Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Capacity for self-monitoring reading comprehension in Elementary School

ABSTRACT

Purpose

To investigate the capacity for self-monitoring reading comprehension in Brazilian Elementary School students.

Methods

Fifty-three Elementary students in the 5th and 9th grades from two Public Schools in the city of São Paulo were assessed. They were selected based on their oral reading rate and grouped according to their performance in reading comprehension in the following categories: Group with best comprehension: students with adequate rate and accuracy, without difficulties in reading comprehension; Group with worst comprehension: students with adequate rate and accuracy but with difficulties in reading comprehension. Two narrative texts followed by eight questions to assess reading comprehension were presented. Two sentences and two words were replaced by ungrammatical elements and pseudo-words. Under the condition of spontaneous monitoring, students read the text aloud and answered the questions. The analysis considered the calculation of hesitation, self-correction, repetitions and mistakes. Under the condition of directed monitoring, students were instructed to read the text, either aloud or silently, after being told that certain parts of the text could not make sense, and they were oriented to underline such parts. The analysis was carried out by counting of underlined items. The comparisons were made with the Mann-Whitney test.

Results

A difference was observed between the groups only at the sentence level among the 9th grade schoolchildren under the spontaneous monitoring and among the 5th grade schoolchildren under directed monitoring.

Conclusion

Students with worst comprehension had a poorer performance to monitor the presence of ungrammatical sentences than their peers with best comprehension.

Keywords
Reading; Comprehension; Reading Comprehension; Monitoring Comprehension; Cognition

RESUMO

Purpose

Investigar a capacidade de automonitoramento da compreensão leitora de escolares brasileiros do Ensino Fundamental.

Método

Avaliaram-se 53 alunos do 5º e 9º anos do Ensino Fundamental de escolas da rede pública de São Paulo, selecionados pelo valor de taxa de leitura oral de texto e agrupados a partir de seu desempenho em compreensão leitora em: Grupo de melhor compreensão: escolares com taxa e acurácia adequadas, sem dificuldades na compreensão leitora; Grupo de pior compreensão: escolares com taxa e acurácia adequadas e dificuldades na compreensão leitora. Dois textos narrativos acompanhados de oito questões para avaliar a compreensão leitora foram apresentados. Duas sentenças e duas palavras foram substituídas por elementos agramaticais, pseudopalavras. Na condição de monitoramento espontâneo, os escolares leram o texto oralmente e responderam a questões. A análise considerou o cômputo de hesitações, autocorreções, repetições e erros. Na condição de monitoramento dirigido, leram o texto, informados de que algumas partes poderiam não fazer sentido e que as sublinhassem. A análise se deu pelo cômputo dos itens sublinhados. Os grupos foram comparados pelo Teste de Mann-Whitney.

Resultados

Observou-se diferença entre os grupos em nível frasal no 9º ano, na condição de monitoramento espontâneo, e no 5º ano, na condição de monitoramento dirigido.

Conclusão

Escolares de pior compreensão leitora apresentaram desempenho inferior ao monitorar a presença de sentenças agramaticais.

Descritores
Leitura; Compreensão; Compreensão Leitora; Monitoramento da Compreensão; Cognição

INTRODUCTION

Efficient reading comprehension requires monitoring the content read and the strategies used for reading, until the understanding of the text is reached. Among the indispensable skills, the role of self-monitoring and evaluation of the acquired knowledge stands out. These executive functions(11 Tirapu-Ustárroz J, García-Molina A, Luna-Lario P, Roig-Rovira T, Pelegrín-Valero C. Modelos de funciones y control ejecutivo (II). Rev Neurol. 2008;46(12):742-50. PMid:18543201.,22 Oosterlaan J, Scheres A, Sergeant JA. Which executive functioning deficits are associated with AD/HD, ODD/CD and comorbid AD/HD+ ODD/CD. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2005;33(1):69-85. PMid:15759592. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-005-0935-y.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-005-093...
) must be directed during reading in order to achieve the very goal of this action: to grasp the meaning or a coherent representation of what is proposed in the text. Effective monitoring must clearly show the perception that there was a failure in comprehension and that this must be resolved to ensure full understanding of the text(33 Paris SG, Myers M 2nd. Comprehension monitoring, memory, and study strategies of good and poor readers. J Literacy Res. 1981;13(1):5-22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10862968109547390.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10862968109547...

4 Perfetti CA, Marron MA, Foltz PW. Sources of comprehension failure: Theoretical perspective and case studies. In: Cornoldi C, Oakhill J, organizadores. Reading comprehension difficulties: processes and intervention. Mahwah: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates; 1996. p. 137-165.
-55 Baker L, Zeliger-Kandasamy A, DeWyngaert LU. Neuroimaging evidence of comprehension monitoring. Psychological Topics. 2014;23(1):167-87.). The ability to define strategies to improve comprehension can distinguish good from poor readers(33 Paris SG, Myers M 2nd. Comprehension monitoring, memory, and study strategies of good and poor readers. J Literacy Res. 1981;13(1):5-22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10862968109547390.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10862968109547...
,66 Garner R, Taylor N. Monitoring of understanding: an investigation of attentional assistance needs at different grade and reading proficiency levels. Reading Psychology: An International Quarterly. 1982;3(1):1-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0270271820030102.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02702718200301...

7 Golinkoff R. A comparison of reading comprehension processes in good and poor comprehenders. Read Res Q. 1976;11(4):623-59. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/747459.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/747459...

8 Weaver CA 3rd, Bryant DS. Monitoring of comprehension: the role of texto difficulty in metamemory for narrative and expository text. Mem Cognit. 1995;23(1):12-22. PMid:7885261. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03210553.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03210553...
-99 Snowling MJ, Hulme C. Evidence‐based interventions for reading and language difficulties: creating a virtuous circle. Br J Educ Psychol. 2011;81(1):1-23. PMid:21391960. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279.2010.02014.x.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279.20...
). The hypothesis that conditions of the self-monitoring ability during reading may be associated with specific deficits in reading comprehension(1010 Oakhill J, Hartt J, Samols D. Levels of comprehension monitoring and working memory in good and poor comprehenders. Read Writ. 2005;18(7-9):657-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3355-z.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-335...
,1111 Yuill N, Oakhill J, Parkin A. Working memory, comprehension ability and the resolution of text anomaly. Br J Psychol. 1989;80(3):351-61. PMid:2790393. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1989.tb02325.x.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.19...
) guided this research.

This study will show preliminary data from a research that aimed to investigate the ability of self-monitoring reading comprehension among Brazilian elementary school students. The experiment was based on procedures described in the literature to distinguish good from poor readers.

METHODS

This is a prospective observational study (CAAE: 33421614500005505) that had the Terms of Consent and the Informed Consent forms signed by the Educational Institutions, the schoolchildren and their guardians.

Sample

Fifty-three students were evaluated (34 girls). They were enrolled in the fifth (N = 34) and ninth (N = 19) grade of Elementary School of two public municipal schools of the city of São Paulo. Teachers indicated them after checking the inclusion criteria: absence of complaints or indicators of hearing or visual (uncorrected) deficits or neurological, behavioral and cognitive disorders as well as history of school retention.

In order to ensure the level of reading comprehension, students with reading rate values ​​below 91 p.p.m. in the case of fifth-grade students, and below 110 p.p.m. in the case of ninth-grade students, were excluded(1212 Carvalho CAF. Função pragmática da linguagem e compreensão de textos na dislexia [tese]. São Paulo: Universidade Federal de São Paulo; 2013.). Participants were grouped based on their performance in reading comprehension tests carried out with appropriate texts for the schooling level(1313 Avila CRB, Kida ASB, Carvalho CAF, Cogo-Moreira H, Lúcio PS. Construção de uma Bateria para Avaliação da Compreensão Leitora para Escolares do Ciclo I do Ensino Fundamental [relatório científico Projeto de Pesquisa Regular]. São Paulo: Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo; 2014.,1414 Saraiva RA, Moojen SMP, Munarski R. Avaliação da compreensão leitora de textos expositivos. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2006.): Group with best comprehension (G1); Group with worst comprehension (G2).

Material

In order to evaluate the ability of self-monitoring reading comprehension, a team composed of 6 speech therapists and 1 linguist developed the protocol that ultimately consisted of 4 narrative texts - 2 for each school year, and 8 literal and inference questions for each text. One of them, as well as the 8 questions attached to it, composed the original protocol(1010 Oakhill J, Hartt J, Samols D. Levels of comprehension monitoring and working memory in good and poor comprehenders. Read Writ. 2005;18(7-9):657-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3355-z.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-335...
) which was translated and linguistically/culturally adapted to Brazilian Portuguese. The other 3 narrative texts and their questions were developed by the same team. In each text, two sentences were changed on the order of words, resulting in ungrammatical structures. In addition to these modifications, two pseudo-words replaced the original words in different paragraphs. This research was preceded by pilots that allowed verifying the linguistic adequacy of the texts.

Procedures

Participants were individually evaluated in times established in advance by the direction of the school.

Each student read 2 school texts: one for spontaneous monitoring assessment and the other for directed monitoring assessment. Because besides the fact that the sample was small, it was collected in two different schools, the order of presentation of texts for the total sample was kept fixed in this experiment in order to avoid bias in the procedure. Thus, the condition of spontaneous monitoring was evaluated first and then, directed monitoring. The instructions followed what is indicated by Oakhill et al.(1010 Oakhill J, Hartt J, Samols D. Levels of comprehension monitoring and working memory in good and poor comprehenders. Read Writ. 2005;18(7-9):657-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3355-z.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-335...
).

Spontaneous monitoring

Schoolchildren were given the instruction: “You must read this text aloud and then answer the questions”. The analysis was performed by computing hesitations, repetitions or errors during the reading of the text.

Directed monitoring

Schoolchildren were given the instruction: “You must read this text in undertone or aloud and then answer the questions. Some parts of the text may not make sense. Underline the words or phrases that you do not understand”.

The number of underlined pseudo-words and sentences were counted (underlined words were not considered mistakes).

Comparisons between groups were made using the Mann-Whitney test (significant values: p-value < 0.05).

RESULTS

Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics of Pseudo-words 1 and 2 and Ungrammatical Sentences 1 and 2 under spontaneous monitoring. Only the variable “hesitation” determined significant differences in the ninth grade, with the largest number of hesitations of G2 for mistakes of syntactic reorganization 1.

Table 1
Descriptive statistics for the no word 1 and 2 and phrasal reorganization 1 and 2 in the situation of spontaneous monitoring and Mann-Whitney test results for comparisons between groups

Table 2 presents the descriptive statistics and comparisons under directed monitoring: Pseudo-words 1 and 2 and Ungrammatical Sentences 1 and 2.

Table 2
Descriptive statistics and Mann-Whitney test results for comparisons between groups in the directed monitoring tasks

DISCUSSION

Monitoring sense is a necessary skill for efficient comprehension(11 Tirapu-Ustárroz J, García-Molina A, Luna-Lario P, Roig-Rovira T, Pelegrín-Valero C. Modelos de funciones y control ejecutivo (II). Rev Neurol. 2008;46(12):742-50. PMid:18543201.,22 Oosterlaan J, Scheres A, Sergeant JA. Which executive functioning deficits are associated with AD/HD, ODD/CD and comorbid AD/HD+ ODD/CD. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2005;33(1):69-85. PMid:15759592. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-005-0935-y.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-005-093...
). Differences in self-monitoring ability may determine best or worst results in reading comprehension tasks. For this reason, the hesitations observed throughout the test with the ninth grade, specifically the experiment for identification of ungrammatical sentences, indicated a difference between the best and worst reading comprehension groups (Table 1). The accuracy to detect inconsistencies in a text proved to be lower when the G2 was compared to the G1.

On the other hand, not all the tasks designed to assess self-monitoring while reading showed evidence of the expected difference. The assessment of spontaneous self-monitoring at word level showed no differences in the performance of schoolchildren with best and worst comprehension skills(1010 Oakhill J, Hartt J, Samols D. Levels of comprehension monitoring and working memory in good and poor comprehenders. Read Writ. 2005;18(7-9):657-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3355-z.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-335...
) (Table 1). G1 as G2 presented similar average performance. The vocabulary level of schoolchildren (uncontrolled variable in this experiment) may have influenced this result.

With regard to the investigation of the condition of directed monitoring (Table 2), it was observed that children with best comprehension of the fifth grade identified more sentence-level mistakes than children with worst comprehension, as expected. These results corroborate the literature: schoolchildren with bad comprehension in the fourth grade do not get involved in precise monitoring, especially when directly assessed(33 Paris SG, Myers M 2nd. Comprehension monitoring, memory, and study strategies of good and poor readers. J Literacy Res. 1981;13(1):5-22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10862968109547390.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10862968109547...
).

Thus, the best performance of the G1 of the fifth and ninth grade in identifying sentence-level mistakes partially corroborates the findings of Oakhill et al.(1010 Oakhill J, Hartt J, Samols D. Levels of comprehension monitoring and working memory in good and poor comprehenders. Read Writ. 2005;18(7-9):657-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3355-z.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-335...
), whose experiment demonstrated the superior performance of participants with good reading comprehension, although only at the sentence level.

CONCLUSION

Schoolchildren with worst reading comprehension had lower performance while monitoring the presence of ungrammatical sentences. In the ninth year, the difference was evidenced under spontaneous self-monitoring and in the fifth grade, under directed monitoring.

  • Study carried out at Universidade Federal de São Paulo – UNIFESP - São Paulo (SP), Brazil.
  • Financial support: nothing to declare.

REFERÊNCIAS

  • 1
    Tirapu-Ustárroz J, García-Molina A, Luna-Lario P, Roig-Rovira T, Pelegrín-Valero C. Modelos de funciones y control ejecutivo (II). Rev Neurol. 2008;46(12):742-50. PMid:18543201.
  • 2
    Oosterlaan J, Scheres A, Sergeant JA. Which executive functioning deficits are associated with AD/HD, ODD/CD and comorbid AD/HD+ ODD/CD. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2005;33(1):69-85. PMid:15759592. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-005-0935-y
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-005-0935-y
  • 3
    Paris SG, Myers M 2nd. Comprehension monitoring, memory, and study strategies of good and poor readers. J Literacy Res. 1981;13(1):5-22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10862968109547390
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10862968109547390
  • 4
    Perfetti CA, Marron MA, Foltz PW. Sources of comprehension failure: Theoretical perspective and case studies. In: Cornoldi C, Oakhill J, organizadores. Reading comprehension difficulties: processes and intervention. Mahwah: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates; 1996. p. 137-165.
  • 5
    Baker L, Zeliger-Kandasamy A, DeWyngaert LU. Neuroimaging evidence of comprehension monitoring. Psychological Topics. 2014;23(1):167-87.
  • 6
    Garner R, Taylor N. Monitoring of understanding: an investigation of attentional assistance needs at different grade and reading proficiency levels. Reading Psychology: An International Quarterly. 1982;3(1):1-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0270271820030102
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0270271820030102
  • 7
    Golinkoff R. A comparison of reading comprehension processes in good and poor comprehenders. Read Res Q. 1976;11(4):623-59. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/747459
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/747459
  • 8
    Weaver CA 3rd, Bryant DS. Monitoring of comprehension: the role of texto difficulty in metamemory for narrative and expository text. Mem Cognit. 1995;23(1):12-22. PMid:7885261. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03210553
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03210553
  • 9
    Snowling MJ, Hulme C. Evidence‐based interventions for reading and language difficulties: creating a virtuous circle. Br J Educ Psychol. 2011;81(1):1-23. PMid:21391960. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279.2010.02014.x
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279.2010.02014.x
  • 10
    Oakhill J, Hartt J, Samols D. Levels of comprehension monitoring and working memory in good and poor comprehenders. Read Writ. 2005;18(7-9):657-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3355-z
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3355-z
  • 11
    Yuill N, Oakhill J, Parkin A. Working memory, comprehension ability and the resolution of text anomaly. Br J Psychol. 1989;80(3):351-61. PMid:2790393. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1989.tb02325.x
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1989.tb02325.x
  • 12
    Carvalho CAF. Função pragmática da linguagem e compreensão de textos na dislexia [tese]. São Paulo: Universidade Federal de São Paulo; 2013.
  • 13
    Avila CRB, Kida ASB, Carvalho CAF, Cogo-Moreira H, Lúcio PS. Construção de uma Bateria para Avaliação da Compreensão Leitora para Escolares do Ciclo I do Ensino Fundamental [relatório científico Projeto de Pesquisa Regular]. São Paulo: Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo; 2014.
  • 14
    Saraiva RA, Moojen SMP, Munarski R. Avaliação da compreensão leitora de textos expositivos. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2006.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    2017

History

  • Received
    22 Feb 2016
  • Accepted
    19 Oct 2016
Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia Al. Jaú, 684, 7º andar, 01420-002 São Paulo - SP Brasil, Tel./Fax 55 11 - 3873-4211 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista@codas.org.br