Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Open field behavior and intra-nucleus accumbens dopamine release in vivo in virgin and lactating rats

Abstract

In adult female mammals, reproductive experience (e.g., mating, pregnancy, parturition, and lactation) has long-term behavioral, endocrine, and neurochemical implications. This experience causes behavioral and neurochemical changes that involve several brain areas important for the expression of maternal behavior. The present study showed that lactating rats exhibited reduced general locomotor activity in the open field test compared with virgin animals. Our hypothesis was that nucleus accumbens dopamine, which regulates maternal behavior in lactating rats, is also involved in the low expression of maternal locomotion in the open field test observed during the early stages of lactation and reflects decreased motivation. Initially we compared open field behavior in virgin and lactating rats to confirm our previous data. Thus, the in vivo release of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens in virgin and lactating female rats was measured. Perfusate concentrations of extracellular dopamine and its metabolites showed no differences between virgin and lactating rats. Thus, the reduced general activity observed in lactating rats might not be related to intra-nucleus accumbens dopamine control.

maternal behavior; virgin rats; lactating rats; nucleus accumbens; dopamine


PLASTICITY AND NEURAL DEVELOPMENT

Open field behavior and intra-nucleus accumbens dopamine release in vivo in virgin and lactating rats

Soraya Ferreira HabrI, II; Maria Martha BernardiI; Isaltino Marcelo ConceiçãoIII; Thalma Ariani FreitasIII; Luciano Freitas FelicioI

IUniversidade Paulista, São Paulo, SP, Brazil

IIUniversidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, SP, Brazil

IIIInstituto Butantan, São Paulo, SP, Brazil

Correspondence regarding this article should be directed to Correspondence regarding this article should be directed to: Soraya Ferreira Habr e-mail address: soraya.vet@gmail.com

ABSTRACT

In adult female mammals, reproductive experience (e.g., mating, pregnancy, parturition, and lactation) has long-term behavioral, endocrine, and neurochemical implications. This experience causes behavioral and neurochemical changes that involve several brain areas important for the expression of maternal behavior. The present study showed that lactating rats exhibited reduced general locomotor activity in the open field test compared with virgin animals. Our hypothesis was that nucleus accumbens dopamine, which regulates maternal behavior in lactating rats, is also involved in the low expression of maternal locomotion in the open field test observed during the early stages of lactation and reflects decreased motivation. Initially we compared open field behavior in virgin and lactating rats to confirm our previous data. Thus, the in vivo release of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens in virgin and lactating female rats was measured. Perfusate concentrations of extracellular dopamine and its metabolites showed no differences between virgin and lactating rats. Thus, the reduced general activity observed in lactating rats might not be related to intra-nucleus accumbens dopamine control.

Keywords: maternal behavior, virgin rats, lactating rats, nucleus accumbens, dopamine.

Introduction

The maternal brain undergoes remarkable physiological and behavioral changes in the peripartum period to meet the demands of the offspring (Numan & Woodside, 2010; Bosch, 2011). Changes in the physiological state of mammals are usually related to endocrine and neural changes (Numan & Stolzenberg, 2008; Stolzenberg et al., 2007; Nasello, Tieppo, & Felicio, 1995; Felicio, Florio, Sider, Cruz-Casallas, & Bridges, 1996; Bridges, Felicio, Pellerin, Stuer, & Mann, 1993) and reflect behavioral adjustments or alterations (Numan, 1994; Numan & Woodside, 2010; Teodorov, Bernardi, Ferrari, Fior-Chadi, & Felicio, 2010). These changes ultimately reflect the biologically useful appearance of some behaviors and modulation of other behaviors, such as caring for pups and aggression (Maestripieri & D'Amato, 1991; Numan, 1994; Numan & Woodside, 2010).

The open field test was initially proposed to measure emotionality (Hall, 1934), but it is also useful for measuring behavioral responses, such as locomotor activity (Patti et al., 2005), hyperactivity (Fukushiro et al., 2008), and exploratory behavior (Crawley, 1985; Alvarez et al., 2006). The open field is also used to measure anxiety (Prut & Belzung, 2003). Rats and mice tend to avoid brightly illuminated, novel, and open spaces, so the open field environment is an anxiogenic stimulus that allows for the measurement of anxiety-induced locomotor activity and exploratory behavior. The anxiogenic response occurs in the first trial, with little or no impact on the animal's subsequent behavior. When the laboratory conditions are maintained with low illumination and without environmental stimuli, rats or mice express mainly exploratory and motor activity rather than anxiety-like responses.

Much data from the literature suggest that lactating rats exhibit less fear in response to several stimuli (Hard & Hansen, 1985; Pereira, Uriarte, Agrati, Zuluaga, & Ferreira, 2005), including in the open field task (Ferreira, Hansen, Nielsen, Archer & Minor, 1989), compared with other stages of the female reproductive cycle. This reduction in general fearfulness observed postpartum appears to be related to the caring of offspring. However, environmental factors may modify open field behavior in lactating dams. Lactating dams of the WAG/Rij (i.e., a genetic model of absence epilepsy) and Wistar rat strains repeatedly placed in the open field arena with their pups showed significantly poorer maternal behavior and were slower in forming the pup location response. Administration of low-dose haloperidol (0.1 mg/kg, i.p.) reduced locomotor activity in dams of both strains and maternal behavior under conditions of bright illumination. Under conditions of red-light illumination, haloperidol increased the number of approaches to the pups and the number of pup transportations in WAG/Rij, but not Wistar, rats and reduced the latencies of these behaviors. Dopaminergic regulation of maternal behavior has been suggested to depend on both genetic and environmental factors (Dobriakova, Dubynin, & van Luijtelaar, 2010). Additionally, we previously found that the physiological state (e.g., lactation) influenced behavior in the open field and elevated plus maze in adult female rats (Silva, Bernardi, Nasello, & Felicio, 1997). Lactating rats exhibited decreased total locomotion in the open field and a decreased percentage of time spent in the open arms of the elevated plus maze, suggesting increased anxiety/emotionality in these behavioral tests.

Dopamine affects motivation via the mesolimbic system, acting mainly in the nucleus accumbens (NAc; Carlezon & Thomas, 2009; Anstrom, Miczek, & Budygin, 2009). Maternal motivation is also regulated by the NAc (Champagne et al., 2004; Afonso, Grella, Chatterjee, & Fleming, 2008; Numan & Wodside, 2010). In rodents, ongoing maternal behavior requires dopamine receptor activation. We previously showed that dopamine receptors in the NAc play a role in ongoing maternal behavior (Silva, Bernardi, Cruz-Casallas, & Felicio, 2003).

We hypothesized that NAc dopamine, which regulates maternal behavior in lactating rats, is also involved in the decreased expression of maternal locomotion in the open field observed in the early stages of lactation, reflected by decreased motivation. We initially compared open field behavior in virgin and lactating rats to confirm our previous data. Thus, in vivo dopamine release in the NAc in virgin and lactating female rats was measured. A microdialysis experiment was performed in another group of rats because this procedure requires a specialized system to collect the dialysate.

Methods

Subjects

Fifteen virgin rats and 14 lactating rats (Department of Pathology, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of São Paulo, Brazil) were divided into four groups: eight virgin and seven lactating rats were used in the behavioral studies, and seven virgin and five lactating rats were used for the microdialysis studies. For lactating rats, 13 pregnant Wistar rats from our own colony, weighing 200-250g each, were used (gestational day [GD] 0 = spermatozoa in the vaginal smear). The dams were individually housed in polypropylene cages (38 x 32 x 16 cm) with controlled room temperature (22°C), humidity (65-70%), and artificial lighting (12 h/12 h light/dark cycle; lights on at 6:00 AM) with free access to Nuvilab rodent chow (Nuvital, São Paulo, Brazil) and filtered water. Sterile and residue-free wood shavings were used as the bedding in the cages. After birth (day 2), the pups were culled to eight pups per litter, and no cross-fostering procedure was used. The animals used in this study were maintained in accordance with the guidelines of the Committee on Care and Use of Laboratory Animal Resources of the School of Veterinary Medicine, University of São Paulo, Brazil (protocol 109, sheet 6, Book 2). Virgin rats (n = 15, two females per cage) were maintained in the same laboratory conditions as lactating rats until the beginning of the experiments.

Drugs

Anesthetics (2% xylazine hydrochloride and 5% ketamine hydrochloride; Konig) were administered intraperitoneally according to the body weights of the rats (1:1). Benzathine benzylpenicillin (Bayer) was administered subcutaneously at a dose of 75 U per animal in a single dose in the dorsal region immediately after stereotaxic surgery.

Stereotaxic surgery

Stereotaxic surgery was performed on GD14 to implant guide cannulae aimed directly at the left NAc (Figure 1). We used the following coordinates relative to bregma (Paxinos & Watson, 1986): dorsal/ventral, 6.4 mm; rostral/caudal, + 1.2 mm; lateral, 1.4 mm.


Open field test

The apparatus was similar to Broadhurst (1960) and consisted of a round 96 cm diameter arena surrounded by a 25 cm high wall painted white, with the floor divided into 19 painted black parts. Hand-operated counters were used to score locomotor frequency (i.e., number of squares crossed with all four paws) and rearing frequency (i.e., number of times a rat stood erect on its hindlegs with its forelegs in the air). A chronometer was used to measure the duration of immobility (i.e., total time in seconds without spontaneous movements). Two groups of female rats (i.e., virgin group and lactating group) were individually placed in the center of the arena, and the behavioral parameters were observed for 5 min on day 5 of lactation. One hour before the experiment, the pups of the lactating rats were maintained separately in another room. The virgin and lactating rats were intermixed and observed. These observations were made between 2:00 PM and 5:00 PM.

Microdialysis

A continuous infusion pump (Harvard Apparatus, model 2274) was equipped with a syringe (Hamilton Model 1002 LT, 2.5 ml) connected to the microdialysis membrane by polyethylene tubes. The female rats were previously habituated to the microdialysis system manipulation. On the day of the experiment, 1 h before the experiments, the dams were separated from their pups and placed in the microdialysis system in another room. After 1 h of acclimation to the system, dialysate was collected at 0, 20, and 40 min. The perfusion fluid entered an inlet tube, exited an outlet tube, and then was collected in Eppendorf tubes that contained 5 μl of 1 M perchloric acid (ClHO4) placed at the end of the outlet tube. The samples were frozen at -80ºC for at most 1 month until they were injected into a high-performance liquid chromatography (HPLC) system with electrochemical detection to quantify monoamine levels.

Confirmation of cannula placements

The rats were deeply anesthetized. Using a peristaltic pump (Cole Parme), the rats were perfused intracardially through the left ventricle with 150 ml of 0.9% saline, followed by 500 ml of 10% paraformaldehyde fixative solution diluted in milli-Q water. The brains were then removed and transferred to a saturated solution of sucrose in 10% paraformaldehyde, where they remained for approximately 24 h. Serial frontal sections, 30 μm thick, were then obtained using a freezing microtome (Leica). The sections were collected sequentially in six compartments so that the distance between the cuts in the same compartment was 180 mm. The sections were stained using the Nissl method and viewed under an optical microscope to determine the placement of the guide cannula in the region of interest. Only data from animals with correct cannula placements were considered for the statistical analysis.

Determination of neurotransmitter and metabolite levels

The samples were analyzed by HPLC (Shimadsu) with an injector, column (Chromolith RP-18e, Merck), and electrochemical detector (1049 A, Hewlett Packard). The levels of dopamine, 3,4-dihidroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC), and homovanillic acid (HVA) were measured to calculate absolute values (pmol) and DOPAC/dopamine, HVA/dopamine and (DOPAC + HVA)/dopamine ratios.

Statistical analysis

The general locomotor activity results were analyzed using an unpaired t-test, and the neurochemical data were analyzed using two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by the Bonferroni post hoc test. In all cases, the results were considered significant at p < .05.

Results

Experiment 1: general activity in virgin and lactating rats in the open field

Table 1 shows the general activity of virgin and lactating rats in the open field. Compared with virgin rats, the unpaired t-test revealed decreased locomotor frequency (F = 5.495; p = .006) and rearing frequency (F = 1.129; p = .0002). No significant differences were detected between groups in the duration of immobility (F = 33.648; p = .388).

Experiment 2: in vivo dopamine release in the NAc in virgin and lactating rats

Figure 2 shows in vivo dopamine and dopamine metabolite concentrations obtained by microdialysis in virgin and lactating rats. The two-way ANOVA of the concentrations of dopamine (lactating vs. virgin: F1,30 = 0.67; p = .41; sessions: F2,30 = 1.18; p = .32; interaction: F2,30 = 1.17; p = .32), HVA (lactating vs. virgin: F1,30 = .96; p = 0.33; sessions: F2,30 = 0; p = 1.0; interaction: F2,30 = .49, p = .62), and DOPAC (lactating vs. virgin: F1,30 = .92, p = .34; sessions: F2,30 = .65; p = 0.95; interaction: F2,30 = 1.24; p = .30) did not reveal differences between virgin and lactating rats. The DOPAC/dopamine (lactating vs. virgin: F1,30 = 0.41; p = .52; sessions: F2,30 = 1.80; p = .18; interaction: F2,30 = 1.26; p = .77), HVA/dopamine (lactating vs. virgin: F1,30 = 0.25; p = .52; sessions: F2,30 = 0.94; p = .40; interaction: F2,30 = 0.66; p = .52), and HVA+DOPAC/dopamine (lactating vs. virgin: F1,30 = 0.10, p = .75; sessions: F2,30 = 1.14; p = .75; interaction: F2,30 = 0.50; p = .61) ratios were also not significantly different between groups.


Discussion

The present study confirmed previous results by showing that lactating rats exhibit decreased locomotor frequency in the open field compared with virgin animals (Silva et al., 1997). Rearing frequency in lactating rats also decreased compared with virgin females. No significant difference was detected in the duration of immobility, suggesting a lack of effect on motor function.

When confronted with a novel environment, the behavior of rats in the open field is determined by two conflicting behaviors: the drive to explore and motivation to avoid potential danger (Bertoglio & Carobrez, 2000). Rats with high levels of emotionality exhibit decreased locomotion and rearing. Thus, the decreased locomotor and rearing behavior observed in the present study may be attributable to increased emotionality in lactating rats compared with virgin females. However, Silva et al. (1997) studied the influence of this physiological state (i.e. lactation) in the elevated plus maze and open field in adult female rats and showed conflicting data. In the open field, total locomotion significantly decreased, and central locomotion (i.e., a parameter related to anxiety) did not differ from virgin rats. In the elevated plus maze, lactating rats displayed a significant reduction in the percentage of time spent on the open arms and a trend toward a reduction in the percentage of entries into the open arms, suggesting an increased anxiogenic-like state. The discrepancies between the two studies may be attributable to the different methods used to assess anxiety levels.

In the present study, both locomotor frequency and rearing frequency decreased in lactating rats compared with virgin rats. Rearing is one of several components of a rat's behavior that, together with other components, comprise what is commonly referred to as general activity (Walsh & Cummins, 1976). Rearing is also a part of stereotyped behavior (Setler, Sarau, Zirkle, & Saunders, 1978) induced in rats by activation of postsynaptic dopaminergic receptors (Randrup & Munkvad, 1974). Decreases in open field locomotion and rearing frequency without changes in the duration of immobility could be interpreted as reduced exploratory behavior or increased anxiety. Exposure to the open field in conditions of intensive light and a noisy environment can have aversive connotations. In contrast, when an animal is exposed to this apparatus under less extreme conditions, such as low illumination and an absence of noise (i.e., conditions used in the present study), the duration of locomotion may reflect and be used to quantify other types of behavior, such as exploratory and motor behavior. Clearly, the emotional component does not completely disappear in such conditions but becomes less relevant. In the present study, general activity in lactating and virgin rats was observed in conditions of low illumination and the absence of noise. Thus, the lactating female rats may have had a decreased motivational response to novelty compared with virgin rats.

During the lactation period, the typical behaviors directed toward pups increase, whereas less relevant behaviors, such as environmental exploration, decrease. Such adaptations in the maternal brain are necessary to optimize the care toward the offspring. Motherhood in rodents induces reduced reactivity to normally aversive stimuli (Fleming & Luebke, 1981; Ferreira, Hansens, Nielsen, Archer, & Minor, 1989). Lactating rats are less inclined to run away from an intruder (Fleming & Luebke, 1981) and exhibit lower reactivity in classic conflict tests (Ferreira et al., 1989). In a light/dark choice situation, lactating mice displayed less avoidance of the aversive illuminated environment than virgin animals (Maestripieri & D'Amato, 1991). Changes in anxiety-like behavior during lactation might reflect other adaptations of the maternal brain. Therefore, a link between maternal behavior and reduced maternal anxiety has been repeatedly postulated, suggesting the existence of changes in the motivational state for some behaviors (Silva et al., 1997; Lonstein, 2005, 2007; Numann & Wodside, 2010).

Neumann (2003) reported that during pregnancy and lactation several neuroendocrine alterations occur that are accompanied by marked behavioral changes, including emotional responsiveness to external challenging situations. A downregulation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis responses to physical or emotional stimuli, particularly anxiety-related behavior, was reported in pregnant and lactating rats (Toufexis, Tesolin, Huang, & Walker, 1999; Johnstone et al., 2000). Reduced anxiety levels could be observed only after separating the lactating rats from their pups immediately before testing in the elevated plus maze after transportation to a neighboring test room, but this was not observed when the rats were kept in the testing room for at least 24 h and separated from their pups 2 h before testing (Picazo & Fernandez-Guasti, 1993; Neumann, 2003).

The NAc is a region rich in dopaminergic inputs. Several authors stated that this limbic site modulates emotional behaviors, including aggression and defensive tasks (Garattini, Giacalone, & Valzelli, 1967; Louilot, Le Moal, & Simon, 1986; Oliver et al., 1987; Almeida & Lucion, 1997; Boer, Lesourd, Mocaer, & Koolhaas, 1999; Volavka, 1999). Dopamine affects motivation via the mesolimbic system, mainly in the NAc (Willner & Scheel-Kruger, 1991; Robbins & Everitt, 1996), and reproductive experience decreases prolactin levels in lactating rats. We investigated the possible relationship between the reduction in open field behaviors and NAc dopamine concentration using in vivo methods. Earlier reports showed that PRL stimulates the in vitro release of dopamine from the rat superfused corpus striatum (Chen & Ramirez, 1982) and increases the in vivo output of DOPAC from the rat caudate nucleus when directly infused into the brain (Ramirez, 1985). However, in the present study, the release of dopamine and its metabolites in the NAc was not different between virgin and lactating female rats.

Previous work showed that lesions of the NAc affected the behavior of retrieving pups to the nest (Hansen, Harthon, Wallin, Lofberg, & Swensson, 1991). Administration of the dopamine receptor antagonist haloperidol and dopamine D2 receptor antagonist pimozide affected search behavior, including grouping offspring and rebuilding the nest. The effects of pimozide on maternal behavior were less pronounced compared with the effects of haloperidol (Silva, Bernardi, & Felicio, 2001; Silva et al., 2003). Our hypothesis is that NAc dopamine, which regulates maternal behavior in lactating rats, is involved in the low maternal locomotion observed in the open field during the early stages of lactation, possibly reflecting decreased motivation. The present study found reduced locomotor and rearing frequency in the open field and no changes in NAc dopamine levels in lactating rats compared with virgin rats, suggesting that the motivational changes observed in lactating rats might occur independently of dopamine release in NAc.

Acknowledgements

This research report is part of doctoral thesis of Soraya Ferreira Habr to the Biomedical Sciences Institute of the University of São Paulo, Brazil. The research was supported by a fellowship from the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior - CAPES.

Received 4 February 2011; received in revised form 27 April 2011; accepted 15 May 2011. Available on line 15 June 2011

Soraya Ferreira Habr, Science Institute of Health, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Paulista University, Brazil and Department of Pharmacology, Biomedical Science Institute, University of São Paulo, Brazil. Maria Martha Bernardi, Science Institute of Health, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Paulista University, Brazil. Isaltino Marcelo Conceição and Thalma Ariani Freitas, Laboratory of Pharmacology, Instituto Butantan, Brazil. Luciano Freitas Felicio, Department of Pathology, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of São Paulo, Brazil.

  • Afonso, V. M., Grella, S. L., Chatterjee, D., & Fleming, A. S. (2008). Previous maternal experience affects accumbal dopaminergic responses to pup-stimuli. Brain Research, 1198, 115-123.
  • Almeida, D.R.M.M., & Lucion, A.B. (1997). 8-OH-DPAT in the median raphe, dorsal periaqueductal gray and corticomedial amygdala nucleus decreases, but the medial septal area it can increase maternal aggressive behavior in rats. Psychopharmacology, 134, 392-400.
  • Alvarez, J.do, Fukushiro, D.F., Tatsu, J.A., de Carvalho, E.P., Gandolfi, A.C., Tsuchiya, J.B.,... & Frussa-Filho R. (2006). Amphetamine-induced rapid-onset sensitization: role of novelty, conditioning and behavioral parameters. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior,83,500-507.
  • Anstrom, K. K., Miczek, K. A., & Budygin, E. A. (2009). Increased phasic dopamine signaling in the mesolimbic pathway during social defeat in rats. Neuroscience, 161, 3-12.
  • Bertoglio, L.J., & Carobrez, A.P. (2000). Previous maze experience required to increase open arms avoidance in rats submitted to the elevated plus-maze model of anxiety. Behavior Brain Research, 108, 197-203.
  • Boer, D.S.F., Lesourd, M., Mocaer, E., & Koolhaas, J.M. (1999). Selective antiaggressive effects of alnespirone in resident-intruder test are mediated via 5-hydroxytryptamine1A receptors: a comparative pharmacological study with 8-hydroxy-2-dipropylaminotetralin, ipsapirone, buspirone, eltoprazine, and WAY-100635. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 288, 1125-1133.
  • Bosch, O.J. (2011). Maternal nurturing is dependent on her innate anxiety: The behavioral roles of brain oxytocin and vasopressin. Hormones and Behavior, 59,202-212.
  • Bridges, R.S, Felicio, L.F, Pellerin L.J., Stuer A.M., & Mann P.E. (1993). Prior parity reduces post-coital diurnal and nocturnal prolactin surges in rats. Life Sciences, 53, 439-445.
  • Broadhurst, P.L. (1960). The place of animal psychology in the development of psychosomatic research. Fortschritte Psychosomatic Medicine, 1, 63-69.
  • Carlezon, W. A., Jr., & Thomas, M. J. (2009). Biological substrates of reward and aversion: A ncleus accumbens activity hypothesis. Neuropharmacology, 56(Suppl 1), 122-132.
  • Champagne, F. A., Chretien, P., Stevenson, C. W., Zhang, T. Y., Gratton, A., & Meaney, M. J. (2004). Variations in nucleus accumbens dopamine associated with individual differences in maternal behavior in the rat. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 4113-4123.
  • Chen, Y.F., & Ramirez, V.D. (1982). Prolactin stimulates dopamine release from male but not from female rat striatal tissue superfused in vitro. Endocrinology,111, 1740-1742.
  • Crawley, JN. (1985). Exploratory behavior models of anxiety in mice. Neuroscience and Biobehavior, 9, 37-44.
  • Dobriakova, Iu.V, Dubynin, V.A., & van Luijtelaar, G. (2010). [Maternal behavior of WAG/Rij strain rats in normal conditions and during intervention in the activity of dopaminergic brain system]. Zh Vyssh Nerv Deiat Im I P Pavlova, 60,372-380.
  • Fukushiro, D.F., Carvalho, R.de C., Ricardo, V.P., Alvarez, J.do N., Ribeiro, L.T., & Frussa-Filho, R. (2008). Haloperidol (but not ziprasidone) withdrawal potentiates sensitization to the hyperlocomotor effect of cocaine in mice. Brain Research Bulletin, 77,124-128.
  • Felicio, L.F., Florio, J.C., Sider, L.H., Cruz-Casallas P.E., & Bridges, R.S. (1996). Reproductive experience increases striatal and hypothalamic dopamine levels in pregnant rats. Brain Research Bulletin, 40, 253-256.
  • Ferreira, A., Hansen, S., Nielsen, M., Archer, T., & Minor, B.G. (1989). Behavior of mother rats in conflict tests sensitive to antianxiety agents. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 193-201.
  • Fleming, A.S., & Luebke, C. (1981). Timidity prevents the virgin female from being a good mother: Emotionality differences between nulliparous and parturient females. Physiology and Behavior, 27, 863-868.
  • Garattini, S., Giacalone, E., & Valzelli, L. (1967) Isolation, aggressiveness and brain 5-hydroxytryptamine turnover. Journal of Pharmacy and Pharmacology, 19, 338-339.
  • Hall, C.S. (1934). Emotional behvior in the rat. 1. defecation and urination as measures of individual differences in emotionality. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 18, 382-403.
  • HÅrd, E., & Hansen, S. (1985). Reduced fearfulness in the lactating rat. Physiology and Behavior, 35: 641-643.
  • Hansen, S., Harthon, C., Wallin, E., Lofberg, L., & Swensson, K. (1991). The effects of 6-OHDA-induced dopamine depletions in ventral ou dorsal striatum on maternal and sexual behavior in the female rat. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 39, 71-77.
  • Johnstone, H.A., Wigger, A., Douglas, A.J., Neumann, I.D., Landgraf, R.,Seckl, J.R., & Russell, J.A. (2000). Attenuation of hypothalamic-pituitaryadrenal axis stress responses in late pregnancy: Changes in feedforward and feedback mechanisms. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 12, 811-822.
  • Lonstein, J. S. (2005). Reduced anxiety in postpartum rats requires recent physical interactions with pups, but is independent of suckling and peripheral sources of hormones. Hormones and Behavior, 47, 241-255.
  • Lonstein, J. S. (2007). Regulation of anxiety during the postpartum period. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 28, 115-141.
  • Louilot, A., Le Moal, M., & Simon, H. (1986). Differential reactivity of dopaminergic neurons in the Nucleus accumbens in response to different behavioral situations: an in vivo voltammetric study in free moving rats. Brain Research, 397, 395-400.
  • Maestripieri, D., & D.Amato, F.R. (1991). Anxiety and maternal aggression in house mice (Mus musculus): a look at interindividual variability. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 105, 295-301.
  • Nasello, A.G., Tieppo, C.A., & Felicio, L.F. (1995). Apomorphine-induced yawning in rat: influence of fasting and day time. Physiology and Behavior, 57, 967-971.
  • Neumann, I.D. (2003). Brain mechanisms underlying emotional alterations in the peripartum period in rats. Depression and Anxiety, 17,111-121.
  • Numan, M. (1994). Maternal behavior. In E. Knobil & J.D. Neill (Eds.), The Physiology of Reproduction (pp. 221-302). New York: Raven Press.
  • Numan, M., & Stolzenberg, D.S.(2009). Medial preoptic area interactions with dopamine neural systems in the control of the onset and maintenance of maternal behavior in rats. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology,30,46-64.
  • Numan ,M., & Woodside, B. (2010). Maternity: neural mechanisms, motivational processes, and physiological adaptations. Behavioral Neuroscience 124,715-41
  • Oliver, B., Mos, J., Van Der Heyden, J., Schipper, J., Tulp, M., Berkelmans, B., & Bevan, P. (1987). Serotonergic modulation of agonistic behaviour. In B. Olivier (Ed.), Ethopharmacology of Agonistic Behaviour in Animals and Humans (pp. 162-186). Dordrecht: Martinus-Nijhoff.
  • Patti, C.L., Frussa-Filho, R., Silva,R.H., Carvalho, R.C., Kameda, S.R., Takatsu-Coleman, A.L.,... Abílio, V.C.(2005). Behavioral characterization of morphine effects on motor activity in mice. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior,81,923-927.
  • Paxinos, G., & Watson, C. (1986). The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. San Diego: Academic Press Inc.
  • Pereira, M., Uriarte, N., Agrati, D., Zuluaga, M.J., & Ferreira, A. (2005).Motivational aspects of maternal anxiolysis in lactating rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl),180,241-8.
  • Prut, L., & Belzung, C.(2003).The open field as a paradigm to measure the effects of drugs on anxiety-like behaviors: a review. European Journal of Pharmacology, 463, 3- 33.
  • Picazo, O., & Fernández-Guasti, A. (1993).Changes in experimental anxiety during pregnancy and lactation. Physiology and Behavior,54, 295-299.
  • Ramirez, V.D. (1985). The push-pull perfusion technique in neuroendocrinology. In A. Bayon & R. Drucker-Colin (Eds), In vivo perfusion and release of neuroactive substances (pp. 249-270, Orlando: Academic Press.
  • Randrup, A., & Munkvad, I. (1974). Pharmacology and physiology of stereotyped behavior. Journal of Psychiatric Research ,11, 1-10.
  • Robbins, T.W., & Everitt, B. J. (1996). Neurobehavioural mechanisms of reward and motivation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 6, 228-236.
  • Setler, P.E., Sarau, H.M., Zirkle, C.L., & Saunders, H.L.(1978). The central effects of a novel dopamine agonist. European Journal of Pharmacology, 50, 419-430.
  • Silva, M.R.P., Bernardi, M.M., Nasello, A.G., & Felicio, L.F. (1997). Influence of lactation on motor activity and elevated plus maze behavior. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 30, 241-244.
  • Silva, M.R.P., Bernardi, M. M., & Felicio, L.F. (2001). Effects of dopamine receptor antagonists on ongoing maternal behavior in rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 68, 461-468.
  • Silva, M.R.P., Bernardi, M. M., Cruz- Casallas, P.E., & Felício, L.F. (2003). Pimozide injections into the Nucleus accumbens disrupt maternal behavior in lactating rats. Pharmacology and Toxicology, 93, 42-47.
  • Stolzenberg, D.S., McKenna, J.B., Keough, S., Hancock, R., Numan, M.J., & Numan, M.(2007). Dopamine D1 receptor stimulation of the nucleus accumbens or the medial preoptic area promotes the onset of maternal behavior in pregnancy-terminated rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 121, 907-919.
  • Teodorov, E., Bernardi, M.M., Ferrari, M.F., Fior-Chadi, D.R., & Felicio, L.F. (2011). Plasticity of opioid receptors in the female periaqueductal gray: multiparity-induced increase in the activity of genes encoding for mu and kappa receptors and a post-translational decrease in delta receptor expression. Journal of Molecular Neuroscience 43,175-181.
  • Toufexis, D.J., Tesolin, S., Huang, N., & Walker, C.D. (1999). Altered pituitary sensitivity to corticotropin-releasing factor and arginine vasopressin participates in the stress hyporesponsiveness of lactation in the rat. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 11, 757-764.
  • Volavka, J. (1999). The effects of clozapine on aggression and substance abuse in schizophrenic patients. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 60 (Suppl. 12), 43-46.
  • Walsh,R.N., & Cummins, R.A.(1976). The Open-Field Test: a critical review. Psychology Bulletin.83, 482-504.
  • Willner, P., & Scheel-Kruger, J. (1991). The mesolimbic dopamine system: From motivation to action. New York: John Wiley; SONS.
  • Correspondence regarding this article should be directed to:
    Soraya Ferreira Habr
    e-mail address:
  • Publication Dates

    • Publication in this collection
      23 Aug 2011
    • Date of issue
      June 2011

    History

    • Reviewed
      27 Apr 2011
    • Received
      04 Feb 2011
    • Accepted
      15 May 2011
    Pontificia Universidade Católica do Rio de Janeiro, Universidade de Brasília, Universidade de São Paulo Rua Marques de São Vicente, 225, 22453-900 Rio de Janeiro/RJ Brasil, Tel.: (55 21) 3527-2109, Fax: (55 21) 3527-1187 - Rio de Janeiro - RJ - Brazil
    E-mail: psycneuro@psycneuro.org