Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Influence of color and brightness on ontogenetic shelter preference by the prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii (Decapoda: Palaemonidae)

ABSTRACT

The giant river prawn, Macrobrachium rosenbergii (de Man, 1879), is a species of great commercial importance. It is farmed under different conditions that translate to a great range of light environments, which impact their behavior and productivity. We present the first study employing both visual modeling and beha vioral data to evaluate the ontogenetic changes in color preferences of juveniles and adults of M. rosenbergii. We offered ten shelters of different colors to juveniles and adults and registered their preferences. Our results show that shelter preference changed with ontogeny: juveniles chose shelters based on chromaticity (preference for blue), while adults based their decisions on brightness (preference for dark grey). This preference adults show for dark colors is probably associated with light avoidance behavior. We recommend providing blue shelters for juveniles and dark shelters for adults.

KEY WORDS:
Animal behavior; aquaculture; color vision; ontogeny; visual modeling; welfare

INTRODUCTION

The giant river prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii (de Man, 1879) is native to a region that encompasses Malaysia, East India, West Indonesia, Gulf of Bengal, and Gulf of Thailand (Holthuis and Ng 2010Holthuis LB, Ng PKL (2010) Nomenclature and taxonomy. In: New NB, Valenti WC, Tidwell JW, D’Abramo LR, Kutty MN (Eds) Freshwater prawns: biology and farming . Wiley Blackwell, Oxford, 12-17.). In their natural environment, the larvae inhabit estuarine environments (Sandifer and Smith 1985Sandifer P, Smith TIJ (1985) Freshwater prawns. In: Huner J, Brown EE (Eds) Crustacean and Mollusk: Aquaculture in the United States. The AVI Publishing Company, Westport, 63-125. https://www.amazon.com/Crustacean-Mollusk-Aquaculture-United-States/dp/1468415050
https://www.amazon.com/Crustacean-Mollus...
), are planktonic (New 2002New MB (2002) Farming freshwater prawns: a manual for the culture of the giant river prawn (Machrobrachium rosenbergii). Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations, Rome , Technical Paper 428. https://www.fao.org/3/y4100e/y4100e.pdf
https://www.fao.org/3/y4100e/y4100e.pdf...
) and go through 11 larval stages (from Zoea I to Zoea XI) (Sandifer and Smith 1985Sandifer P, Smith TIJ (1985) Freshwater prawns. In: Huner J, Brown EE (Eds) Crustacean and Mollusk: Aquaculture in the United States. The AVI Publishing Company, Westport, 63-125. https://www.amazon.com/Crustacean-Mollusk-Aquaculture-United-States/dp/1468415050
https://www.amazon.com/Crustacean-Mollus...
). After a larval period of 20 to 50 days, the larvae undergo metamorphosis into post-larvae (Sandifer and Smith 1985Sandifer P, Smith TIJ (1985) Freshwater prawns. In: Huner J, Brown EE (Eds) Crustacean and Mollusk: Aquaculture in the United States. The AVI Publishing Company, Westport, 63-125. https://www.amazon.com/Crustacean-Mollusk-Aquaculture-United-States/dp/1468415050
https://www.amazon.com/Crustacean-Mollus...
), which are benthic and begin migration to freshwater environments, where they remain until adulthood (New 2002New MB (2002) Farming freshwater prawns: a manual for the culture of the giant river prawn (Machrobrachium rosenbergii). Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations, Rome , Technical Paper 428. https://www.fao.org/3/y4100e/y4100e.pdf
https://www.fao.org/3/y4100e/y4100e.pdf...
) and mate (Sandifer and Smith 1985Sandifer P, Smith TIJ (1985) Freshwater prawns. In: Huner J, Brown EE (Eds) Crustacean and Mollusk: Aquaculture in the United States. The AVI Publishing Company, Westport, 63-125. https://www.amazon.com/Crustacean-Mollusk-Aquaculture-United-States/dp/1468415050
https://www.amazon.com/Crustacean-Mollus...
). Then, the ovige rous female migrates to estuarine environments, where the larvae hatch from the eggs, restarting the cycle (Sandifer and Smith 1985Sandifer P, Smith TIJ (1985) Freshwater prawns. In: Huner J, Brown EE (Eds) Crustacean and Mollusk: Aquaculture in the United States. The AVI Publishing Company, Westport, 63-125. https://www.amazon.com/Crustacean-Mollusk-Aquaculture-United-States/dp/1468415050
https://www.amazon.com/Crustacean-Mollus...
).

Macrobrachium rosenbergii is a species of great commercial importance (Zeng et al. 2012Zeng C, Cheng Y, Lucas J, Southgate PC (2012) Other decapod crustaceans. In: Lucas JS, Southgate PC (Eds) Aquaculture: farming aquatic animals and plants. Wiley-Blackwell, Oxford, 2nd ed., 514-540. https://www.amazon.com.br/Aquaculture-Farming-Aquatic-Animals-Plants/dp/1405188588
https://www.amazon.com.br/Aquaculture-Fa...
, Engle et al. 2016Engle CR, Quagrainie Kwamena K, Dey MM (2016) Seafood and aquaculture marketing concepts. In: Engle CR, Quagrainie KK, Dey MM (Eds) Seafood and Aquaculture Marketing Handbook. Wiley Blackwell, Oxford , 2nd ed., 47-79. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118859223
https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118859223...
) that, in 2018 alone, accounted for the production of 234,400 tons of food worldwide (FAO 2020FAO (2020) The state of world fisheries and aquaculture 2020: Sustainability in Action. Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations, Rome. https://doi.org/10.4060/ca9229en
https://doi.org/10.4060/ca9229en...
). These prawns are farmed under various conditions (Coyle et al. 2010Coyle SD, Alston DE, Sampaio CMS (2010) Nursery Systems and Management. In: New NB, Valenti WC, Tidwell JW, D’Abramo LR, Kutty MN (Eds) Freshwater prawns: biology and farming. Wiley Blackwell, Oxford, 108-126., Daniels et al. 2010Daniels WH, Cavalli RO, Smullen RP (2010) Broodstock management. In: New NB, Valenti WC, Tidwell JW, D’Abramo LR, Kutty MN (Eds) Freshwater prawns: biology and farming . Wiley Blackwell, Oxford, 40-54., Valenti et al. 2010Valenti WC, Daniels WH, New MB, Correia ES (2010a) Hatchery Systems and Management. In: New NB, Valenti WC, Tidwell JW, D’abramo LR, Kutty MN (Eds) Freshwater prawns: biology and farming . Wiley Blackwell, Oxford , 55-85.a, 2010Valenti WC, New MB, Salin KR, Ye J (2010b) Grow-out systems -monoculture. In: New NB, Valenti WC, Tidwell JW, D’abramo LR, Kutty MN (Eds) Freshwater prawns: biology and farming . Wiley Blackwell, Oxford , 154-179.b), which might translate to a great range of light environments, which have the potential to impact the biology and productivity of prawns. In fact, one study has already shown that the color of the environment can change the color of the prawns themselves, in order to favor camouflage (Borisov et al. 2022Borisov RR, Nikonova IN, Parshin-Chudin AV, Kovacheva NP (2022) Effect of background color on the coloration of juvenile giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii. Russian Journal of Ecology 53(1): 40-47. https://doi.org/10.1134/S1067413622010039
https://doi.org/10.1134/S106741362201003...
). Another study has already shown that food color affects prawns’ larvae feeding behavior (Yong et al. 2018Yong ASK, Kawamura G, Lim LS, Gwee PX (2018) Growth performance and survival of giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii larvae fed coloured feed. Aquaculture Research 49(8): 2815-2821. https://doi.org/10.1111/are.13744
https://doi.org/10.1111/are.13744...
). In different species, color prefe rence has also been related to the selection of appropriate habitats (Strader et al. 2015Strader ME, Davies SW, Matz MV (2015) Differential responses of coral larvae to the colour of ambient light guide them to suitable settlement microhabitat. Royal Society Open Science 2(10): 150358. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.150358
https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.150358...
, Gu et al. 2017Gu X, Zhuang P, Zhao F, Shi X, Huang X, Feng G, Zhang T, Liu J, Zhang L, Kynard B (2017) Substrate color preference and feeding by juvenile Chinese sturgeon Acipenser sinensis: exploration of a behavioral adaptation. Environmental Biology of Fishes 100(1): 27-33. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10641-016-0549-y
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10641-016-0549-...
, Havel and Fuiman 2017Havel LN, Fuiman LA (2017) Depth preference of settling red drum (Sciaenops ocellatus) Larvae in relation to benthic habitat color and water-column depth. Estuaries and Coasts 40(2): 573-579. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12237-016-0160-7
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12237-016-0160-...
). For instance, changes in color preference throughout the ontogenetic development of some caridean shrimps (Lysmatidae) have been related to the physical properties of the environments occupied by them at different stages of development (Johnson and Rhyne 2015Johnson KB, Rhyne AL (2015) Ontogenetic shift of spectral sensitivity in the larval phototaxis of two sympatric caridean shrimp, Lysmata wurdemanni and L. boggessi (Decapoda: Lysmatidae). Marine Biology 162(6): 1265-1273. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00227-015-2667-z
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00227-015-2667-...
). Therefore, the study of color preference by species of economic interest might exert an important role in animal welfare and food productivity.

Morphologically, the eyes of M. rosenbergii change throughout ontogenetic development, since larvae have functionally apposition eyes, whereas adults have functionally reflecting superposition eyes (Nilsson 1983Nilsson D-E (1983) Evolutionary links between apposition and superposition optics in crustacean eyes. Nature 302: 818-821. https://doi.org/10.1038/302818a0
https://doi.org/10.1038/302818a0...
). The superposition eyes are more efficient for gathering light and can be advantageous in low light conditions, but the transformations necessary for the appearance of a superposition eye may not be complete in post-larvae, as seen in a caridean shrimp (Douglass and Forward 1989Douglass JK, Forward RB (1989) The ontogeny of facultative superposition optics in a shrimp eye: hatching through metamorphosis. Cell and Tissue Research 258(2): 289-300. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00239449
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00239449...
). Regarding the visual sensitivity of the species, it was found that dark-adapted individuals exhibit a light absorption peak at 563 nm, which corresponds to the yellow-red region of the spectrum (Matsuda and Wilder 2014Matsuda K, Wilder MN (2014) Eye structure and function in the giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii. Fisheries Science 80(3): 531-541. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-014-0734-x
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-014-0734-...
), although the authors don’t specify how many photoreceptor types would be accounting for the sensitivity curve. Still, in spite of the large number of experiments that have already been conducted with M. rosenbergii (Chong-Carrillo et al. 2016Chong-Carrillo O, Arencibia-Jorge R, Chávez-Chong CO, Akintola SL, García-Guerrero MU, Michán-Aguirre L, Nolasco-Soria H, Cupul-Magaña F, Vega-Villasante F (2016) The prawns of the genus Macrobrachium (Crustacea, Decapoda, Palaemonidae) with commercial importance: a patentometric view. Latin American Journal of Aquatic Research 44(3): 602-609. https://doi.org/10.3856/vol44-issue3-fulltext-19
https://doi.org/10.3856/vol44-issue3-ful...
), only recently their visual system began to be studied through behavioral experiments (Kawamura et al. 2016Kawamura G, Bagarinao T, Yong ASK, Jeganathan IMX, Lim LS (2016) Colour preference and colour vision of the larvae of the giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 474: 67-72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2015.10.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2015.10....
, 2017Kawamura G, Bagarinao T, Yong ASK, Fen TC, Lim LS (2017) Shelter colour preference of the postlarvae of the giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii. Fisheries Science 83(2): 259-264. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-017-1062-8
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-017-1062-...
, 2018Kawamura G, Bagarinao T, Yong ASK, Faisal AB, Lim LS (2018) Limit of colour vision in dim light in larvae of the giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii. Fisheries Science 84(2): 365-371. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-018-1179-4
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-018-1179-...
, 2020Kawamura G, Yong ASK, Wong JS, Tuzan AD, Lim LS (2020) The giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii alters background colour preference after metamorphosis from larvae to postlarvae: In association with nature of phototaxis. Aquaculture Research 51(9): 3711-3717. https://doi.org/10.1111/are.14720
https://doi.org/10.1111/are.14720...
).

Through visual modeling studies, a strategy that has currently gained popularity, it is possible to assess which spectral information available in the environment could be exerting an adaptive function. During visual modeling, we infer how the visual system of a given animal is stimulated by observing a particular object under a specific illuminant (Olsson et al. 2018Olsson P, Lind O, Kelber A (2018) Chromatic and achromatic vision: Parameter choice and limitations for reliable model predictions. Behavioral Ecology 29(2): 273-282. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arx133
https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arx133...
). In other words, just by knowing how many kinds of photoreceptors (and their peak sensitivities) there are in the eye of an animal, the spectrum of ambient light, and the color of an object of interest, we can suppose how the object should be seen by that observer. Yet, although variations in the type and number of photoreceptors are usually related to the dimensionality of color vision, only behavioral tests can verify an animals’ color perception (Jacobs 1996Jacobs GH (1996) Primate photopigments and primate color vision. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 93(2): 577-581. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.93.2.577
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.93.2.577...
). So, it is important to couple visual modeling data with behavioral experiments that can validate them (Lind and Kelber 2009Lind O, Kelber A (2009) Avian colour vision: Effects of variation in receptor sensitivity and noise data on model predictions as compared to behavioural results. Vision Research 49(15): 1939-1947. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.05.003
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.05...
).

To our knowledge, the availability of chromatic and achromatic cues to M. rosenbergii has never been properly analyzed by means of visual modeling. The experiments already carried out either did not try to control the brightness of the stimuli or tried to do so without correctly taking into account the prawns’ visual system. In color preference experiments, the choice of stimuli colors should consider the eye of the beholder (Hill 2002Hill GE (2002) In the eye of the beholder. In: Hill GE (Ed.) A red bird in a brown bag: the function and evolution of colorful plumage in the house finch. Oxford University Press, Oxford, 39-52. https://oxford.universitypressscholarship.com/view/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195148480.001.0001/acprof-9780195148480
https://oxford.universitypressscholarshi...
), not human vision. By using visual modeling, we can control the colors of the stimuli to be presented to prawns in a rigorous manner, as in studies with other animal species (Detto 2007Detto T (2007) The fiddler crab Uca mjoebergi uses colour vision in mate choice. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 274(1627): 2785-2790. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2007.1059
https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2007.1059...
, Siebeck et al. 2014Siebeck UE, Wallis GM, Litherland L, Ganeshina O, Vorobyev M (2014) Spectral and spatial selectivity of luminance vision in reef fish. Frontiers in Neural Circuits 8: 118. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2014.00118
https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2014.00118...
, Olsson et al. 2015Olsson P, Lind O, Kelber A (2015) Bird colour vision: Behavioural thresholds reveal receptor noise. Journal of Experimental Biology 218(2): 184-193. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.111187
https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.111187...
, Escobar-Camacho et al. 2019Escobar-Camacho D, Taylor MA, Cheney KL, Green NF, Marshall NJ, Carleton KL (2019) Color discrimination thresholds in a cichlid fish: Metriaclima benetos. The Journal of Experimental Biology 222(17): jeb201160. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.201160
https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.201160...
).

Therefore, here we developed the first color preference study on M. rosenbergii employing visual modeling, as a more rigorous control for stimuli brightness and chromaticity, according to the visual system of prawns. Our aim is to investigate whether their preference for different colors and brightness changes over their development. Since M. rosenbergii spontaneously occupies experimental shelters (Santos et al. 2015Santos DB, Pontes CS, Campos PMO, Arruda MF (2015) Behavioral profile of Macrobrachium rosenbergii in mixed and monosex culture submitted to shelter of different colors. Acta Scientiarum, Biological Sciences 37(3): 273-279. https://periodicos.uem.br/ojs/index.php/ActaSciBiolSci/article/view/24656
https://periodicos.uem.br/ojs/index.php/...
), we analyzed the preference that two benthic developmental stages (i.e. juveniles and adults) show for shelters of different chromaticities and brightness. Our hypothesis is that M. rosenbergii changes its color preference gradually throughout ontogeny, regardless of the farming conditions. This ontogenetic shift would be adaptive in natural populations because the spectral composition (i.e. color) of the surrounding natural photic environment changes as they start migrating from estuaries to freshwater ecosystems. Since this migration only begins when M. rosenbergii reaches its post-larvae developmental stage, we predict that adults will be more prone than juveniles to exhibit strong preferences for longer wavelengths, such as yellow and orange, which are colors associated with freshwater bodies (Stoffels 2013Stoffels RJ (2013) Trophic Ecology. In: Humphries P, Walker K (Eds) Ecology of Australian freshwater fishes. CSIRO Publishing, Melbourn, 131-158. https://www.publish.csiro.au/book/6515/#:~:text=The%20science%20of%20freshwater%20fish,to%20new%20levels%20of%20understanding
https://www.publish.csiro.au/book/6515/#...
).

MATERIAL AND METHODS

Ethical statement

Our research was approved by the Ethics Committee on the Use of Animals of our institution (protocol 042/2018) and is in accordance with Brazilian law. It complies with ARRIVE guidelines and was carried out in accordance with the U.K. Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986, and associated guidelines, EU Directive 2010/63/EU for animal experiments.

Animal maintenance

At the Sensory Ecology Laboratory of the Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, we kept 39 juveniles of M. rosenbergii. Animals were housed in a collective aquarium (100 x 50 cm, 40 cm water column), with transparent water, sandy substrate, and aeration. They were subjected to a 12-hour light/12-hour dark light cycle (light from 6:00 a.m. to 6:00 p.m.). Two fluorescent lamps provided a light intensity of approximately 320 lux (measured with an Extech Instruments HD 400 Light Meter), which should be sufficient for color discrimination (Kawamura et al. 2018Kawamura G, Bagarinao T, Yong ASK, Faisal AB, Lim LS (2018) Limit of colour vision in dim light in larvae of the giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii. Fisheries Science 84(2): 365-371. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-018-1179-4
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-018-1179-...
).

We kept the physical-chemical parameters of the water at optimum levels for the prawns (pH: 7.0-7.5; ammonia: 0 mg/L; temperature: 26-28 °C) and fed the animals twice a day (morning and afternoon) with commercial food containing 42% crude protein. Twenty percent of the aquarium’s water was changed twice a week.

Experimental apparatus

We also made available transparent water, sandy substrate, and aeration in two experimental aquaria, with aeration positioned, approximately, at the center of each aquarium. In each experimental aquarium, we arranged ten shelters, in two rows of five. We built the shelters with plastic-coated paper, folded in an appropriate way. Each aquarium had six grey shelters (PANTONE 19-1101 TPG, PANTONE P 172-15 C, PANTONE P 173-3 C, PANTONE P 179-3 C, PANTONE 10101 C, PANTONE P 179-1), each one with a different brightness, besides a blue (PANTONE 2728 C), a green (PANTONE 802 C), a yellow (PANTONE 397 C) and an orange (PANTONE 2012 C) shelter (Fig. 1).

Figure 1
Schematic view of the arrangement of shelters in an experimental aquarium. The prawns were individually tested in aquaria with 10 colored shelters arranged in two rows of five.

Shelters’ colors and brightness

For choosing our stimulus colors, we printed 101 different color patches that were coated with plastic and had their reflectance spectra measured by us with a spectrophotometer (USB4000 UV-VIS Fiber Optic Spectrometer, Ocean Optics, Inc.). We coupled the spectrometer to a bifurcated optical fiber (QR450-7-XSR, Ocean Optics, Inc.), also attached to a light source (DH-2000-BAL, Ocean Optics, Inc.). A white standard surface (WS-1-SL, Ocean Optics, Inc.), and the obs truction of the light source and optical fiber, were used as, respectively, the white and the black standards, for system calibration. We also measured the illuminant of the experimental room with the spectrometer coupled to an optical fiber (QP450-2-XSR, Ocean Optics, Inc.), attached to a cosine corrector (CC-3-UV-S, Ocean Optics, Inc.). We calibrated this spectrometric system with a calibration light source (LS-1-CAL, Ocean Optics, Inc.).

We run visual models, through pavo 2 (Maia et al. 2019Maia R, Gruson H, Endler JA, White TE (2019) pavo 2: New tools for the spectral and spatial analysis of colour in r. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 10(7): 1097-1107. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.13174
https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.13174...
), a package for R 3.4.1 (R Development Core Team 2020R Core Team (2020) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna. https://www.R-project.org/
https://www.R-project.org/...
), to infer how each color patch would be seen according to the visual system of the prawns. Our model computed the quantum catches absorbed by each type of photoreceptor described for prawns when the animals visualized each shelter (reflectance spectra in Fig. 2), that was illuminated by the fluorescent lamps of our experimental room (Fig. 3). The absorption peaks of the photoreceptors of another species, the ghost shrimp Palaemon paludosus (Gibbes, 1850) (380 nm and 555 nm) (Goldsmith and Fernandez 1968Goldsmith TH, Fernandez HR (1968) Comparative Studies of Crustacean Spectral Sensitivity. Zeitschrift für Vergleichende Physiologie 60: 156-175. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00878449
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00878449...
), were adopted in our model since data were not available for M. rosenbergii. Palaemon paludosus is the species most closely related to M. rosenbergii for which photoreceptors’ absorption peaks have been established. Using photoreceptors’ absorption peaks of related species is a viable alternative since small variations in estimated peaks do not carry a great influence on visual modeling results (Lind and Kelber 2009Lind O, Kelber A (2009) Avian colour vision: Effects of variation in receptor sensitivity and noise data on model predictions as compared to behavioural results. Vision Research 49(15): 1939-1947. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.05.003
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.05...
, Olsson et al. 2018Olsson P, Lind O, Kelber A (2018) Chromatic and achromatic vision: Parameter choice and limitations for reliable model predictions. Behavioral Ecology 29(2): 273-282. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arx133
https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arx133...
).

Figure 2
Reflectance spectra of experimental shelters. The reflectance of each curve is represented as a percentage, compared to the maximum reflectance of the most reflective curve. Each curve is represented approximately the same color as the respective shelter.

Figure 3
Illuminant spectrum of the experimental room. Illumination was provided by two fluorescent lamps.

In order to calculate the signal of each photoreceptor, we divided the amount of light reflected by a stimulus (shelter) and captured by a specific photoreceptor class (stimulus quantum catch), by the amount of light reflected by a perfectly white surface (white standard) and captured by the same photoreceptor class (maximum quantum catch). Following Detto (2007Detto T (2007) The fiddler crab Uca mjoebergi uses colour vision in mate choice. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 274(1627): 2785-2790. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2007.1059
https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2007.1059...
), we computed achromatic signals for each stimulus, as the sum of signals of short-wavelength (S signals) and long-wavelength (L signals) photoreceptors. For chromatic signals, we divided S signals by L signals (Siebeck et al. 2014Siebeck UE, Wallis GM, Litherland L, Ganeshina O, Vorobyev M (2014) Spectral and spatial selectivity of luminance vision in reef fish. Frontiers in Neural Circuits 8: 118. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2014.00118
https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2014.00118...
). Chromatic and achromatic signals were plotted in a chromaticity-luminance diagram (Fig. 4) that shows how our stimuli varied with respect to color and brightness. We chose not to use the RNL model (Vorobyev and Osorio 1998Vorobyev M, Osorio D (1998) Receptor noise as a determinant of colour thresholds. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 265(1394): 351-358. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1998.0302
https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1998.0302...
) because it demands visual parameters (Olsson et al. 2018Olsson P, Lind O, Kelber A (2018) Chromatic and achromatic vision: Parameter choice and limitations for reliable model predictions. Behavioral Ecology 29(2): 273-282. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arx133
https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arx133...
) that are still unknown for prawns.

Figure 4
Chromaticity-luminance diagram for the visual system of prawns. Inferred chromatic and achromatic signals, determined for a perfectly white surface (asterisk depicted in the diagram) and for shelters employed in our experiments (squares), are represented. Distance between squares indicates color and brightness differences in chromatic and achromatic signal axes, respectively. Short λ = short wavelengths (e.g. blues), Long λ = long wavelengths (e.g. yellows).

Experimental design

In experiment 1, we individually tested 39 juveniles about two months after metamorphosis (body weight: 0.46 ± 0.13 g; total length: 3.89 ± 0.36 cm), two juveniles per day, each one in a smaller experimental aquarium (30 x 50 cm). The shelters, in experiment 1, measured 7 cm depth x 6 cm width x 3 cm height. The position of the different shelters for each animal was randomized. We completed this experi ment in four weeks.

In experiment 2, we individually tested 24 of the 39 previously tested individuals, when they were already adults about seven months after metamorphosis (body weight: 6.4 ± 3.2 g; total length: 8.49 ± 1.84 cm), one adult prawn per day, in a larger experimental aquarium (50 x 100 cm). The shelters, in experiment 2, measured 7 cm depth x 9 cm width x 5 cm height. We completed this experiment in five weeks. Otherwise, the two experiments were identical. Differences in sample size between experiments 1 and 2 were due to animal mortality. We did not sex the prawns or record their morphotypes in our experiments.

In both experiments, we fed all prawns in the collective aquaria at 7:30 a.m., removed each prawn, that was about to be tested, from the collective aquarium and transferred it to the center of an experimental aquarium at 8:00 a.m. From 9:00 a.m. to 5:30 p.m., every 30 min, we recorded if the only animal was inside/on top of a shelter (shelter occupancy), or if it was away from any shelter (no choice), following Kawamura et al. (2017Kawamura G, Bagarinao T, Yong ASK, Fen TC, Lim LS (2017) Shelter colour preference of the postlarvae of the giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii. Fisheries Science 83(2): 259-264. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-017-1062-8
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-017-1062-...
). This record was made directly, without the use of a video camera. After the experimental session, we removed the prawn from the experimental aquarium, recorded its weight and total length, and transferred it to another collective aquarium, in which individuals that had been already tested were kept. All animals were fed again at the end of the day.

Statistical analysis

We performed ten chi-square tests, one for each shelter, for unequal expected proportions, to verify if the occupancy of each shelter differed from the expected 10% of all cases. We employed the Bonferroni correction to account for multiple comparisons and set our α to 0.005. All tests were performed in BioEstat 5.3 (Ayres et al. 2007Ayres M, Ayres M Jr, Ayres DL, Santos AAS (2007) BioEstat: aplicações estatísticas nas áreas das ciências bio-médicas. https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Alex-De-Assis-Dos-Santos-2/publication/263608962_BIOESTAT_-_aplicacoes_estatisticas_nas_areas_das_Ciencias_Bio-Medicas/links/02e7e53b598e69ebfe000000/BIOESTAT-aplicacoes-estatisticas-nas-areas-das-Ciencias-Bio-Medicas.pdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Ale...
).

RESULTS

Visual modeling

Regarding our stimuli, the blue shelter generated a chromatic signal that stood out from the green, the yellow, and the orange shelters (Fig. 4), a strong indication that the blue shelter could be perceived, by the visual system of the prawns, as being of a different color. In contrast, these four colored stimuli (i.e. blue, green, yellow, and orange shelters) only showed discrete variations of achromatic signals, with the yellow shelter being slightly lighter than the others (Fig. 4). Moreover, all these colored stimuli, according to the visual system of the prawns, were of intermediate brightness when compared to grey shelters. That is, in the absence of chromatic information, animals should not be able to tell colored and grey shelters apart, and would have a great range of achromatic options, including darker (grey shelters), intermediate (grey and colored shelters), and brighter (grey shelters) stimuli, from which they could choose.

Shelter preference

The records of juveniles and adults occupying the shelters are shown in Fig. 5. Juveniles occupied the bluest available shelter (i.e. the one with the highest chromatic signal) significantly more than expected by chance (p = 0.0034), while occupied all the other available shelters (i.e. those with intermediate and low chromatic signals) at expectancy levels (p ≥ 0.009). Adults, on the other hand, occupied two of the darkest available shelters (i.e. those with the lowest achromatic signals), grey 1 (p < 0.0001) and grey 2 (p = 0.0001), significantly more than expected by chance. They occupied the brightest available shelter (i.e. the one with the highest achromatic signal), grey 6 (p = 0.0010), significantly less than expected. Adults occupied all other shelters (i.e. those with more intermediate achromatic signals) at expectancy levels (p ≥ 0.0435).

Figure 5
Preference for shelters of different colors by juvenile and adult prawns. The bars represent the number of records of occupancy for each shelter. Black bars represent shelters that were chosen significantly above the expectancy level. Grey bars represent shelters chosen at expected frequencies. The white bar represents a shelter chosen significantly below expectancy levels.

DISCUSSION

Our study is the first to employ visual modeling to study color preference in M. rosenbergii. The computation of quantum catches, for the visual system of prawns, enabled us to choose blue, green, yellow, and orange stimuli that satisfied two criteria: 1) differed chromatically from the grey shelters, presenting a very high or a very low chromatic signal; and 2) exhibited intermediate brightness levels, when compared to grey stimuli. Our visual model showed that, for the prawns, the blue shelter presented a chromaticity very different from the chromaticity of the green, the yellow, and the orange shelters. On the other hand, the green, the yellow, and the orange shelters were very similar in chromaticity for the prawns, probably due to a dichromatic color vision maintained by the interaction of two different types of photoreceptors (Jacobs 1996Jacobs GH (1996) Primate photopigments and primate color vision. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 93(2): 577-581. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.93.2.577
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.93.2.577...
). Nevertheless, we must emphasize that our results should be interpreted with caution since it is not already established if the visual system of M. rosenbergii works through chromatic opposition.

Our prediction that adult subjects would prefer longer wavelength colors (i.e. yellow or orange shelters) was not corroborated, since juveniles preferred the blue shelter, while adults preferred the darkest ones. In our study, the stimulus that caught the attention of the juveniles (i.e. the blue shelter) was similar in brightness, and chromatically different, from other colored stimuli (i.e. the green, the yellow, and the orange shelters), which were disfavored. This is a strong indication that the preference for blue shown by juveniles was based on color alone, not brightness, which is the first indication of color vision in these animals. Adult preference, on the other hand, was probably based on brightness, since different neutral stimuli (grey shelters) were very similar in chromaticity and very different in brightness. Still, our results are not enough to determine whether adult giant river prawns enjoy color vision since there is a possibility that they prefer darker shelters despite a color vision sense. In fact, it has already been reported that adults favor the occupancy of shaded areas of the pond (Karplus and Harpaz 1990Karplus I, Harpaz S (1990) Preliminary observations on behavioral interactions and distribution patterns of freshwater prawns Macrobrachium rosenbergii under semi-natural conditions (Decapoda, Caridea). Crustaceana 59(2): 193-203. https://doi.org/10.1163/156854090X00660
https://doi.org/10.1163/156854090X00660...
) and that the use of shelters by this species might be linked to light avoidance behavior (Costa and Arruda 2016Costa JS, Arruda MF (2016) Effect of stocking density on the behavior of newly transformed juvenile Macrobrachium rosenbergii. Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology 49(6): 375-389. https://doi.org/10.1080/10236244.2016.1236527
https://doi.org/10.1080/10236244.2016.12...
). So, the preference for darker shelters by adults, in our study, may be explained by a tendency to avoid light expressed by the species, which should facilitate their escape from predators (Dabbagh and Kamrani 2011Dabbagh AR, Kamrani E (2011) Preference of substrate and shelter type by the snapping shrimp, Alpheus lobidens De Haan, 1849 (Crustacea: Caridea: Alpheidae). Pakistan Journal of Zoology 43(3): 606-608. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/283177626_Preference_of_Substrate_and_Shelter_Type_by_the_Snapping_Shrimp_Alpheus_lobidens_De_Haan_1849_Crustacea_Caridea_Alpheidae
https://www.researchgate.net/publication...
). This preference for darker shelters has also been observed in other species of Macrobrachium (Mariappan and Balasundaram 2003Mariappan P, Balasundaram C (2003) Sheltering behaviour of Macrobrachium nobilii (Henderson and Matthai, 1910). Acta Ethologica 5(2): 89-94. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10211-002-0069-y
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10211-002-0069-...
, Balasundaram et al. 2004Balasundaram C, Jeyachitra P, Balamurugan P (2004) Shelter preference in Macrobrachium spp. with reference to aquaculture. Acta Ethologica 7(2): 95-101. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10211-004-0090-4
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10211-004-0090-...
).

The ontogenetic changes in color preference exhibited by our animals, from blue to dark grey, are consistent with what has been found in less controlled experiments performed by other laboratories (Juarez et al. 1987Juarez LM, Holtschmit K, Salmeron JJ, Smith MK (1987) The effects of chemical and visual communication, space availability, and substratum color on growth of the juvenile freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii (De Man). Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 110: 285-295. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0981(87)90006-2
https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0981(87)900...
, Kawamura et al. 2016Kawamura G, Bagarinao T, Yong ASK, Jeganathan IMX, Lim LS (2016) Colour preference and colour vision of the larvae of the giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 474: 67-72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2015.10.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2015.10....
, 2017Kawamura G, Bagarinao T, Yong ASK, Fen TC, Lim LS (2017) Shelter colour preference of the postlarvae of the giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii. Fisheries Science 83(2): 259-264. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-017-1062-8
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-017-1062-...
, 2018Kawamura G, Bagarinao T, Yong ASK, Faisal AB, Lim LS (2018) Limit of colour vision in dim light in larvae of the giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii. Fisheries Science 84(2): 365-371. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-018-1179-4
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-018-1179-...
, Yong et al. 2018Yong ASK, Kawamura G, Lim LS, Gwee PX (2018) Growth performance and survival of giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii larvae fed coloured feed. Aquaculture Research 49(8): 2815-2821. https://doi.org/10.1111/are.13744
https://doi.org/10.1111/are.13744...
), however, our data demonstrate that these shifts in color preference seem to occur later in life. Regarding the preference of juveniles, our findings are in line with what has been shown for the color preference of the larvae of M. rosenbergii in studies employing different colors of beads (Kawamura et al. 2016Kawamura G, Bagarinao T, Yong ASK, Jeganathan IMX, Lim LS (2016) Colour preference and colour vision of the larvae of the giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 474: 67-72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2015.10.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2015.10....
) and feed (Yong et al. 2018Yong ASK, Kawamura G, Lim LS, Gwee PX (2018) Growth performance and survival of giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii larvae fed coloured feed. Aquaculture Research 49(8): 2815-2821. https://doi.org/10.1111/are.13744
https://doi.org/10.1111/are.13744...
). Similarly, the preference for darker stimuli by our adult subjects is in line with what has been verified in previous studies that tested post-larvae of M. rosenbergii (Kawamura et al. 2017Kawamura G, Bagarinao T, Yong ASK, Fen TC, Lim LS (2017) Shelter colour preference of the postlarvae of the giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii. Fisheries Science 83(2): 259-264. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-017-1062-8
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-017-1062-...
), which were shown to prefer black shelters over blue or green ones, and juveniles (Juarez et al. 1987Juarez LM, Holtschmit K, Salmeron JJ, Smith MK (1987) The effects of chemical and visual communication, space availability, and substratum color on growth of the juvenile freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii (De Man). Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 110: 285-295. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0981(87)90006-2
https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0981(87)900...
), that preferred black over blue or white substrates. Kawamura et al. (2020), on the other hand, report that the yellow background was the most preferred by larvae and was avoided by post-larvae. The authors also claim that larvae preferred brighter backgrounds, while post-larvae preferred darker ones. However, these authors were unable to satisfactorily consider the visual system of the animals, since nothing was done to assess the chromaticity of the different stimuli.

The discrepancies found between our results and previous studies might result from the lack of precise control over stimuli and also from methodological differences. For instance, Juarez et al. (1987Juarez LM, Holtschmit K, Salmeron JJ, Smith MK (1987) The effects of chemical and visual communication, space availability, and substratum color on growth of the juvenile freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii (De Man). Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 110: 285-295. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0981(87)90006-2
https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0981(87)900...
) and Kawamura et al. (2017Kawamura G, Bagarinao T, Yong ASK, Fen TC, Lim LS (2017) Shelter colour preference of the postlarvae of the giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii. Fisheries Science 83(2): 259-264. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-017-1062-8
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-017-1062-...
, 2020Kawamura G, Yong ASK, Wong JS, Tuzan AD, Lim LS (2020) The giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii alters background colour preference after metamorphosis from larvae to postlarvae: In association with nature of phototaxis. Aquaculture Research 51(9): 3711-3717. https://doi.org/10.1111/are.14720
https://doi.org/10.1111/are.14720...
) tested M. rosenbergii in groups, while we adopted individual testing. We know these animals are aggressive from a young age (Silva and Arruda 2015Silva PF, Arruda MF (2015) Social status and individual behavioral differences in juvenile Macrobrachium rosenbergii. Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology 48(1): 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1080/10236244.2014.968424
https://doi.org/10.1080/10236244.2014.96...
), and since Kawamura et al. (2017Kawamura G, Bagarinao T, Yong ASK, Fen TC, Lim LS (2017) Shelter colour preference of the postlarvae of the giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii. Fisheries Science 83(2): 259-264. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-017-1062-8
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-017-1062-...
) reported agonistic interactions among their experimental individuals, we can’t rule out the possibility that their results were biased by hierarchy. For example, in a group context, it is plausible to imagine that a prawn might have avoided occupying a preferred spot that was already occupied by a more dominant individual, forcing it to opt for a suboptimal choice.

Hierarchy can even influence the expression of vision-related genes. Aziz et al. (2018Aziz D, Rahi ML, Hurwood DA, Mather PB (2018) Analysis of candidate gene expression patterns of adult male Macrobrachium rosenbergii morphotypes in response to a social dominance hierarchy. Hydrobiologia 825(1): 121-136. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10750-018-3721-x
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10750-018-3721-...
) compared gene expression of the three morphotypes found in populations of M. rosenbergii and found higher expression levels of long-wavelength opsin genes in the eyestalk and hepatopancreas of small males, which are the most subordinate morphotype. Hence, they suggest that small males could present better color discrimination, which would help avoid dominant and subdominant morphotypes, which carry blue and orange claws, respectively. In addition, Santos et al. (2015Santos DB, Pontes CS, Campos PMO, Arruda MF (2015) Behavioral profile of Macrobrachium rosenbergii in mixed and monosex culture submitted to shelter of different colors. Acta Scientiarum, Biological Sciences 37(3): 273-279. https://periodicos.uem.br/ojs/index.php/ActaSciBiolSci/article/view/24656
https://periodicos.uem.br/ojs/index.php/...
) noticed a difference in color preference between sex, verifying that females preferred red and orange shelters, while males preferred black ones. Since we did not sex our prawns, and neither did Juarez et al. (1987Juarez LM, Holtschmit K, Salmeron JJ, Smith MK (1987) The effects of chemical and visual communication, space availability, and substratum color on growth of the juvenile freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii (De Man). Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 110: 285-295. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0981(87)90006-2
https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0981(87)900...
) or Kawamura et al. (2017Kawamura G, Bagarinao T, Yong ASK, Fen TC, Lim LS (2017) Shelter colour preference of the postlarvae of the giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii. Fisheries Science 83(2): 259-264. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-017-1062-8
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-017-1062-...
, 2020Kawamura G, Yong ASK, Wong JS, Tuzan AD, Lim LS (2020) The giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii alters background colour preference after metamorphosis from larvae to postlarvae: In association with nature of phototaxis. Aquaculture Research 51(9): 3711-3717. https://doi.org/10.1111/are.14720
https://doi.org/10.1111/are.14720...
), we should not rule out sex bias as a possible explanation for the ontogenetic discrepancies found between studies. Additional behavioral studies should be encouraged, with careful control of stimuli’s chromaticity and brightness, in order to study the development of a preference for chromatic and achromatic cues in males and females of M. rosenbergii and to elucidate whether adults have color vision.

The use of shelters can improve the welfare of animals used in aquaculture, for example, by preventing the appearance of body damage resulting, in part, from agonistic interactions (Gesto and Jokumsen 2022Gesto M, Jokumsen A (2022) Effects of simple shelters on growth performance and welfare of rainbow trout juveniles. Aquaculture 551: 737930. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquaculture.2022.737930
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquaculture.20...
). A higher frequency of shelter use could reduce the occurrence of predation events and favor access to food (Santos et al. 2015Santos DB, Pontes CS, Campos PMO, Arruda MF (2015) Behavioral profile of Macrobrachium rosenbergii in mixed and monosex culture submitted to shelter of different colors. Acta Scientiarum, Biological Sciences 37(3): 273-279. https://periodicos.uem.br/ojs/index.php/ActaSciBiolSci/article/view/24656
https://periodicos.uem.br/ojs/index.php/...
). According to Huntingford et al. (2012Huntingford F, Jobling M, Kadri S (2012) Aquaculture and Behavior. Wiley Blackwell, Oxford, 240 pp.), in aquaculture, animal welfare and production frequently go hand in hand. In fact, Santos et al. (2015Santos DB, Pontes CS, Campos PMO, Arruda MF (2015) Behavioral profile of Macrobrachium rosenbergii in mixed and monosex culture submitted to shelter of different colors. Acta Scientiarum, Biological Sciences 37(3): 273-279. https://periodicos.uem.br/ojs/index.php/ActaSciBiolSci/article/view/24656
https://periodicos.uem.br/ojs/index.php/...
) propose that providing the shelters preferred by M. rosenbergii would favor the growth of the prawns and reduce production costs.

Our results strongly indicate that M. rosenbergii uses chromaticity alone to distinguish between stimuli. Juveniles show a higher preference for blue shelters, which is based on chromaticity, since, according to our visual model, other shelters with comparable brightness were not preferred by the animals in the same way. Later in life, prawns direct their preference to darker shelters and avoid brighter ones, which seems to be based, mostly, on brightness cues. We suggest that farmers should provide blue shelters for juveniles and dark shelters for adults, to favor shelter occupancy, which can improve productivity and animal welfare.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We would like to thank Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior, finance codes 001, and 043/2012, which financed in part our study and that also provided a PhD. Scholarship to F.P.C. We would also like to thank Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico, finance codes 478222/2006-8, 25674/2009 and 474392/2013-9, which financed in part our study and that also provided a Researcher Scholarship to D.M.A.P. We would like to thank Felipe N. Castro, for helpful statistical advice; Beatriz A. de Souza, Charles S. Torres, Eider R. de Farias Filho, Thiago F. Cordeiro and Tainá S. Araujo for helping with data collection and animal care; Marilia F. Erickson, Larissa M.O. Prado and Vinícius G.S. Santana for contributing with reflectance measurements and visual modeling; and Louise M.O. Prado for figure design. Any inadequacies of proficiency are the sole responsibility of the authors.

LITERATURE CITED

  • Ayres M, Ayres M Jr, Ayres DL, Santos AAS (2007) BioEstat: aplicações estatísticas nas áreas das ciências bio-médicas. https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Alex-De-Assis-Dos-Santos-2/publication/263608962_BIOESTAT_-_aplicacoes_estatisticas_nas_areas_das_Ciencias_Bio-Medicas/links/02e7e53b598e69ebfe000000/BIOESTAT-aplicacoes-estatisticas-nas-areas-das-Ciencias-Bio-Medicas.pdf
    » https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Alex-De-Assis-Dos-Santos-2/publication/263608962_BIOESTAT_-_aplicacoes_estatisticas_nas_areas_das_Ciencias_Bio-Medicas/links/02e7e53b598e69ebfe000000/BIOESTAT-aplicacoes-estatisticas-nas-areas-das-Ciencias-Bio-Medicas.pdf
  • Aziz D, Rahi ML, Hurwood DA, Mather PB (2018) Analysis of candidate gene expression patterns of adult male Macrobrachium rosenbergii morphotypes in response to a social dominance hierarchy. Hydrobiologia 825(1): 121-136. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10750-018-3721-x
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10750-018-3721-x
  • Balasundaram C, Jeyachitra P, Balamurugan P (2004) Shelter preference in Macrobrachium spp. with reference to aquaculture. Acta Ethologica 7(2): 95-101. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10211-004-0090-4
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10211-004-0090-4
  • Borisov RR, Nikonova IN, Parshin-Chudin AV, Kovacheva NP (2022) Effect of background color on the coloration of juvenile giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii Russian Journal of Ecology 53(1): 40-47. https://doi.org/10.1134/S1067413622010039
    » https://doi.org/10.1134/S1067413622010039
  • Chong-Carrillo O, Arencibia-Jorge R, Chávez-Chong CO, Akintola SL, García-Guerrero MU, Michán-Aguirre L, Nolasco-Soria H, Cupul-Magaña F, Vega-Villasante F (2016) The prawns of the genus Macrobrachium (Crustacea, Decapoda, Palaemonidae) with commercial importance: a patentometric view. Latin American Journal of Aquatic Research 44(3): 602-609. https://doi.org/10.3856/vol44-issue3-fulltext-19
    » https://doi.org/10.3856/vol44-issue3-fulltext-19
  • Costa JS, Arruda MF (2016) Effect of stocking density on the behavior of newly transformed juvenile Macrobrachium rosenbergii Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology 49(6): 375-389. https://doi.org/10.1080/10236244.2016.1236527
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/10236244.2016.1236527
  • Coyle SD, Alston DE, Sampaio CMS (2010) Nursery Systems and Management. In: New NB, Valenti WC, Tidwell JW, D’Abramo LR, Kutty MN (Eds) Freshwater prawns: biology and farming. Wiley Blackwell, Oxford, 108-126.
  • Dabbagh AR, Kamrani E (2011) Preference of substrate and shelter type by the snapping shrimp, Alpheus lobidens De Haan, 1849 (Crustacea: Caridea: Alpheidae). Pakistan Journal of Zoology 43(3): 606-608. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/283177626_Preference_of_Substrate_and_Shelter_Type_by_the_Snapping_Shrimp_Alpheus_lobidens_De_Haan_1849_Crustacea_Caridea_Alpheidae
    » https://www.researchgate.net/publication/283177626_Preference_of_Substrate_and_Shelter_Type_by_the_Snapping_Shrimp_Alpheus_lobidens_De_Haan_1849_Crustacea_Caridea_Alpheidae
  • Daniels WH, Cavalli RO, Smullen RP (2010) Broodstock management. In: New NB, Valenti WC, Tidwell JW, D’Abramo LR, Kutty MN (Eds) Freshwater prawns: biology and farming . Wiley Blackwell, Oxford, 40-54.
  • Detto T (2007) The fiddler crab Uca mjoebergi uses colour vision in mate choice. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 274(1627): 2785-2790. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2007.1059
    » https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2007.1059
  • Douglass JK, Forward RB (1989) The ontogeny of facultative superposition optics in a shrimp eye: hatching through metamorphosis. Cell and Tissue Research 258(2): 289-300. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00239449
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00239449
  • Engle CR, Quagrainie Kwamena K, Dey MM (2016) Seafood and aquaculture marketing concepts. In: Engle CR, Quagrainie KK, Dey MM (Eds) Seafood and Aquaculture Marketing Handbook. Wiley Blackwell, Oxford , 2nd ed., 47-79. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118859223
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118859223
  • Escobar-Camacho D, Taylor MA, Cheney KL, Green NF, Marshall NJ, Carleton KL (2019) Color discrimination thresholds in a cichlid fish: Metriaclima benetos The Journal of Experimental Biology 222(17): jeb201160. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.201160
    » https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.201160
  • FAO (2020) The state of world fisheries and aquaculture 2020: Sustainability in Action. Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations, Rome. https://doi.org/10.4060/ca9229en
    » https://doi.org/10.4060/ca9229en
  • Gesto M, Jokumsen A (2022) Effects of simple shelters on growth performance and welfare of rainbow trout juveniles. Aquaculture 551: 737930. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquaculture.2022.737930
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquaculture.2022.737930
  • Goldsmith TH, Fernandez HR (1968) Comparative Studies of Crustacean Spectral Sensitivity. Zeitschrift für Vergleichende Physiologie 60: 156-175. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00878449
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00878449
  • Gu X, Zhuang P, Zhao F, Shi X, Huang X, Feng G, Zhang T, Liu J, Zhang L, Kynard B (2017) Substrate color preference and feeding by juvenile Chinese sturgeon Acipenser sinensis: exploration of a behavioral adaptation. Environmental Biology of Fishes 100(1): 27-33. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10641-016-0549-y
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10641-016-0549-y
  • Havel LN, Fuiman LA (2017) Depth preference of settling red drum (Sciaenops ocellatus) Larvae in relation to benthic habitat color and water-column depth. Estuaries and Coasts 40(2): 573-579. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12237-016-0160-7
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s12237-016-0160-7
  • Hill GE (2002) In the eye of the beholder. In: Hill GE (Ed.) A red bird in a brown bag: the function and evolution of colorful plumage in the house finch. Oxford University Press, Oxford, 39-52. https://oxford.universitypressscholarship.com/view/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195148480.001.0001/acprof-9780195148480
    » https://oxford.universitypressscholarship.com/view/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195148480.001.0001/acprof-9780195148480
  • Holthuis LB, Ng PKL (2010) Nomenclature and taxonomy. In: New NB, Valenti WC, Tidwell JW, D’Abramo LR, Kutty MN (Eds) Freshwater prawns: biology and farming . Wiley Blackwell, Oxford, 12-17.
  • Huntingford F, Jobling M, Kadri S (2012) Aquaculture and Behavior. Wiley Blackwell, Oxford, 240 pp.
  • Jacobs GH (1996) Primate photopigments and primate color vision. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 93(2): 577-581. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.93.2.577
    » https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.93.2.577
  • Johnson KB, Rhyne AL (2015) Ontogenetic shift of spectral sensitivity in the larval phototaxis of two sympatric caridean shrimp, Lysmata wurdemanni and L. boggessi (Decapoda: Lysmatidae). Marine Biology 162(6): 1265-1273. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00227-015-2667-z
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s00227-015-2667-z
  • Juarez LM, Holtschmit K, Salmeron JJ, Smith MK (1987) The effects of chemical and visual communication, space availability, and substratum color on growth of the juvenile freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii (De Man). Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 110: 285-295. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0981(87)90006-2
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0981(87)90006-2
  • Karplus I, Harpaz S (1990) Preliminary observations on behavioral interactions and distribution patterns of freshwater prawns Macrobrachium rosenbergii under semi-natural conditions (Decapoda, Caridea). Crustaceana 59(2): 193-203. https://doi.org/10.1163/156854090X00660
    » https://doi.org/10.1163/156854090X00660
  • Kawamura G, Bagarinao T, Yong ASK, Jeganathan IMX, Lim LS (2016) Colour preference and colour vision of the larvae of the giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 474: 67-72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2015.10.001
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2015.10.001
  • Kawamura G, Bagarinao T, Yong ASK, Fen TC, Lim LS (2017) Shelter colour preference of the postlarvae of the giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii Fisheries Science 83(2): 259-264. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-017-1062-8
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-017-1062-8
  • Kawamura G, Bagarinao T, Yong ASK, Faisal AB, Lim LS (2018) Limit of colour vision in dim light in larvae of the giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii Fisheries Science 84(2): 365-371. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-018-1179-4
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-018-1179-4
  • Kawamura G, Yong ASK, Wong JS, Tuzan AD, Lim LS (2020) The giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii alters background colour preference after metamorphosis from larvae to postlarvae: In association with nature of phototaxis. Aquaculture Research 51(9): 3711-3717. https://doi.org/10.1111/are.14720
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/are.14720
  • Lind O, Kelber A (2009) Avian colour vision: Effects of variation in receptor sensitivity and noise data on model predictions as compared to behavioural results. Vision Research 49(15): 1939-1947. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.05.003
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.05.003
  • Maia R, Gruson H, Endler JA, White TE (2019) pavo 2: New tools for the spectral and spatial analysis of colour in r. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 10(7): 1097-1107. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.13174
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.13174
  • Mariappan P, Balasundaram C (2003) Sheltering behaviour of Macrobrachium nobilii (Henderson and Matthai, 1910). Acta Ethologica 5(2): 89-94. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10211-002-0069-y
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10211-002-0069-y
  • Matsuda K, Wilder MN (2014) Eye structure and function in the giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii Fisheries Science 80(3): 531-541. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-014-0734-x
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-014-0734-x
  • New MB (2002) Farming freshwater prawns: a manual for the culture of the giant river prawn (Machrobrachium rosenbergii). Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations, Rome , Technical Paper 428. https://www.fao.org/3/y4100e/y4100e.pdf
    » https://www.fao.org/3/y4100e/y4100e.pdf
  • Nilsson D-E (1983) Evolutionary links between apposition and superposition optics in crustacean eyes. Nature 302: 818-821. https://doi.org/10.1038/302818a0
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/302818a0
  • Olsson P, Lind O, Kelber A (2015) Bird colour vision: Behavioural thresholds reveal receptor noise. Journal of Experimental Biology 218(2): 184-193. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.111187
    » https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.111187
  • Olsson P, Lind O, Kelber A (2018) Chromatic and achromatic vision: Parameter choice and limitations for reliable model predictions. Behavioral Ecology 29(2): 273-282. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arx133
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arx133
  • R Core Team (2020) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna. https://www.R-project.org/
    » https://www.R-project.org/
  • Sandifer P, Smith TIJ (1985) Freshwater prawns. In: Huner J, Brown EE (Eds) Crustacean and Mollusk: Aquaculture in the United States. The AVI Publishing Company, Westport, 63-125. https://www.amazon.com/Crustacean-Mollusk-Aquaculture-United-States/dp/1468415050
    » https://www.amazon.com/Crustacean-Mollusk-Aquaculture-United-States/dp/1468415050
  • Santos DB, Pontes CS, Campos PMO, Arruda MF (2015) Behavioral profile of Macrobrachium rosenbergii in mixed and monosex culture submitted to shelter of different colors. Acta Scientiarum, Biological Sciences 37(3): 273-279. https://periodicos.uem.br/ojs/index.php/ActaSciBiolSci/article/view/24656
    » https://periodicos.uem.br/ojs/index.php/ActaSciBiolSci/article/view/24656
  • Siebeck UE, Wallis GM, Litherland L, Ganeshina O, Vorobyev M (2014) Spectral and spatial selectivity of luminance vision in reef fish. Frontiers in Neural Circuits 8: 118. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2014.00118
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2014.00118
  • Silva PF, Arruda MF (2015) Social status and individual behavioral differences in juvenile Macrobrachium rosenbergii Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology 48(1): 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1080/10236244.2014.968424
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/10236244.2014.968424
  • Stoffels RJ (2013) Trophic Ecology. In: Humphries P, Walker K (Eds) Ecology of Australian freshwater fishes. CSIRO Publishing, Melbourn, 131-158. https://www.publish.csiro.au/book/6515/#:~:text=The%20science%20of%20freshwater%20fish,to%20new%20levels%20of%20understanding
    » https://www.publish.csiro.au/book/6515/#:~:text=The%20science%20of%20freshwater%20fish,to%20new%20levels%20of%20understanding
  • Strader ME, Davies SW, Matz MV (2015) Differential responses of coral larvae to the colour of ambient light guide them to suitable settlement microhabitat. Royal Society Open Science 2(10): 150358. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.150358
    » https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.150358
  • Valenti WC, Daniels WH, New MB, Correia ES (2010a) Hatchery Systems and Management. In: New NB, Valenti WC, Tidwell JW, D’abramo LR, Kutty MN (Eds) Freshwater prawns: biology and farming . Wiley Blackwell, Oxford , 55-85.
  • Valenti WC, New MB, Salin KR, Ye J (2010b) Grow-out systems -monoculture. In: New NB, Valenti WC, Tidwell JW, D’abramo LR, Kutty MN (Eds) Freshwater prawns: biology and farming . Wiley Blackwell, Oxford , 154-179.
  • Vorobyev M, Osorio D (1998) Receptor noise as a determinant of colour thresholds. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 265(1394): 351-358. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1998.0302
    » https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1998.0302
  • Yong ASK, Kawamura G, Lim LS, Gwee PX (2018) Growth performance and survival of giant freshwater prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii larvae fed coloured feed. Aquaculture Research 49(8): 2815-2821. https://doi.org/10.1111/are.13744
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/are.13744
  • Zeng C, Cheng Y, Lucas J, Southgate PC (2012) Other decapod crustaceans. In: Lucas JS, Southgate PC (Eds) Aquaculture: farming aquatic animals and plants. Wiley-Blackwell, Oxford, 2nd ed., 514-540. https://www.amazon.com.br/Aquaculture-Farming-Aquatic-Animals-Plants/dp/1405188588
    » https://www.amazon.com.br/Aquaculture-Farming-Aquatic-Animals-Plants/dp/1405188588

ADDITIONAL NOTES

  • Zoobank register

    https://zoobank.org/F4B36A13-9CA7-4F17-8A39-36B91AEB893B
  • How to cite this article

    Costa FP, Arruda MF, Ribeiro K, Pessoa DMA (2022) Influence of colour and brightness on ontogenetic shelter preference by the prawn Macrobrachium rosenbergii (Decapoda: Palaemonidae). Zoologia 40: e22023. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1984-4689.v40.e22023
  • 6
    Published by Sociedade Brasileira de Zoologia at Scientific Electronic Library Online (https://www.scielo.br/zool)

Edited by

Editorial responsibility

Kleber Del Claro

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    13 Mar 2023
  • Date of issue
    2023

History

  • Received
    03 May 2022
  • Accepted
    11 Aug 2022
Sociedade Brasileira de Zoologia Caixa Postal 19020, 81531-980 Curitiba PR Brasil, Tel./Fax: (55 41) 3266-6823 - Curitiba - PR - Brazil
E-mail: sbz@sbzoologia.org.br