Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Subjective position sliding in utterances perceived as echolalic in a child with Language Disorder: a case study

ABSTRACT

Within a linguistic-discursive framework, subject markers in a chain of utterances considered to be echolalia based on the recurring linguistic structure does X want Y? were investigated. This chain was produced during a speech therapy session by J., a female child, 10-years-old at the time of data collection, and with a speech-language pathology diagnosis of language disorder and a medical diagnosis of early psychosis. A set of linguistic fluctuations indicated a sliding of the subject position in the analyzed chain. Such fluctuations involved syntactic, lexical, semantic, morphological and prosodic elements. Discursively, the fluctuations left traces of a sliding of the subject position in the chain formed by these utterances, from a spoken subject (do you want Y?) to a speaking/desiring one (I want Y.). In this way, utterances considered echolalia can provide clues, via their linguistic fluctuations and discursive slippages, about the subject's desire in their relationship with the O/other. Given this, although they do not emerge in a conventional way, such utterances can demonstrate possibilities for changes in subject position. A contribution of the present research for clinical practice involving language in therapeutic settings therefore, was to highlight a listening to utterances, which could be seen as connected/grounded in the speech of the other. In clinical practice involving language, it is possible to create space for new/other senses for utterances, to allow the constitution of the subject of/in language, based on utterances often interpreted as being devoid of subjectivity.

Keywords:
Child Language; Language Disorder; Echolalia; Linguistics; Speech Therapy

RESUMO

Sob ótica linguístico-discursiva de orientação francesa, foram investigadas marcas de subjetividade numa cadeia de enunciados tidos como ecolálicos, ancorados na estrutura linguística recorrente X quer Y?. No interior de uma sessão de fonoterapia, essa cadeia foi produzida por J., uma criança do sexo feminino, com 10 anos de idade à época da coleta dos dados, com diagnóstico fonoaudiológico de distúrbio de linguagem e diagnóstico médico de psicose precoce. Um conjunto de flutuações linguísticas indiciaram um deslizamento de posição subjetiva na cadeia analisada. Tais flutuações envolveram elementos sintáticos, lexicais, semânticos, morfológicos e prosódicos. Discursivamente, as flutuações deixaram rastros de um deslizamento de posição subjetiva, ou seja, de sujeito falado (cê quer Y?) para falante/desejante (eu quero Y.) na cadeia formada por esses enunciados. Desse modo, enunciados tidos como ecolálicos podem dar pistas do desejo do sujeito em sua relação com o O/outro, por meio de suas flutuações linguísticas e de seus deslizamentos discursivos. Portanto, embora não irrompam de forma convencional, tais enunciados podem mostrar possibilidades de mudanças de posição subjetiva. Por conseguinte, uma contribuição da investigação relatada à clínica de linguagem é a de que, no setting terapêutico, pode haver escuta para enunciados que, em primeira instância, poderiam ser tidos como colados/enraizados no dizer do outro. Na clínica de linguagem é possível, então, dar lugar a novos/outros sentidos a tais enunciados, de maneira a favorecer a constituição do sujeito da/na linguagem a partir de enunciados frequentemente interpretados como esvaziados de subjetividade.

Descritores:
Linguagem Infantil; Distúrbio de Linguagem; Ecolalia; Linguística; Fonoaudiologia

INTRODUCTION

In pathological contexts involving language disorders, so-called echolalias were initially viewed as “[…] productions devoid of sense, only an echo, similar to the speech of a parrot: a repetition without communicative intent, without interlocutors […] simply a decontextualized empty repetition, […]”(11 Kanner L. Problems of nosology and psychodinamics in early infantile autism. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 1949;19(3):416-26. PMid:18146742.:416). They have subsequently begun to be investigated through different approaches(22 Oliveira MT. Quem fala nessa voz? [dissertação]. São Paulo: Pontíficia Universidade Católica de São Paulo; 2001.).

Approaches related to pragmatic, communicative or social interaction aspects have predominated(33 Freitas FAF, Montenegro ACA, Fernandes FDM, Delgado IC, Almeida LNA, Alves GÂS. Communicative skills of children with autistic spectrum disorder: clinical and family perception. Rev CEFAC. 2021;23(4):e1521. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216/20212341521.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216/2021...

4 Hage SVR, Hter LYSY, Fernandes FDM. Social Communication and pragmatic skills of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Developmental Language Disorder. CoDAS. 2021;34(2):e20210075. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20212021075. PMid:34932641.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/2021...
-55 Balestro JI, Fernandes FDM. Percepção de cuidadores de crianças com Transtorno do Espectro do Autismo quanto ao perfil comunicativo de seus filhos após um programa de orientação fonoaudiológica. CoDAS. 2019;31(1):e20170222. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20182018222. PMid:30843922.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/2018...
). However, based on a multimodal and enunciative language approach, echolalias can also be understood as a metaphor, in their relation to gestures in autism(11 Kanner L. Problems of nosology and psychodinamics in early infantile autism. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 1949;19(3):416-26. PMid:18146742.,66 Barros I, Fonte R, Souza A. Ecolalia e gestos no autismo: reflexões em torno da metáfora enunciativa. Forma y Función. 33(1):173-89. http://dx.doi.org/10.15446/fyf.v33n1.84184.
http://dx.doi.org/10.15446/fyf.v33n1.841...
). However, they can also be understood, based on the subject/language relationship, as repetitions of sonorous segments that lead to changes in verbalization(77 Moraes MEA, Carvalho GMM. Repetição em aquisição de linguagem: notas sobre o espelhamento sonoro infantil e a ecolalia no autismo. Signótica. 2022;34:e70277. http://dx.doi.org/10.5216/sig.v34.70277.).

Therefore, based on this prior case, we can see that only more recently we encounter in its investigation, a focus on the subject, who beyond the echolalias, “speaks in this voice”(22 Oliveira MT. Quem fala nessa voz? [dissertação]. São Paulo: Pontíficia Universidade Católica de São Paulo; 2001.:2), a perspective that produces a significant change of direction in the trajectory of the investigation of speech understood as echolalia.

Adopting this latter perspective and distancing ourselves from the theoretical-methodological understanding that views echolalias as echoes of the other(s), we seek to investigate how this speech could be interpreted from the linguistic-discursive point of view of a French orientation, whose characteristics will be highlighted subsequently. Within this framework, possible subject markers were sought in utterances with recurring linguistic structures in the linguistic-discursive production of J. - a child with a speech therapy diagnosis of language disorder and a medical diagnosis of early psychosis.

In the expression linguistic-discursive, “linguistic” is understood as a structural organization of the different dimensions of language. In this organization, the syntactic, semantic, morphological and phonological (in its prosodic component) dimensions have been privileged in speech understood as echolalia. In this same expression, “discursive” is understood as a process of sense production, “[…] whose specificity resides in the type of materiality of its base, that is, linguistic materiality […].”(88 Pêcheux M, Fuchs C. A propósito da análise automática do discurso: atualização e perspectivas. In: Gadet F, Hak T, organizadores. Por uma análise automática do discurso: uma introdução à obra de Michel Pêcheux. 3. ed. Campinas: Editora da Unicamp; 1990. p. 179.:179), materiality that manifests itself in the form of utterances. However, we also highlight that the prosodic component of the phonological dimension of language, in addition to maintaining a strict relationship with the syntactic and semantic components of language, also indicates subjective aspects of the discursive process.

What we understand here as an utterance is not a structural unit of language. In other words, the utterance is not confused with the syntactic organization of a sentence or clause; it is a concrete and singular materialization of language in a discursive process. In this process, it arises as “[…] a link in a very complex chain of other utterances […]”(99 Bakhtin MM. Os gêneros do discurso. In: Bakhtin MM. Estética da criação verbal. 3. ed. São Paulo: Martins Fontes; 1992. cap. 3, p. 291.:291) - which means that an utterance does not begin in itself, given that it is anchored in a network of already produced utterances.

In summary, the conception of the subject adopted in our proposal is that he/she is constituted by the O/other, in/through language (88 Pêcheux M, Fuchs C. A propósito da análise automática do discurso: atualização e perspectivas. In: Gadet F, Hak T, organizadores. Por uma análise automática do discurso: uma introdução à obra de Michel Pêcheux. 3. ed. Campinas: Editora da Unicamp; 1990. p. 179.,1010 Authier-Revuz J. Heterogeneidade(s) enunciativa(s). Cad Estud Lingüíst. 1990;19:25-42. https://doi.org/10.20396/cel.v19i0.8636824.
https://doi.org/10.20396/cel.v19i0.86368...
,1111 Pêcheux M. Análise automática do discurso. In: Gadet F, Hak T, organizadores. Por uma análise automática do discurso: uma introdução à obra de Michel Pêcheux. 3. ed. Campinas: Editora da Unicamp; 1990. p. 163-252. ). From this perspective, the subject is not the empirical subject, given that they are not understood as the origin of (their) speech, but as its substrate and effect(1010 Authier-Revuz J. Heterogeneidade(s) enunciativa(s). Cad Estud Lingüíst. 1990;19:25-42. https://doi.org/10.20396/cel.v19i0.8636824.
https://doi.org/10.20396/cel.v19i0.86368...
). Further “[…] they are always simultaneously, the subject of ideology and the unconscious […] traversed by language prior to cognition”(1212 Henry P. A ferramenta imperfeita — língua, sujeito e discurso. Campinas: Unicamp; 1992. p. 188-189.:188-189).

In the Other (ideology and the unconscious) is the position “[…] of the truth regarding the symptom and desire”(1313 Tfouni LV. Entre a análise do discurso e a psicanálise, a verdade do sujeito — análise de narrativas orais. Rev Invest – Ling. 2005;18(2):1-15.). As such, the subject’s desire marked in their discursive process by sliding, will be considered in detail in the present study. It is notable however, that in a discursive process, the Other is linguistically marked. We are dealing in this case, with that instance that we can understand as the “other”, that is, the individual who, interpellated as subject by the language in the discursive process, is configured as a linguistically marked interlocutor in this process. In summary, the “other” corresponds to the alterity that is, in some way, revealed in the discursive chain.

Among the linguistic-discursive characteristics that justify our interest in the subject markers present in utterances with a recurring linguistic structure produced by the child being considered, is the fact that the linguistic markers most commonly observed in children without language acquisition difficulties that indicate subjective constitution were not observed here: the first-person pronouns such as I, my, me, mine, with me. As such, in the utterances that one expects as declaratives of J., a prevalence of second person pronouns (instead of first person) in the interrogative form was observed. Therefore, utterances that we would generally expect to appear in J.’s speech such as I want to go, I want to sleep, I want water appear as do you want to go? Do you want to sleep? Do you want water? In the last instance, the conventional and expected form in first person of her utterances is displaced to third person - such as Does the girl want cake? instead of I want cake.

In these utterances, their recurring structure in syntactic terms is does X want Y. In this structure, both the first and third elements can either be missing or undergo lexical variation during speech production. The following linguistic characteristics, however, remain constant: semantic, volitional in the verb to want; morphological, in the present indicative of this verb; and prosodic, in the interrogative intonational form of the utterance. Given the repeated presence of this linguistic form in J.’s utterances, we ask: is it possible to discursively observe traces of subject position sliding, that is, from the spoken subject (do you want Y?) to a speaking/desiring one (I want Y.) in the chain formed by these utterances? If so, how does such a sliding manifest?

The hypothesis associated with such a question is that J.’s chain of utterances, presenting this recurring linguistic structure (traditionally called echolalias) would make up a sequence in which they are not simply “echoes” of the other’s speech, but a manifestation/marker of subjectivity in discursive production.

Notably, two studies have focused on the speech production of J., seeking, similar to here, the linguistic-discursive markers of her subjectivity. In one of these(1414 Bonatto J, Chacon L. Indícios de subjetividade na fala de uma criança psicótica: um olhar para as hesitações. Revista Distúrbios da Comunicação. [Internet]. 2012 [citado em 2022 Dez 13];24(2):185-97. Disponível em: http://hdl.handle.net/11449/114713
http://hdl.handle.net/11449/114713...
), the hesitations were viewed as such markers; in the other(1515 Coelho NF. Características dos enunciados dispersos de uma criança com diagnóstico fonoaudiológico de distúrbio de linguagem [dissertação]. Cidade São José do Rio Preto: Instituto de Biociências, Letras e Ciências Exatas, Universidade Estadual Paulista; 2012.), it was the markers of refusal that were understood in this manner. However, we believe that utterances with recurring linguistic structures, similar to those that constitute the object of the present investigation, can provide other (and different) indicators of subjectivity in J.’s speech, increasing our understanding of how, in a non-conventional manner, subjectivity can be observed in clinical cases such as J.’s.

PRESENTATION OF THE CLINICAL CASE

The present study was developed according to the following ethical procedures: a review by the Research Ethics Commission of the Faculty of Philosophy and Science of the São Paulo State University (Faculdade de Filosofia e Ciências/Universidade Estadual Paulista - FFC/UNESP), number 0138/2010; and signing of a Free and Informed Consent Form (ICF) by the child’s legal guardian, using the model provided by the Centre for Studies in Education and Health of the Faculty of Philosophy and Sciences of the São Paulo State University (CEES/UNESP).

Filmed and transcribed records of a speech therapy session of around 40 minutes were used, with a child (identified in the data as J.) of female sex, 10 years old at the time of data collection. The session underwent three transcription phases: (i) by a first transcriber; (ii) a revision of the transcription by the same transcriber and by the supervisor monitoring the child’s clinical progress; and finally, (iii) a third reviser, who specifically added information to the transcription regarding the immediate situation in which the therapy session was undertaken. Such information included gestures, facial expressions and basic prosodic information. The final transcription was prepared according to the norms proposed for the Research Project of the São Paulo Study of Urban Linguistic Norms (Project NURC/SP), which studies spoken Portuguese. According to these norms: + corresponds to a silent pause; ? corresponds to interrogative intonation; (( )) corresponds to observations by the transcriber; : corresponds to elongations; / corresponds to interruptions; ( ) corresponds to moments of speech unintelligible for the transcriber or uncertainties in the transcription; and finally, [ ] corresponds to overlapping voices.

The child was given a speech therapy diagnosis of language disorder and a medical diagnosis of early psychosis. At the time of data collection, she had already been undergoing speech therapy for three and a half years. In carrying out therapy, grounded in a clinical framework with a pragmatic perspective, the sessions sought to “restructure” the child’s linguistic expression. From this perspective, strategies such as play activities were employed, that involve “pretending” games that simulate conventional, day-to-day situations in which language is involved. This is the case of the session under consideration here based on a “pretending” game of giving a baby a bath.

Regarding the speech therapy diagnosis of Language Disorder, beyond the alterations at the formal language level, J. mainly presented changes in discursive aspects of language. Regarding these, the I/(O)other relationship showed itself to be particularly weakened, which could be observed in her discursive production, by: (i) an absence of the first-person pronoun; (ii) dispersion of the syntagmatic chain; and (iii) a return of utterances (not necessarily present in the therapeutic scene). Among these utterances, those previously presented, with a recurring linguistic structure of the type does X want Y?, stood out. Therefore, given this prominence, it is in/through the irruption of these utterances that the subject markers manifest in them, were perceived.

DISCUSSION

Here, we outline the investigation’s guiding questions: is it possible to discursively observe signs of the sliding of the subject position, that is, from the spoken subject (Do you want Y?) to the speaking/desiring subject (I want Y.) in the chain formed by these utterances? If so, how does the sliding manifest?

Notably, in the structure does X want Y?, the first and third elements can be missing. When filled in, in position X, fluctuations between the second and third person can occur, with a predominance of second person. In position Y, verbal elements can occur (such as to sleep or to write) or nominal elements such as (water). As we can see, the elements that can occupy the positions X and Y could either be missing or vary, while the (i) verbal volitional element to want and (ii) the interrogative intonation of the utterance, can remain constant.

Chart 1 presents a sample of the therapy session that will be analyzed, during which, mainly utterances with this structure arose. Such utterances are highlighted in bold in the chart.

Chart 1
Sample of a therapy session with different irruptions of utterances with the structure does X want Y?

The first appearance of an utterance with the structure in question can be observed in the table, that is: do you want to sleep? (utterance 37). It initiates a chain of utterances in which the fluctuations of linguistic elements in the positions that accompany the verb allow us to detect the sliding of the subject position. The scene under analysis unfolded around the discursive object making the baby go to sleep. Therefore, in general, it could have favoured the emergence of this utterance. The non-verbal elements could be at the root of such an irruption, given that a small bed and pillow were present in the scene.

The participation of interlocutor T. also stood out (the linguistically marked other in this discursive process) in the irruption of the chain of utterances presenting the linguistic structure under analysis. Therefore, such an utterance, which could have been, on the part of T, heard as I [J.] want to sleep, was heard as T., do you want to sleep?, wherein J. receives as a reply from T.: yes … I won’t go to sleep now … will you go to sleep now?. Given this “expression of her desire” not being heard, J. responds with a laugh and steps away from the bed. T., in turn, continues not to hear, suggesting that J. put the doll to bed, probably trying to operationalize the therapeutic plan - the pretend play of routine care of a baby - within the clinical-theoretical framework that underpins its realization, the approach called pragmatic.

Not hearing J.’s “expression of desire”, together with the contextual elements of the discursive process, could be at the root of the irruption of another of the utterances with the structure being analysed here: do you want water? (utterance 47). In this utterance, the elimination of the position X and a lexical fluctuation on the position Y, from to sleep (a verb) to water (a noun), occurs. Another utterance irrupts soon after the utterance do you want water? (utterance 49). The slippage in discursive production between, do you want to go to sleep? and do you want water? could have occurred, once again, due to another failure on the part of T. to listen to J.’s “expression of desire”. Additionally, we once again observe T. in the scene drawing attention to the doll, given their not noticing that the utterance do you want water? could be contextually linked to care of the doll. Therefore, the sliding between the two utterances could also have occurred due to the non-verbal elements of the therapeutic scene. A toy bathtub and bathroom kit were part of the scene. It was notable that soon after the new utterance do you want water?, J. picks up a toy belonging to the bathroom kit off the floor, an action that suggests an association between this utterance and the toy bathtub present in the physical space of the therapy session - that is, in linguistic terms, a marker of semantic association between bathtub and water.

The ongoing failure to hear J.’s “expression of desire” means that a new utterance with the same structure irrupts in the chain: do you want to write? (utterance 51). This utterance by J. receives the following response from T.: no, we won’t write today … today I didn’t bring any paper:: to write:: … nor a pen::cil. A new fluctuation in the lexical element that fills the Y position can be observed: from water (a noun) to to write (a verb). Notably, in the physical space where the scene took place, there was a small table and child’s stool, non-verbal elements that would have stimulated a connection with writing activities, such as those undertaken in a school environment.

Once again, a failure to hear on the part of T. seems to provoke a further irruption of an utterance with the same structure - do you want some water? (utterance 53) -, equally with a fluctuation in the lexical filling of the position Y, that is, of to write (a verb) to water (a noun). Once again, it is an utterance that irrupts in the discursive process as one more unheard “expression of desire” of (her) place.

Finally, after a series of utterances (53 to 58) in which the discursive object making the baby go to sleep shows itself in conflict with the object give the baby a bath, an utterance irrupts in J.’s speech, which, although discursively connected to the chain already underway, breaks the linguistic structure that underpinned the utterances constituting this chain: I’ll go to sleep Mila … what are you doing? It is the moment when, linguistically and discursively, the sliding of the subject position reveals itself: from the spoken subject (by the other) you to the speaking/desiring subject (of themself) I. The following elements further highlight this sliding: (i) the lexical fluctuation between the verb to want and the verbal locution to go to sleep; (ii) the fluctuation of the morphological characteristics of the verb (present) and the locution (future); and, finally, (iii) the fluctuation of its semantic characteristics (respectively, from the volitional to affirmative).

Some observations should be made regarding the irruption of utterances with the linguistic structure does X want Y? highlighted above. The progress of the child’s clinical case showed that utterances with this structure occurred in all the therapeutic scenes observed. It is also notable that such utterances irrupt even in therapeutic scenes where no physical object present would apparently explain their irruption. Therefore, the emergence of utterances with the structure does X want Y? can be instigated by both verbal and non-verbal elements that echo in the scene and from the already-said/already-experienced background that anchored it. Or possibly and mainly, through the encounter between what was underway in the scene under analysis and echoes from other scenes (not only therapeutic) that make up J.’s discursive memory.

It is evident therefore, that an utterance does not begin in itself, but emerges as a link in a chain of utterances(99 Bakhtin MM. Os gêneros do discurso. In: Bakhtin MM. Estética da criação verbal. 3. ed. São Paulo: Martins Fontes; 1992. cap. 3, p. 291.) in a discursive process. Ultimately, a discursive process does not begin in itself, given that it establishes itself over a prior discourse (or inter-discourse), made up, in the case under analysis, not only of the set of therapeutic scenes between J. and T. but also, of the multiple scenes (non-therapeutic) that J. was invited to participate in her daily life. Therefore, the utterances that emerged in the scene under consideration would be “[…] strictly speaking, an effect of the interdiscourse over itself […].”(88 Pêcheux M, Fuchs C. A propósito da análise automática do discurso: atualização e perspectivas. In: Gadet F, Hak T, organizadores. Por uma análise automática do discurso: uma introdução à obra de Michel Pêcheux. 3. ed. Campinas: Editora da Unicamp; 1990. p. 179.:167), which reinforces the fact that, as we anticipated, the subject is not the source of (his/her) discourse, but its substrate and effect(1010 Authier-Revuz J. Heterogeneidade(s) enunciativa(s). Cad Estud Lingüíst. 1990;19:25-42. https://doi.org/10.20396/cel.v19i0.8636824.
https://doi.org/10.20396/cel.v19i0.86368...
).

Through the fluctuations between linguistic elements that make up the structure does X want Y?, in some manner related to verbal and non-verbal elements of the therapeutic scene and, as we just indicated, also possibly to discursive memory, the chain of utterances manifested the sliding of J.’s subject position. This sliding did not occur in a linear/progressive manner, as we saw in the description of the sample. By contrast, the lexical fluctuations reflected: (i) developments not favoured in the discursive process due to (very) little negotiation of sense between T. and J.; (ii) conflicts between discursive objects; and, finally, (iii) movements that combined echoes of other utterances from the scene, and non-verbal elements that integrated it. These are fluctuations that “construct” the desired discursive sliding - which, from the first to the final utterance, we can linguistically describe through displacements: lexical (to want/to go to sleep); semantic (volitional/affirmative); morphological (present/future); prosodic (interrogative/declarative); and pronominal (I/you). In this context, semantic and pronominal displacements especially reveal a possible sign of the subject’s desire, marked in its extreme positions, by the affirmative and volitional character of the verbs, and by the change of grammatical person, that is, the linguistic and conventional expression of subjectivity.

We see this in I will go to sleep Mila, in which the first-person markers point towards the subjectification of J. towards the position of a speaking/desiring subject, and the continuation of the utterance … what are you doing? allows us to identify a possible distance of the child in relation to their own utterance - which concomitantly, can provide another indicator of their change of subject position. Therefore, when viewed within a linguistic-discursive framework such as that adopted in our study, utterances understood as echolalias can demonstrate, through their supposed structural repeatability (but not discursive), a series of displacements between subject positions.

FINAL CONSIDERATIONS

We believe that the investigation presented here has presented elements for the understanding that a chain of utterances understood as echolalias (given their recurring linguistic structure) can, at the same time, show traces of being anchored (i) in contextual elements from the therapeutic scene, and (ii) in an interdiscursive network that makes the chain possible. In this anchoring, the chain can further reveal indicators of subjectivity, if the irruption of these utterances is understood as an “expression of desire” wherein displacements of the subject position become visible.

However, the functioning of the does X want Y? structure during the therapy sessions of the child J., was found to be quite complex, the reason for our proposal of investigating it. As we can see, the linguistic fluctuations in the chain of utterances with a recurring linguistic structure showed that “[…] waywardness [is] always ready to implant itself in discourse, as a result of weakly constituted subjectivity […].”(1515 Coelho NF. Características dos enunciados dispersos de uma criança com diagnóstico fonoaudiológico de distúrbio de linguagem [dissertação]. Cidade São José do Rio Preto: Instituto de Biociências, Letras e Ciências Exatas, Universidade Estadual Paulista; 2012.:9) However, the movement of the waywardness observed here can indicate a sliding of subject position during discursive production, given that discursive production is a “[…] logically destabilizing site, marked by the tension between the spoken and the unspoken, […]”(1313 Tfouni LV. Entre a análise do discurso e a psicanálise, a verdade do sujeito — análise de narrativas orais. Rev Invest – Ling. 2005;18(2):1-15.:27).

We believe that the present study can provide contributions for the literature and for clinical speech therapy that focuses on aspects of language. As we sought to show, recurring linguistic structures can, in the utterances that contain them, provide clues through linguistic fluctuations and discursive slippages, regarding the subject’s desire in their relation to the O/other. Therefore, although they do not irrupt in a conventional manner, such slippages can show the possibility of changes to subject position. Given this, a contribution to clinical practice involving language offered by an investigation of utterances, anchored in recurring linguistic structures can be observed, to the extent that, in the therapeutic setting, there can be a listening to utterances that, in the first instance, could be understood as connected/rooted in the speech of the other. In clinical practice with language, it is possible to create space for new/alternative senses for such utterances, so as to favour the constitution of the subject of/in language, based on utterances frequently interpreted as being devoid of subjectivity.

Therefore, in cases of children with language disorders of an implied psychotic nature, speech therapy can go beyond structural/formal analyses and reach a significant result as such (verbal and non-verbal). As we sought to demonstrate, a linguistic-discursive perspective such as that outlined here, rather than analysing expressions in a decontextualized manner with structures that could be interpreted as echolalias, allows us to analyse them in relation to the complexity and nuance of their emergence during the discursive process.

  • Study conducted at Programa de Pós-graduação em Fonoaudiologia, Universidade Estadual Paulista - UNESP - Marília (SP), Brasil.
  • Financial support: Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq) (process 305639/2021-8); and Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES).

REFERÊNCIAS

  • 1
    Kanner L. Problems of nosology and psychodinamics in early infantile autism. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 1949;19(3):416-26. PMid:18146742.
  • 2
    Oliveira MT. Quem fala nessa voz? [dissertação]. São Paulo: Pontíficia Universidade Católica de São Paulo; 2001.
  • 3
    Freitas FAF, Montenegro ACA, Fernandes FDM, Delgado IC, Almeida LNA, Alves GÂS. Communicative skills of children with autistic spectrum disorder: clinical and family perception. Rev CEFAC. 2021;23(4):e1521. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216/20212341521
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216/20212341521
  • 4
    Hage SVR, Hter LYSY, Fernandes FDM. Social Communication and pragmatic skills of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Developmental Language Disorder. CoDAS. 2021;34(2):e20210075. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20212021075 PMid:34932641.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20212021075
  • 5
    Balestro JI, Fernandes FDM. Percepção de cuidadores de crianças com Transtorno do Espectro do Autismo quanto ao perfil comunicativo de seus filhos após um programa de orientação fonoaudiológica. CoDAS. 2019;31(1):e20170222. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20182018222 PMid:30843922.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20182018222
  • 6
    Barros I, Fonte R, Souza A. Ecolalia e gestos no autismo: reflexões em torno da metáfora enunciativa. Forma y Función. 33(1):173-89. http://dx.doi.org/10.15446/fyf.v33n1.84184
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.15446/fyf.v33n1.84184
  • 7
    Moraes MEA, Carvalho GMM. Repetição em aquisição de linguagem: notas sobre o espelhamento sonoro infantil e a ecolalia no autismo. Signótica. 2022;34:e70277. http://dx.doi.org/10.5216/sig.v34.70277.
  • 8
    Pêcheux M, Fuchs C. A propósito da análise automática do discurso: atualização e perspectivas. In: Gadet F, Hak T, organizadores. Por uma análise automática do discurso: uma introdução à obra de Michel Pêcheux. 3. ed. Campinas: Editora da Unicamp; 1990. p. 179.
  • 9
    Bakhtin MM. Os gêneros do discurso. In: Bakhtin MM. Estética da criação verbal. 3. ed. São Paulo: Martins Fontes; 1992. cap. 3, p. 291.
  • 10
    Authier-Revuz J. Heterogeneidade(s) enunciativa(s). Cad Estud Lingüíst. 1990;19:25-42. https://doi.org/10.20396/cel.v19i0.8636824
    » https://doi.org/10.20396/cel.v19i0.8636824
  • 11
    Pêcheux M. Análise automática do discurso. In: Gadet F, Hak T, organizadores. Por uma análise automática do discurso: uma introdução à obra de Michel Pêcheux. 3. ed. Campinas: Editora da Unicamp; 1990. p. 163-252.
  • 12
    Henry P. A ferramenta imperfeita — língua, sujeito e discurso. Campinas: Unicamp; 1992. p. 188-189.
  • 13
    Tfouni LV. Entre a análise do discurso e a psicanálise, a verdade do sujeito — análise de narrativas orais. Rev Invest – Ling. 2005;18(2):1-15.
  • 14
    Bonatto J, Chacon L. Indícios de subjetividade na fala de uma criança psicótica: um olhar para as hesitações. Revista Distúrbios da Comunicação. [Internet]. 2012 [citado em 2022 Dez 13];24(2):185-97. Disponível em: http://hdl.handle.net/11449/114713
    » http://hdl.handle.net/11449/114713
  • 15
    Coelho NF. Características dos enunciados dispersos de uma criança com diagnóstico fonoaudiológico de distúrbio de linguagem [dissertação]. Cidade São José do Rio Preto: Instituto de Biociências, Letras e Ciências Exatas, Universidade Estadual Paulista; 2012.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    18 Dec 2023
  • Date of issue
    2024

History

  • Received
    13 Dec 2022
  • Accepted
    04 July 2023
Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia Al. Jaú, 684, 7º andar, 01420-002 São Paulo - SP Brasil, Tel./Fax 55 11 - 3873-4211 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista@codas.org.br