Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

“Sign exchanges”: characterization of phonological processes occurred during the Libras acquisition by deaf preschoolers

ABSTRACT

Purpose

To describe the characteristics of the phonological processes found in deaf preschoolers’ sign language.

Methods

Fifteen deaf children aged 3 to 7 years participated in the study. Data collection was conducted by filming the application of the FONOLIBRAS test, which requires participants to name 50 figures in Libras (Brazilian sign language). Transcription was performed on SignWriting; Libras signals with phonological processes were classified by type, parameter, and hand(s) involved. Finally, the data received statistical treatment.

Results

All deaf children showed signs of linguistic variation in relation to the adult pattern, that is, phonological processes, confirming that these processes are a natural phenomenon of child development in sign languages ​​as in spoken languages. The most frequent process was assimilation, while replacement, epenthesis, elision, and metathesis were also present. The hand configuration parameter was the most affected. The occurrence was greater in the passive hand.

Conclusion

As phonological processes are part of the acquisition of sign languages and can be naturally suppressed or not, these phenomena deserve the attention of professionals who work in the area of deafness in order to optimize the communication development of users of these languages.

Keywords:
Language development; Phonetics; Deafness

RESUMO

Objetivo

Descrever as características dos processos fonológicos encontrados na língua de sinais de pré-escolares surdos.

Métodos

Participaram da pesquisa 15 crianças surdas, com idade entre 3 e 7 anos. A coleta de dados ocorreu por meio de filmagem da aplicação do Instrumento de avaliação fonológica da língua de sinais brasileira: FONOLIBRAS, que propõe nomear, em Libras (Língua Brasileira de Sinais), 50 figuras. Foi realizada transcrição em SignWriting e os sinais de Libras com processos fonológicos foram classificados quanto ao tipo, parâmetro e mão(s) envolvida(s). Por fim, os dados receberam tratamento estatístico.

Resultados

Todas as crianças surdas apresentaram produção de sinais com variações linguísticas, em relação ao padrão adulto, ou seja, processos fonológicos, confirmando que é um fenômeno natural do desenvolvimento infantil também nas línguas de sinais. O tipo mais frequente foi assimilação, também estando presentes substituição, epêntese, elisão e metátese. O parâmetro configuração de mão foi o mais afetado e a ocorrência maior, na mão passiva.

Conclusão

De que modo os processos fonológicos fazem parte da aquisição das línguas de sinais, podendo ser naturalmente suprimidos, ou não, é um assunto que merece a atenção dos profissionais que atuam na área da surdez, de modo a aperfeiçoar o desenvolvimento da comunicação dos usuários destas línguas.

Palavras-chave:
Desenvolvimento da linguagem; Fonética; Surdez

INTRODUCTION

In sign language, it is possible to observe a phenomenon similar to what is popularly called “speech exchanges” in oral language, more technically known as phonological processes.

From a more comprehensive view, a phonological process is any change observed in the phonological level of languages that may result in linguistic modification or variation(11 Diniz HG. A história da língua de sinais brasileira (libras): um estudo descritivo de mudanças fonológicas e lexicais [tese]. Florianópolis (SC): Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina; 2010. ). Modification occurs when over time, an old form of language is substituted with a more present one; and variation occurs when this modification adjusts to the individual’s stylistic, social, or linguistic factors(22 Schembri A, Mckee D, Mckee R, Pivac S, Johnston T, Goswell D. Phonological variation and change in Australian and New Zealand Sign Language: the location variable. Lang Var Change. 2009;21(2):193-231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954394509990081.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S095439450999...
).

In the literature, the emphasis of research has usually fallen on the phonological processes of the variation type that occurs in childhood, more specifically in oral-aural languages.

Although the interest of the present research relates to phonological processes occurring in visuomotor languages, the background studies in this area, even if in another modality, have served as a starting point.

In Classical Generative Theory, the definitions and possible causes of phonological processes in childhood have been described as a “[…] problem in the phonological functioning of one or more distinctive features […]”(33 Matzenauer-Hernandorena CL. Teorias fonológicas e aquisição fonológica. Estudos Linguísticos [Internet]. 2002 [citado em 2017 set 5];31(1). Disponível em: http://www.gel.org.br/estudoslinguisticos/volumes/31/htm/mesaredo/MR01.htm
http://www.gel.org.br/estudoslinguistic...
).

In Natural Generative Theory, Stampe was the pioneer of the term “phonological process” and defined it as:

[...] a mental operation that applies in speech to substitute, for a class of sounds or sound sequences presenting a specific common difficulty to the speech capacity of the individual (44 Lamprecht RR. Aquisição fonológica do português: perfil de desenvolvimento e subsídios para terapia. Porto Alegre: Artmed; 2004. :41).

Lamprecht defined the term “repair strategies” as:

[...] strategies adopted by children to tailor the achievement of the target system - the language spoken by the adults of their social group - to their phonological system. Basically, it refers to what children do in place of the segment and/or the syllabic structure that they do not know yet or whose production they do not dominate(44 Lamprecht RR. Aquisição fonológica do português: perfil de desenvolvimento e subsídios para terapia. Porto Alegre: Artmed; 2004. :28).

According to the Autosegmental Theory, it is the “[…] exchange of one segment for another that already integrates the child's system […]”(33 Matzenauer-Hernandorena CL. Teorias fonológicas e aquisição fonológica. Estudos Linguísticos [Internet]. 2002 [citado em 2017 set 5];31(1). Disponível em: http://www.gel.org.br/estudoslinguisticos/volumes/31/htm/mesaredo/MR01.htm
http://www.gel.org.br/estudoslinguistic...
).

In the view of the Optimality Theory, this is an inappropriate ordering of the target language constraints hierarchy(55 Lamprecht RR. Diferenças no ranqueamento de restrições como origem de diferenças na aquisição fonológica. Let Hoje. 1999;34(3):65-82. ).

Studies indicate that the number of phonological processes or repair strategies is directly proportional to the degree of difficulty of acquisition and production of the minimal linguistic units involved, as well as to the degree of phonological deviation present(66 Ghisleni MRL, Keske-Soares M, Mezzomo CL. O uso das estratégias de reparo, considerando a gravidade do desvio fonológico evolutivo. Rev CEFAC. 2010;12(5):766-71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462010005000030.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-1846201...
,77 Wiethan FM, Mota HB. Emprego de estratégias de reparo para os fonemas fricativos no desvio fonológico. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2012;17(1):572-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-80342012000100007.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-8034201...
).

Thus, it can be concluded that in general, phonological processes manifested in childhood are usually related to the restrictions of immaturity in the capacity needed to reproduce the adult pattern, resulting in phonetic-phonological simplifications, allowing an approximate production of the target linguistic system.

In other words, the child communicates even if full maturity has not been reached. Consequently, the child’s vocabulary will present modifications/simplifications in the signifier (form) of the sign (word or sign), compared to the adult standard.

Phonological processes can be classified as(88 Teixeira ER. Os processos de simplificação fonológica na descrição do desenvolvimento de crianças falantes do português em situações aquisicionais típicas e atípicas. Prolíngua. 2015;10(1):79-92. ):

  • Substitution processes, manifested in the paradigmatic axis of the sub-lexical units’ contrasts. These processes are characterized by the composition of traces. Simplification occurs by replacing members of a class with members of another natural class* * Natural class “[…] is the set of segments that share similar traces and undergo common phonological rules […]”(9:27). In the example, both the /s/ and /t/ phonemes are produced by the tongue in contact with the alveolar region without vocal fold vibration, but they are differentiated by the trace relative to the breath support /s/ is [+continuous] and /t/ is [-continuous]. . Example: “tapo” instead of “sapo”;

  • Structural modifier processes, manifested in the syntagmatic axis of the sequences of the sub-lexical units. These processes are characterized by the combination of phonemes for the formation of morphological and lexical units. Example: “rávore” instead of “árvore”;

  • Context-sensitive processes, manifested in the paradigmatic and syntagmatic axes, influenced by contextual factors. These processes are characterized by the substitution of features or segments by others more similar to the near phonological context. Example: “pepeta” instead of “chupeta.”

Returning attention to sign language, the Natural Phonology Theory argues that phonological processes are innate, natural, and universal(1010 Lima LM, Queiroga BAM. Aquisição fonológica em crianças com antecedentes de desnutrição. Rev CEFAC. 2017;9(1):13-20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462007000100003.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-1846200...
). Thus, they can be found in both oral-aural and visual-spatial language modalities (1111 Carvalho S, Moita M, Mineiro A. As palavras e os gestos que nos “tornam” humanos. Cadernos de Saúde. 2013;6:13-5. ).

In sign languages, such changes in the phonetic-phonological level are manifested as alterations in one or more constitutive parameters of the sign(11 Diniz HG. A história da língua de sinais brasileira (libras): um estudo descritivo de mudanças fonológicas e lexicais [tese]. Florianópolis (SC): Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina; 2010. ). As such, it can be affirmed that a sign contains phonological process(es) when observed, in relation to the target language, and articulated differences are manifested in hand configuration, point of articulation, movement, orientation, number of hands and/or non-manual markings.

The literature indicates that Liddel and Johnson were probably the first authors to study the occurrence of phonological processes in sign languages, in relation to American Sign Language (ASL), and classified the processes as: epenthesis of the movement, deletion of the prehension, metathesis, twinning, assimilation, reduction, perseveration and anticipation(1212 Liddell SK, Johnson RE. American sign language: the phonological base. Sign Lang Stud. 1989;64(1):195-277. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/sls.1989.0027.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/sls.1989.0027...
).

In Brazil, Lodenir Karnopp was one of the pioneers of studies on the subject.

In 1994, when investigating the acquisition of Libras in four deaf children of deaf parents between the ages of 2 years and 8 months and 5 years and 9 months, it was observed that the highest substitution rate occurred in the hand configuration parameter, when compared to the movement and point of articulation, and these substitutions were systematic(1313 Karnopp LB. Aquisição fonológica nas línguas de sinais. Let Hoje. 1997;32(4):147-62. ). In 1999, when conducting a longitudinal study on the phonological acquisition of Libras by a deaf girl with deaf parents and sisters, a higher occurrence of substitutions in this parameter was confirmed(1414 Karnopp LB. Aquisição fonológica na língua brasileira de sinais: estudo longitudinal de uma criança surda [tese]. Porto Alegre (RS): Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul; 1999. ).

When the “phonological awareness assessment instrument, configuration parameter for deaf children using Brazilian Sign Language” was applied to 15 deaf children aged between 6 years and 11 years and 1 month, it was found that the signs classified as “expected modified denomination” (phonological processes) occurred less frequently in children with a longer period of linguistic exposure to Libras(1515 Cruz CR, Lamprecht RR. Proposta de instrumento de avaliação da consciência fonológica, parâmetro configuração de mão, para crianças surdas utentes da Língua de Sinais Brasileira. Let Hoje. 2008;43(3):98-106. ).

In longitudinal observation of spontaneous productions in a deaf child of deaf parents, between the ages of 1 year and 6 months and 2 years and 6 months, processes classified as substitution were identified, affecting more than one parameter in the same sign, and more than half involved the hand configuration parameter(1616 Bento NA. Os parâmetros fonológicos: configuração de mãos, ponto de articulação e movimento na aquisição da língua brasileira de sinais: um estudo de caso [dissertação]. Salvador, BA: Universidade Federal da Bahia; 2010. ).

Regarding the creation and application of the “Instrument of phonological evaluation of Brazilian Sign Language: FONOLIBRAS”, four deaf children of hearing parents, aged between 6 and 12 years, presented phonological processes classified as epenthesis, elision, metathesis, and assimilation(1717 Costa RCR. Proposta de instrumento para a avaliação fonológica da língua brasileira de sinais: FONOLIBRAS [dissertação]. Salvador, BA: Universidade Federal da Bahia; 2012. ).

In another application of the “phonological awareness assessment instrument, configuration parameter, for deaf children using the Brazilian Sign Language,” the presence of phonological processes was observed in the sign language of a deaf girl with neurological comorbidity aged 5 years and 10 months, and it was noted that the parameter that had the most changes in relation to the target language was the hand configuration(1818 Rizzon M, Vidor DCGM, Cruz CR. Avaliação de linguagem em um caso de associação entre surdez e paquigiria. Audiol Commun Res. 2013;18(3):220-30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2317-64312013000300012.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2317-6431201...
).

The occurrence and disappearance of the phonological processes must obey certain chronology, and consequently, the persistence of these beyond the expected age constitutes a delay in the development process and possible atypicity(88 Teixeira ER. Os processos de simplificação fonológica na descrição do desenvolvimento de crianças falantes do português em situações aquisicionais típicas e atípicas. Prolíngua. 2015;10(1):79-92. ).

Atypical phonological development can be classified as(1919 Ferreira-Gonçalves G. Representação fonológica em uma abordagem conexionista: formalização dos contrastes encobertos. Let Hoje. 2008;43(3):61-8. ):

  • Delayed development: the production identifies with earlier stages of acquisition;

  • Variable development: production with overlapping stages (delayed or advanced, in relation to the normal production);

  • Different development: production with processes not found in the normal acquisition.

The investigation and distinction of each phonological process in sign languages, with regards to normality and atypia, are still a challenge(1717 Costa RCR. Proposta de instrumento para a avaliação fonológica da língua brasileira de sinais: FONOLIBRAS [dissertação]. Salvador, BA: Universidade Federal da Bahia; 2012. ). That is to say, it is still necessary to establish parameters on the phonetic-phonological acquisition of sign languages that specify which phonological processes are part of the development and until what age they usually occur (typical x age processes) and also, which processes are not common in the development (atypical processes).

The aim of the present study was to describe the characteristics of the phonological processes found in the sign language of deaf preschoolers.

METHODS

This research is part of a master’s thesis project approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Fluminense Federal University under the nº 57068916.6.0000.5243. All those responsible for the subjects involved signed the Free and Informed Consent form.

This article focused on the phonological processes that occurred during the naming task carried out by the students of the Primary Education sector in a teaching institution for the deaf.

A total of 25 deaf students of both genders from the EI 3, EI 4, and EI 5 groups and of either deaf or hearing parents were invited to participate in the study, regardless of whether they were newcomers to the institution, and including those with multiple disabilities.

Excluded from the sample were the students whose parents or guardians did not authorize their participation (one student), those who refused to participate (three students), those who dropped out of the test (three students), and those who did not attend on the dates scheduled for data collection (three students). Thus, the total sample size was 15 deaf preschoolers (n=15).

Data collection was carried out in the facilities of the Primary Education sector itself, in the year 2016, in the months of October (first collection) and December (second collection). The dissertation investigated a mini-course of continuous professional training that proposed that teachers incorporate cognitive and psychomotor stimulation in their classes, aiming to improve the acquisition of Libras by their students. However, in this paper, only the data from the first stage (October 2016) is presented in order to demonstrate the natural behavior of this group, that is, free of such specific interventions.

The evaluation instrument used was the FONOLIBRAS test(1717 Costa RCR. Proposta de instrumento para a avaliação fonológica da língua brasileira de sinais: FONOLIBRAS [dissertação]. Salvador, BA: Universidade Federal da Bahia; 2012. ), in which participants are required to name 50 figures in sign language. The individual evaluation time did not exceed 15 minutes.

Filming was carried out by means of a Sony Xperia Z3 mobile phone while the researcher interacted with the children, showing them the binder of figures.

As predicted by the author of the FONOLIBRAS test, due to the social linguistic variations, the signs on the answer sheet needed to be adjusted to the Libras used in that deaf community.

The videos were analyzed and the students' responses were transcribed in SignWriting (using the SW-Edit program). As anticipated by the author of the test, the answers were classified as: expected name, name in the same semantic field, absent name, or home sign.

When phonological processes occurred in Libras signs (expected name and name in the same semantic field), these were classified as: assimilation, substitution, epenthesis, elision, or metathesis and the parameters (hand configuration, point of articulation, movement, orientation, and number of hands** ** Being an appointment task, the facial and/or corporal expression parameter was not evaluated and the “number of hands,” also called “hand configuration,” despite being less recognized as a parameter, was included, since it was relevant to the purposes of this study. ) and the hand(s) involved (passive, active, or both) were specified.

In order to verify the influence of the gender variable on the production of Libras signs with phonological processes, the t-Test was applied for independent samples, and to verify the influence of the age and class variables, ANOVA tests were implemented.

The quantitative data was arranged in Excel spreadsheets and analyzed by a professional statistician.

RESULTS

Regarding the influence of the gender variable on the production of Libras signs with phonological processes, there was no evidence that female preschoolers (mean= 40.8 and deviation= 17.8) presented more deviations than male preschoolers (mean= 27.7 and deviation= 6.4) (p = 0.111). Similarly, with regards to the influence of the age and class variables, there was no evidence of differences in the percentages among the ages (p = 0.554) nor among the groups (p = 0.207) ( Table 1 ). In other words, it was not possible to predict the performance against the gender, age, or class groups to which deaf children belonged.

Table 1
Characteristics of the subjects of the sample, with their respective performances

Concerning the group's behavior (n= 15), the average percentage of signs with phonological processes was 35.53%, standard deviation 15.50%, minimum 14.89%, maximum 67.86%, and median 35.00%, drawing attention to the fact that all subjects presented phonological processes in the signs ( Figure 1 ).

Figure 1
Percentage distribution of Libras signs with phonological process(es) (n=15)

Substitution, epenthesis, elision, metathesis, and assimilation types of phonological processes were found ( Chart 1 ).

Chart 1
Classification of phonological processes, with respective definitions and sample examples

It was observed that the hand configuration parameter was the most frequently affected by phonological processes, followed by point of articulation, movement, orientation, and finally, number of hands ( Figure 2 ).

Figure 2
Phonological processes characteristics: sign language parameter involved

In some instances, the signs presented more than one type of phonological process in more than one parameter ( Chart 2 ).

Chart 2
Examples of sample signs with more than one type of phonological process, in more than one parameter

The phonological process of the assimilation type was the most frequent in the subjects' production, followed by substitution, epenthesis, elision, and metathesis ( Figure 3 ).

Figure 3
Phonological processes characteristics: type

In general, the phonological processes occurred more frequently in the passive hand, followed by the active hand, and less frequently, in both hands ( Figure 4 ).

Figure 4
Phonological processes characteristics: hand(s) involved

DISCUSSION

Based on the data obtained, it can be confirmed that phonological processes are universal, innate, and natural(1010 Lima LM, Queiroga BAM. Aquisição fonológica em crianças com antecedentes de desnutrição. Rev CEFAC. 2017;9(1):13-20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462007000100003.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-1846200...
), given that they appeared in the language sample of all 15 subjects of the research. Phonological processes are also manifested in the linguistic development of deaf children that use sign language(1111 Carvalho S, Moita M, Mineiro A. As palavras e os gestos que nos “tornam” humanos. Cadernos de Saúde. 2013;6:13-5. ).

Regarding the factors that influenced the results, although it is expected that older female children with greater level of schooling will perform better in language, with less number of phonological processes than their younger male counterparts, with lower level of schooling, this could not be confirmed. In view of this, it would be interesting to investigate what other factors could influence the phonetic-phonological performance of deaf children during the acquisition phases of sign language.

It should be remembered that, among the subjects of the research, there were children of both hearing parents and deaf Libras-using parents, as well as students who had recently joined the bilingual teaching institution. Consequently, it can be observed that these variables alone already promote an enormous heterogeneity of the linguistic-environmental realities relative to Libras communication that these children have been and are exposed to.

Researchers have stated that there is a relation between the initial linguistic experience (the moment/age of exposure to the language, the quality and quantity of the linguistic stimulus), the phonological work memory, and language acquisition results(2020 Pierce LJ, Genesee F, Delcenserie A, Morgan G. Variations in phonological working memory: linking early language experiences and language learning outcomes. Appl Psycholinguist. 2017;38(6):1265-300. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000236.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S014271641700...
). Therefore, one limitation of the present research was not to foresee the need to request the authorization of those responsible to disclose the variables that relate to the linguistic experience and thus, identify whether or not there was a correlation with the performance in the articulation of the signs.

Regarding the characteristics of the phonological processes manifested in the sample of deaf preschoolers, there were more difficulties (more phonological processes) in the hand configuration parameter, which confirms the information found in the scientific literature(1313 Karnopp LB. Aquisição fonológica nas línguas de sinais. Let Hoje. 1997;32(4):147-62. ,1414 Karnopp LB. Aquisição fonológica na língua brasileira de sinais: estudo longitudinal de uma criança surda [tese]. Porto Alegre (RS): Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul; 1999. ,1616 Bento NA. Os parâmetros fonológicos: configuração de mãos, ponto de articulação e movimento na aquisição da língua brasileira de sinais: um estudo de caso [dissertação]. Salvador, BA: Universidade Federal da Bahia; 2010. ,1818 Rizzon M, Vidor DCGM, Cruz CR. Avaliação de linguagem em um caso de associação entre surdez e paquigiria. Audiol Commun Res. 2013;18(3):220-30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2317-64312013000300012.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2317-6431201...
). This fact could be explained by the high degree of development of cognitive (visual perception, attention, and memory) and psychomotor abilities (tone, balance, laterality, body motion, spatio-temporal structuring, and fine and broad motor coordination) required for the reception and expression of this parameter.

As for the phonological processes classification types, all of those described in Libras were found, both by Bento (substitution)(1616 Bento NA. Os parâmetros fonológicos: configuração de mãos, ponto de articulação e movimento na aquisição da língua brasileira de sinais: um estudo de caso [dissertação]. Salvador, BA: Universidade Federal da Bahia; 2010. ), and by Costa (epenthesis, elision, metathesis, and assimilation)(1717 Costa RCR. Proposta de instrumento para a avaliação fonológica da língua brasileira de sinais: FONOLIBRAS [dissertação]. Salvador, BA: Universidade Federal da Bahia; 2012. ). Since they fall within the classification proposed by Teixeira (substitution processes, structural modifier processes, and context-sensitive processes)(88 Teixeira ER. Os processos de simplificação fonológica na descrição do desenvolvimento de crianças falantes do português em situações aquisicionais típicas e atípicas. Prolíngua. 2015;10(1):79-92. ), they can be considered complementary.

The assimilation type was the phonological process with the highest incidence, which may be explained by the tendency of the non-dominant hand to mirror (imitate) the dominant hand, leading to preference for signs with equal parameters in both hands, a tendency of child motor coordination called “Complicity or solidarity”(2121 Meier RP. Modalidade e aquisição da língua: estratégias e restrições na aprendizagem dos primeiros sinais. In: Quadros RM, Vasconcelos MLB, organizadores. Questões teóricas das pesquisas em línguas de sinais. Florianópolis: Arara Azul; 2006. p. 211-24. ). In other words, assimilation appears before the need to dissociate movements, modifying the sign to overcome this type of difficulty.

It was anticipated that the phonological processes would have a greater tendency to manifest in the passive hand, since this frequently corresponds to the non-dominant hand which, by definition, would be the non-favored hand for more complex actions and therefore, the less skilled.

CONCLUSIONS

Considering that the phonological processes are manifestations observed during the acquisition of sign languages by the children, and which can be naturally suppressed or not, it is a matter that deserves to be given more attention with the aim of creating evaluative, preventive, and remedial actions for language acquisition by the users of these languages.

It is believed that the results of this research will contribute to the expansion of knowledge about sign languages, specifically with regards to the phonetic-phonological linguistic level, serving as a basis for future studies and practices in the area of deafness.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

To the National Institute of Education of the Deaf (INES), for the support granted to carry out this research.

  • Study carried out at Instituto Nacional de Educação de Surdos – INES – Rio de Janeiro (RJ), Brasil.
  • Funding: None.
  • *
    Natural class “[…] is the set of segments that share similar traces and undergo common phonological rules […]”(99 Schardosim CR, Trombetta N. Fonologia: breve histórico dos estudos no Estruturalismo e Gerativismo. Revista E-scrita. 2012;3(2A):17-31. :27). In the example, both the /s/ and /t/ phonemes are produced by the tongue in contact with the alveolar region without vocal fold vibration, but they are differentiated by the trace relative to the breath support /s/ is [+continuous] and /t/ is [-continuous].
  • **
    Being an appointment task, the facial and/or corporal expression parameter was not evaluated and the “number of hands,” also called “hand configuration,” despite being less recognized as a parameter, was included, since it was relevant to the purposes of this study.

REFERÊNCIAS

  • 1
    Diniz HG. A história da língua de sinais brasileira (libras): um estudo descritivo de mudanças fonológicas e lexicais [tese]. Florianópolis (SC): Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina; 2010.
  • 2
    Schembri A, Mckee D, Mckee R, Pivac S, Johnston T, Goswell D. Phonological variation and change in Australian and New Zealand Sign Language: the location variable. Lang Var Change. 2009;21(2):193-231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954394509990081.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954394509990081
  • 3
    Matzenauer-Hernandorena CL. Teorias fonológicas e aquisição fonológica. Estudos Linguísticos [Internet]. 2002 [citado em 2017 set 5];31(1). Disponível em: http://www.gel.org.br/estudoslinguisticos/volumes/31/htm/mesaredo/MR01.htm
    » http://www.gel.org.br/estudoslinguisticos/volumes/31/htm/mesaredo/MR01.htm
  • 4
    Lamprecht RR. Aquisição fonológica do português: perfil de desenvolvimento e subsídios para terapia. Porto Alegre: Artmed; 2004.
  • 5
    Lamprecht RR. Diferenças no ranqueamento de restrições como origem de diferenças na aquisição fonológica. Let Hoje. 1999;34(3):65-82.
  • 6
    Ghisleni MRL, Keske-Soares M, Mezzomo CL. O uso das estratégias de reparo, considerando a gravidade do desvio fonológico evolutivo. Rev CEFAC. 2010;12(5):766-71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462010005000030.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462010005000030
  • 7
    Wiethan FM, Mota HB. Emprego de estratégias de reparo para os fonemas fricativos no desvio fonológico. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2012;17(1):572-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-80342012000100007.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-80342012000100007
  • 8
    Teixeira ER. Os processos de simplificação fonológica na descrição do desenvolvimento de crianças falantes do português em situações aquisicionais típicas e atípicas. Prolíngua. 2015;10(1):79-92.
  • 9
    Schardosim CR, Trombetta N. Fonologia: breve histórico dos estudos no Estruturalismo e Gerativismo. Revista E-scrita. 2012;3(2A):17-31.
  • 10
    Lima LM, Queiroga BAM. Aquisição fonológica em crianças com antecedentes de desnutrição. Rev CEFAC. 2017;9(1):13-20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462007000100003.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462007000100003
  • 11
    Carvalho S, Moita M, Mineiro A. As palavras e os gestos que nos “tornam” humanos. Cadernos de Saúde. 2013;6:13-5.
  • 12
    Liddell SK, Johnson RE. American sign language: the phonological base. Sign Lang Stud. 1989;64(1):195-277. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/sls.1989.0027.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/sls.1989.0027
  • 13
    Karnopp LB. Aquisição fonológica nas línguas de sinais. Let Hoje. 1997;32(4):147-62.
  • 14
    Karnopp LB. Aquisição fonológica na língua brasileira de sinais: estudo longitudinal de uma criança surda [tese]. Porto Alegre (RS): Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul; 1999.
  • 15
    Cruz CR, Lamprecht RR. Proposta de instrumento de avaliação da consciência fonológica, parâmetro configuração de mão, para crianças surdas utentes da Língua de Sinais Brasileira. Let Hoje. 2008;43(3):98-106.
  • 16
    Bento NA. Os parâmetros fonológicos: configuração de mãos, ponto de articulação e movimento na aquisição da língua brasileira de sinais: um estudo de caso [dissertação]. Salvador, BA: Universidade Federal da Bahia; 2010.
  • 17
    Costa RCR. Proposta de instrumento para a avaliação fonológica da língua brasileira de sinais: FONOLIBRAS [dissertação]. Salvador, BA: Universidade Federal da Bahia; 2012.
  • 18
    Rizzon M, Vidor DCGM, Cruz CR. Avaliação de linguagem em um caso de associação entre surdez e paquigiria. Audiol Commun Res. 2013;18(3):220-30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2317-64312013000300012.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2317-64312013000300012
  • 19
    Ferreira-Gonçalves G. Representação fonológica em uma abordagem conexionista: formalização dos contrastes encobertos. Let Hoje. 2008;43(3):61-8.
  • 20
    Pierce LJ, Genesee F, Delcenserie A, Morgan G. Variations in phonological working memory: linking early language experiences and language learning outcomes. Appl Psycholinguist. 2017;38(6):1265-300. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000236.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000236
  • 21
    Meier RP. Modalidade e aquisição da língua: estratégias e restrições na aprendizagem dos primeiros sinais. In: Quadros RM, Vasconcelos MLB, organizadores. Questões teóricas das pesquisas em línguas de sinais. Florianópolis: Arara Azul; 2006. p. 211-24.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    08 Nov 2018
  • Date of issue
    2018

History

  • Received
    05 Sept 2017
  • Accepted
    20 Mar 2018
Academia Brasileira de Audiologia Rua Itapeva, 202, conjunto 61, CEP 01332-000, Tel.: (11) 3253-8711, Fax: (11) 3253-8473 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista@audiologiabrasil.org.br