012 of the Brazilian National Health Council, Ministry of Health.

The study sample comprised 60 volunteers from the Brazilian capital, stratified by age into 2 groups: aged adults (≥60 years of age) and younger adults (19‒39 years of age). Fifteen volunteers from each group had 2‒7 years of education, whereas 15 had ≥8 years of education.

This study is part of a larger project by the FortaleçaMente Group, involved in education, research, and extension activities on communication involving adults and elderly from the FD. The inclusion and exclusion criteria applied to participants have been published elsewhere by the FortaleçaMente Group.1010. Cera ML, Silva LD. Narrative discourse of younger and older adults from Brazil associated with demographic factors. CoDAS. 2020;32(5):e20190130. Inclusion criteria were: having a typical stable state of health, with no history of psychiatric or neurologic diseases, being right-handed, and having lived in the FD for 10 years or longer. Exclusion criteria were: having a prior or current history of alcoholism or illegal drug use; having visual or hearing deficits or being in use of a hearing aid or poorly adapted dental prosthesis that could negatively affect performance on the tasks performed; and having below expected results on screening tests for cognition and depression.

The phonetic-phonological assessments were carried out in a quiet space at the volunteers’ homes, institutions or other venue of their choice. As described by Cera and Silva,1010. Cera ML, Silva LD. Narrative discourse of younger and older adults from Brazil associated with demographic factors. CoDAS. 2020;32(5):e20190130. information on age, gender, education, and economic status were collected, according to the Brazilian Economic Classification Criteria,1111. Associação Brasileira de Empresas de Pesquisa. Critério de Classificação Econômica Brasil. Brazil; 2016 [accessed on Feb 16, 2020]. Available at: Available at: http://www.abep.org/criterio-brasil
http://www.abep.org/criterio-brasil...
to determine socioeconomic levels; and assessments were performed using the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE)44. Bertolucci PHF, Brucki SMD, Campacci SR, Juliano Y. The Mini-Mental State Examination in an outpatient population: influence of literacy. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 1994;52(1):1-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X1994000100001
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
,1212. Folstein MF, Folstein ME, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189-98. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
for global cognitive screening; and by the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS)1313. Yesavaje JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, Lum O, Huang V, Adey M, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res. 1982-1983;17(1):37-49. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
to identify signs of depression. These instruments are described below:

  • Brazilian Association of Research Companies (Associação Brasileira de Empresas de Pesquisa - ABEP):1111. Associação Brasileira de Empresas de Pesquisa. Critério de Classificação Econômica Brasil. Brazil; 2016 [accessed on Feb 16, 2020]. Available at: Available at: http://www.abep.org/criterio-brasil
    http://www.abep.org/criterio-brasil...
    questionnaire collecting data on items in household, householder education, and access to public utility services, to provide an estimate of household income, scored on a scale of 0 to 100, with greater scores indicating higher economic class;

  • MMSE:44. Bertolucci PHF, Brucki SMD, Campacci SR, Juliano Y. The Mini-Mental State Examination in an outpatient population: influence of literacy. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 1994;52(1):1-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X1994000100001
    https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
    ,1212. Folstein MF, Folstein ME, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189-98. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
    https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
    participants’ scores exceeded the cut-off values for the Brazilian population published by Bertolucci et al.,44. Bertolucci PHF, Brucki SMD, Campacci SR, Juliano Y. The Mini-Mental State Examination in an outpatient population: influence of literacy. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 1994;52(1):1-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X1994000100001
    https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
    of 18 points for education of 1‒7 years, and 26 for ≥8 years;

  • GDS:1313. Yesavaje JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, Lum O, Huang V, Adey M, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res. 1982-1983;17(1):37-49. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4
    https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
    the short form of the scale with a maximum score of 15 was used, in which scores ≥5 points suggest depression. All study participants scored >5 points.

In addition, the Verbal agility subtest of the BDAE77. Goodglass H, Kaplan E. The assessment of aphasia and related disorders. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger; 1983. was applied for the phonetic-phonological assessment. In this test, subjects are instructed to repeat words as quickly as possible and the number of repetitions in 5 seconds is registered. The test stimuli for the Brazilian version are as follows: 1) vovó-vovó, 2) tic-tac, 3) pula-pula, 4) saúde-saúde, 5) Filomena, 6) ponta direita, and 7) lagartixa. The sequence of repetition for each stimuli from the subtest is scored with 0, 1 or 2 points according to pre-established repetition bands, with 2 points assigned for producing a greater number of correct repetitions of the stimuli; 1 point for lower numbers of correct repetitions; and 0 points for correct repetitions below the minimum number expected. Total score ranges from 0 (worst performance) to 14 (best performance) points. The Brazilian normative mean values collected in SP are 12.6 points for the overall sample, 11.9 for subjects with educational level of 1‒8 years, and 13.0 for those with >9 years of education.99. Radanovic M, Mansur LL, Scaff M. Normative data for the Brazilian population in the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination: influence of schooling. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(11):1731-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001100019
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...

Statistical method

The Kolmogorov-Smirnov determined the data distribution, defining the selection of statistical tests for the inferential analysis. The IBM SPSS Statistics 22 software was used for statistical tests.

Fisher’s exact and Mann-Whitney tests were used to compare sociodemographic, neuropsychiatric, and cognitive variables between the groups.

The Mann-Whitney test was used to compare verbal agility performance of the younger and aged adults.

Student’s t-test was employed to the compare phonological performance of the FD sample against Brazilian normative mean values.

Spearman’s correlation coefficient was used to analyze the association of verbal agility with sociodemographic, neuropsychiatric, and cognitive variables.

A p value of 0.05 was adopted to indicate statistical significance.

RESULTS

A total of 65 volunteers from the FD took part in the study. Five subjects were subsequently excluded for scoring lower than Brazilian means on the cognitive screening and depression tests. The proportion of females in each group ranged from 87 to 90%. Mean score on the ABEP was 31.2 (9.8) for younger adults and 26.8 (7.8) for older adults, both classified with social economic stratum B2. Mean age of the older adults was 66.8 (4.6) years and younger adults was 28.7 (7.9) years; mean education was 7.6 (3.7) years and 8.2 (4.1) years; mean time residing in the FD was 47.6 (9.5) years and 24.0 (7.5) years; GDS score was 1.5 (2.0) and 2.9 (2.3); and MMSE score was 26.2 (0.2) and 26.6 (0.8), respectively. There was no statistically significant difference in economic class, education or cognition between the two age groups, except for depression screening score, which proved lower among the younger group.1010. Cera ML, Silva LD. Narrative discourse of younger and older adults from Brazil associated with demographic factors. CoDAS. 2020;32(5):e20190130.

Regarding findings of the present study for phonetic-phonological aspects, Table 1 presents a comparison of the performance by the groups of younger and aged adults from the FD, revealing no statistically significant difference for age.

Table 1.
Comparison of phonetic-phonological performance of younger and aged adults from the Brazilian capital, Federal District, using the Mann-Whitney test.

A comparison of verbal agility performance of the FD volunteers against Brazilian normative mean values for SP is shown in Table 2. There was a statistically significant difference between the groups, with the FD sample exhibiting worse verbal agility performance.

Table 2.
Comparison of verbal agility performance of Federal District volunteers with Brazilian normative mean values for São Paulo.

Results of the correlation between verbal agility performance and sociodemographic, neuropsychiatric, and cognitive variables are given in Table 3. The association of age, education, economic status, and cognitive performance with verbal agility proved to be statistically significant. For the overall sample, verbal agility performance correlated with education, socioeconomic class, and score on the screening test. In the low-educated group, no association was found between speech production and the variables studied. For the high-educated group, verbal agility performance was correlated with both age and education.

Table 3.
Correlation between verbal agility performance of Federal District volunteers and sociodemographic variables.

DISCUSSION

The main finding of this study was that there was no statistically significant difference in phonetic-phonological performance between younger and aged adults from the FD - capital of Brazil (Table 1) -, but the performance of the FD sample was worse than the normative national mean, derived from SP data (Table 2). Also, verbal agility was associated with the sociodemographic variables and with scores on the cognitive screening tests (Table 3). These findings are discussed below.

The results for the comparison of verbal agility performance of younger and aged adults corroborate the Brazilian study, which observed similar phonetic-phonological performance across the three age groups studied,99. Radanovic M, Mansur LL, Scaff M. Normative data for the Brazilian population in the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination: influence of schooling. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(11):1731-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001100019
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
yet differed from a study conducted in another country.88. Borod JC, Goodglass H, Kaplan E. Normative data on the Boston diagnostic aphasia examination, parietal lobe Battery, and the Boston naming test. J Clin Neuropsychol. 1980;2(3):209-15. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688638008403793
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...
Decline in phonological processing with age has been observed in other studies.1414. Diaz MT, Johnson MA, Burke DM, Madden DJ. Age-related differences in the neural bases of phonological and semantic processes. J Cogn Neurosci. 2014;26(12):2798-811. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00665
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1162/...
,1515. Rizio AA, Moyer KJ, Diaz MT. Neural evidence for phonologically based language production deficits in older adults: an fMRI investigation of age-related differences in picture-word interference. Brain Behav. 2017;7(4):e00660. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.660
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1002/...
,1616. Zhuang J, Johnson MA, Madden DJ, Burke DM, Diaz MT. Age-related differences in resolving semantic and phonological competition during receptive language tasks. Neuropsychologia. 2016;93(Pt A):189-99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.10.016
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
The difference compared to a previous study assessing verbal agility88. Borod JC, Goodglass H, Kaplan E. Normative data on the Boston diagnostic aphasia examination, parietal lobe Battery, and the Boston naming test. J Clin Neuropsychol. 1980;2(3):209-15. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688638008403793
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...
was likely due to sociodemographic disparities between the countries or differences in the languages and their variations. In the present study, verbal agility was assessed using a repetition task, i.e., phonological output processing, whereas other studies which detected impairments in phonological processing with aging assessed input processing1414. Diaz MT, Johnson MA, Burke DM, Madden DJ. Age-related differences in the neural bases of phonological and semantic processes. J Cogn Neurosci. 2014;26(12):2798-811. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00665
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1162/...
,1616. Zhuang J, Johnson MA, Madden DJ, Burke DM, Diaz MT. Age-related differences in resolving semantic and phonological competition during receptive language tasks. Neuropsychologia. 2016;93(Pt A):189-99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.10.016
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
or utterances using the naming task.1515. Rizio AA, Moyer KJ, Diaz MT. Neural evidence for phonologically based language production deficits in older adults: an fMRI investigation of age-related differences in picture-word interference. Brain Behav. 2017;7(4):e00660. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.660
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1002/...
Thus, the differences in the present results relative to those of other studies might be related to the stage of language processing assessed, input or output; to the method of assessing phonological processing, repetition or naming; or to sociodemographic and linguistic differences.

During the aging process, compensatory activities are active in conjunction with neural plasticity.1717. Tyler LK, Shafto MA, Randall B, Wringht P, Marslen-Wilson WD, Stamatakis EA. Preserving syntactic processing across the adult life span: the modulation of the frontotemporal language system in the context of age-related atrophy. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20(2):352-64. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp105
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1093/...
It is therefore believed that, for verbal agility, Brazilians use compensatory strategies to maintain phonetic-phonological performance, despite the effects of aging. This notion is supported by the present study and by a previous investigation carried out in another part of the country,99. Radanovic M, Mansur LL, Scaff M. Normative data for the Brazilian population in the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination: influence of schooling. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(11):1731-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001100019
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
both of which reported similar output phonetic-phonological processing results. Moreover, in the present study, the similarities in sex, education, economic class, and cognitive performance for the groups studied may also have contributed to the absence of difference in verbal agility between younger and aged adults,1010. Cera ML, Silva LD. Narrative discourse of younger and older adults from Brazil associated with demographic factors. CoDAS. 2020;32(5):e20190130. given that age was the only variable that differed between the groups.

Sociodemographic disparities can be found in different regions of Brazil - a country with major socioeconomic and cultural diversity. Therefore, phonetic-phonological performance of volunteers from FD were compared to national normative averages for SP.99. Radanovic M, Mansur LL, Scaff M. Normative data for the Brazilian population in the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination: influence of schooling. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(11):1731-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001100019
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
The volunteers from the Brazilian capital had worse performance (Table 2), corroborating the findings regarding the association of sociodemographic variables with cognitive,1818. Faria CA, Lourenço RA, Ribeiro PC, Lopes CS. Cognitive performance and frailty in older adults clients of a private health care plan. Rev Saude Publica. 2013;47(5):923-30. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0034-8910.2013047004451
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/...
,1919. Scarmeas N, Zarahn E, Anderson KE, Habeck CG, Hilthon J, Flynn J, et al. Association of life activities with cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer disease: implications for the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Arch Neurol. 2003;60(3):359-65. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.60.3.359
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1001/...
,2020. Zahodne LB, Stern Y, Manly JJ. Differing effects of education on cognitive decline in diverse elders with low versus high educational attainment. Neuropsychology. 2015;29(4):649-57. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000141
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/...
language,1414. Diaz MT, Johnson MA, Burke DM, Madden DJ. Age-related differences in the neural bases of phonological and semantic processes. J Cogn Neurosci. 2014;26(12):2798-811. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00665
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1162/...
,1515. Rizio AA, Moyer KJ, Diaz MT. Neural evidence for phonologically based language production deficits in older adults: an fMRI investigation of age-related differences in picture-word interference. Brain Behav. 2017;7(4):e00660. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.660
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1002/...
,1616. Zhuang J, Johnson MA, Madden DJ, Burke DM, Diaz MT. Age-related differences in resolving semantic and phonological competition during receptive language tasks. Neuropsychologia. 2016;93(Pt A):189-99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.10.016
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
,2121. Miotto EC, Sato J, Lúcia MCS, Camargo CHP, Scaff M. Development of an adapted version of the Boston Naming Test for Portuguese speakers. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2010;32(3):279-82. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-44462010005000006
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/...
,2222. Olabarrieta-Landa L, Rivera D, Morlett-Paredes A, Jaimes-Bautista A, Garza MT, Garza-del-Angel J. Standard form of Boston Naming Test: normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. NeuroRehabilitation. 2015;37(4):501-13. https://doi.org/10.3233/nre-151278
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.3233/...
and specifically with verbal agility performance.88. Borod JC, Goodglass H, Kaplan E. Normative data on the Boston diagnostic aphasia examination, parietal lobe Battery, and the Boston naming test. J Clin Neuropsychol. 1980;2(3):209-15. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688638008403793
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...
,99. Radanovic M, Mansur LL, Scaff M. Normative data for the Brazilian population in the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination: influence of schooling. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(11):1731-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001100019
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
Differences in verbal agility between the samples from different regions of Brazil may also be related to linguistic variations, especially in light of the previous observation that speakers from SP city have regional variations in speech that differ from other regions of the country.2323. Peres DO. A identificação das variedades regionais do português brasileiro através da informação entoacional. Let Hoje. 2017;52(1):40-50. https://doi.org/10.15448/1984-7726.2017.1.25408
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.15448...
In addition, it is important to bear in mind that most elderly in the FD were originally from other regions of the country, thus conferring more diverse speech characteristics. Moreover, results showed that average time living in the FD was 48 years for aged adults and 24 years for younger ones, factors which may further the systematization of speech characteristics of the population from the country’s capital. This language variation typical of the FD was previously reported,2424. Dettoni RV, Pacheco CS, Andrade CQ, Scherre MMP. Linguistic variation project in the Midwest region of Brazil - Valco. Alfa Rev Linguíst. 2012;56(3):807-33. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1981-57942012000300004
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/...
although the study in question did not assess speech rate in this region of the country. Based on the difference between the Brazilian normative means and those found in the present study, it can be concluded that speakers from the FD have lower verbal agility performance, suggesting a slower speech rate compared to speakers from SP. For neurologic disorders to be properly diagnosed, regional phonetic-phonological characteristics should be taken into account. The present study reports the verbal agility performance of Brazilians from the FD, capital of Brazil since 1960. Previous Brazilian studies on verbal agility have been conducted in the SP region in both typical speakers99. Radanovic M, Mansur LL, Scaff M. Normative data for the Brazilian population in the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination: influence of schooling. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(11):1731-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001100019
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
and individuals with neurological disorders.2525. Cera ML, Ortiz KZ, Bertolucci PHF, Minett TSC. Speech and orofacial apraxias in Alzheimer’s disease. Int Psychogeriatr. 2013;25(10):1679-85. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610213000781
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
Interestingly, the mean age of the overall sample from the FD was 47.7 (20.2) years, almost identical to that of the SP sample, of 47.2 (17.6) years.99. Radanovic M, Mansur LL, Scaff M. Normative data for the Brazilian population in the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination: influence of schooling. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(11):1731-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001100019
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
It therefore follows that normative differences for the FD and SP found in the present study were not attributed to participants’ age.

In addition, the association between phonetic-phonological performance and sociodemographic, cognitive, and neuropsychiatric variables is explored below.

Age was associated with verbal agility in the high-educated group (Table 3). Thus, this study suggests that the areas responsible for linguistic-phonological and motor-phonetic processing remain activated in a similar fashion in both younger and aged adults. This holds true, except for higher educational level, in which case better verbal agility performance is associated with lower age. With regard to the neuroanatomical substrates of acquired phonological and phonetic disorders, Ripamonti et al. listed the following regions as responsible for phonetic processing: left central operculum, left inferior frontal gyrus, left precentral gyrus, left insula, left parietal operculum, left supramarginal gyrus, left supplementary motor cortex, left frontal operculum, and left putamen.2626. Ripamonti E, Frustaci M, Zonca G, Aggujaro S, Monteni F. Disentangling phonological and articulatory processing: a neuroanatomical study in aphasia. Neuropsychologia. 2018;121:175-85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.10.015
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
The regions described as responsible for phonological processing were: posterior portion of the left superior temporal gyrus, left supramarginal gyrus, Heschl’s gyrus, left temporal plane, posterior portion of the left middle temporal gyrus, and left parietal operculum. 2626. Ripamonti E, Frustaci M, Zonca G, Aggujaro S, Monteni F. Disentangling phonological and articulatory processing: a neuroanatomical study in aphasia. Neuropsychologia. 2018;121:175-85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.10.015
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
Thus, differences in the functioning of the regions responsible for linguistic-phonological and motor-phonetic processing with greater length of formal study may have influenced the results found in this study of better verbal agility performance associated with lower age in high-educated subjects.

The result showing an association of education with verbal agility, where the greater the verbal agility, the longer the period of formal study (Table 3), corroborates studies on verbal agility measured using the BDAE88. Borod JC, Goodglass H, Kaplan E. Normative data on the Boston diagnostic aphasia examination, parietal lobe Battery, and the Boston naming test. J Clin Neuropsychol. 1980;2(3):209-15. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688638008403793
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...
,99. Radanovic M, Mansur LL, Scaff M. Normative data for the Brazilian population in the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination: influence of schooling. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(11):1731-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001100019
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
and faster cognitive processing speed for higher educated individuals.2727. Teixeira-Fabrício A, Lima-Silva TB, Kissaki PT, Vieira MG, Ordonez TN, Oliveira TB, et al. Cognitive training in older adults and the elderly: impact of educational strategies according age. Psico-USF. 2012;17(1):85-95. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-82712012000100010
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/...

The present study revealed that, the greater the educational level, the stronger the association of phonetic-phonological performance with age and education. This result corroborates the study by Rentz et al., which suggested a neuroprotective effect and less vulnerability to the neurotoxic effects among subjects with greater cognitive reserve.2828. Rentz DM, Locascio JJ, Becker JA, Moran EK, Eng E, Buckner RL, et al. Cognition, reserve and amyloid deposition in normal aging. Ann Neurol. 2010;67(3):353-64. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.21904
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1002/...
Cognitive reserve is determined by various factors, including socioeconomic and educational,1919. Scarmeas N, Zarahn E, Anderson KE, Habeck CG, Hilthon J, Flynn J, et al. Association of life activities with cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer disease: implications for the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Arch Neurol. 2003;60(3):359-65. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.60.3.359
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1001/...
while sociodemographic aspects influence cognitive performance.1818. Faria CA, Lourenço RA, Ribeiro PC, Lopes CS. Cognitive performance and frailty in older adults clients of a private health care plan. Rev Saude Publica. 2013;47(5):923-30. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0034-8910.2013047004451
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/...
,2020. Zahodne LB, Stern Y, Manly JJ. Differing effects of education on cognitive decline in diverse elders with low versus high educational attainment. Neuropsychology. 2015;29(4):649-57. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000141
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/...
In a study involving participants from Latin American countries, age influenced language performance in only some countries, leading to norms adjusted for age in just some of these regions, while the effect of education on language performance was seen in all countries studied.2222. Olabarrieta-Landa L, Rivera D, Morlett-Paredes A, Jaimes-Bautista A, Garza MT, Garza-del-Angel J. Standard form of Boston Naming Test: normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. NeuroRehabilitation. 2015;37(4):501-13. https://doi.org/10.3233/nre-151278
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.3233/...
In Brazil, the influence of age and education on language performance was found in many different regions.2121. Miotto EC, Sato J, Lúcia MCS, Camargo CHP, Scaff M. Development of an adapted version of the Boston Naming Test for Portuguese speakers. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2010;32(3):279-82. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-44462010005000006
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/...

It is interesting to note that, besides education, economic status and cognitive score were also associated with verbal agility performance in the sample of 60 volunteers assessed (Table 3). Scarmeas et al. argued that occupational and educational aspects are life experiences that may allow some people to better cope with cognitive changes and that challenging experiences may attenuate neurodegenerative processes.1919. Scarmeas N, Zarahn E, Anderson KE, Habeck CG, Hilthon J, Flynn J, et al. Association of life activities with cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer disease: implications for the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Arch Neurol. 2003;60(3):359-65. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.60.3.359
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1001/...
Although conducted in a society with relatively low income inequality, the results of the study by Nishizawa et al. suggest that socioeconomic status in childhood may be an element of cognitive reserve, with important implications for late-life subjective memory complaints.2929. Nishizawa T, Morita A, Fujiwara T, Kondo K. Association between childhood socioeconomic status and subjective memory complaints among older adults: results from the Japan Gerontological Evaluation Study 2010. Int Psychogeriatr. 2019;31(12):1699-707. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1041610219000814
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
Thus, reports in the literature are consistent with our conclusions, confirming that sociodemographic factors are associated with cognitive abilities, specifically phonetic-phonological performance.

The present study has some limitations that deserve to be highlighted, such as the fact that most volunteers were female. Consequently, generalizing these results for men should be done with caution. However, previous studies observed no differences between men and women for speech fluency.3030. Andrade CRF, Martins VO. Influence of gender and educational status on fluente adults’ speech fluency. Rev Logop Foniatr Audiol. 2011;31(2):74-81. Furthermore, the present study analyzed the quantitative performance for phonetic-phonological processing, based on scores on the verbal agility subtest. However, in clinical practice, in addition to quantitative analyses of performance in the verbal agility subtest, it is fundamental that the qualitative analyses of phonetic symptoms be differentiated from phonological ones, as per the differential diagnosis of aphasia and apraxia of speech.3131. Cera ML, Ortiz KZ, Bertolucci PHF, Minett TSC. Phonetic and phonological aspects of speech in Alzheimer’s disease. Aphasiology. 2017;32(1):88-102. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2017.1362687
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...
,3232. McNeil MR, Robin DA, Schmidt RA. Apraxia of speech. In: McNeil MR, editor. Clinical management of sensorimotor speech disorders. New York: Thieme ; 2009. p. 249-68.

Therefore, study results confirmed no difference in agility between the younger and aged adults from the FD. The FD sample, however, had statistically worse phonetic-phonological performance relative to Brazilian normative means. Lastly, the sociodemographic and cognitive factors were associated with verbal ability performance.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors would like to thank the volunteers who agreed to take part in this study.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística - IBGE. 2018. Ministério do Planejamento Orçamento e Gestão. Projeção da população do Brasil por sexo e idade para o período 2010/2060: projeção da população das unidades da federação por sexo e idade para o período 2010/2060, Brasília, Brazil [accessed on Feb 16, 2020]. Available at: Available at: https://www.ibge.gov.br/estatisticas/sociais/populacao/9109-projecao-da-populacao.html?=&t=resultados
    » https://www.ibge.gov.br/estatisticas/sociais/populacao/9109-projecao-da-populacao.html?=&t=resultados
  • 2
    Brasil, 1994, Lei nº 8.842, de 4 de janeiro de 1994. Dispõe sobre a política nacional do idoso, cria o Conselho Nacional do Idoso e dá outras providências. Diário Oficial da República Federativa do Brasil, Brasília, Brazil [accessed on Feb 16, 2020]. Available at: Available at: http://www.planalto.gov.br/ccivil_03/leis/L8842.htm
    » http://www.planalto.gov.br/ccivil_03/leis/L8842.htm
  • 3
    Grivol M, Hage S. Phonological working memory: a comparative study between different age groups. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;23(3):245-51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2179-64912011000300010
    » https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2179-64912011000300010
  • 4
    Bertolucci PHF, Brucki SMD, Campacci SR, Juliano Y. The Mini-Mental State Examination in an outpatient population: influence of literacy. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 1994;52(1):1-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X1994000100001
    » https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X1994000100001
  • 5
    Salazar-Fraile J, Balanzá-Martínez V, Selva-Vera G, Martínez-Aran A, Sánchez-Moreno J, Rubio C, et al. Motor speed predicts stability of cognitive deficits in both schizophrenic and bipolar I patients at one-year follow-up. Eur J Psychiat. 2009;23(3):184-97.
  • 6
    van der Merwe A. A theoretical framework for the characterization of pathological speech sensorimotor control. In: McNeil MR, editor. Clinical management of sensorimotor speech disorders. New York: Thieme; 2009. p.3-18.
  • 7
    Goodglass H, Kaplan E. The assessment of aphasia and related disorders. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger; 1983.
  • 8
    Borod JC, Goodglass H, Kaplan E. Normative data on the Boston diagnostic aphasia examination, parietal lobe Battery, and the Boston naming test. J Clin Neuropsychol. 1980;2(3):209-15. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688638008403793
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/01688638008403793
  • 9
    Radanovic M, Mansur LL, Scaff M. Normative data for the Brazilian population in the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination: influence of schooling. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(11):1731-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001100019
    » https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001100019
  • 10
    Cera ML, Silva LD. Narrative discourse of younger and older adults from Brazil associated with demographic factors. CoDAS. 2020;32(5):e20190130.
  • 11
    Associação Brasileira de Empresas de Pesquisa. Critério de Classificação Econômica Brasil. Brazil; 2016 [accessed on Feb 16, 2020]. Available at: Available at: http://www.abep.org/criterio-brasil
    » http://www.abep.org/criterio-brasil
  • 12
    Folstein MF, Folstein ME, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189-98. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
  • 13
    Yesavaje JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, Lum O, Huang V, Adey M, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res. 1982-1983;17(1):37-49. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4
  • 14
    Diaz MT, Johnson MA, Burke DM, Madden DJ. Age-related differences in the neural bases of phonological and semantic processes. J Cogn Neurosci. 2014;26(12):2798-811. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00665
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00665
  • 15
    Rizio AA, Moyer KJ, Diaz MT. Neural evidence for phonologically based language production deficits in older adults: an fMRI investigation of age-related differences in picture-word interference. Brain Behav. 2017;7(4):e00660. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.660
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.660
  • 16
    Zhuang J, Johnson MA, Madden DJ, Burke DM, Diaz MT. Age-related differences in resolving semantic and phonological competition during receptive language tasks. Neuropsychologia. 2016;93(Pt A):189-99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.10.016
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.10.016
  • 17
    Tyler LK, Shafto MA, Randall B, Wringht P, Marslen-Wilson WD, Stamatakis EA. Preserving syntactic processing across the adult life span: the modulation of the frontotemporal language system in the context of age-related atrophy. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20(2):352-64. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp105
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp105
  • 18
    Faria CA, Lourenço RA, Ribeiro PC, Lopes CS. Cognitive performance and frailty in older adults clients of a private health care plan. Rev Saude Publica. 2013;47(5):923-30. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0034-8910.2013047004451
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/S0034-8910.2013047004451
  • 19
    Scarmeas N, Zarahn E, Anderson KE, Habeck CG, Hilthon J, Flynn J, et al. Association of life activities with cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer disease: implications for the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Arch Neurol. 2003;60(3):359-65. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.60.3.359
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.60.3.359
  • 20
    Zahodne LB, Stern Y, Manly JJ. Differing effects of education on cognitive decline in diverse elders with low versus high educational attainment. Neuropsychology. 2015;29(4):649-57. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000141
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000141
  • 21
    Miotto EC, Sato J, Lúcia MCS, Camargo CHP, Scaff M. Development of an adapted version of the Boston Naming Test for Portuguese speakers. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2010;32(3):279-82. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-44462010005000006
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-44462010005000006
  • 22
    Olabarrieta-Landa L, Rivera D, Morlett-Paredes A, Jaimes-Bautista A, Garza MT, Garza-del-Angel J. Standard form of Boston Naming Test: normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. NeuroRehabilitation. 2015;37(4):501-13. https://doi.org/10.3233/nre-151278
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.3233/nre-151278
  • 23
    Peres DO. A identificação das variedades regionais do português brasileiro através da informação entoacional. Let Hoje. 2017;52(1):40-50. https://doi.org/10.15448/1984-7726.2017.1.25408
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.15448/1984-7726.2017.1.25408
  • 24
    Dettoni RV, Pacheco CS, Andrade CQ, Scherre MMP. Linguistic variation project in the Midwest region of Brazil - Valco. Alfa Rev Linguíst. 2012;56(3):807-33. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1981-57942012000300004
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/S1981-57942012000300004
  • 25
    Cera ML, Ortiz KZ, Bertolucci PHF, Minett TSC. Speech and orofacial apraxias in Alzheimer’s disease. Int Psychogeriatr. 2013;25(10):1679-85. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610213000781
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610213000781
  • 26
    Ripamonti E, Frustaci M, Zonca G, Aggujaro S, Monteni F. Disentangling phonological and articulatory processing: a neuroanatomical study in aphasia. Neuropsychologia. 2018;121:175-85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.10.015
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.10.015
  • 27
    Teixeira-Fabrício A, Lima-Silva TB, Kissaki PT, Vieira MG, Ordonez TN, Oliveira TB, et al. Cognitive training in older adults and the elderly: impact of educational strategies according age. Psico-USF. 2012;17(1):85-95. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-82712012000100010
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-82712012000100010
  • 28
    Rentz DM, Locascio JJ, Becker JA, Moran EK, Eng E, Buckner RL, et al. Cognition, reserve and amyloid deposition in normal aging. Ann Neurol. 2010;67(3):353-64. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.21904
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.21904
  • 29
    Nishizawa T, Morita A, Fujiwara T, Kondo K. Association between childhood socioeconomic status and subjective memory complaints among older adults: results from the Japan Gerontological Evaluation Study 2010. Int Psychogeriatr. 2019;31(12):1699-707. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1041610219000814
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/s1041610219000814
  • 30
    Andrade CRF, Martins VO. Influence of gender and educational status on fluente adults’ speech fluency. Rev Logop Foniatr Audiol. 2011;31(2):74-81.
  • 31
    Cera ML, Ortiz KZ, Bertolucci PHF, Minett TSC. Phonetic and phonological aspects of speech in Alzheimer’s disease. Aphasiology. 2017;32(1):88-102. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2017.1362687
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2017.1362687
  • 32
    McNeil MR, Robin DA, Schmidt RA. Apraxia of speech. In: McNeil MR, editor. Clinical management of sensorimotor speech disorders. New York: Thieme ; 2009. p. 249-68.
  • 1
    This study was conducted at the Faculdade de Ceilândia, Universidade de Brasília, Brasília, DF, Brazil.
  • Funding: This work was supported by the Fundação de Apoio à Pesquisa do Distrito Federal (grant number 193.001.519/2017).

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    18 Sept 2020
  • Date of issue
    Jul-Sep 2020

History

  • Received
    03 Mar 2020
  • Accepted
    04 May 2020
Academia Brasileira de Neurologia, Departamento de Neurologia Cognitiva e Envelhecimento R. Vergueiro, 1353 sl.1404 - Ed. Top Towers Offices, Torre Norte, São Paulo, SP, Brazil, CEP 04101-000, Tel.: +55 11 5084-9463 | +55 11 5083-3876 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistadementia@abneuro.org.br | demneuropsy@uol.com.br