Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

School-age children with specific language impairment produce more speech disfluencies than their peers

Abstracts

PURPOSE:

To compare the occurrence of speech disfluencies during narrative production in children with specific language impairment (SLI) and their age-matched peers.

METHODS:

The study included 60 children aged between 7 and 10 years, 40 with typical language development and 20 with SLI. For data collection, a series of 15 stories was used, each one represented by pictures composed of four scenes. Narratives were transcripted and the speech disfluencies presented on them were classified as stuttering-like disfluencies (part-word repetition, single-syllable word repetition, and dysrhythmic phonation - prolongations, blocks and broken words) or other disfluencies (interjection, revision/abandoned utterances, and multisyllable/phrase repetition). The disfluency categories were compared in each group and its occurrence was also compared between groups.

RESULTS:

The occurrence of stuttering-like and other disfluencies did not differ among children with typical language development, whereas children with SLI produced other disfluencies. Between-group comparison showed that children with SLI produced more disfluencies of both types than their age-matched peers.

CONCLUSION:

Children with SLI showed more speech disfluencies during narrative production than their age-matched peers, and the most common disfluencies used by them were not typical of people who stutter (interjection, revision/abandoned utterances, and multisyllable/phrase repetition).


OBJETIVO:

Comparar a ocorrência de rupturas de fala durante a produção de narrativas em escolares com distúrbio específico de linguagem (DEL) e seus pares cronológicos.

MÉTODOS:

Participaram do estudo 60 crianças com idade entre 7 e 10 anos, sendo 40 em desenvolvimento típico de linguagem e 20 com DEL. Para a coleta de dados, foi utilizada uma série de 15 histórias, representadas por figuras, compostas por quatro cenas cada. As histórias foram transcritas e as rupturas de fala presentes nessas narrativas foram classificadas em gagas (repetição de parte da palavra, repetição de uma sílaba da palavra e fonação disrítmica - prolongamentos, bloqueios e palavras rompidas) ou outras (interjeição, revisão/abandono de segmentos e repetição de frases ou multissilábica). O tipo de ruptura foi comparado em cada grupo, e a ocorrência de cada tipo de ruptura foi comparada entre os grupos.

RESULTADOS:

Enquanto a ocorrência dos tipos de rupturas não diferiu nos escolares em desenvolvimento típico de linguagem, os escolares com DEL tiveram maior ocorrência de outras rupturas. A comparação entre os grupos revelou que os escolares com DEL produzem mais rupturas de ambos os tipos do que seus pares.

CONCLUSÃO:

A ocorrência de rupturas de fala durante a produção de narrativas foi maior em escolares com DEL do que seus pares cronológicos, sendo que as rupturas classificadas como não gagas (interjeição, revisão/abandono de segmentos e repetição de frases ou multissilábica) foram mais frequentes para essa população.


INTRODUCTION

Fluent speech production depends on the activation of semantic, phonological, and syntactic information, influenced by contextual information( 11. Seiger-Gardner L, Schwartz RG. Lexical access in children with and without specific language impairment: a cross-modal picture-word interference study. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2008;43(5):528-51. ). The soft and accurate movement of several parts of the vocal tract is essential in this process, which requires that motor planning can occur without intercurrences( 22. Andrade C. Abordagem neurolinguística e motora da gagueira. In: Ferreira LP, Befi-Lopes DM, Limongi SCO, editors. Tratado de Fonoaudiologia. São Paulo: Roca; 2004. p. 1001-16. ). Therefore, when this process presents flaws, the fluency in speech production is interrupted, thus generating ruptures or disfluencies( 22. Andrade C. Abordagem neurolinguística e motora da gagueira. In: Ferreira LP, Befi-Lopes DM, Limongi SCO, editors. Tratado de Fonoaudiologia. São Paulo: Roca; 2004. p. 1001-16. , 33. Guo LY, Tomblin JB, Samelson V. Speech disruptions in the narratives of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;51(3):722-38. ).

In normal language development, several studies have shown that speech disfluencies are more frequent when linguistic representations are still developing, especially in lexical and syntactic aspects( 33. Guo LY, Tomblin JB, Samelson V. Speech disruptions in the narratives of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;51(3):722-38.

4. Rispoli M, Hadley P. The leading-edge: the significance of sentence disruptions in the development of grammar. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2001;44(5):1131-43.
- 55. Rispoli M. Changes in the nature of sentence production during the period of grammatical development. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2003;46(4):818-30. ). From the lexical point of view, the initial representation of unknown words is limited( 66. Gray S. Word-learning by preschoolers with specific language impairment: what predicts success? J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2003;46(1):56-67. , 77. Gershkoff-Stowe L, Hahn ER. Fast mapping skills in the developing lexicon. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2007;50(3):682-97. ), and this representation is reinforced by the experience with the word that, up to that point, is unknown. This allows the children to make fewer efforts to access that word in memory and, consequently, produce less disfluencies( 77. Gershkoff-Stowe L, Hahn ER. Fast mapping skills in the developing lexicon. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2007;50(3):682-97.

8. Hall NE. Lexical development and retrieval in treating children who stutter. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2004;35(1):57-69.
- 99. Capone NC, McGregor KK. The effect of semantic representation on toddlers' word retrieval. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2005;48(6):1468-80. ). From the syntactic point of view, a similar process exists in which a higher level of experience with the language enables better fluency of speech( 33. Guo LY, Tomblin JB, Samelson V. Speech disruptions in the narratives of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;51(3):722-38. ).

The occurrence of speech disfluencies during normal language development can also be related to the speech mismatch between lexical and syntactic skills during specific periods of development, thus determining asynchronies in speech production that trigger flaws in speech planning( 88. Hall NE. Lexical development and retrieval in treating children who stutter. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2004;35(1):57-69. ). Besides, speech disfluencies can be used as strategies to buy some time or fix errors during sentence production( 33. Guo LY, Tomblin JB, Samelson V. Speech disruptions in the narratives of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;51(3):722-38. ).

In the case of children with specific language impairment (SLI), who normally present difficulties in lexical acquisition, delay in the appearance of first words and limitations in lexical access, as well as important morphosyntactic deficit( 11. Seiger-Gardner L, Schwartz RG. Lexical access in children with and without specific language impairment: a cross-modal picture-word interference study. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2008;43(5):528-51. ), it is usually necessary to reinforce language-related experiences, so that their linguistic representations can be efficiently established( 66. Gray S. Word-learning by preschoolers with specific language impairment: what predicts success? J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2003;46(1):56-67. ).

This disorder is diagnosed with the presence of significant changes in the processes of language acquisition and development that do not result from broader changes, such as hearing loss, neurological damage, intellectual disabilities, or pervasive developmental disorders( 1010. Stark RE, Tallal P. Selection of children with specific language deficits. J Speech Hear Disord. 1981;46(2):114-22. , 1111. Bishop DV. The underlying nature of specific language impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1992;33(1):3-66. ).

Therefore, it is reasonable to state that children with SLI may present disfluencies in speech production when presenting weak linguistic representations( 88. Hall NE. Lexical development and retrieval in treating children who stutter. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2004;35(1):57-69. , 1212. Boscolo B, Ratner N, Rescorla L. Fluency of school-age children with a history of specific expressive language impairment: an exploratory study. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2002;11(1):41-9. ). Besides, these children need to work hard to learn the language, and they constitute a risk group for fluency changes during speech production activities that overload their linguistic system( 1212. Boscolo B, Ratner N, Rescorla L. Fluency of school-age children with a history of specific expressive language impairment: an exploratory study. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2002;11(1):41-9. ).

Effectively, a higher rate of speech disfluencies is associated with children with SLI, because they would require more time to plan and formulate sentences. A low level of automaticity to formulate the language is also attributed to them as a result of the delay to domain certain grammatical forms, which would also lead to the higher incidence of word repetition in their discourse( 1313. Finneran DA, Leonard LB, Miller CA. Speech disruptions in the sentence formulation of school-age children with specific language impairment. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2009;44(3):271-86. , 1414. Peets KF. Profiles of dysfluency and errors in classroom discourse among children with language impairment. J Commun Disord. 2009;42(2):136-54. ).

The inclusion of the assessment of speech fluency is pointed out as being essential in the process of diagnosing specific language changes, because it enables us to verify the effective use of language. Besides, children suspected of having SLI aged between 3 and 4 years showed reduced speech rate measured in syllables and words per minute( 1515. Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Juste FS, Cáceres-Assenço AM, Fortunato-Tavares TM. Aspectos da fluência da fala em crianças com Distúrbio Específico de Linguagem. Audiol, Commun Res. 2014;19(3):252-7. ).

An essential aspect to be considered is that speech disfluencies tend to reduce with age in children with typical development, because they only occur in longer and syntactically more complex segments( 1616. Yaruss JS, Newman RM, Flora T. Language and disfluency in nonstuttering children's conversational speech. J Fluency Disord. 1999;24(3):185-207. ). However, because children with SLI present strong difficulties in more complex linguistic tasks, such as narratives, there may be an increasing occurrence of speech disfluencies in this type of linguistic activity, even if they produce shorter, less cohesive narratives, with semantic, morphological, and syntactic errors( 1414. Peets KF. Profiles of dysfluency and errors in classroom discourse among children with language impairment. J Commun Disord. 2009;42(2):136-54. , 1717. Liles BZ, Duffy RJ, Merritt DD, Purcell SL. Measurement of narrative discourse ability in children with language disorders. J Speech Hear Res. 1995;38(2):415-25. ).

There is still evidence that the pattern of disfluencies used by the population with SLI is different than that of children with typical language development because fluency is associated with the effective use of language. Although children with history of language disorder significantly presented more stuttering-like disfluencies (part-word repetition, prolongations, blocks, and broken words) than their peers, the use of common disfluencies (word repetition and revisions) was similar in both groups( 1212. Boscolo B, Ratner N, Rescorla L. Fluency of school-age children with a history of specific expressive language impairment: an exploratory study. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2002;11(1):41-9. ).

Other studies did not find significant differences between the use of common disfluencies when comparing children with SLI and their peers( 33. Guo LY, Tomblin JB, Samelson V. Speech disruptions in the narratives of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;51(3):722-38. , 1515. Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Juste FS, Cáceres-Assenço AM, Fortunato-Tavares TM. Aspectos da fluência da fala em crianças com Distúrbio Específico de Linguagem. Audiol, Commun Res. 2014;19(3):252-7. ). According to the authors, this result was unexpected, but it can be explained by the fact that the speech sample is based on narratives, which does not require a shift change, and, therefore, does not favor the use of resources to elaborate the message during speech( 33. Guo LY, Tomblin JB, Samelson V. Speech disruptions in the narratives of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;51(3):722-38. ).

Considering that morphosyntax also interferes in fluency, and because we did not find studies about the use of disfluency by Portuguese-speaking school-age children with SLI in the literature, this analysis aimed at comparing the occurrence of speech disfluencies during the production of narratives among school-age children with SLI and their age-matched peers.

METHODS

The project was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the School of Medicine of the University of São Paulo (number 1150/09), and people in charge of the children signed the informed consent.

The study included 60 children aged between 7 and 10 years, being 40 with typical language development (18 male participants) and 20 with SLI (14 male participants). Individuals were paired by age group, in a ratio of two participants with typical development to one in the SLI group. All individuals lived in the city of São Paulo and studied in a regular public school.

Individuals with typical language development were selected from a state school located in the west zone of the City of São Paulo. At first, all subjects were individually screened during the regular classroom period in a room reserved for this study. Inclusion criteria for the typical development group were as follows: absence of complaints regarding language development; attending regular classrooms, compatible with the age group; not presenting any productive phonological process or idiosyncratic changes in imitation and naming tests of phonology( 1818. Wertzner HF. Fonologia. In: Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner HF, editors. ABFW - Teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de Fonologia, Vocabulário, Fluência e Pragmática. 2ª edição. Barueri: Pró-Fono; 2004. p. 5-32. ), besides having a compatible performance according to the expectations in tasks involving phonological awareness, reading and writing skills( 1919. Andrade C, Befi-Lopes D, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner H. Manual de avaliação de linguagem do Serviço de Fonoaudiologia do Centro de Saúde Escola Samuel B. Pessoa. São Paulo: 1997. ).

Members of the SLI group were undergoing weekly speech and language treatment, and had been previously diagnosed based on international criteria - exclusive linguistic deficit and nonverbal intelligence quotient within normal stardarts ( 1111. Bishop DV. The underlying nature of specific language impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1992;33(1):3-66. ). For that diagnosis, the child should have preserved hearing and results below expected in at least two standardized language tests, from the child language assessment ABFW( 2020. Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner HF, editors. ABFW - Teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de Fonologia, Vocabulário, Fluência e Pragmática. 2ª edição. Barueri: Pró-Fono; 2004. ) and the evaluation of the mean length utterance( 2121. Araujo K. Desempenho gramatical com crianças em desenvolvimento normal e com Distúrbio Específico de Linguagem [Tese]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2007. ). The minimum time of speech and language therapy for these individuals was 6 months, and the average period was 3 years.

For data collection, a series of 15 stories was used, represented by figures, composed of four scenes each( 2222. Baron-Cohen S, Leslie A, Frith U. Mechanical, behavioural and intencional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. Br J Dev Psychol. 1986;4(2):113-25. , 2323. Perissinoto J. Avaliação fonoaudiológica da criança com Autismo. In: Perissinoto J, editor. Conhecimentos essenciais para atender bem a criança com Autismo. São José dos Campos: Pulso; 2003. p. 45-55. ). During data collection, participants were told that the sequence of four figures composed a story. The first figure was presented to the child, who was asked to organize the others and narrate the respective story. In case the figures were not ordered in the usual manner, the researcher wrote down the adopted order, however, the child was not advised to rearrange or narrate according to the usual order; therefore, all the children elaborated stories that were compatible with their understanding of the sequence. To eliminate possible variables, such as short-term memory deficit, figures were visible for the child during the entire narrative.

After data collection, two graduate students of speech language pathology transcribed each story narrated by all the subjects. After the transcriptions, the same speech language pathologists analyzed the disfluencies according to the following classification: stuttering-like disfluencies (part-word repetition, single-syllable word repetition, and dysrhythmic phonation - prolongations, blocks, and broken words) and other disfluencies (interjection, revision/abandoned utterances, and multisyllable/phrase repetition)( 2424. Ambrose NG, Yairi E. Normative disfluency data for early childhood stuttering. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1999;42(4):895-909. ).

After that stage, the reliability calculation of the analyses was performed using the Pearson correlation coefficient, r, and the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC). For the average stuttering-like disfluencies, rates were r=89.9 and ICC=87.9, both with p<0.001; and for the average of other disfluencies, rates were r=80.9 and ICC=80.8, both with p<0.001. Therefore, it was possible to observe that the minimum agreement was 80.8%, which is in accordance with literature.

To respond to the objective, descriptive and inferential statistical analysis was performed using the software Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, version 18. For intragroup comparison, the Wilcoxon test was used, and for between-group comparison in each type of disfluency, the Mann-Whitney test was adopted. The initial significance level was 5%.

RESULTS

Descriptive analysis indicates that the mean and median occurrence of stuttering-like disfluencies and other disfluencies are lower in the group of typical language development. The intragroup comparison between disfluencies did not differ among school-age children with typical language development, however, the group with SLI had higher occurrence of other disfluencies (Table 1).

Table 1.
Descriptive statistics and intragroup comparison of disfluencies

The between-group comparison showed that school-age children with SLI produced more stuttering-like disfluencies and other disfluencies than those with typical language development (Table 2).

Table 2.
Between-group comparison of the occurrence of each type of disfluency

DISCUSSION

To compare the occurrence of speech disfluencies during the production of narratives among school-age students with SLI and their age-matched peers, it was first necessary to establish the profile of stuttering-like disfluencies and other disfluencies for both analyzed groups, and then verify if the use of each type of disfluency was different between groups.

With regard to the profile of the occurrence of each type of disfluency, in the group with typical language development there was not a prevalent type of disfluency, however, in the group with SLI, the other disfluencies were more common. Interestingly, disfluencies that were prevalent in that group were interjection, utterance revision, and sentence or word repetition, because these are more associated with linguistic content than with motor planning in the act of speaking( 22. Andrade C. Abordagem neurolinguística e motora da gagueira. In: Ferreira LP, Befi-Lopes DM, Limongi SCO, editors. Tratado de Fonoaudiologia. São Paulo: Roca; 2004. p. 1001-16. ).

The difficulty related to content would really be more expected in the population with SLI, because it presents lower domain of language rules in its several linguistic aspects( 1212. Boscolo B, Ratner N, Rescorla L. Fluency of school-age children with a history of specific expressive language impairment: an exploratory study. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2002;11(1):41-9.

13. Finneran DA, Leonard LB, Miller CA. Speech disruptions in the sentence formulation of school-age children with specific language impairment. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2009;44(3):271-86.
- 1414. Peets KF. Profiles of dysfluency and errors in classroom discourse among children with language impairment. J Commun Disord. 2009;42(2):136-54. ). However, even if this finding confirms that the pattern of disfluencies used by the population with SLI is different from its peers, it is different from previous studies that found no significant differences concerning the use of disfluencies considered as non-stuttering-like( 33. Guo LY, Tomblin JB, Samelson V. Speech disruptions in the narratives of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;51(3):722-38. , 1212. Boscolo B, Ratner N, Rescorla L. Fluency of school-age children with a history of specific expressive language impairment: an exploratory study. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2002;11(1):41-9. , 1515. Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Juste FS, Cáceres-Assenço AM, Fortunato-Tavares TM. Aspectos da fluência da fala em crianças com Distúrbio Específico de Linguagem. Audiol, Commun Res. 2014;19(3):252-7. ).

This difference can be a result of the type of used material, because, in this sample, students were asked to elaborate narratives based on logical-temporal sequences. In this type of task, the linguistic demand is higher than when static figures are presented( 1515. Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Juste FS, Cáceres-Assenço AM, Fortunato-Tavares TM. Aspectos da fluência da fala em crianças com Distúrbio Específico de Linguagem. Audiol, Commun Res. 2014;19(3):252-7. ), a book with no words( 1212. Boscolo B, Ratner N, Rescorla L. Fluency of school-age children with a history of specific expressive language impairment: an exploratory study. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2002;11(1):41-9. ).

However, other methodological aspects may have interfered in this result. Whereas in this study the classification of disfluencies divides them into stuttering-like (part-word repetition, prolongations, blocks, and broken words) and others (interjection, revision, and phrase repetition)( 2424. Ambrose NG, Yairi E. Normative disfluency data for early childhood stuttering. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1999;42(4):895-909. ), in the other mentioned studies disfluencies were classified as silent pauses and vocal hesitation (filled-in pauses, interjections, word or part-word repetition, revision)( 33. Guo LY, Tomblin JB, Samelson V. Speech disruptions in the narratives of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;51(3):722-38. ), as normal (word/phrase repetition, revision, and interjection) and stuttering-like disfluencies (part-word repetition, prolongation, and block)( 1212. Boscolo B, Ratner N, Rescorla L. Fluency of school-age children with a history of specific expressive language impairment: an exploratory study. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2002;11(1):41-9. ), or as common disfluencies (hesitation, word repetition, revision, interjection, unfinished word, segment repetition, and phrase repetition) and stuttering-like disfluencies (syllable repetition, blocks, prolongation, sound repetition, pause, and intrusion)( 1515. Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Juste FS, Cáceres-Assenço AM, Fortunato-Tavares TM. Aspectos da fluência da fala em crianças com Distúrbio Específico de Linguagem. Audiol, Commun Res. 2014;19(3):252-7. ). Therefore, it is possible that differences between these classifications, even if subtle, led to different results. So, for further analyses, it would be interesting to compare the occurrence of each type of disfluency in these groups, because then it would be possible to identify which one of them, common or stuttering-like, distinguishes the groups.

Another aspect that could explain such a divergence is the age group, which was different between studies and could justify the difference in the linguistic domain of these subjects, therefore interfering in the occurrence of disfluencies. In this study, students were aged between 7 and 10 years; in another national study, the analyzed age group ranged between 3 and 7 years( 1515. Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Juste FS, Cáceres-Assenço AM, Fortunato-Tavares TM. Aspectos da fluência da fala em crianças com Distúrbio Específico de Linguagem. Audiol, Commun Res. 2014;19(3):252-7. ) and, in international analyses, individuals were students aged 9 years( 33. Guo LY, Tomblin JB, Samelson V. Speech disruptions in the narratives of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;51(3):722-38. , 1212. Boscolo B, Ratner N, Rescorla L. Fluency of school-age children with a history of specific expressive language impairment: an exploratory study. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2002;11(1):41-9. ).

Besides, while in this study the participants had been diagnosed and were undergoing speech language and audiology therapy, in the other national study they were still being diagnosed( 1515. Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Juste FS, Cáceres-Assenço AM, Fortunato-Tavares TM. Aspectos da fluência da fala em crianças com Distúrbio Específico de Linguagem. Audiol, Commun Res. 2014;19(3):252-7. ) and, in the international one, they kept a linguistic performance that was inferior to that of their peers, however, they were not undergoing therapy( 1212. Boscolo B, Ratner N, Rescorla L. Fluency of school-age children with a history of specific expressive language impairment: an exploratory study. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2002;11(1):41-9. ). So, the linguistic strategies learned in therapy may favor changes in the pattern of speech fluency.

When disfluencies were compared in the groups, students with SLI showed to use more disfluencies of both types. This finding confirms that the linguistic difficulty of this population is also clear in the pattern of speech fluency, because even stuttering-like disfluencies are more frequent.

The use of syllable or part-word repetition, prolongations, blocks, and broken words is more associated with speech motor planning( 22. Andrade C. Abordagem neurolinguística e motora da gagueira. In: Ferreira LP, Befi-Lopes DM, Limongi SCO, editors. Tratado de Fonoaudiologia. São Paulo: Roca; 2004. p. 1001-16. ), which indicates that the linguistic damage in the case of SLI can affect the language process, thus leading to imbalance that generates these disfluencies( 33. Guo LY, Tomblin JB, Samelson V. Speech disruptions in the narratives of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;51(3):722-38. ).

In situations when demand overcomes the linguistic capacity of these individuals, as is the case of elaborating narratives from logical-temporal sequences( 2525. Bento AC, Befi-Lopes DM. Story organization and narrative by school-age children with typical language development. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2010;22(4):503-8. ), speech disfluencies can also be used as processing strategies to buy some time for planning and formulating phrases, or fixing errors during the production of phrases( 33. Guo LY, Tomblin JB, Samelson V. Speech disruptions in the narratives of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;51(3):722-38. , 1313. Finneran DA, Leonard LB, Miller CA. Speech disruptions in the sentence formulation of school-age children with specific language impairment. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2009;44(3):271-86. ).

The occurrence of disfluencies in complex linguistic tasks is predicted for individuals without language changes( 1616. Yaruss JS, Newman RM, Flora T. Language and disfluency in nonstuttering children's conversational speech. J Fluency Disord. 1999;24(3):185-207. ); however, if we consider that for individuals with SLI the elaboration of narratives is experienced as a complex linguistic task, it would be justified due to the difficulty to integrate lexical, morphosyntactic, and contextual aspects( 1414. Peets KF. Profiles of dysfluency and errors in classroom discourse among children with language impairment. J Commun Disord. 2009;42(2):136-54. , 1717. Liles BZ, Duffy RJ, Merritt DD, Purcell SL. Measurement of narrative discourse ability in children with language disorders. J Speech Hear Res. 1995;38(2):415-25. ). Recent national studies pointed out that the population with SLI faces difficulties related to fluency exactly because of the difficulty to functionally use closed class words, and to elaborate narratives that require knowledge of the world and pragmatic skills( 2626. Befi-Lopes DM, Bacchin LB, Pedott PR, Cáceres-Assenço AM. Complexidade da história e pausas silentes em crianças com e sem distúrbio específico de linguagem. CoDAS. 2013;25(4):325-9.

27. Befi-Lopes DM, Pedott PR, Bacchin LB, Cáceres AM. Relação entre pausas silentes e classe gramatical em narrativas de crianças com distúrbio específico de linguagem. CoDAS. 2013;25(1):64-9.
- 2828. Pedott PR, Bacchin LB, Cáceres-Assenço AM, Befi-Lopes DM. A duração da pausa silente difere entre palavras de classe aberta ou fechada? Audiol, Commun Res. 2014;19(2):153-7. ).

Therefore, the findings in this study reinforce the importance of assessing the fluency in situations of language disorder, because the profile of fluency of these individuals distinguishes them from speakers who do not have such a disorder. This aspect also needs to be considered in the therapeutic process, because this awareness shows the need to improve the fluency of speech of these individuals. Without strategies that help them elaborate speech more efficiently, it is very likely that these individuals will no longer be interested in speech, considering to be insecure or with little domain about the subject they approach.

So, it is interesting that new studies be developed to verify other fluency-related aspects of this population, such as speech rate, and also to propose clear and scientifically based forms to intervene in such cases, thus contributing with the improvement in the communication of these individuals, and, consequently, to strengthen Speech Language Pathology and Audiology.

CONCLUSION

By comparing the occurrence of speech disfluencies during the production of narratives, it was possible to observe that school-age children with SLI produced more disfluencies than their age-matched peers, and disfluencies classified as non-stuttering-like (interjection/revision/abandoned utterance and phrase/multisyllable repetition) were more common for this population.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Seiger-Gardner L, Schwartz RG. Lexical access in children with and without specific language impairment: a cross-modal picture-word interference study. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2008;43(5):528-51.
  • 2
    Andrade C. Abordagem neurolinguística e motora da gagueira. In: Ferreira LP, Befi-Lopes DM, Limongi SCO, editors. Tratado de Fonoaudiologia. São Paulo: Roca; 2004. p. 1001-16.
  • 3
    Guo LY, Tomblin JB, Samelson V. Speech disruptions in the narratives of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;51(3):722-38.
  • 4
    Rispoli M, Hadley P. The leading-edge: the significance of sentence disruptions in the development of grammar. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2001;44(5):1131-43.
  • 5
    Rispoli M. Changes in the nature of sentence production during the period of grammatical development. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2003;46(4):818-30.
  • 6
    Gray S. Word-learning by preschoolers with specific language impairment: what predicts success? J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2003;46(1):56-67.
  • 7
    Gershkoff-Stowe L, Hahn ER. Fast mapping skills in the developing lexicon. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2007;50(3):682-97.
  • 8
    Hall NE. Lexical development and retrieval in treating children who stutter. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2004;35(1):57-69.
  • 9
    Capone NC, McGregor KK. The effect of semantic representation on toddlers' word retrieval. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2005;48(6):1468-80.
  • 10
    Stark RE, Tallal P. Selection of children with specific language deficits. J Speech Hear Disord. 1981;46(2):114-22.
  • 11
    Bishop DV. The underlying nature of specific language impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1992;33(1):3-66.
  • 12
    Boscolo B, Ratner N, Rescorla L. Fluency of school-age children with a history of specific expressive language impairment: an exploratory study. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2002;11(1):41-9.
  • 13
    Finneran DA, Leonard LB, Miller CA. Speech disruptions in the sentence formulation of school-age children with specific language impairment. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2009;44(3):271-86.
  • 14
    Peets KF. Profiles of dysfluency and errors in classroom discourse among children with language impairment. J Commun Disord. 2009;42(2):136-54.
  • 15
    Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Juste FS, Cáceres-Assenço AM, Fortunato-Tavares TM. Aspectos da fluência da fala em crianças com Distúrbio Específico de Linguagem. Audiol, Commun Res. 2014;19(3):252-7.
  • 16
    Yaruss JS, Newman RM, Flora T. Language and disfluency in nonstuttering children's conversational speech. J Fluency Disord. 1999;24(3):185-207.
  • 17
    Liles BZ, Duffy RJ, Merritt DD, Purcell SL. Measurement of narrative discourse ability in children with language disorders. J Speech Hear Res. 1995;38(2):415-25.
  • 18
    Wertzner HF. Fonologia. In: Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner HF, editors. ABFW - Teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de Fonologia, Vocabulário, Fluência e Pragmática. 2ª edição. Barueri: Pró-Fono; 2004. p. 5-32.
  • 19
    Andrade C, Befi-Lopes D, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner H. Manual de avaliação de linguagem do Serviço de Fonoaudiologia do Centro de Saúde Escola Samuel B. Pessoa. São Paulo: 1997.
  • 20
    Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner HF, editors. ABFW - Teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de Fonologia, Vocabulário, Fluência e Pragmática. 2ª edição. Barueri: Pró-Fono; 2004.
  • 21
    Araujo K. Desempenho gramatical com crianças em desenvolvimento normal e com Distúrbio Específico de Linguagem [Tese]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo; 2007.
  • 22
    Baron-Cohen S, Leslie A, Frith U. Mechanical, behavioural and intencional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. Br J Dev Psychol. 1986;4(2):113-25.
  • 23
    Perissinoto J. Avaliação fonoaudiológica da criança com Autismo. In: Perissinoto J, editor. Conhecimentos essenciais para atender bem a criança com Autismo. São José dos Campos: Pulso; 2003. p. 45-55.
  • 24
    Ambrose NG, Yairi E. Normative disfluency data for early childhood stuttering. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1999;42(4):895-909.
  • 25
    Bento AC, Befi-Lopes DM. Story organization and narrative by school-age children with typical language development. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2010;22(4):503-8.
  • 26
    Befi-Lopes DM, Bacchin LB, Pedott PR, Cáceres-Assenço AM. Complexidade da história e pausas silentes em crianças com e sem distúrbio específico de linguagem. CoDAS. 2013;25(4):325-9.
  • 27
    Befi-Lopes DM, Pedott PR, Bacchin LB, Cáceres AM. Relação entre pausas silentes e classe gramatical em narrativas de crianças com distúrbio específico de linguagem. CoDAS. 2013;25(1):64-9.
  • 28
    Pedott PR, Bacchin LB, Cáceres-Assenço AM, Befi-Lopes DM. A duração da pausa silente difere entre palavras de classe aberta ou fechada? Audiol, Commun Res. 2014;19(2):153-7.
  • Financial support: Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo - FAPESP.
  • *DMBL was in charge of orientation, elaboration of the Project and aprova of the final version of the paper; AMCA revised data tabulation and analysis, literature survey, and was in charge of statistical analysis, interpretation of results and writing the article; SFM and MV were responsible for data tabulation and analysis, and literature survey. Study carried out at the Speech-Language Pathology Laboratory for Research into Language Development and Language Development Disorders, Department of Physiotherapy, Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, and Occupational Therapy. School of Medicine, Universidade de São Paulo - USP - São Paulo (SP), Brazil.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Dec 2014

History

  • Received
    10 July 2014
  • Accepted
    22 Oct 2014
Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia Al. Jaú, 684, 7º andar, 01420-002 São Paulo - SP Brasil, Tel./Fax 55 11 - 3873-4211 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista@codas.org.br