Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Mean length utterance in Brazilian children: a comparative study between Down syndrome, specific language impairment, and typical language development

Abstracts

PURPOSE:

To describe the linguistic performance of Brazilian Portuguese-speaking children with Down syndrome by analyzing their Mean Length Utterance; to compare their performance to that of children with Specific Language Impairment and Typical Development; and to verify whether children with Down syndrome present developmental language delay or disorder.

METHOD:

Participants were 25 children with Down syndrome (Research Group), matched by mental age to a Control Group of typically developing children, and to a Control Group of children with Specific Language Impairment. Participants were divided into subgroups, according to age range (three, four and five years). Speech samples were collected for the Research Group, and the Mean Length Utterance was analyzed for morphemes and words.

RESULTS:

Differences were observed between the performance of the Research Group and both Control Groups, and the former presented inferior Mean Length Utterance values for all age ranges, characterizing a delay in grammar and general language development.

CONCLUSION:

The description of the linguistic abilities of Brazilian Portuguese-speaking children with Down syndrome indicated important grammatical deficits, especially regarding the use of functional words.

Down Syndrome; Language development; Child development; Child; Linguistics; Language disorders


OBJETIVO:

Descrever o desempenho linguístico de crianças com síndrome de Down falantes do Português Brasileiro por meio da análise da Extensão Média do Enunciado, comparar esse desempenho ao de crianças com Distúrbio Específico de Linguagem e com Desenvolvimento Típico e verificar se as crianças com síndrome de Down apresentam atrasos ou desvios do desenvolvimento linguístico.

MÉTODO:

Participaram do estudo 25 crianças com síndrome de Down (grupo pesquisa), pareadas pela idade mental ao grupo controle de crianças com desenvolvimento típico e ao grupo controle de crianças com Distúrbio Específico de Linguagem. Os participantes foram divididos em subgrupos de acordo com a faixa etária (três, quatro e cinco anos). Foram colhidas amostras de fala do grupo pesquisa e realizadas análises por meio da Extensão Média do Enunciado medida em morfemas e em palavras.

RESULTADOS:

Observaram-se diferenças de desempenhos entre o grupo pesquisa e os grupos controle, sendo que o primeiro apresentou valores de Extensão Média do Enunciado inferiores em todas as faixas etárias, o que caracteriza o atraso do desenvolvimento gramatical e linguístico geral.

CONCLUSÃO:

A descrição das habilidades linguísticas de crianças com síndrome de Down falantes do Português Brasileiro apontou para déficits gramaticais importantes, principalmente no que se refere ao uso de palavras funcionais.

Síndrome de Down; Desenvolvimento da linguagem; Desenvolvimento infantil; Criança; Linguística; Transtornos da linguagem


INTRODUCTION

Populations with Down syndrome (DS), the most frequent chromosome pathology and the most common genetic cause of intellectual deficiency, present great variability in language development. Studies report that these individuals have linguistic deficits that are reflected on difficulties with all aspects related to language (phonology, pragmatics, semantics, syntax, and morphology). These difficulties are seen as delays instead of language development disorders. Deficits in expressive language are more marked than in receptive language, particularly in morphosyntax( 11. Vicari S, Caselli MC, Tonucci F. Asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic development in children with Down Syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(5):634-44.

2. Abbeduto L, Warren SF, Conners FA. Language development in Down syndrome: from the prelinguistic period to the acquisition of literacy. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2007;13(3):247-61.

3. Caselli MC, Monaco L, Trasciani M, Vicari S. Language in Italian children with Down syndrome and with specific language impairment. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(1):27-35.

4. Martin GE, Klusek J, Estigarribia B, Roberts JE. Language characteristics of individuals with Down Syndrome. Top Lang Disord. 2009;29(2):112-32.

5. Rondal JA. Spoken language in persons with Down Syndrome: a life-span perspective. INT-JECSE. 2009;1(2):138-63.

6. Marques SF, Limongi SCO. A extensão média do enunciado (EME) como medida do desenvolvimento de linguagem de crianças com síndrome de Down. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;23(2):152-7.

7. Kover ST, McDuffie A, Abbeduto L, Brown WT. Effects of sampling context on spontaneous expressive language in males with fragile X syndrome or Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2012;55(4):1022-38.
- 88. Van Bysterveldt AK, Westerveld MF, Gillon G, Foster-Cohen S. Personal narrative skills of school-aged children with Down syndrome. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2012;47(1):95-105. ), which is a clinical marker of language in this population.

With the purpose of explaining and understanding the morphosyntactic difficulties of individuals with DS more clearly, international researchers have conducted increasingly more studies in this area. The difficulties presented by these individuals in regard to morphosyntactic aspects are confirmed by researchers who have used the mean length of utterance (MLU) in their studies.

The MLU was proposed as an index to measure and describe the grammatical and morphological development of children undergoing typical development (TD), as well as of those with language impairment( 99. Brown R. A first language: the early stages. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1973. ). This measurement is conducted based on the analysis of speech samples, and it is used and widely accepted as the most effective technique to be used in quantitative assessments of language development in children, both in research studies and in the clinical sphere. It can be measured through morphemes (MLU-m) or words (MLU-w)( 11. Vicari S, Caselli MC, Tonucci F. Asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic development in children with Down Syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(5):634-44. , 77. Kover ST, McDuffie A, Abbeduto L, Brown WT. Effects of sampling context on spontaneous expressive language in males with fragile X syndrome or Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2012;55(4):1022-38. , 1010. Araújo K. Aspectos do desempenho gramatical de crianças pré-escolares em desenvolvimento normal de linguagem [dissertação]. São Paulo: Faculdade de Filosofia, Letras e Ciências Humanas da Universidade de São Paulo; 2003.

11. Parker MD, Brorson KA. A comparative study between mean length of utterance in morphemes (MLU-m) and mean length utterance in words (MLU-w). First Language. 2005;25(3):365- 76.
- 1212. Araujo K. Desempenho gramatical de criança em desenvolvimento normal e com distúrbio específico de linguagem [tese]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2007. ). In the case of Brazilian Portuguese (BP), the MLU is measured by adding type-1 grammatical morphemes (GM-1), related to articles, nouns, and verbs, as well as by adding type-2 grammatical morphemes (GM-2), related to pronouns, prepositions, and conjunctions. These criteria were proposed on consideration of the linguistic differences between BP and English( 1010. Araújo K. Aspectos do desempenho gramatical de crianças pré-escolares em desenvolvimento normal de linguagem [dissertação]. São Paulo: Faculdade de Filosofia, Letras e Ciências Humanas da Universidade de São Paulo; 2003. ). BP is a richer and more inflected language than English. In this sense, it is comparable to Italian, a language that is more difficult to be mastered( 11. Vicari S, Caselli MC, Tonucci F. Asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic development in children with Down Syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(5):634-44. , 33. Caselli MC, Monaco L, Trasciani M, Vicari S. Language in Italian children with Down syndrome and with specific language impairment. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(1):27-35. ).

The MLU is also considered an effective and reliable tool to assess the aforementioned aspects in the DS population( 11. Vicari S, Caselli MC, Tonucci F. Asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic development in children with Down Syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(5):634-44. , 1313. Berglund E, Eriksson M, Johansson I. Parental reports of spoken language skills in children with Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2001;44(1):179-91.

14. Eadie PA, Fey ME, Douglas JM, Parsons CL. Profiles of grammatical morphology and sentence imitation in children with specific language impairment and Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2002;45(4):720-32.

15. Chapman RS. Language learning in Down syndrome: the speech and language profile compared to adolescents with cognitive impairment of unknown origin. Downs Syndr Res Pract. 2006;10(2):61-6.
- 1616. Laws G, Bishop DV. A comparison of language abilities in adolescents with Down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2003;46(6):1324-39. ). The results of studies in which this tool was used present frequent omissions of grammatical morphemes as the main morphosyntactic deficit, particularly of functional words( 11. Vicari S, Caselli MC, Tonucci F. Asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic development in children with Down Syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(5):634-44. , 44. Martin GE, Klusek J, Estigarribia B, Roberts JE. Language characteristics of individuals with Down Syndrome. Top Lang Disord. 2009;29(2):112-32. , 55. Rondal JA. Spoken language in persons with Down Syndrome: a life-span perspective. INT-JECSE. 2009;1(2):138-63. , 1313. Berglund E, Eriksson M, Johansson I. Parental reports of spoken language skills in children with Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2001;44(1):179-91. ).

A scarcity of studies in which the MLU is considered in children with DS is noticeable in the literature on this topic in Brazil. This may be due to a difficulty to find large and homogeneous samples, to the great variability observed in their language performances, to the lack of standardized tests, and to the lack of knowledge about this tool on the part of speech-language pathologists( 66. Marques SF, Limongi SCO. A extensão média do enunciado (EME) como medida do desenvolvimento de linguagem de crianças com síndrome de Down. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;23(2):152-7. ).

The literature presents studies in which the authors compare the language performance of children with DS and specific language impairment (SLI)( 33. Caselli MC, Monaco L, Trasciani M, Vicari S. Language in Italian children with Down syndrome and with specific language impairment. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(1):27-35. , 1414. Eadie PA, Fey ME, Douglas JM, Parsons CL. Profiles of grammatical morphology and sentence imitation in children with specific language impairment and Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2002;45(4):720-32. , 1616. Laws G, Bishop DV. A comparison of language abilities in adolescents with Down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2003;46(6):1324-39. ). The researchers point to similarities between the performances of both populations concerning morphological aspects, as both present poorer performances than children with TD of the same mental age (MA). They also point to differences, considered subtle, in the omission or incorrect production of morphemes.

Considering the scarcity of studies with a focus on BP in relation to the language abilities of children with DS that provide parameters for scientific studies and clinical practice, in this study our purpose was to describe the language development of children with DS by using the MLU-m, considering GM-1 and GM-2, as well as the MLU-w; to compare the linguistic performance of these children to the performances of children with TD and SLI; and to verify whether the children with DS presented delays or disorders in their language development, based on the children with TD.

METHODS

This study was approved by the ethics committee of the institution in question (protocol number 1004/08). The participants' parents or legal guardians signed the Informed Consent form.

Three groups of individuals comprised this study's sample, each with 25 participants who spoke BP: one study group with children with DS (SG-DS) and two control groups (CG), one control group that counted children with TD (CG-TD), and another with children with SLI (CG-SLI).

The data pertaining to both CG were obtained from a previously conducted study( 1212. Araujo K. Desempenho gramatical de criança em desenvolvimento normal e com distúrbio específico de linguagem [tese]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2007. ), in which the inclusion criteria were as follows:

1. For the CG-TD: presenting performances adequate to chronological age (CA) in a speech and language triage( 1717. Fluharty NB. Fluharty preschool speech and language screening tests. Austin: Pro-Ed, 1978. ); having no complaints and no previous submission to any intervention related to the areas of speech-language pathology and audiology, psychology, or neurology; being a speaker of BP exposed only to this language; attending a day-care center subsidized by the city of São Paulo;

2. For the CG-SLI: presenting performances that were poorer than expected for the CA on a speech and language assessment that addressed vocabulary, phonology, pragmatics( 1818. Andrade CRF, Béfi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner HF. ABFW: teste de linguagem Infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. Barueri: Pró-Fono, 2004. ), fluency, and discursive ability (Frog, where are you?) when orality was sufficient; relying on oral language as the predominant mode of communication( 1919. Fernandes FDM. Pragmática. In: Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner HF. ABFW - Teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. Barueri: Pró-Fono, 2004. p.83-97. ); speaking intelligibly enough so that speech could be transcribed( 2020. Wertzner HF. Fonologia. In: Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner HF. ABFW - Teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. Barueri: Pró-Fono. 2004. p.5-40. ); having normal hearing, confirmed by an audiological assessment; being under treatment at the Laboratory for Investigation of Language Development and Its Alterations (LIF-DLA) for a period between 6 and 18 months; attending a day-care center subsidized by the city of São Paulo.

Concerning the assessment of the participants' intelligence quotient, the children in the CG-SLI were not formally evaluated by a qualified professional at the time of the study because it can only be conducted after 5 years of age. Thus, as it is reported in the literature, each individual's cognitive ability was estimated through an assessment of symbolic maturity.

We considered the following inclusion criteria for the DS group:

1. Relying on oral language as the predominant mode of communication, determined by a Test of Pragmatic Language( 1919. Fernandes FDM. Pragmática. In: Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner HF. ABFW - Teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. Barueri: Pró-Fono, 2004. p.83-97. );

2. Being in the preoperational stage of cognitive development, determined by an Assessment of Language and Cognition( 2121. Limongi SCO, Carvallo RMM, Souza ER. Auditory processing and language in Down syndrome. J Med Speech Lang Pathol. 2000;8(1):27-34. );

3. Speaking intelligibly enough so that enunciation could be transcribed, determined by a Speech and Language Test( 2020. Wertzner HF. Fonologia. In: Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner HF. ABFW - Teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. Barueri: Pró-Fono. 2004. p.5-40. ) and intelligible utterance of at least 50% of the words( 2222. Wertzner HF. Fonologia: desenvolvimento e alterações. In: Ferreira LP, Befi-Lopes DM, Limongi SCO (orgs.). Tratado de Fonoaudiologia. 2ª ed. São Paulo: Editora Roca, 2010. p.281-90. );

4. Presenting simple trisomy karyotype of chromosome 21;

5. Being under speech, language, and audiological treatment for at least 1 year;

6. Having no comorbidities, such as moderate to severe conductive or neurosensory hearing loss; visual deficiency; severe cardiomyopathy that required surgery; and psychological and/or psychiatric conditions;

7. Having undergone physical therapy until achieving independent and stable gait;

8. Attending a municipal or state preschool or regular school in the state of São Paulo for at least 2 years.

The last five criteria were obtained from the participants' medical charts.

In the CG-TD and CG-SLI, the individuals' age ranged from 3 to 5 years and 11 months of CA. The groups were divided into three subgroups based on age (3, 4, and 5 years). In the SG-DS, the individuals' age ranged from 5 to 9 years and 11 months of CA. This group was divided in subgroups based on MA, obtained through the application of the Primary Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (PTONI)( 2323. Ehrler DJ, Mcghee RL. Primary Test of Nonverbal Intelligence - PTONI. Texas: Pro-Ed, 2008. ).

It is worth highlighting that we used retrospective data about previously studied populations (TD and SLI) that do not have intellectual disabilities. Thus, the PTONI was used to obtain the MA, necessary when pairing the participants from the CG-TD and the CG-SLI with the purpose of exploring developmental relations among linguistic abilities( 2424. Thomas MS, Annaz D, Ansari D, Scerif G, Jarrold C, Karmiloff-Smith A. Using developmental trajectories to understand developmental disorders. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2009;52(2):336-58. ).

We paired each individual in the SG-DS to participants in the CG-TD and CG-SLI according to the MA of the children with DS and a difference of 1 month and over in relation to the children with DS. We highlight that, in regard to this pairing, the CAs in both CG ranged between 3 and 5 years, which calls for a remark about the CG-SLI. An SLI diagnosis can only be conducted after 5 years of age in children with a history of a language impairment that persists after language rehabilitation. Up until this age, the best term to designate these children is specific language alteration, as it is possible that children with language delays will be part of this group( 2525. Reed AV. An introduction to children with language disorders. 2a ed. New York: McMillan, 1994. p.95-116. ). However, because the term SLI is used in the international literature, in the present study the terminology SLI will be maintained.

We recorded 30 minutes of interaction between the researcher and each participant. In these sessions, the researcher used materials that enabled symbolic play and that were adequate for the cognitive developmental stage of the participants. The data pertaining to the first 100 enunciations were recorded after the first five initial minutes of interaction were discarded, as this was considered the child's period of adaptation to being observed. This resulted in 2.5 thousand enunciations. The criteria used to transcribe and analyze the speech samples to obtain the MLU were the same as those proposed and adopted in previous studies( 99. Brown R. A first language: the early stages. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1973. , 1212. Araujo K. Desempenho gramatical de criança em desenvolvimento normal e com distúrbio específico de linguagem [tese]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2007. ).

In the statistical analyses, we used a descriptive analysis to obtain the descriptive measures of the variables analyzed (GM-1, GM-2, MLU-m, MLU-w); Kolmogorov-Smirnov test to compare the age ranges and verify data normality; Levene test to observe the homogeneity of the variances; and one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) to be compared between the age ranges (3-5 years) in regard to each variable (GM-1, GM-2, MLU-m, MLU-w) in each group (TD, SLI, and DS) separately. Tukey's post hoc test was used in case significant differences were found between the groups and age ranges. Mixed ANOVAs were carried out for each age range, separately, to investigate possible differences between the groups. When statistically significant differences were found, the analyses were calculated by means of contrasts and graphics with confidence intervals. The level of significance adopted was 0.05 (5%).

To ensure the reliability of the analysis of the recorded data related to the SG-DS, we submitted 20% of the speech samples, drawn randomly, to a compatibility analysis performed by two judges, one M.Sc. and one Ph.D. in speech-language pathology and audiology, with experience in language and children with DS and in the methodology used in this study. Their analyses yielded 85 and 88% of concordance, respectively.

RESULTS

Table 1 displays the descriptive analysis of the variables GM-1, GM-2, MLU-m, and MLU-w in the SG-DS, CG-TD, and CG-SLI.

Table 1
Descriptive analysis of the variables type 1 grammatical morphemes, type 2 grammatical morphemes, mean lenght utterances for morphemes, mean length utterances for words for the study group Down syndrome, control group typical development and control group specific language impairment

It is observable that the averages increased as age advanced in the three groups analyzed and that this increase occurred more markedly among the children with TD, who always achieved the highest averages, followed by those with SLI and those with DS, respectively.

An exception to this increase in average as age progresses was observed in the 4-year age range of the SG-DS in regard to the GM-1 and to the MLU-m and MLU-w, which maintained the same average of the 3-year age range. Regarding the GM-2 of the 5-year-old participants of the CG-SLI, we verified a slight average decrease compared to the 5-year age range ones.

It is also observable that an increase in the average of the variables was always more expressive in the 4- and 5-year age ranges in the SG-DS and CG-TD.

On comparison of the averages among the age ranges of the groups, the 5-year-old participants in the SG-DS presented values close to those obtained by the 3-year-old individuals in the CG-SLI and lower than the 3-year-olds in the CG-TD, in all variables.

Table 2 presents a comparison between the age ranges (3-5 years) in relation to each variable (GM-1, GM-2, MLU-m, and MLU-w) in the CG-TD.

Table 2
Comparison of each variable among the age ranges (3-5 years) in the control group with typical development

Statistically significant differences were verified between the age ranges for all variables. Tukey's post hoc test was applied. The results indicate that in the CG-TD, statistically significant differences were found concerning GM-1 and MLU-w only between the 3- and 5-year age range (p=0.000 for both variables) and between the 4- and 5-year age range (p=0.000 for both variables). The same occurred in the case of GM-2 and MLU-m in all age ranges (3X4, p=0.003; 3X5, p=0.000; 4X5, p=0.020; and 3X4, p=0.021; 3X5, p=0.000; 4X5, p=0.000, respectively).

Table 3 displays the comparison between the age ranges (3-5 years) in relation to each variable (GM-1, GM-2, MLU-m, and MLU-w) in the CG-SLI.

Table 3
Comparison of each variable among the age ranges (3-5 years) in the control group with specific language impairment

We observed statistically significant differences among the age ranges only in regard to the variable GM-2. Tukey's post hoc test, calculated only for GM-2, indicates that statistically significant differences were verified only between the 3- and 4-year age ranges (p=0.017).

Table 4 shows the comparison between the age ranges (3-5 years) in relation to each variable (GM-1, GM-2, MLU-m, and MLU-w) in the SG-DS.

Table 4
Comparison of each variable among the age ranges (3-5 years) in the study group with Down syndrome

Statistically significant differences were observed among the age ranges in relation to all variables. Tukey's post hoc test was applied. On the basis of the results for the SG-DS, statistically significant differences can be observed for all variables between the 3- and 5-year age ranges, and between the 4- and 5-year age ranges (GM-1: 3X5, p=0.016; 4X5, p=0.006; GM-2: 3X5, p=0.013; 4X5, p=0.048; MLU-m: 3X5, p=0.005; 4X5, p=0.003; MLU-w: 3X5, p=0.001; 4X5, p=0.000).

Table 5 displays the comparison among the groups (TD, SLI, and DS) regarding the three age ranges.

Table 5
Comparison of the three age ranges among the groups

Statistically significant differences were verified among the groups (TD, SLI, and DS), which indicates that the values of GM-1, GM-2, MLU-m, and MLU-w vary depending on the group analyzed.

DISCUSSION

In studies on language development of children with DS whose authors have used MLU as a way to assess the development of language skills or as a method to pair groups, the participants are organized by MA, vocabulary, or lexical or morphosyntactic development( 11. Vicari S, Caselli MC, Tonucci F. Asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic development in children with Down Syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(5):634-44. , 77. Kover ST, McDuffie A, Abbeduto L, Brown WT. Effects of sampling context on spontaneous expressive language in males with fragile X syndrome or Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2012;55(4):1022-38. , 88. Van Bysterveldt AK, Westerveld MF, Gillon G, Foster-Cohen S. Personal narrative skills of school-aged children with Down syndrome. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2012;47(1):95-105. , 1313. Berglund E, Eriksson M, Johansson I. Parental reports of spoken language skills in children with Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2001;44(1):179-91.

14. Eadie PA, Fey ME, Douglas JM, Parsons CL. Profiles of grammatical morphology and sentence imitation in children with specific language impairment and Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2002;45(4):720-32.
- 1515. Chapman RS. Language learning in Down syndrome: the speech and language profile compared to adolescents with cognitive impairment of unknown origin. Downs Syndr Res Pract. 2006;10(2):61-6. ). It is worth highlighting that these researchers rely on standardized tests, especially for the English language, which does not occur in the case of BP.

In Brazil, studies that approach the language skills of children with DS through MLU have been initiated, and the first results point to the efficacy of this tool in the population in question( 66. Marques SF, Limongi SCO. A extensão média do enunciado (EME) como medida do desenvolvimento de linguagem de crianças com síndrome de Down. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;23(2):152-7. , 2626. Araújo K, Muhler LP, Telles P, Surian AC, Befi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FD, et al. Extensão média de enunciados de crianças com distúrbio específico de linguagem, síndrome de Down e do espectro autístico [resumo]. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2006; Suplemento especial. ). As reported in one of these studies( 66. Marques SF, Limongi SCO. A extensão média do enunciado (EME) como medida do desenvolvimento de linguagem de crianças com síndrome de Down. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;23(2):152-7. ), the results obtained in the present study show that MLU-m and MLU-w can be considered reliable and efficacious measures to point out the index to be used to describe the grammar and language development of children with DS who speak BP. This fact is corroborated by studies conducted on SLI and TD in Brazil( 1010. Araújo K. Aspectos do desempenho gramatical de crianças pré-escolares em desenvolvimento normal de linguagem [dissertação]. São Paulo: Faculdade de Filosofia, Letras e Ciências Humanas da Universidade de São Paulo; 2003. , 1212. Araujo K. Desempenho gramatical de criança em desenvolvimento normal e com distúrbio específico de linguagem [tese]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2007. ), as well as in other countries( 33. Caselli MC, Monaco L, Trasciani M, Vicari S. Language in Italian children with Down syndrome and with specific language impairment. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(1):27-35. , 1515. Chapman RS. Language learning in Down syndrome: the speech and language profile compared to adolescents with cognitive impairment of unknown origin. Downs Syndr Res Pract. 2006;10(2):61-6. , 2727. Leonard LB, Eyer JA, Bedore LM, Grela BG. Three accounts of the grammatical morpheme difficulties of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1997;40(4):741-53. , 2828. Rice ML, Redmond SM, Hoffman L. Mean length of utterance in children with specific language impairment and in younger control children shows concurrent validity and stable and parallel growth trajectories. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2006;49(4):793-808. ).

The MLU-m described grammar development because it refers to the use of both GM-1 and GM-2, which occurred not only in relation to an increase in the quantity of words in the participants' vocabulary, but also to the use of morphemes that indicate their inflection, that is, to morphosyntactic knowledge. As pointed out in the literature, the acquisition of inflectional morphemes is influenced by aspects such as the frequency with which they occur in the language and environment exposed to the child, the load of semantic information, and phonological structure( 11. Vicari S, Caselli MC, Tonucci F. Asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic development in children with Down Syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(5):634-44. , 99. Brown R. A first language: the early stages. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1973.

10. Araújo K. Aspectos do desempenho gramatical de crianças pré-escolares em desenvolvimento normal de linguagem [dissertação]. São Paulo: Faculdade de Filosofia, Letras e Ciências Humanas da Universidade de São Paulo; 2003.

11. Parker MD, Brorson KA. A comparative study between mean length of utterance in morphemes (MLU-m) and mean length utterance in words (MLU-w). First Language. 2005;25(3):365- 76.
- 1212. Araujo K. Desempenho gramatical de criança em desenvolvimento normal e com distúrbio específico de linguagem [tese]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2007. , 2929. Balason DV, Dollaghan CA. Grammatical morphemes production in four-year-old children. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2002;45(5):961-9. ). MLU-w, which refers to lexical information beyond the grammatical classes of MLU-m (articles, nouns, verbs, pronouns, prepositions, and conjunctions) and encompasses adverbs, adjectives, numbers, and interjections, fulfilled the purpose of describing and comparing the overall language development of the participants( 11. Vicari S, Caselli MC, Tonucci F. Asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic development in children with Down Syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(5):634-44. , 1010. Araújo K. Aspectos do desempenho gramatical de crianças pré-escolares em desenvolvimento normal de linguagem [dissertação]. São Paulo: Faculdade de Filosofia, Letras e Ciências Humanas da Universidade de São Paulo; 2003.

11. Parker MD, Brorson KA. A comparative study between mean length of utterance in morphemes (MLU-m) and mean length utterance in words (MLU-w). First Language. 2005;25(3):365- 76.
- 1212. Araujo K. Desempenho gramatical de criança em desenvolvimento normal e com distúrbio específico de linguagem [tese]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2007. , 2828. Rice ML, Redmond SM, Hoffman L. Mean length of utterance in children with specific language impairment and in younger control children shows concurrent validity and stable and parallel growth trajectories. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2006;49(4):793-808. ).

In this study, the children with DS reached averages pertaining to the variables GM-1, GM-2, MLU-m, and MLU-w that differentiated them from the participants in the CG and were lower than those of the CG-SLI, which, in turn, also presented lower averages than the CG-TD. The differences among the TD, SLI, and DS groups were confirmed through a comparative analysis, which indicated that the values of the variables GM-1, GM-2, MLU-m, and MLU-w varied depending on the group and age range analyzed. Expressive language deficits are shown in DS by an MLU that is poorer than expected, based both on CA and MA, or poorer than that of control individuals paired by MA( 11. Vicari S, Caselli MC, Tonucci F. Asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic development in children with Down Syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(5):634-44. , 33. Caselli MC, Monaco L, Trasciani M, Vicari S. Language in Italian children with Down syndrome and with specific language impairment. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(1):27-35.

4. Martin GE, Klusek J, Estigarribia B, Roberts JE. Language characteristics of individuals with Down Syndrome. Top Lang Disord. 2009;29(2):112-32.

5. Rondal JA. Spoken language in persons with Down Syndrome: a life-span perspective. INT-JECSE. 2009;1(2):138-63.
- 66. Marques SF, Limongi SCO. A extensão média do enunciado (EME) como medida do desenvolvimento de linguagem de crianças com síndrome de Down. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;23(2):152-7. , 1313. Berglund E, Eriksson M, Johansson I. Parental reports of spoken language skills in children with Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2001;44(1):179-91.

14. Eadie PA, Fey ME, Douglas JM, Parsons CL. Profiles of grammatical morphology and sentence imitation in children with specific language impairment and Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2002;45(4):720-32.
- 1515. Chapman RS. Language learning in Down syndrome: the speech and language profile compared to adolescents with cognitive impairment of unknown origin. Downs Syndr Res Pract. 2006;10(2):61-6. ).

The poorer performance of the SG-DS compared to both control groups (TD and SLI) is related to difficulties in regard to the morphosyntactic aspects presented by individuals with DS. Although they acquire GM-1 words (articles, nouns, and verbs), these children have difficulty to acquire and use the necessary inflections, such as morphemes that mark the number, gender, and case of nouns, as well as the tense, person, and mood of verbs, in addition to articles. Difficulties in acquisition and use are also verified in words with more syntactic information (GM-2) that work as relational elements, such as pronouns, prepositions, and conjunctions. The less frequent use of these words leads to the production of simple telegraphic sentences( 11. Vicari S, Caselli MC, Tonucci F. Asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic development in children with Down Syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(5):634-44. , 66. Marques SF, Limongi SCO. A extensão média do enunciado (EME) como medida do desenvolvimento de linguagem de crianças com síndrome de Down. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;23(2):152-7. , 1313. Berglund E, Eriksson M, Johansson I. Parental reports of spoken language skills in children with Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2001;44(1):179-91. ).

Studies in the literature corroborate the difficulties shown by the participants with DS in the present study concerning the use of functional words, especially in languages more grammatically complex, highly inflected, and Latin based, which is the case of BP( 11. Vicari S, Caselli MC, Tonucci F. Asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic development in children with Down Syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(5):634-44. , 33. Caselli MC, Monaco L, Trasciani M, Vicari S. Language in Italian children with Down syndrome and with specific language impairment. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(1):27-35. , 66. Marques SF, Limongi SCO. A extensão média do enunciado (EME) como medida do desenvolvimento de linguagem de crianças com síndrome de Down. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;23(2):152-7. , 1313. Berglund E, Eriksson M, Johansson I. Parental reports of spoken language skills in children with Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2001;44(1):179-91. ).

The findings of this study show that children with DS had poorer performances than those with TD in regard to all variables studied, especially in older ages. As it happens in the Brazilian population with SLI( 1212. Araujo K. Desempenho gramatical de criança em desenvolvimento normal e com distúrbio específico de linguagem [tese]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2007. ), children with DS experience the establishment of a more persistent grammatical difficulty to form morphological rules as age progresses. It is considered that children with DS are unable to generalize the knowledge of these rules and the use of lexical items that do not have fixed characteristics, such as pronouns, prepositions, and conjunctions( 11. Vicari S, Caselli MC, Tonucci F. Asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic development in children with Down Syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(5):634-44. , 33. Caselli MC, Monaco L, Trasciani M, Vicari S. Language in Italian children with Down syndrome and with specific language impairment. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(1):27-35. , 66. Marques SF, Limongi SCO. A extensão média do enunciado (EME) como medida do desenvolvimento de linguagem de crianças com síndrome de Down. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;23(2):152-7. , 1313. Berglund E, Eriksson M, Johansson I. Parental reports of spoken language skills in children with Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2001;44(1):179-91. ).

In two studies carried out with children who spoke English, the authors compared the language skills of children with DS, TD, and SLI paired by MLU-m( 1414. Eadie PA, Fey ME, Douglas JM, Parsons CL. Profiles of grammatical morphology and sentence imitation in children with specific language impairment and Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2002;45(4):720-32. ) and nonverbal cognitive abilities( 1616. Laws G, Bishop DV. A comparison of language abilities in adolescents with Down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2003;46(6):1324-39. ). The results did not yield any statistically significant differences between the SLI and DS groups, and the performances of both groups were poorer than those of individuals with TD. Despite the similarities, subtle differences were observed, such as, the fact that the children with SLI omitted more verbal inflections, whereas those with DS produced more incorrect forms( 1616. Laws G, Bishop DV. A comparison of language abilities in adolescents with Down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2003;46(6):1324-39. ).

Differently than these works, the results of a study carried out with children with DS, SLI, and TD who spoke Italian( 33. Caselli MC, Monaco L, Trasciani M, Vicari S. Language in Italian children with Down syndrome and with specific language impairment. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(1):27-35. ), paired individually based on MA (ages ranging from 3 years and 8 months to 5 years and 7 months), showed that the children with DS had poorer performances in morphosyntactic production than those with SLI and those with TD. These findings point to greater differences between the DS and SLI groups than in English language studies. This fact can be explained by the morphosyntactic demands of Italian, which is more complex grammatically and can, therefore, pose more difficulties. These characteristics can show differences in the linguistic profile of children with various pathologies.

In this sense, considering the complexity of BP, as well as of the other Romance languages (French, Italian, Romanian, and Spanish), also highly inflected, it is possible to verify that the children with DS who participated in this study had poorer performances than the individuals with SLI in regard to morphosyntactic abilities, as reported in the literature( 44. Martin GE, Klusek J, Estigarribia B, Roberts JE. Language characteristics of individuals with Down Syndrome. Top Lang Disord. 2009;29(2):112-32. ). The more marked difficulty shown by the children with DS speakers of BP concerning morphosyntactic aspects, in comparison to those with SLI, can be justified by the fact that the Portuguese language is relatively richer and more grammatically marked compared to English, and it is therefore more difficult to be mastered. This finding has also been reported in studies on children with TD( 1010. Araújo K. Aspectos do desempenho gramatical de crianças pré-escolares em desenvolvimento normal de linguagem [dissertação]. São Paulo: Faculdade de Filosofia, Letras e Ciências Humanas da Universidade de São Paulo; 2003. ) and SLI( 1212. Araujo K. Desempenho gramatical de criança em desenvolvimento normal e com distúrbio específico de linguagem [tese]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2007. ) whose MLU presented lower values than those found in studies carried out in English.

It is pointed out in the literature( 1010. Araújo K. Aspectos do desempenho gramatical de crianças pré-escolares em desenvolvimento normal de linguagem [dissertação]. São Paulo: Faculdade de Filosofia, Letras e Ciências Humanas da Universidade de São Paulo; 2003. ) that, as age progresses, children with TD use grammatical morphemes more frequently. They also manipulate and combine linguistic structures more easily and quickly. The same can be observed in regard to DS, but the MA factor cannot be disregarded.

This finding is confirmed by the increase in the averages of the variables GM-1, GM-2, MLU-m, and MLU-w between the 4- and 5-year age ranges, except in regard to GM-2, in which this increase was observed in all age ranges. These data agree with studies conducted abroad( 11. Vicari S, Caselli MC, Tonucci F. Asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic development in children with Down Syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(5):634-44. , 33. Caselli MC, Monaco L, Trasciani M, Vicari S. Language in Italian children with Down syndrome and with specific language impairment. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(1):27-35. , 1313. Berglund E, Eriksson M, Johansson I. Parental reports of spoken language skills in children with Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2001;44(1):179-91. ) and in Brazil( 66. Marques SF, Limongi SCO. A extensão média do enunciado (EME) como medida do desenvolvimento de linguagem de crianças com síndrome de Down. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;23(2):152-7. ), which report an increase in MLU as individuals with DS become older.

The findings concerning the CG-TD with Brazilian children are corroborated by other studies in the international literature( 1111. Parker MD, Brorson KA. A comparative study between mean length of utterance in morphemes (MLU-m) and mean length utterance in words (MLU-w). First Language. 2005;25(3):365- 76. , 2828. Rice ML, Redmond SM, Hoffman L. Mean length of utterance in children with specific language impairment and in younger control children shows concurrent validity and stable and parallel growth trajectories. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2006;49(4):793-808. , 3030. Thordardottir ET. Early lexical and syntactic development in Quebec French and English: implications for cross-linguistic and bilingual assessment. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2005;40(3):243-78. ). It is worth highlighting that in the 4- and 5-year age ranges, the increase in the averages of the variables was more marked, except for GM-2, in which the children presented a more evident increase between the 3- and 4-year age ranges. In another study carried out with Brazilian children( 1010. Araújo K. Aspectos do desempenho gramatical de crianças pré-escolares em desenvolvimento normal de linguagem [dissertação]. São Paulo: Faculdade de Filosofia, Letras e Ciências Humanas da Universidade de São Paulo; 2003. ), the authors state that these individuals use a reduced number of morphemes in their early years. As age progresses, they deal with linguistic structures more easily and quickly. In this sense, the authors of a study with preschool Brazilian children between 2 and 4 years of age verified an instability in verbal morphology and pointed out that, in regard to the nominal inflection of numbers, productive use occurs at 5 years of age.

Concerning the Brazilian children with SLI, an increase in the averages of the variables according to the progression of age was also observed, more markedly between 3 and 4 years. Concerning the variable GM-2, an increase in average was verified only between 3 and 4 years. An international( 2828. Rice ML, Redmond SM, Hoffman L. Mean length of utterance in children with specific language impairment and in younger control children shows concurrent validity and stable and parallel growth trajectories. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2006;49(4):793-808. ) and a Brazilian study( 1212. Araujo K. Desempenho gramatical de criança em desenvolvimento normal e com distúrbio específico de linguagem [tese]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2007. ) point out the difficulty of this population to use GM-2 in older ages.

Thus, we observed that the children with DS in this study had more similarities with the CG-DT regarding the more advanced morphosyntactic development in older ages, despite the linguistic gap between both groups, than with the CG-SLI.

In regard to all the variables analyzed (GM-1, GM-2, MLU-m, and MLU-w), in the 5-year age range the values achieved by the SG-DS were close to those found in the 3-year age range of the CG-SLI and lower than the 3-year age range of the CG-TD, which points to more expressive language difficulties in children with DS( 11. Vicari S, Caselli MC, Tonucci F. Asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic development in children with Down Syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(5):634-44.

2. Abbeduto L, Warren SF, Conners FA. Language development in Down syndrome: from the prelinguistic period to the acquisition of literacy. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2007;13(3):247-61.
- 33. Caselli MC, Monaco L, Trasciani M, Vicari S. Language in Italian children with Down syndrome and with specific language impairment. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(1):27-35. ).

These difficulties, along with the similar morphosyntactic development of the SG-DS and CG-TD, especially when the 4- and 5-year age ranges are compared, indicate language delay, not impairment. It is considered that delays are any similarities between a study group and a control group(s) in terms of overall proficiency or error typology on a language test( 2828. Rice ML, Redmond SM, Hoffman L. Mean length of utterance in children with specific language impairment and in younger control children shows concurrent validity and stable and parallel growth trajectories. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2006;49(4):793-808. ). Although the language delays observed in the population with DS vary, they follow a characteristic profile( 55. Rondal JA. Spoken language in persons with Down Syndrome: a life-span perspective. INT-JECSE. 2009;1(2):138-63. ). These individuals have difficulties to deal simultaneously with communicative intention, semantic content, pragmatics, lexical selection, morphosyntactic markers, and speech rules. These results agree with authors who affirm that individuals with DS present language development delays and not impairments( 22. Abbeduto L, Warren SF, Conners FA. Language development in Down syndrome: from the prelinguistic period to the acquisition of literacy. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2007;13(3):247-61.

3. Caselli MC, Monaco L, Trasciani M, Vicari S. Language in Italian children with Down syndrome and with specific language impairment. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(1):27-35.

4. Martin GE, Klusek J, Estigarribia B, Roberts JE. Language characteristics of individuals with Down Syndrome. Top Lang Disord. 2009;29(2):112-32.

5. Rondal JA. Spoken language in persons with Down Syndrome: a life-span perspective. INT-JECSE. 2009;1(2):138-63.
- 66. Marques SF, Limongi SCO. A extensão média do enunciado (EME) como medida do desenvolvimento de linguagem de crianças com síndrome de Down. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;23(2):152-7. ).

In international studies, the focus of researchers is the language development of individuals with DS during childhood and adolescence( 11. Vicari S, Caselli MC, Tonucci F. Asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic development in children with Down Syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(5):634-44. , 33. Caselli MC, Monaco L, Trasciani M, Vicari S. Language in Italian children with Down syndrome and with specific language impairment. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(1):27-35. , 1515. Chapman RS. Language learning in Down syndrome: the speech and language profile compared to adolescents with cognitive impairment of unknown origin. Downs Syndr Res Pract. 2006;10(2):61-6. ). They affirm that the cognitive deficit present in this population justifies the less frequent use of words that work as relational elements (GM-2). For the authors in question, this fact influences the acquisition and oral expression of these linguistic elements, as they consider the importance of cognitive development for language development and its structuration. In this sense, our findings could characterize the linguistic differences between the children in the SG-DS and those in the CG-TD and CG-SLI.

While conducting this study, we carefully considered any factors that could potentially influence the results, to ensure that our purpose, to describe the language development of children with DS through MLU-m, was fulfilled. Thus, we considered the variables MA, schooling, type of school attended (public), and therapeutic interventions, as described in the criteria for selection of the participants.

CONCLUSION

In this study, the children with DS, speakers of BP, showed important grammatical deficits characterized by delays in overall language development. Despite these delays, we observed that morphosyntactic abilities were acquired as age progressed, especially between 4 and 5 years of MA. The MLU proved to be a reliable and efficacious tool to identify the grammatical and language development of the population with DS, which confirms the validity of using this index. Studies with larger populations of individuals with DS are necessary, in spite of their great interindividual variability, to provide more representative data both for the scientific area and for evidence-based clinical practice.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Vicari S, Caselli MC, Tonucci F. Asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic development in children with Down Syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(5):634-44.
  • 2
    Abbeduto L, Warren SF, Conners FA. Language development in Down syndrome: from the prelinguistic period to the acquisition of literacy. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2007;13(3):247-61.
  • 3
    Caselli MC, Monaco L, Trasciani M, Vicari S. Language in Italian children with Down syndrome and with specific language impairment. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(1):27-35.
  • 4
    Martin GE, Klusek J, Estigarribia B, Roberts JE. Language characteristics of individuals with Down Syndrome. Top Lang Disord. 2009;29(2):112-32.
  • 5
    Rondal JA. Spoken language in persons with Down Syndrome: a life-span perspective. INT-JECSE. 2009;1(2):138-63.
  • 6
    Marques SF, Limongi SCO. A extensão média do enunciado (EME) como medida do desenvolvimento de linguagem de crianças com síndrome de Down. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;23(2):152-7.
  • 7
    Kover ST, McDuffie A, Abbeduto L, Brown WT. Effects of sampling context on spontaneous expressive language in males with fragile X syndrome or Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2012;55(4):1022-38.
  • 8
    Van Bysterveldt AK, Westerveld MF, Gillon G, Foster-Cohen S. Personal narrative skills of school-aged children with Down syndrome. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2012;47(1):95-105.
  • 9
    Brown R. A first language: the early stages. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1973.
  • 10
    Araújo K. Aspectos do desempenho gramatical de crianças pré-escolares em desenvolvimento normal de linguagem [dissertação]. São Paulo: Faculdade de Filosofia, Letras e Ciências Humanas da Universidade de São Paulo; 2003.
  • 11
    Parker MD, Brorson KA. A comparative study between mean length of utterance in morphemes (MLU-m) and mean length utterance in words (MLU-w). First Language. 2005;25(3):365- 76.
  • 12
    Araujo K. Desempenho gramatical de criança em desenvolvimento normal e com distúrbio específico de linguagem [tese]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2007.
  • 13
    Berglund E, Eriksson M, Johansson I. Parental reports of spoken language skills in children with Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2001;44(1):179-91.
  • 14
    Eadie PA, Fey ME, Douglas JM, Parsons CL. Profiles of grammatical morphology and sentence imitation in children with specific language impairment and Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2002;45(4):720-32.
  • 15
    Chapman RS. Language learning in Down syndrome: the speech and language profile compared to adolescents with cognitive impairment of unknown origin. Downs Syndr Res Pract. 2006;10(2):61-6.
  • 16
    Laws G, Bishop DV. A comparison of language abilities in adolescents with Down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2003;46(6):1324-39.
  • 17
    Fluharty NB. Fluharty preschool speech and language screening tests. Austin: Pro-Ed, 1978.
  • 18
    Andrade CRF, Béfi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner HF. ABFW: teste de linguagem Infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. Barueri: Pró-Fono, 2004.
  • 19
    Fernandes FDM. Pragmática. In: Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner HF. ABFW - Teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. Barueri: Pró-Fono, 2004. p.83-97.
  • 20
    Wertzner HF. Fonologia. In: Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner HF. ABFW - Teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. Barueri: Pró-Fono. 2004. p.5-40.
  • 21
    Limongi SCO, Carvallo RMM, Souza ER. Auditory processing and language in Down syndrome. J Med Speech Lang Pathol. 2000;8(1):27-34.
  • 22
    Wertzner HF. Fonologia: desenvolvimento e alterações. In: Ferreira LP, Befi-Lopes DM, Limongi SCO (orgs.). Tratado de Fonoaudiologia. 2ª ed. São Paulo: Editora Roca, 2010. p.281-90.
  • 23
    Ehrler DJ, Mcghee RL. Primary Test of Nonverbal Intelligence - PTONI. Texas: Pro-Ed, 2008.
  • 24
    Thomas MS, Annaz D, Ansari D, Scerif G, Jarrold C, Karmiloff-Smith A. Using developmental trajectories to understand developmental disorders. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2009;52(2):336-58.
  • 25
    Reed AV. An introduction to children with language disorders. 2a ed. New York: McMillan, 1994. p.95-116.
  • 26
    Araújo K, Muhler LP, Telles P, Surian AC, Befi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FD, et al. Extensão média de enunciados de crianças com distúrbio específico de linguagem, síndrome de Down e do espectro autístico [resumo]. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2006; Suplemento especial.
  • 27
    Leonard LB, Eyer JA, Bedore LM, Grela BG. Three accounts of the grammatical morpheme difficulties of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1997;40(4):741-53.
  • 28
    Rice ML, Redmond SM, Hoffman L. Mean length of utterance in children with specific language impairment and in younger control children shows concurrent validity and stable and parallel growth trajectories. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2006;49(4):793-808.
  • 29
    Balason DV, Dollaghan CA. Grammatical morphemes production in four-year-old children. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2002;45(5):961-9.
  • 30
    Thordardottir ET. Early lexical and syntactic development in Quebec French and English: implications for cross-linguistic and bilingual assessment. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2005;40(3):243-78.
  • Study carried out at the Department of Physical Therapy, Speech-Language Pathology/Audiology, and Occupational Therapy, School of Medicine, Universidade de São Paulo - USP - São Paulo (SP), Brazil.
  • *
    AMAC was responsible for data collection, tabulation and analysis, and for writing the article; DBML collaborated with the discussion and final version of the manuscript; SCOL was responsible for the project, study outline, and overall supervision of the stages of manuscript writing and elaboration.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    June 2014

History

  • Received
    06 June 2013
  • Accepted
    02 May 2014
Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia Al. Jaú, 684, 7º andar, 01420-002 São Paulo - SP Brasil, Tel./Fax 55 11 - 3873-4211 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista@codas.org.br