Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Sequential production of motor-action verb subtypes in Parkinson's disease patients

PRODUÇÃO SEQUENCIAL DE SUBTIPOS DE VERBOS DE AÇÃO MOTORA EM PACIENTES COM DOENÇA DE PARKINSON

ABSTRACT

Motor-action verbs (MAVs) production and comprehension are compromised in patients with Parkinson's disease (PD).

Objectives:

The aim of this study was to characterize the sequential production of three subtypes of MAVs in PD patients: whole body (e.g., run), specific body part (e.g., kick), and instrumental (e.g., saw). This study also aimed to identify the production characteristics for each of the two main phases in fluency performance: selection (initial abundant item production) and retrieval (more paced and scarce production).

Methods:

This study involved a group of 20 nondemented, on-medication PD patients, with an average age of 66.59 years (standard deviation = 4.13), and a comparison group (CG) of 20 normal elderly individuals, matched by years of education and controlled for cognitive performance and depression. Both groups performed a classical verb fluency task. Sequential word-by-word analyses were conducted.

Results:

Significant differences were found at the initial production of whole-body MAVs and the overall production of instrumental verbs (both measures were lower in the PD group). A repeated-measures analysis of variance confirmed the linear CG performance and the quadratic PD performance.

Conclusions:

PD patients present altered production of whole-body and instrumental MAVs. This proposal for the semantic sequential analysis of motor verbs deserves further investigation, as a new methodology for the evaluation of fluency performance in motor-related disease.

Keywords:
Parkinson's Disease; Aging; Executive Functions; Language

RESUMO

A produção e a compreensão das ações motoras (MAVs) estão comprometidas em pacientes com doença de Parkinson (DP).

Objetivos:

Caracterizar a produção sequencial de três subtipos de verbos de MAVs: corpo inteiro (por exemplo, corre), parte corporal específica (por exemplo, chute) e instrumental (por exemplo, serra) em pacientes com DP. Identificar as características de produção para cada uma das duas principais fases em desempenho de fluência: seleção (produção inicial abundante de itens) e recuperação (produção mais acelerada e escassa).

Métodos:

Um grupo de 20 pacientes com DP não demência, com idade média de 66,59 (desvio padrão — DP= 4,13), e um grupo de comparação (GC) de 20 idosos normais, dísticos com anos de estudo e controle para desempenho cognitivo e depressão. Ambos os grupos realizaram uma tarefa clássica de fluência de verbo. Foram realizadas análises sequenciais palavra por palavra.

Resultados:

Diferenças significativas foram encontradas na produção inicial de MAVs de corpo inteiro e na produção global de verbos instrumentais (ambas as medidas foram menores no grupo PD). Uma medida repetida na análise de variância (ANOVA) confirmou o desempenho linear de CG e o desempenho quadrático de DP.

Conclusões:

Os pacientes com DP apresentam produção alterada de MAVs corporais inteiras e instrumentais. Esta proposta para a análise sequencial semântica dos verbos motores merece uma nova investigação, como uma nova metodologia para a avaliação do desempenho da fluência em doenças motoras.

Palavras-chave:
Doença de Parkinson; Envelhecimento; Função Executiva; Idioma

INTRODUCTION

Action fluency (verb production) and action language processing (comprehension, semantic comparison, etc.) are compromised in patients with Parkinson's disease (PD)11 Roberts A, Nguyen P, Orange JB, Jog M, Nisbet KA, McRae K. Differential impairments of upper and lower limb movements influence action verb processing in Parkinson disease. Cortex. 2017;97:49-59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09.022
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09...
. Actions performed by specific parts of the body are particularly affected, which can be mainly explained by dopamine deficiency: Herrera et al.22 Herrera E, Bermúdez-Margaretto B, Ribacoba R, Cuetos F. The motor-semantic meanings of verbs generated by Parkinson's disease patients on/off dopamine medication in a verbal fluency task. Journal of Neurolinguistics. 2015;36:72-8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2015.06.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.201...
studied the on/off medication effect (levodopa) on verbal fluency performance in a group of 34 nondemented PD patients and concluded that patients on medication produced a greater number of verbs with high motor specificity (e.g., sew, knit, and bounce) than those with low motor specificity (e.g., swim, run, and sleep).

Subtypes of motor-action verbs

In the literature, three types of motor-action verbs (MAVs) have been described: whole body (WBAVs), specific body verbs (SBAVs), and instrumental verbs (InstVs), where each type of verb has a different neuropsychological and functional neuroimaging correlate33 Yang J, Shu H, Bi Y, Liu Y, Wang X. Dissociation and association of the embodied representation of tool-use verbs and hand verbs: an fMRI study. Brain Lang. 2011;119(3):167-74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2011.06.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2011.06....
:

Whole-body action verbs: actions performed by or with most of the body, such as running, swimming, and jumping44 Kemmerer D, Castillo JG, Talavage T, Patterson S, Wiley C. Neuroanatomical distribution of five semantic components of verbs: evidence from fMRI. Brain Lang. 2008;107(1):16-43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2007.09.003
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2007.09....

Specific body part action verbs: actions performed by specific body parts, such as kicking, biting, and blinking55 Pulvermüller F. Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6(7):576-82. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1706
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1706...

Instrumental verbs: actions performed using an instrument or an object, such as cutting and sawing66 Pelgrims B, Olivier E, Andres M. Dissociation between manipulation and conceptual knowledge of object use in the supramarginalis gyrus. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32(11):1802-10. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21149
https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21149...

Neuropsychology of verbal fluency paradigms

Verbal fluency tests, where only a specific category of item (e.g., animals, words with a specific letter, or verbs) is required to be produced in a limited time (usually within 1 min), produce a funnel effect due to the cognitive constraint of a specific item or category. Two production phases have been described for fluency paradigms77 Snyder HR, Munakata Y. So many options, so little control: abstract representations can reduce selection demands to increase children's self-directed flexibility. J Exp Child Psychol. 2013;116(3):659-73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.010
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.0...
: selection (the initial abundant production) and retrieval (slower paced production). Selection occurs approximately from 1 to 15 s, when words are highly available and abundantly produced. Retrieval begins approximately from second 20, when words are less available, and a significant retrieval effort is required to produce only a few words.

This study

To date, most studies have not performed a sequential-semantic analysis of participant performance in fluency testing. The global score (the total number of words produced) is the most frequent measurement used in fluency testing88 Salmazo-Silva H, Parente MAMP, Rocha MS, Baradel RR, Cravo AM, Sato JR, et al. Lexical-retrieval and semantic memory in Parkinson's disease: the question of noun and verb dissociation. Brain Lang. 2017;165:10-20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2016.10.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2016.10....
. However, this scoring criterion does not reflect the production sequence, both in general and in each of the two phases (selection and retrieval), where two different neuropsychological processes are involved. What types of verbs are initially produced? What is the overall production sequence for each subtype of verb? To answer these questions, we performed a sequential analysis on fluency testing based on paper and pencil testing. This study aimed to explore differences in performance between the PD group (PDG) and the comparison group (CG) across three criteria:

  • The overall frequency and relative percentage of all MAVs,

  • The frequency and relative percentage of each of the three types of MAVs, and

  • The sequential production of each of the three types of MAVs.

METHODS

Participants

A group of 20 nondemented patients diagnosed with PD (55% males), with a mean age of 66.59 years, and a CG of 20 normal elderly individuals (45% males) matched by age and years of education, with a mean age of 67.74 years, participated in the study. The average number of years of progression after diagnosis was 10.63 (standard deviation=4.71) in PD patients.

The inclusion criteria included a clinical diagnosis of PD. Individuals had to be between 50 and 70 years old and achieve at least 23 score on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), according to normative data adjusted for low education99 Villaseñor-Cabrera T, Guárdia-Olmos J, Jiménez-Maldonado M, Rizo-Curiel G, Peró-Cebollero M. Sensitivity and specificity of the Mini-Mental State Examination in the Mexican population. Quality & Quantity: International Journal of Methodology. 2010;44(6):1105-12. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-009-9263-6
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-009-9263-...
. Patients should have mild or no symptoms of depression (Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale1010 Montgomery SA, Asberg M. A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br J Psychiatry. 1979;134:382-9. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.134.4.382
https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.134.4.382...
, cutoff score=20). All patients were treated with levodopa. Not all Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale data were available for all patients.

The exclusion criteria were diagnosis with an atypical PD syndrome (i.e., Lewy body dementia), symptoms of severe depression, or cognitive impairment.

Patients were diagnosed with PD according to UK Parkinson's Disease Society Brain Bank clinical diagnostic criteria1111 Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, Lees AJ. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1992;55(3):181-4. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.55.3.181
https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.55.3.181...
. All patients were at the beginning stage of a protocol to be considered the candidates for surgery to provide deep brain stimulation. The criteria for this protocol included the presence of typical PD accompanied by untreatable motor fluctuations and dyskinesia, and at least 30% motor improvement in the levodopa test. All participants in this study gave their written consent to participate. All data were de-identified using an alpha-numeric code. This research was approved by the ethical committee of the hospital where it was conducted.

Instruments

Mini-Mental State Examination1212 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189-98. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)900...

Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) is a brief cognitive assessment that evaluates temporal orientation, spatial orientation, memory, attention, calculation, and language.

The Hoehn and Yahr scale1313 Hoehn MM, Yahr MD. Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology. 1967;17(5):427-42. https://doi.org/10.1212/wnl.17.5.427
https://doi.org/10.1212/wnl.17.5.427...

The Hoehn and Yahr (H&Y) Scale is clinical rating scale that defines broad categories of motor function in PD (modified version).

Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale88 Salmazo-Silva H, Parente MAMP, Rocha MS, Baradel RR, Cravo AM, Sato JR, et al. Lexical-retrieval and semantic memory in Parkinson's disease: the question of noun and verb dissociation. Brain Lang. 2017;165:10-20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2016.10.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2016.10....

This scale evaluates the core symptoms of depression.

The Geriatric Depression Scale1414 Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, Lum O, Huang V, Adey M, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res. 1982;17(1):37-49. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4
https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(82)900...

This depression scale was used for the CG.

Verb fluency task. In this task, participants were asked to follow the instructions: “Tell me as many verbs as you can in 1 min, or words that describe what people do.” No specific instructions were given to generate WBAVs, SBAVs, or InstVs. Responses were recorded by the examiner.

Procedure

All patients were registered for regular clinical services at the National Institute of Neurology and Neurosurgery (Mexico), clinically diagnosed with PD, and individually evaluated with the cognitive tests in the “on” medication stage; depending on each patient, one or two sessions were needed to complete the evaluation. All results, including the specific sequence of verbs produced by each participant or patient, were stored in databases.

Fluency analysis

The fluency test results were analyzed by a semantic-sequential approach that focused on the three different types of MAVs: WBAVs, SBAVs, and InstVs. A double-check classification procedure was performed, where two of the authors separately classified the verbs and then compared their classifications. Inter-rater agreement was 98.25. We analyzed the one-by-one sequence of production of each verb, and the motor content of each verb was determined according to San Miguel Abella and González-Nosti1515 San Miguel Abella RA, González-Nosti M. Motor content norms for 4,565 verbs in Spanish. Behav Res Methods. 2020;52(2):447-54. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-019-01241-1
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-019-01241...
.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive and correlational (nonparametric Spearman's correlation) analyses were performed, focusing on the correlation of clinical measures and cognitive scores. A repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to detect differences in the production sequence.

RESULTS

The participant's demographics and overall results are presented in Table 1. Although the PDG produced a slightly lower number of all verbs on average (lower overall fluency), the difference was not significant from the CG (t-test analysis). Significant differences were found in the overall production of MAVs (the sum of all three motor types), and the PDG presented a lower performance. The InstVs production was the most affected in the PDG (lower than the CG); in contrast, the overall production of WBAVs and SBAVs was not significantly different between the groups.

Table 1
Demographic data, mean differences, and ANOVA (repeated measures).

The within-group analysis of the relative percentage of production indicated that the most produced verbs in each group were WBAVs, followed by SBAVs and InstVs. However, the relative percentage of InstVs was different between the groups, with significantly fewer InstVs produced in the PDG. No significant differences were found in the average motor content for each subtype of MAV produced (Table 2). Other types of verbs were produced by participants, but not included in further analysis; these included psychological, emotional, and abstract.

Table 2
The most frequent verbs produced (initial production order); the rest of the verbs (up to teen) are presented by the sequence of appearance in the overall production.

Correlation analyses

Overall fluency correlated with years of education in both groups (CG: r=0.772, p<0.001; PDG: r=0.653, p=0.002), but overall fluency only correlated with the MMSE score in the CG (r=0.429, p=0.046). Overall fluency correlated with all MAVs (CG: r=0.772, p=0.000; PDG: r=0.653, p=0.002) and with SBAVs (CG: r=0.771, p=0.000; PDG: r=0.646, p=0.004) in both groups, but only correlated with WBAVs (r=0.456, p=0.033) in the CG. None of the clinical measures presented statistically significant correlations with fluency test performance. In the PDG, only the years of evolution (after PD diagnosis) correlated with the H&Y scale (r=0.589, p=0.008), and in the CG, only MMSE scores correlated with the depression scores (r=-0.463, p=0.030).

Sequential analysis

Based on the PDG performance (an average production of 12 verbs), we decided to analyze three-verb blocks, focusing on the first four blocks produced (Figure 1). In the CG, the initial production of WBAVs was higher and had the highest fluency value for all four blocks. These effects were not found in the PDG.

Figure 1
Sequential performance. CG: control group; PDG: Parkinson's disease group.

The groups had different patterns of MAVs production. The CG had a high initial MAVs production with a slow linear-type decay, due to the decline in WBAVs production. In contrast, the PDG had a pyramid-like MAVs production: a slow ascending production with a significantly more rapid decay, due to the combined production of SBAVs and WBAVs. The results of the repeated-measures ANOVA on all motor verbs confirm the linear model for CG production and a quadratic model for PDG production (Table 1). The within-subject effects for the CG and the PDG were all significant.

DISCUSSION

This study found significant decrements in the production of all types of MAVs in PD patients, which is consistent with the literature11 Roberts A, Nguyen P, Orange JB, Jog M, Nisbet KA, McRae K. Differential impairments of upper and lower limb movements influence action verb processing in Parkinson disease. Cortex. 2017;97:49-59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09.022
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09...
,22 Herrera E, Bermúdez-Margaretto B, Ribacoba R, Cuetos F. The motor-semantic meanings of verbs generated by Parkinson's disease patients on/off dopamine medication in a verbal fluency task. Journal of Neurolinguistics. 2015;36:72-8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2015.06.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.201...
. The new findings presented here highlight the instrumental category as the least produced in PD patients and at disproportionately lower rates than the CG.

In general, these results suggest that the brain networks that support the production of InstVs may be more compromised in PD. InstVs are not only intransitively complex (agent, object, and recipient of the action) but also require context specification. For example, the verb “to cut” can be used in many different contexts: different cutting tools exist and even the same tools may require different semantic and pragmatic decisions in different real-life contexts66 Pelgrims B, Olivier E, Andres M. Dissociation between manipulation and conceptual knowledge of object use in the supramarginalis gyrus. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32(11):1802-10. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21149
https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21149...
. Aging studies on healthy participants have found a significant decrement in usage (mechanical/pragmatic) and semantic/cognitive instrumental knowledge1616 Lesourd M, Baumard J, Jarry C, Le Gall D, Osiurak F. A cognitive-based model of tool use in normal aging. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2017;24(4):363-86. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2016.1218822
https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2016.12...
.

Among all MAVs, the InstVs category requires the highest within-network coupling and the most complex cognitive processing decisions66 Pelgrims B, Olivier E, Andres M. Dissociation between manipulation and conceptual knowledge of object use in the supramarginalis gyrus. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32(11):1802-10. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21149
https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21149...
,1616 Lesourd M, Baumard J, Jarry C, Le Gall D, Osiurak F. A cognitive-based model of tool use in normal aging. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2017;24(4):363-86. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2016.1218822
https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2016.12...
. Our results suggest that these types of verbs may be the most sensitive to PD motor-related cognitive effects.

The findings of the semantic-sequential analysis indicated a high initial production of WBAVs in the CG but a significantly different and diminished WBAVs production in the PDG. This is due to a different pattern of sequential production between groups (linear vs. quadratic). To the best of our knowledge, these findings have not previously been reported in the literature. The initial high production of WBAVs in the CG may be explained by three main factors: the great majority of these verbs are intransitive (grammatically simple)1717 Earles JL, Kernsten AW. Why are verbs so hard to remember? Effects of semantic context on memory for verbs and nouns. Cogn Sci. 2017;41 Suppl 4:780-807. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12374
https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12374...
, they describe frequent everyday actions, and they present high imageability, which is a priming factor in verb processing1818 Xu X, Kang C, Guo T. Imageability and semantic association in the representation and processing of event verbs. Cog Process. 2016;17(2):175-84. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-015-0747-0
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-015-0747-...
.

No correlations between clinical or cognitive scores and fluency performance on the PDG were found. Moreover, expected clinical correlations were present (i.e., years of evolution and MMSE scores). Covariable effects (repeated-measures ANOVA) indicate that in both groups, only the education years and the depression scores influenced the sequential performance. However, a higher number of participants is needed to perform more precise covariable statistics.

The semantic-sequential analysis of MAVs presents several new findings:

  • The initial production of WBAVs was significantly reduced in the PDG.

  • The trajectory of production was different between the groups (quadratic vs. linear).

  • Although the lexical span and the motor content were similar in both groups, MAV fluency was altered (diminished) in the PDG, mainly in the dimension of instrumental actions.

Our proposal for the semantic-sequential analysis of motor verbs contributes to more specific data already reported in the literature and deserves further investigation, which may probe the possible advantages as a standard evaluation of fluency performance in motor-related disease.

The main limitation of our study was the small sample size that makes the findings difficult to generalize. Further research is necessary to achieve a wider normative characterization of the aging process on MAV fluency.

  • The study was conducted by National Institute of Neurology MVS, Mexico City, Mexico.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors thank the Neurology Department at the National Institute of Neurology and Neurosurgery MVS for the neurological evaluation of patients.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Roberts A, Nguyen P, Orange JB, Jog M, Nisbet KA, McRae K. Differential impairments of upper and lower limb movements influence action verb processing in Parkinson disease. Cortex. 2017;97:49-59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09.022
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09.022
  • 2
    Herrera E, Bermúdez-Margaretto B, Ribacoba R, Cuetos F. The motor-semantic meanings of verbs generated by Parkinson's disease patients on/off dopamine medication in a verbal fluency task. Journal of Neurolinguistics. 2015;36:72-8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2015.06.001
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2015.06.001
  • 3
    Yang J, Shu H, Bi Y, Liu Y, Wang X. Dissociation and association of the embodied representation of tool-use verbs and hand verbs: an fMRI study. Brain Lang. 2011;119(3):167-74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2011.06.001
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2011.06.001
  • 4
    Kemmerer D, Castillo JG, Talavage T, Patterson S, Wiley C. Neuroanatomical distribution of five semantic components of verbs: evidence from fMRI. Brain Lang. 2008;107(1):16-43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2007.09.003
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2007.09.003
  • 5
    Pulvermüller F. Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6(7):576-82. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1706
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1706
  • 6
    Pelgrims B, Olivier E, Andres M. Dissociation between manipulation and conceptual knowledge of object use in the supramarginalis gyrus. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32(11):1802-10. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21149
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21149
  • 7
    Snyder HR, Munakata Y. So many options, so little control: abstract representations can reduce selection demands to increase children's self-directed flexibility. J Exp Child Psychol. 2013;116(3):659-73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.010
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.010
  • 8
    Salmazo-Silva H, Parente MAMP, Rocha MS, Baradel RR, Cravo AM, Sato JR, et al. Lexical-retrieval and semantic memory in Parkinson's disease: the question of noun and verb dissociation. Brain Lang. 2017;165:10-20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2016.10.006
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2016.10.006
  • 9
    Villaseñor-Cabrera T, Guárdia-Olmos J, Jiménez-Maldonado M, Rizo-Curiel G, Peró-Cebollero M. Sensitivity and specificity of the Mini-Mental State Examination in the Mexican population. Quality & Quantity: International Journal of Methodology. 2010;44(6):1105-12. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-009-9263-6
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-009-9263-6
  • 10
    Montgomery SA, Asberg M. A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br J Psychiatry. 1979;134:382-9. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.134.4.382
    » https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.134.4.382
  • 11
    Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, Lees AJ. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1992;55(3):181-4. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.55.3.181
    » https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.55.3.181
  • 12
    Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189-98. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
  • 13
    Hoehn MM, Yahr MD. Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology. 1967;17(5):427-42. https://doi.org/10.1212/wnl.17.5.427
    » https://doi.org/10.1212/wnl.17.5.427
  • 14
    Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, Lum O, Huang V, Adey M, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res. 1982;17(1):37-49. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4
  • 15
    San Miguel Abella RA, González-Nosti M. Motor content norms for 4,565 verbs in Spanish. Behav Res Methods. 2020;52(2):447-54. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-019-01241-1
    » https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-019-01241-1
  • 16
    Lesourd M, Baumard J, Jarry C, Le Gall D, Osiurak F. A cognitive-based model of tool use in normal aging. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2017;24(4):363-86. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2016.1218822
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2016.1218822
  • 17
    Earles JL, Kernsten AW. Why are verbs so hard to remember? Effects of semantic context on memory for verbs and nouns. Cogn Sci. 2017;41 Suppl 4:780-807. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12374
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12374
  • 18
    Xu X, Kang C, Guo T. Imageability and semantic association in the representation and processing of event verbs. Cog Process. 2016;17(2):175-84. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-015-0747-0
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-015-0747-0

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    06 Feb 2023
  • Date of issue
    2023

History

  • Received
    07 Apr 2022
  • Reviewed
    19 Sept 2022
  • Accepted
    08 Oct 2022
Academia Brasileira de Neurologia, Departamento de Neurologia Cognitiva e Envelhecimento R. Vergueiro, 1353 sl.1404 - Ed. Top Towers Offices, Torre Norte, São Paulo, SP, Brazil, CEP 04101-000, Tel.: +55 11 5084-9463 | +55 11 5083-3876 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistadementia@abneuro.org.br | demneuropsy@uol.com.br