Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Developing a dynamic virtual stimulation protocol to induce linear egomotion during orthostatic posture control test

Abstract

Introduction

In this work, the effect of a dynamic visual stimulation (DS) protocol was used to induce egomotion, the center of pressure (COP) displacement response.

Methods

DS was developed concerning the scenario structure (chessboard-pattern floor and furniture) and luminance. To move the scenario in a discrete forward (or backward) direction, the furniture is expanded (or reduced) and the black and white background is reversed during floor translation while the luminance is increased (or reduced) by steps of 2 cd/m2. This protocol was evaluated using COP signals from 29 healthy volunteers: standing on a force platform observing the virtual scene (1.72 × 1.16 m) projected 1 m ahead (visual incidence angle: θl = 81.4° and θv = 60.2°), which moves with constant velocity (2 m/s) during 250 ms. A set of 100 DS was applied in random order, interspersed by a 10 s of static scene.

Results

The Tukey post-hoc test (p < 0.001) indicated egomotion in the same direction of DS. COP displacement increased over stimulation (8.4 ± 1.7 to 22.6 ±5.3 mm), as well as time to recover stability (4.1 ± 0.4 to 7.2 ± 0.6 s). The peak of egomotion during DSF occurred 200 ms after DSB (Wilcoxon, p = 0.002).

Conclusion

The dynamic configuration of this protocol establishes virtual flow effects of linear egomotion dependent on the direction of the dynamic visual stimulation. This finding indicates the potential application of the proposed virtual dynamic stimulation protocol to investigate the cortical visual evoked response in postural control studies.

Keywords:
Center of pressure; Dynamic visual stimulation; Egomotion; Postural control; Virtual reality; Visual optic flow

Introduction

Virtual reality technologies have been used in postural research and neurorehabilitation to compose a dynamic visual stimulation scene (DS) during balance control test (Akizuki et al., 2005Akizuki H, Uno A, Arai K, Morioka S, Ohyama S, Nishiike S, Tamura K, Takeda N. Effects of immersion in virtual reality on postural control. Neuroscience Letters. 2005; 379(1):23-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2004.12.041. PMid:15814192.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2004....
; Darekar et al. 2015Darekar A, McFadyen BJ, Lamontagne A, Fung J. Efficacy of virtual reality-based intervention on balance and mobility disorders post-stroke: a scoping review. Journal of Neuroengineering and Rehabilitation. 2015; 12(1):46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12984-015-0035-3. PMid:25957577.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12984-015-003...
; Keshner and Kenyon, 2004Keshner EA, Kenyon RV. Using immersive technology for postural research and rehabilitation. Assistive Technology. 2004; 16(1):54-62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400435.2004.10132074. PMid:15357148.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400435.2004....
; Streepey et al., 2007Streepey JW, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Field of view and base of support width influence postural responses to visual stimuli during quiet stance. Gait & Posture. 2007; 25(1):49-55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.12.013. PMid:16464594.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
; Wang et al., 2010Wang Y, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Identifying the control of physically and perceptually evoked sway responses with coincident visual scene velocities and tilt of the base of support. Experimental Brain Research. 2010; 201(4):663-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-2082-0. PMid:19924408.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-208...
). This technique has the advantage of deploying specific scenarios, similar to the real world, to be applied simultaneously with a force platform in order to investigate body sway response due to subject’s self-motion illusion, also called egomotion (Akizuki et al., 2005Akizuki H, Uno A, Arai K, Morioka S, Ohyama S, Nishiike S, Tamura K, Takeda N. Effects of immersion in virtual reality on postural control. Neuroscience Letters. 2005; 379(1):23-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2004.12.041. PMid:15814192.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2004....
; Guerraz and Bronstein, 2008Guerraz M, Bronstein AM. Mechanisms underlying visually induced body sway. Neuroscience Letters. 2008; 443(1):12-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.07.053. PMid:18672020.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008....
; Keshner et al., 2006Keshner EA, Dokka K, Kenyon RV. Influences of the perception of self-motion on postural parameters. Cyberpsychology & Behavior. 2006; 9(2):163-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2006.9.163. PMid:16640471.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2006.9.163...
; Slobounov et al., 2006Slobounov S, Wu T, Hallett M, Shibasaki H, Slobounov E, Newell K. Neural underpinning of postural responses to visual field motion. Biological Psychology. 2006; 72(2):188-97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.10.005. PMid:16338048.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.20...
; Streepey et al., 2007Streepey JW, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Field of view and base of support width influence postural responses to visual stimuli during quiet stance. Gait & Posture. 2007; 25(1):49-55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.12.013. PMid:16464594.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
; Tossavainen et al., 2003Tossavainen T, Juhola M, Pyykkö I, Aalto H, Toppila E. Development of virtual reality stimuli for force platform posturography. International Journal of Medical Informatics. 2003; 70(2-3):277-83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1386-5056(03)00034-0. PMid:12909179.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1386-5056(03)...
). Nevertheless, this evoked postural response is dependent on the visual motion processing and the central brain integration of the environment information with the proprioceptive and vestibular system (Akizuki et al., 2005Akizuki H, Uno A, Arai K, Morioka S, Ohyama S, Nishiike S, Tamura K, Takeda N. Effects of immersion in virtual reality on postural control. Neuroscience Letters. 2005; 379(1):23-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2004.12.041. PMid:15814192.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2004....
; Guerraz and Bronstein, 2008Guerraz M, Bronstein AM. Mechanisms underlying visually induced body sway. Neuroscience Letters. 2008; 443(1):12-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.07.053. PMid:18672020.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008....
; Keshner et al., 2004Keshner EA, Kenyon RV, Langston J. Postural responses exhibit multisensory dependencies with discordant visual and support surface motion. Journal of Vestibular Research. 2004; 14(4):307-19. PMid:15328445.).

In real situation, the body displacement produces motion of the visual scene at the retina (Berthoz et al., 1975Berthoz A, Pavard B, Young LR. Perception of linear horizontal self-motion induced by peripheral vision (linearvection): basic characteristics and visual-vestibular interactions. Experimental Brain Research. 1975; 23(5):471-89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916. PMid:1081949.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916...
; 1979Berthoz A, Lacour M, Soechting JF, Vidal PP. The role of vision in the control of posture during linear motion. Progress in Brain Research. 1979; 50:197-209. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60820-1. PMid:551426.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(08)...
; Gibson, 1950Gibson JJ. The perception of the visual world. Oxford: Houghton Mifflin; 1950.; 1954Gibson JJ. The visual perception of objective motion and subjective movement. Psychological Review. 1954; 61(5):304-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0061885. PMid:13204493.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0061885...
; Mestre, 1992Mestre DR. Visual perception of self-motion. In: Proteau L, Elliott D, editors. Vision and motor control. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers B.V.; 1992. p. 421-38.; Paulus et al., 1984Paulus W, Straube A, Brandt T. Visual stabilization of posture: physiological stimulus characteristics and clinical aspects. Brain. 1984; 107(Pt 4):1143-63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/107.4.1143. PMid:6509312.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/107.4.11...
; 1989Paulus W, Straube A, Krafczyk S, Brandt T. Differential effects of retinal target displacement, changing size and changing disparity in the control of anterior/posterior and lateral body-sway. Experimental Brain Research. 1989; 78(2):243-52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00228896. PMid:2599035.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00228896...
; Warren 1995Warren WH. Self-motion: visual perception and visual control. In: Epstein W, Rogers S, editors. Perception of space and motion. London: Academic Press; 1995. p. 263-325.; Warren and Hannon, 1988Warren WH Jr, Hannon DJ. Direction of self-motion is perceived from optical flow. Nature. 1988; 336(6195):162-3. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/336162a0.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/336162a0...
). This displacement generates a optic flow that specifies the kinematic properties of body sway and also the three-dimensional (3-D) layout of the environment (Guerraz and Bronstein, 2008Guerraz M, Bronstein AM. Mechanisms underlying visually induced body sway. Neuroscience Letters. 2008; 443(1):12-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.07.053. PMid:18672020.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008....
; Guerraz et al., 2001aGuerraz M, Gianna CC, Burchill PM, Gresty MA, Bronstein AM. Effect of visual surrounding motion on body sway in a three-dimensional environment. Perception & Psychophysics. 2001a; 63(1):47-58. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03200502. PMid:11304016.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03200502...
; 2001bGuerraz M, Thilo KV, Bronstein AM, Gresty MA. Influence of action and expectation on visual control of posture. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research. 2001b; 11(2):259-66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00080-X. PMid:11275487.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(00)...
; O’Connor et al., 2008O’Connor KW, Loughlin PJ, Redfern MS, Sparto PJ. Postural adaptations to repeated optic flow stimulation in older adults. Gait & Posture. 2008; 28(3):385-91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.01.010. PMid:18329878.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
; Pretto et al., 2009Pretto P, Ogier M, Bülthoff HH, Bresciani JP. Influence of size of the field of view on motion perception. Computer Graphics. 2009; 33(2):139-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01.003.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01....
). However, in the moving room paradigm, the egomotion may be enhanced by the properties of the surround motion that can provide spatial reference for postural response (Berthoz et al., 1975Berthoz A, Pavard B, Young LR. Perception of linear horizontal self-motion induced by peripheral vision (linearvection): basic characteristics and visual-vestibular interactions. Experimental Brain Research. 1975; 23(5):471-89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916. PMid:1081949.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916...
; Dijkstra et al., 1994Dijkstra TMH, Schöner G, Gielen CCAM. Temporal stability of the action-perception cycle for postural control in a moving visual environment. Experimental Brain Research. 1994; 97(3):477-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241542. PMid:8187859.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241542...
; Gibson, 1950Gibson JJ. The perception of the visual world. Oxford: Houghton Mifflin; 1950.; Lestienne et al., 1977Lestienne F, Soechting J, Berthoz A. Postural readjustments induced by linear motion of visual scenes. Experimental Brain Research. 1977; 28(3-4):363-84. PMid:885185.; Masson et al., 1995Masson G, Mestre DR, Pailhous J. Effects of the spatio-temporal structure of optical flow on postural readjustments in man. Experimental Brain Research. 1995; 103(1):137-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241971. PMid:7615029.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241971...
; Paulus et al., 1989Paulus W, Straube A, Krafczyk S, Brandt T. Differential effects of retinal target displacement, changing size and changing disparity in the control of anterior/posterior and lateral body-sway. Experimental Brain Research. 1989; 78(2):243-52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00228896. PMid:2599035.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00228896...
; Stoffregen, 1985Stoffregen TA. Flow structure versus retinal location in the optical control of stance. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 1985; 11(5):554-65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.11.5.554. PMid:2932530.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.11.5...
; 1986Stoffregen TA. The role of optical velocity in the control of stance. Perception & Psychophysics. 1986; 39(5):355-60. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03203004. PMid:3737368.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03203004...
; Van Asten et al., 1988Van Asten WNJC, Gielen CCAM, Van der Gon JJD. Postural adjustments induced by simulated motion of differently structured environments. Experimental Brain Research. 1988; 73(2):371-83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00248230. PMid:3215313.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00248230...
). On this basis, the optic flow from the moving virtual environment can be manipulated to evoke egomotion (Dokka et al. 2009Dokka K, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Influence of visual scene velocity on segmental kinematics during stance. Gait & Posture. 2009; 30(2):211-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.05.001. PMid:19505827.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
; Keshner and Kenyon, 2000Keshner EA, Kenyon RV. The influence of an immersive virtual environment on the segmental organization of postural stabilizing responses. Journal of Vestibular Research. 2000; 10(4-5):201-19.; O’Connor et al., 2008O’Connor KW, Loughlin PJ, Redfern MS, Sparto PJ. Postural adaptations to repeated optic flow stimulation in older adults. Gait & Posture. 2008; 28(3):385-91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.01.010. PMid:18329878.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
; Pretto et al., 2009Pretto P, Ogier M, Bülthoff HH, Bresciani JP. Influence of size of the field of view on motion perception. Computer Graphics. 2009; 33(2):139-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01.003.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01....
; Seno et al., 2009Seno T, Ito H, Sunaga S. The object and background hypothesis for vection. Vision Research. 2009; 49(24):2973-82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.09.017. PMid:19782099.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009....
; 2010Seno T, Nakamura S, Ito H, Sunaga S. Static visual components without depth modulation alter the strength of vection. Vision Research. 2010; 50(19):1972-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010.07.004. PMid:20633575.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010....
; Wang et al., 2010Wang Y, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Identifying the control of physically and perceptually evoked sway responses with coincident visual scene velocities and tilt of the base of support. Experimental Brain Research. 2010; 201(4):663-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-2082-0. PMid:19924408.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-208...
).

An important source of information for egomotion relies mostly on the visual properties. Hence, it is affected by the visual incidence angle, distance from the objects, motion velocity, luminance and contrast content of the scenario (Dijkstra et al., 1992Dijkstra TMH, Gielen CCAM, Melis BJM. Postural responses to stationary and moving scenes as a function of distance to the scene. Human Movement Science. 1992; 11(1-2):195-203. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-9457(92)90060-O.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-9457(92)9...
; Masson et al., 1994Masson G, Mestre DR, Blin O, Pailhous J. Low luminance contrast sensitivity: effect of training on psychophysical and optokinetic nystagmus thresholds in man. Vision Research. 1994; 34(14):1893-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(94)90313-1. PMid:7941391.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(94)9...
; Seno et al., 2009Seno T, Ito H, Sunaga S. The object and background hypothesis for vection. Vision Research. 2009; 49(24):2973-82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.09.017. PMid:19782099.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009....
; 2010Seno T, Nakamura S, Ito H, Sunaga S. Static visual components without depth modulation alter the strength of vection. Vision Research. 2010; 50(19):1972-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010.07.004. PMid:20633575.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010....
; Streepey et al., 2007Streepey JW, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Field of view and base of support width influence postural responses to visual stimuli during quiet stance. Gait & Posture. 2007; 25(1):49-55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.12.013. PMid:16464594.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
). Moreover, linear egomotion can be generated by object movement within the peripheral visual field and by the floor translation parallel to the anterior-posterior (A/P) axis of the human body (Berthoz et al., 1975Berthoz A, Pavard B, Young LR. Perception of linear horizontal self-motion induced by peripheral vision (linearvection): basic characteristics and visual-vestibular interactions. Experimental Brain Research. 1975; 23(5):471-89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916. PMid:1081949.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916...
; 1979Berthoz A, Lacour M, Soechting JF, Vidal PP. The role of vision in the control of posture during linear motion. Progress in Brain Research. 1979; 50:197-209. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60820-1. PMid:551426.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(08)...
; Da-Silva et al., 2015Da-Silva PJG, Cagy M, Infantosi AFC. A new dynamic virtual stimulation protocol to induce vection. IFMBE Proceedings, 2015; 45:497-500. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-11128-5_124.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-1112...
; Gibson, 1950Gibson JJ. The perception of the visual world. Oxford: Houghton Mifflin; 1950.; Haibach et al., 2008Haibach PS, Slobounov SM, Newell KM. The potential applications of a virtual moving environment for assessing falls in elderly adults. Gait & Posture. 2008; 27(2):303-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.04.004. PMid:17524647.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
; Warren and Hannon, 1988Warren WH Jr, Hannon DJ. Direction of self-motion is perceived from optical flow. Nature. 1988; 336(6195):162-3. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/336162a0.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/336162a0...
), changing COP position at the same direction of the dynamic visual stimulation. Therefore, there is a direct relationship between the direction (and velocity) of visual optic flow and the orientation (and amplitude) of COP displacement (Dokka et al., 2009Dokka K, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Influence of visual scene velocity on segmental kinematics during stance. Gait & Posture. 2009; 30(2):211-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.05.001. PMid:19505827.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
; 2010Dokka K, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA, Kording KP. Self versus environment motion in postural control. PLoS Computational Biology. 2010; 6(2):1-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000680. PMid:20174552.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1...
; Masson et al., 1995Masson G, Mestre DR, Pailhous J. Effects of the spatio-temporal structure of optical flow on postural readjustments in man. Experimental Brain Research. 1995; 103(1):137-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241971. PMid:7615029.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241971...
; Reed-Jones et al., 2008Reed-Jones RJ, Vallis LA, Reed-Jones JG, Trick LM. The relationship between postural stability and virtual environment adaptation. Neuroscience Letters. 2008; 435(3):204-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.02.047. PMid:18359162.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008....
; Tossavainen et al., 2003Tossavainen T, Juhola M, Pyykkö I, Aalto H, Toppila E. Development of virtual reality stimuli for force platform posturography. International Journal of Medical Informatics. 2003; 70(2-3):277-83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1386-5056(03)00034-0. PMid:12909179.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1386-5056(03)...
; Wang et al., 2010Wang Y, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Identifying the control of physically and perceptually evoked sway responses with coincident visual scene velocities and tilt of the base of support. Experimental Brain Research. 2010; 201(4):663-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-2082-0. PMid:19924408.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-208...
).

Previous studies of orthostatic postural control and visual optic flow have been designed from many different perspectives. One of them consists in determining whether a particular visual property is used to control postural sway (Andersen and Dyre, 1989Andersen GJ, Dyre BP. Spatial orientation from optic flow in the central visual field. Perception & Psychophysics. 1989; 45(5):453-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03210719. PMid:2726408.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03210719...
; Kuno et al., 1999Kuno S, Kawakita T, Kawakami O, Miyake Y, Watanabe S. Postural adjustment response to depth direction moving patterns produced by virtual reality graphics. Japanese Journal of Physiology. 1999; 49(5):417-24. http://dx.doi.org/10.2170/jjphysiol.49.417. PMid:10603425.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2170/jjphysiol.49.4...
; Van Asten et al., 1988Van Asten WNJC, Gielen CCAM, Van der Gon JJD. Postural adjustments induced by simulated motion of differently structured environments. Experimental Brain Research. 1988; 73(2):371-83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00248230. PMid:3215313.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00248230...
) or to describe the postural feedback loop (Day and Guerraz, 2007Day BL, Guerraz M. Feedforward versus feedback modulation of human vestibular-evoked balance responses by visual self-motion information. The Journal of Physiology. 2007; 582(1):153-61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2007.132092. PMid:17446222.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2007....
). Moreover, the virtual moving environment has been used to assess time to recover stability and fall risk (Haibach et al., 2008Haibach PS, Slobounov SM, Newell KM. The potential applications of a virtual moving environment for assessing falls in elderly adults. Gait & Posture. 2008; 27(2):303-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.04.004. PMid:17524647.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
). However, in the dynamic scene stimulation, not all visual information can be used to induce egomotion (Fushiki et al., 2005Fushiki H, Kobayashi K, Asai M, Watanabe Y. Influence of visually induced self-motion on postural stability. Acta Oto-Laryngologica. 2005; 125(1):60-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00016480410015794. PMid:15799576.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00016480410015...
; O’Connor et al., 2008O’Connor KW, Loughlin PJ, Redfern MS, Sparto PJ. Postural adaptations to repeated optic flow stimulation in older adults. Gait & Posture. 2008; 28(3):385-91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.01.010. PMid:18329878.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
; Pretto et al., 2009Pretto P, Ogier M, Bülthoff HH, Bresciani JP. Influence of size of the field of view on motion perception. Computer Graphics. 2009; 33(2):139-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01.003.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01....
; Wang et al., 2010Wang Y, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Identifying the control of physically and perceptually evoked sway responses with coincident visual scene velocities and tilt of the base of support. Experimental Brain Research. 2010; 201(4):663-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-2082-0. PMid:19924408.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-208...
). Instead, it was stated that the visual optic flow structure as a whole determines the orientation and the magnitude of the postural reaction to the motion of the environment. The optic flow pattern corresponds to the optic motion of environmental elements which indicate the magnitude, velocity and direction of its displacement. The velocity contains information about distance along the object (motion perspective), whereas the direction provides information about self-motion (visual kinesthesis) (Gibson, 1950Gibson JJ. The perception of the visual world. Oxford: Houghton Mifflin; 1950.; Lishman and Lee, 1973Lishman JR, Lee DN. The autonomy of visual kinaesthesis. Perception. 1973; 2(3):287-94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p020287. PMid:4546578.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p020287...
). In particular, the focus of scene expansion corresponds to the current direction of egomotion (representing an anterior translation of the body). Thus, the dynamic structure of the optic flow (the 3-D layout) over a stationary observer can be the functional element to induce egomotion during stabilometric test.

Alternatively, since the visually evoked postural responses are dependent of the optic flow (Dokka et al. 2009Dokka K, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Influence of visual scene velocity on segmental kinematics during stance. Gait & Posture. 2009; 30(2):211-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.05.001. PMid:19505827.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
; Pretto et al., 2009Pretto P, Ogier M, Bülthoff HH, Bresciani JP. Influence of size of the field of view on motion perception. Computer Graphics. 2009; 33(2):139-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01.003.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01....
; Seno et al., 2009Seno T, Ito H, Sunaga S. The object and background hypothesis for vection. Vision Research. 2009; 49(24):2973-82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.09.017. PMid:19782099.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009....
; 2010Seno T, Nakamura S, Ito H, Sunaga S. Static visual components without depth modulation alter the strength of vection. Vision Research. 2010; 50(19):1972-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010.07.004. PMid:20633575.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010....
; Wang et al., 2010Wang Y, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Identifying the control of physically and perceptually evoked sway responses with coincident visual scene velocities and tilt of the base of support. Experimental Brain Research. 2010; 201(4):663-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-2082-0. PMid:19924408.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-208...
), the dynamic visual protocol could be designed from the perspective to understand the cortical interaction underlying the visual motion signal processing for postural control. Thus, to investigate this motion-related visual evoked potential, it is important to ensure that the dynamic virtual stimulation protocol generates egomotion. For instance, the present work aims to assess the dynamic virtual stimulation protocol to be applied during stabilometric test: orthostatic posture on a force platform. The visual stimulation was carried out by developing a virtual environmental structure concerning scenario displacement, visual image velocity and incidence angle, distance from the screen and luminance. In this protocol, the reversing black and white background during floor translation parallel to the ground plane was applied simultaneously with the lateral walls and furniture movements while luminance was changed to generate bi-dimensional optic flow stimulation as a tunnel pattern. This dynamic configuration establishes an advantage to generate a virtual flow field that reflects the natural vision and the physiological properties of the body sway perturbation. Indeed, it was employed to evoke illusion of motion in an opposite direction of the dynamic visual stimulation. To this end, we measured and compared the A/P COP displacement during dynamic (forward and backward) and static scenarios, synchronized at motion-onset of the exhibiting dynamic scenes.

Methods

Dynamic virtual stimulation protocol

The virtual scenario reproduces an 8.0 m wide by 3.0 m high by 12.0 m deep room developed using IDE Delphi and API OpenGL (Graphics Library). The room consists of a chessboard-pattern floor (similar to pattern-reversal stimulation), with walls and ceiling designed with distinct texture (Figure 1). Furniture can be added and allocated at any position of the room. Hence, the virtual scenario can be composed with different configurations, setting out the dynamic stimulation in the center and/or periphery of the visual field. As a default, the table and chair was placed in the center of the visual field, while door and drawer in the periphery.

Figure 1
Virtual scenario: chessboard-pattern floor with a table and chair in the center of the screen. Five different views from the most backward to the far forward scenario with displacement in steps of 50 cm and its corresponding luminance in steps of 2 cd/m2 starting in 31 cd/m2.

In order to carry out the dynamic properties of the visual stimulation protocol, the furniture is expanded or reduced, while the floor, walls and ceiling are moved in parallel direction with a constant displacement and velocity. Therefore, the virtual scenario can move in forward (DSF) or backward direction (DSB) in a frame rate of 60 Hz. Furthermore, the luminance was changed for each DS scene (Figure 1) by steps of 2 cd/m2, ranging from 31 to 39 cd/m2 and increasing in forward direction. The DS sequence was randomly applied and interspersed by a Static Scene (SS) at the final position of the exhibiting DS (the initial position of the next DS). Consequently, the subject could not previously predict the DS direction. Moreover, the amount of DS to be applied, the displacement of the scenario and the time duration of exhibiting DS and SS scenes can be set by configuration. Additionally, the DS stimuli were codified by pulses with specific code numbers for forward and backward displacement (synchronized by the onset of exhibiting DS scene). This sequence of pulses generates a trigger signal to be used during signal processing.

DS during stabilometric test

The DS protocol was evaluated using the stabilometric signals from 29 healthy subjects (aging from 20 to 42 years), with normal or corrected-to-normal vision and no history of equilibrium disorder, as reported by Da-Silva et al. (2015)Da-Silva PJG, Cagy M, Infantosi AFC. A new dynamic virtual stimulation protocol to induce vection. IFMBE Proceedings, 2015; 45:497-500. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-11128-5_124.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-1112...
. All participants previously signed an informed consent form, as approved by the ethics committee of clinical research of IESC/UFRJ - Instituto de Estudos em Saúde Coletiva/Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro: 100/2011 (Rio de Janeiro, Brazil).

The stabilometric tests were performed under controlled environmental condition, with the subject in orthostatic posture, quietly standing on a force platform located 1 m ahead of a white wall. The first test was acquired without virtual scenario stimulation, with the subject just observing the white wall during 5 min (condition denotes as WW). The DS protocol trial was performed with the virtual environment projected on the white wall: displayed with dimension of 1.72 × 1.16 m and centered at the subjects vision line as reference. For this evaluation, the DS configuration was set up to generate a moving scene with constant velocity, Vi = 2 m/s, which is considered as one that elicits more postural changes due to illusory egomotion (Berthoz et al. 1975Berthoz A, Pavard B, Young LR. Perception of linear horizontal self-motion induced by peripheral vision (linearvection): basic characteristics and visual-vestibular interactions. Experimental Brain Research. 1975; 23(5):471-89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916. PMid:1081949.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916...
; 1979Berthoz A, Lacour M, Soechting JF, Vidal PP. The role of vision in the control of posture during linear motion. Progress in Brain Research. 1979; 50:197-209. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60820-1. PMid:551426.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(08)...
; Dokka et al., 2009Dokka K, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Influence of visual scene velocity on segmental kinematics during stance. Gait & Posture. 2009; 30(2):211-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.05.001. PMid:19505827.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
). Thus, the amplitude of each virtual scene motion was 50 cm; displayed during 250 ms. A set of 100 DS stimuli (50 to each direction) was applied in random order, interspersed by 10 s of SS to allow the balance reestablishment and thus to avoid the risk of falling. This set up configuration generates a dynamic effect of discrete forward and backward stimulation to induce an illusion of egomotion, i.e. self-motion in an opposite direction of the dynamic scenario. All participants were instructed to maintain the orthostatic posture while looking straight ahead at the scene, keeping the gaze at the virtual chair. Moreover, at the final of the experiment, the participants were asked about their perception of self-motion.

Visual optic flow structures

The visual optic flow was composed based on the visual incidence angle (θ), the distance between the eyes and the screen (H) and the images velocity (Vi) (Berthoz et al., 1975Berthoz A, Pavard B, Young LR. Perception of linear horizontal self-motion induced by peripheral vision (linearvection): basic characteristics and visual-vestibular interactions. Experimental Brain Research. 1975; 23(5):471-89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916. PMid:1081949.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916...
). Considering the width and height of the image size (S) as w = 1.72 m and h = 1.16 m, H = 1.0 m and Vi = 2.0 m/s, the instantaneous two-dimensional velocity field was estimated as:

d θ d t = V i H × sin 2 θ (1)

where, θ =2×arctan(S/2H) is the lateral (−40.7º ≤ θl ≤ 40.7º) or vertical (−30.1º ≤ θv ≤ 30.1º) components of the visual incidence angle obtained for the respective width and height of the image size (S). As describe by Da-Silva et al. (2015)Da-Silva PJG, Cagy M, Infantosi AFC. A new dynamic virtual stimulation protocol to induce vection. IFMBE Proceedings, 2015; 45:497-500. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-11128-5_124.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-1112...
, it generated visual optic flow stimulation as a tunnel pattern, with optic flow closer to 0º/s in the central vision. Forwards stimulation (DSF) increases the optic flow of the image up to 4°/s in both periphery and ground plane of the visual field. Otherwise, backwards (DSB), the optic flow decreases.

The A/P COP signal analysis

The COP signal was acquired using a square portable force platform (0.16 m2) composed with four load cells (gain: 600x), digitized at 400 Hz (resolution: 16 bits) with a 60 Hz digital notch filter, and stored in a hard disk for offline signal processing (Matlab v. 7.6.0 - The Mathworks, USA). Only the COP displacement in the anterior-posterior direction (A/P stabilogram) was analyzed in this evaluation.

The A/P COP signal was low-pass filtered (cut-off: 7 Hz) by applying a 2nd order Butterworth with null phase (Da Silva et al., 2012Da Silva PJG, Nadal J, Infantosi AFC. Investigating the center of pressure velocity Romberg’s quotient for assessing the visual role on the body sway. Revista Brasileira de Engenharia Biomédica. 2012; 28(4):319-26. http://dx.doi.org/10.4322/rbeb.2012.039.
http://dx.doi.org/10.4322/rbeb.2012.039...
). In the graphs, a positive A/P COP displacement means a forward displacement of the subject’s COP. Based on the trigger signal, the A/P COP signal of each DSF was identified and segmented into M = 50 epochs of 13 s duration (–3 to 10 s), synchronized by the onset of the DSF (t = 0) and including 3 s of SS immediately preceding it. The postural stability just before DS, the changes in the A/P COP position over the following time and the time to recover stability after egomotion were assessed by applying the multiple-comparison test (for more details, see Da-Silva et al., 2015Da-Silva PJG, Cagy M, Infantosi AFC. A new dynamic virtual stimulation protocol to induce vection. IFMBE Proceedings, 2015; 45:497-500. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-11128-5_124.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-1112...
).

In summary, the linear egomotion was considered when the distribution of the A/P COP position statistically differed (one-way ANOVA, α = 0.05) from those preceding DSF and the COP displacement estimated as the difference of this position identified by the Tukey test (α = 0.05) and the position preceding the dynamic scene. Considering only the COP signals with effect of linear egomotion, the time to recover stability was assessed by applying the Tukey test (α = 0.05) to identify the consecutive COP segments distributions after DSF peak without significant differences. It was established as the instant time of the first initial set that considers COP stability. Finally, those A/P COP signals with effect of linear egomotion were coherently averaged. The same procedure was used for the DSB stimulation.

Results

Figure 2 depicts the A/P COP displacement without virtual stimulation (WW condition) and in response to linear egomotion by applying DSF (red) and DSB (blue) stimulation. It can be noted that, during WW condition (Figure 2a), the A/P COP displacement oscillates around zero, indicating postural stability over the time. Regardless DSF and DSB (Figure 2b), no significant change was observed between COP position during SS before DS (ANOVA, p > 0.8), indicating postural stability just preceding DS. For both DSF and DSB, COP sway increases with the sequence of stimulation, presenting more variability at the final stimuli. Moreover, the postural response after DSF (Tukey post-hoc test, p < 0.001), i.e. the negative peak of the stabilogram signals (indicated by the arrow in Figure 2b), and the instant time to recover stability (Tukey post-hoc test, p > 0.6) are time-shifted along the stimulation. Similar result is observed by applying DSB (positive peak).

Figure 2
Time evolution of the A/P COP displacement segments during: (a) white wall condition (WW); (b) forward (DSF, red) and backward (DSB, blue) virtual scene stimulation. The gray area indicates the 250 ms of DS (motion-onset: t = 0). The arrows indicate the significant changes (Tukey post-hoc test, p < 0.001) of the A/P COP position in response to egomotion.

For the same subject, linear egomotion was observed in 63% of DS. The coherently averaged A/P COP stabilogram during DSF (33 stimuli, red line in Figure 3) evidenced postural instability in the posterior direction of the body axes with mean displacement of –12.3 mm. On the other hand, during DSB (31 stimuli, blue line), the instability occurred at the anterior direction with mean displacement of 12.1 mm. Those displacements indicate that the optic flow stimulation as a tunnel pattern induced linear egomotion with evoked postural response in the same direction of the motion scene. Furthermore, the effect of the linear egomotion during DSF achieves its peak approximately 300 ms after DSB (gray area in Figure 3). Additionally, the subject reestablished the postural stability until the 7th second after DS. Similar results were observed for the rest of the subjects.

Figure 3
The coherent averaged A/P COP displacement from subject #1 in response to egomotion induced by forward (DSF, red) and backward (DSB, blue) scene stimulation. The dashed lines indicate the standard deviation of the A/P COP displacement. The gray area indicates DS exhibition (250 ms). The brown area indicates the latency (300 ms) between evoked postural response by applying DSF and DSB.

Figure 4a depicts the grand-average (29 subjects) of the A/P COP displacement for DSF (red) and DSB (blue) in absolute values. In both DS conditions, the COP mean displacement and its standard deviation increased with the sequence of stimulation (ranging from 8.4 ± 1.7 to 22.6 ±5.3 mm). In addition, the time to recover stability also increased with the sequence of stimulation (Figure 4b), ranging from 4.1 ± 0.4 to 7.2 ± 0.6 s. Although all subjects reported vection, the percentage of the amount of egomotion differs among volunteers, (63% to 80% of DS trials). Moreover, no difference (Wilcoxon rank sum test: p = 0.7) was observed between COP displacement response distributions due to DSF and DSB (absolute values), as well as between time to recover stability (p = 0.8; Figure 5). On the other hand, the median of the instant peak time distribution of egomotion during DSF was different than that obtained for DSB (p = 0.002), occurring 200 ms after DSB.

Figure 4
The grand-average over DS trials (mean ± standard deviation): (a) A/P COP displacement (absolute values); (b) time to recover stability. Red and blue indicates the DSF and DSB, respectively.
Figure 5
Box-plot distribution of A/P COP displacement (absolute values), time to recover stability and instant peak time of egomotion during DSF (red) and DSB (blue).

Discussion

In this dynamic virtual protocol, the moving room paradigm was applied with the environmental structures composing an optic flow as a tunnel pattern to generate discrete forward or backward sway stimulation. This tunnel pattern induced linear egomotion in the stationary observer, changing the center of pressure position, as also observed by Akizuki et al. (2005)Akizuki H, Uno A, Arai K, Morioka S, Ohyama S, Nishiike S, Tamura K, Takeda N. Effects of immersion in virtual reality on postural control. Neuroscience Letters. 2005; 379(1):23-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2004.12.041. PMid:15814192.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2004....
, Dokka et al. (2009)Dokka K, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Influence of visual scene velocity on segmental kinematics during stance. Gait & Posture. 2009; 30(2):211-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.05.001. PMid:19505827.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
, Pretto et al. (2009)Pretto P, Ogier M, Bülthoff HH, Bresciani JP. Influence of size of the field of view on motion perception. Computer Graphics. 2009; 33(2):139-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01.003.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01....
, Streepey et al. (2007)Streepey JW, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Field of view and base of support width influence postural responses to visual stimuli during quiet stance. Gait & Posture. 2007; 25(1):49-55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.12.013. PMid:16464594.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
and Wang et al. (2010)Wang Y, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Identifying the control of physically and perceptually evoked sway responses with coincident visual scene velocities and tilt of the base of support. Experimental Brain Research. 2010; 201(4):663-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-2082-0. PMid:19924408.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-208...
during oscillating room stimulation. The virtual scene was designed with an 8.0 m wide by 3.0 m high by 12.0 m deep room in order to stimulate the peripheral vision while the floor moved parallel to a ground plane. It was projected with dimension 1.72 × 1.16 m in a white wall located 1 m ahead from the standing subject (81.4° × 60.2° visual incidence angle). The size of the lateral view of the present protocol increased the peripheral vision stimulation since it is greater than those used in most virtual reality displays as head-mounted display, shutter glasses or computer monitor (ranging from 30 to 60°). According to Pretto et al. (2009)Pretto P, Ogier M, Bülthoff HH, Bresciani JP. Influence of size of the field of view on motion perception. Computer Graphics. 2009; 33(2):139-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01.003.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01....
, the optic flow carried out by those displays restricts the influence of visual inputs at central visual field. Therefore, the visual incidence angle used in the proposed virtual stimulation protocol establishes the representation of the spatial structure of the employed environment to stimulate the peripheral vision as suggested by Guerraz and Bronstein (2008)Guerraz M, Bronstein AM. Mechanisms underlying visually induced body sway. Neuroscience Letters. 2008; 443(1):12-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.07.053. PMid:18672020.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008....
, O’Connor et al. (2008)O’Connor KW, Loughlin PJ, Redfern MS, Sparto PJ. Postural adaptations to repeated optic flow stimulation in older adults. Gait & Posture. 2008; 28(3):385-91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.01.010. PMid:18329878.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
and Pretto et al. (2009)Pretto P, Ogier M, Bülthoff HH, Bresciani JP. Influence of size of the field of view on motion perception. Computer Graphics. 2009; 33(2):139-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01.003.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01....
.

The optic flow parallel to the A/P axis of the human body was generated by the translation of the floor, as the classical study of locomotion control by Gibson (1950Gibson JJ. The perception of the visual world. Oxford: Houghton Mifflin; 1950.; 1954Gibson JJ. The visual perception of objective motion and subjective movement. Psychological Review. 1954; 61(5):304-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0061885. PMid:13204493.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0061885...
). This source of information parallel to the A/P axis of the human body has been used in most postural control protocols as described by Dokka et al. (2009)Dokka K, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Influence of visual scene velocity on segmental kinematics during stance. Gait & Posture. 2009; 30(2):211-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.05.001. PMid:19505827.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
, O’Connor et al. (2008)O’Connor KW, Loughlin PJ, Redfern MS, Sparto PJ. Postural adaptations to repeated optic flow stimulation in older adults. Gait & Posture. 2008; 28(3):385-91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.01.010. PMid:18329878.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
, Pretto et al. (2009)Pretto P, Ogier M, Bülthoff HH, Bresciani JP. Influence of size of the field of view on motion perception. Computer Graphics. 2009; 33(2):139-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01.003.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01....
, Seno et al. (2010)Seno T, Nakamura S, Ito H, Sunaga S. Static visual components without depth modulation alter the strength of vection. Vision Research. 2010; 50(19):1972-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010.07.004. PMid:20633575.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010....
and Wang et al. (2010)Wang Y, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Identifying the control of physically and perceptually evoked sway responses with coincident visual scene velocities and tilt of the base of support. Experimental Brain Research. 2010; 201(4):663-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-2082-0. PMid:19924408.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-208...
. Distinct of those studies, the reversing black and white background during chessboard-like floor translation applied simultaneously with the lateral walls and furniture movements has accomplished to increase the bi-dimensional optic flow up to 4°/s at the periphery of visual field. Indeed, this optic flow field velocity reflects the natural vision when the virtual scene moves at the same velocity as the physiological body sway perturbation (Berthoz et al., 1975Berthoz A, Pavard B, Young LR. Perception of linear horizontal self-motion induced by peripheral vision (linearvection): basic characteristics and visual-vestibular interactions. Experimental Brain Research. 1975; 23(5):471-89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916. PMid:1081949.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916...
; 1979Berthoz A, Lacour M, Soechting JF, Vidal PP. The role of vision in the control of posture during linear motion. Progress in Brain Research. 1979; 50:197-209. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60820-1. PMid:551426.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(08)...
; Masson et al., 1995Masson G, Mestre DR, Pailhous J. Effects of the spatio-temporal structure of optical flow on postural readjustments in man. Experimental Brain Research. 1995; 103(1):137-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241971. PMid:7615029.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241971...
).

In the proposed protocol, the luminance was changed by steps of 2 cd/m2, ranging from 31 to 39 cd/m2 and increasing (or decreasing) in forward (or backward) direction while the scene is moved with constant velocity (Vi = 2.0 m/s). These luminance thresholds applied here are in the range of those for real image motion detection by the visual system, as reported by Berthoz et al. (1975Berthoz A, Pavard B, Young LR. Perception of linear horizontal self-motion induced by peripheral vision (linearvection): basic characteristics and visual-vestibular interactions. Experimental Brain Research. 1975; 23(5):471-89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916. PMid:1081949.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916...
; 1979Berthoz A, Lacour M, Soechting JF, Vidal PP. The role of vision in the control of posture during linear motion. Progress in Brain Research. 1979; 50:197-209. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60820-1. PMid:551426.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(08)...
); Masson et al. (1995)Masson G, Mestre DR, Pailhous J. Effects of the spatio-temporal structure of optical flow on postural readjustments in man. Experimental Brain Research. 1995; 103(1):137-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241971. PMid:7615029.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241971...
; Pretto et al. (2009)Pretto P, Ogier M, Bülthoff HH, Bresciani JP. Influence of size of the field of view on motion perception. Computer Graphics. 2009; 33(2):139-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01.003.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01....
. However, these authors used a constant luminance during forward displacement to induce peripheral optic flow information for navigation control, without objects in the central visual field. On the other hand, in order to reproduce the natural optical flow information for the orthostatic posture control, the expansion / reduction of the size of the table and chair in the central visual field was employed. Besides, it was used also to highlight the fusion between the black and white moving pattern of the chessboard-like floor, increasing self-motion sensation and hence the linear egomotion.

Such dynamic visual effect is equivalent to the direction of body motion, i.e. body sway in the anterior axes increases the optic flow of the environment, otherwise (posterior sway) the optic flow decreases. As a consequence, the proposed protocol induced the illusion of self-motion in opposite direction of the DS scene, as observed in the literature (Berthoz et al., 1975Berthoz A, Pavard B, Young LR. Perception of linear horizontal self-motion induced by peripheral vision (linearvection): basic characteristics and visual-vestibular interactions. Experimental Brain Research. 1975; 23(5):471-89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916. PMid:1081949.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916...
; Dijkstra et al., 1994Dijkstra TMH, Schöner G, Gielen CCAM. Temporal stability of the action-perception cycle for postural control in a moving visual environment. Experimental Brain Research. 1994; 97(3):477-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241542. PMid:8187859.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241542...
; Dokka et al., 2009Dokka K, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Influence of visual scene velocity on segmental kinematics during stance. Gait & Posture. 2009; 30(2):211-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.05.001. PMid:19505827.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
; Gibson, 1950Gibson JJ. The perception of the visual world. Oxford: Houghton Mifflin; 1950.; Keshner and Kenyon, 2000Keshner EA, Kenyon RV. The influence of an immersive virtual environment on the segmental organization of postural stabilizing responses. Journal of Vestibular Research. 2000; 10(4-5):201-19.; Lestienne et al., 1977Lestienne F, Soechting J, Berthoz A. Postural readjustments induced by linear motion of visual scenes. Experimental Brain Research. 1977; 28(3-4):363-84. PMid:885185.; Masson et al., 1995Masson G, Mestre DR, Pailhous J. Effects of the spatio-temporal structure of optical flow on postural readjustments in man. Experimental Brain Research. 1995; 103(1):137-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241971. PMid:7615029.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241971...
; O’Connor et al., 2008O’Connor KW, Loughlin PJ, Redfern MS, Sparto PJ. Postural adaptations to repeated optic flow stimulation in older adults. Gait & Posture. 2008; 28(3):385-91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.01.010. PMid:18329878.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
; Paulus et al., 1989Paulus W, Straube A, Krafczyk S, Brandt T. Differential effects of retinal target displacement, changing size and changing disparity in the control of anterior/posterior and lateral body-sway. Experimental Brain Research. 1989; 78(2):243-52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00228896. PMid:2599035.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00228896...
; Pretto et al., 2009Pretto P, Ogier M, Bülthoff HH, Bresciani JP. Influence of size of the field of view on motion perception. Computer Graphics. 2009; 33(2):139-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01.003.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01....
; Seno et al., 2009Seno T, Ito H, Sunaga S. The object and background hypothesis for vection. Vision Research. 2009; 49(24):2973-82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.09.017. PMid:19782099.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009....
; 2010Seno T, Nakamura S, Ito H, Sunaga S. Static visual components without depth modulation alter the strength of vection. Vision Research. 2010; 50(19):1972-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010.07.004. PMid:20633575.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010....
; Stoffregen, 1985Stoffregen TA. Flow structure versus retinal location in the optical control of stance. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 1985; 11(5):554-65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.11.5.554. PMid:2932530.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.11.5...
; 1986Stoffregen TA. The role of optical velocity in the control of stance. Perception & Psychophysics. 1986; 39(5):355-60. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03203004. PMid:3737368.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03203004...
; Van Asten et al., 1988Van Asten WNJC, Gielen CCAM, Van der Gon JJD. Postural adjustments induced by simulated motion of differently structured environments. Experimental Brain Research. 1988; 73(2):371-83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00248230. PMid:3215313.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00248230...
; Wang et al., 2010Wang Y, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Identifying the control of physically and perceptually evoked sway responses with coincident visual scene velocities and tilt of the base of support. Experimental Brain Research. 2010; 201(4):663-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-2082-0. PMid:19924408.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-208...
). Therefore, the bi-dimensional optic flow reflects the 3D layout of this virtual environment available to the stationary observer, representing the magnitude, velocity and direction of body sway displacement. Moreover, the proposed setup allows including additional furniture, change the scene velocity or the amplitude of displacement, as well as roll the surrounding. However, these can be properly evaluated in future studies in order to provide evidence that the system can be useful in a broad set of postural control experiments. Hence, such flexibility allows investigating the interaction (or conflict) between the visual system and the proprioception or vestibular system without mechanic devices (as platform tilt, translating sled or roll chair), generally used in most studies. On the other hand, such devices cannot be fully discarded, since the direct stimulation of the other systems provides specific information, which cannot be generated by a visual stimulation system.

The forwards and backwards scenes were applied in random order and hence the subject could not predict the direction of surrounding motion. Additionally, the dynamic virtual stimulation with unexpected visual motion-onset distracter scenes was used to avoid postural adaptation or optic flow habituation, as suggested by O’Connor et al. (2008)O’Connor KW, Loughlin PJ, Redfern MS, Sparto PJ. Postural adaptations to repeated optic flow stimulation in older adults. Gait & Posture. 2008; 28(3):385-91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.01.010. PMid:18329878.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
, Reed-Jones et al. (2008)Reed-Jones RJ, Vallis LA, Reed-Jones JG, Trick LM. The relationship between postural stability and virtual environment adaptation. Neuroscience Letters. 2008; 435(3):204-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.02.047. PMid:18359162.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008....
and Wang et al. (2010)Wang Y, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Identifying the control of physically and perceptually evoked sway responses with coincident visual scene velocities and tilt of the base of support. Experimental Brain Research. 2010; 201(4):663-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-2082-0. PMid:19924408.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-208...
. These authors reported that consecutive visual scene motion at the same direction decreases linear egomotion in about 40% and can raise it in 10% after 10 stimuli. Although Reed-Jones et al. (2008)Reed-Jones RJ, Vallis LA, Reed-Jones JG, Trick LM. The relationship between postural stability and virtual environment adaptation. Neuroscience Letters. 2008; 435(3):204-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.02.047. PMid:18359162.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008....
reported that the A/P COP sway displacement decreases with the repetition of the presentation, this was not observed in our study. Indeed, the unexpected surrounding motion creates a mismatch between the dynamic virtual scene and motion of the body. According to Wang et al. (2010)Wang Y, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Identifying the control of physically and perceptually evoked sway responses with coincident visual scene velocities and tilt of the base of support. Experimental Brain Research. 2010; 201(4):663-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-2082-0. PMid:19924408.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-208...
, this divergence produces a sensory conflict that interferes with the capability to discriminate between visual field motion and body motion, resulting in spatial disorientation and changing the center of pressure position.

Linear egomotion was observed by applying 250 ms of a dynamic scene with a constant velocity of 2 m/s and interspersed by 10 s of static scene at the final position of the exhibiting room movement. The orthostatic postural response occurred at the same direction of the dynamic stimulation, with A/P COP displacement and its standard deviation increasing with the random sequence of stimulation. The results agree with the literature: self-motion in scene velocity greater than 1 m/s and sway amplitude increasing at scene velocities as high as 2.0 m/s (Akizuki et al., 2005Akizuki H, Uno A, Arai K, Morioka S, Ohyama S, Nishiike S, Tamura K, Takeda N. Effects of immersion in virtual reality on postural control. Neuroscience Letters. 2005; 379(1):23-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2004.12.041. PMid:15814192.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2004....
; Darekar et al., 2015Darekar A, McFadyen BJ, Lamontagne A, Fung J. Efficacy of virtual reality-based intervention on balance and mobility disorders post-stroke: a scoping review. Journal of Neuroengineering and Rehabilitation. 2015; 12(1):46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12984-015-0035-3. PMid:25957577.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12984-015-003...
; Dokka et al., 2009Dokka K, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Influence of visual scene velocity on segmental kinematics during stance. Gait & Posture. 2009; 30(2):211-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.05.001. PMid:19505827.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
; 2010Dokka K, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA, Kording KP. Self versus environment motion in postural control. PLoS Computational Biology. 2010; 6(2):1-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000680. PMid:20174552.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1...
; Keshner and Kenyon, 2004Keshner EA, Kenyon RV. Using immersive technology for postural research and rehabilitation. Assistive Technology. 2004; 16(1):54-62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400435.2004.10132074. PMid:15357148.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400435.2004....
; Streepey et al., 2007Streepey JW, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Field of view and base of support width influence postural responses to visual stimuli during quiet stance. Gait & Posture. 2007; 25(1):49-55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.12.013. PMid:16464594.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
; Wang et al., 2010Wang Y, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Identifying the control of physically and perceptually evoked sway responses with coincident visual scene velocities and tilt of the base of support. Experimental Brain Research. 2010; 201(4):663-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-2082-0. PMid:19924408.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-208...
). Those studies have demonstrated that vection and postural response are correlated with the velocity of the visual motion stimulation. Dokka et al. (2009Dokka K, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Influence of visual scene velocity on segmental kinematics during stance. Gait & Posture. 2009; 30(2):211-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.05.001. PMid:19505827.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
; 2010Dokka K, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA, Kording KP. Self versus environment motion in postural control. PLoS Computational Biology. 2010; 6(2):1-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000680. PMid:20174552.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1...
) observed body sway at scene velocity of 1.25 and 1.88 m/s when compared with lower scene velocities (< 0.31 m/s). Additionally, the visually evoked postural responses was lined up with the plane of motion of the visual stimulus, while the magnitude of body displacement increases during linear egomotion when compared to the static scenes, also observed by Akizuki et al. (2005)Akizuki H, Uno A, Arai K, Morioka S, Ohyama S, Nishiike S, Tamura K, Takeda N. Effects of immersion in virtual reality on postural control. Neuroscience Letters. 2005; 379(1):23-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2004.12.041. PMid:15814192.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2004....
, Da-Silva et al., 2015Da-Silva PJG, Cagy M, Infantosi AFC. A new dynamic virtual stimulation protocol to induce vection. IFMBE Proceedings, 2015; 45:497-500. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-11128-5_124.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-1112...
; Dijkstra et al. (1992Dijkstra TMH, Gielen CCAM, Melis BJM. Postural responses to stationary and moving scenes as a function of distance to the scene. Human Movement Science. 1992; 11(1-2):195-203. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-9457(92)90060-O.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-9457(92)9...
; 1994Dijkstra TMH, Schöner G, Gielen CCAM. Temporal stability of the action-perception cycle for postural control in a moving visual environment. Experimental Brain Research. 1994; 97(3):477-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241542. PMid:8187859.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241542...
) and Wang et al. (2010)Wang Y, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Identifying the control of physically and perceptually evoked sway responses with coincident visual scene velocities and tilt of the base of support. Experimental Brain Research. 2010; 201(4):663-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-2082-0. PMid:19924408.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-208...
.

The proposed dynamic virtual protocol induced linear egomotion in more than 63% of the applied trials. Considering that the subject reestablishes the postural stability until the 7th second, the 10 s of the static scene interval interspersed between the dynamic stimulation ensured that, when a new dynamic stimulus was applied, no significant effect was observed from the previous one (ANOVA, p > 0.8). Therefore, the postural instability is also a time-locked activity (Da-Silva et al., 2015Da-Silva PJG, Cagy M, Infantosi AFC. A new dynamic virtual stimulation protocol to induce vection. IFMBE Proceedings, 2015; 45:497-500. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-11128-5_124.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-1112...
). This finding indicates the influence of the static scene to recover stability after the body sway perturbation. According to Dijkstra et al. (1992Dijkstra TMH, Gielen CCAM, Melis BJM. Postural responses to stationary and moving scenes as a function of distance to the scene. Human Movement Science. 1992; 11(1-2):195-203. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-9457(92)90060-O.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-9457(92)9...
; 1994Dijkstra TMH, Schöner G, Gielen CCAM. Temporal stability of the action-perception cycle for postural control in a moving visual environment. Experimental Brain Research. 1994; 97(3):477-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241542. PMid:8187859.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241542...
), Fushiki et al. (2005)Fushiki H, Kobayashi K, Asai M, Watanabe Y. Influence of visually induced self-motion on postural stability. Acta Oto-Laryngologica. 2005; 125(1):60-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00016480410015794. PMid:15799576.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00016480410015...
, Haibach et al., 2008 and Seno et al. (2010)Seno T, Nakamura S, Ito H, Sunaga S. Static visual components without depth modulation alter the strength of vection. Vision Research. 2010; 50(19):1972-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010.07.004. PMid:20633575.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010....
, the static scene provides a time to recover stability during the action-perception cycle for postural control in a moving visual environment, avoiding the risk of fall. On the other hand, the increasing of the A/P COP and time to recover stability variability along sequence of stimulation could be associated to the prolonged upright stance during the protocol applied here. To reduce this variability, we recommend for further study that the amount of the dynamic visual stimulation can be applied into distinct blocks, with subject resting in a comfortable chair between blocks.

The results indicate that DSF peak occurs at approximately 200 ms after DSB peak. This finding could be an indicative that the effect due to forward stimulation elicits distinct control strategies to maintained balance than that observed by applying the backward stimulation. This difference perhaps could represents a higher order cortical processing to maintaining balance, since there are more magnocellular receptive fields regarding to the increasing size of the image at the retina, as well as the motion-processing pathway that can be traced from those magnocellular input to primary visual cortex and allied areas of the parietal cortex (Kandel, 1991Kandel ER. Perception of motion, depth, and form. In: Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessel TM, editors. Principles of neurological science. New York: Elsevier; 1991. p. 440-66.; Kuba et al., 2007Kuba M, Kubová Z, Kremlácek J, Langrová J. Motion-onset VEPs: Characteristics, methods, and diagnostic use. Vision Research. 2007; 47(2):189-202. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2006.09.020. PMid:17129593.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2006....
). Hence, the postural control strategy to maintain balance seems to depend on the virtual scene direction.

All these findings indicate that linear egomotion induced by forward and backward visual motion stimulation was a major feature to promote A/P postural instability. Besides, it is in agreement with the physiological properties of the visual system: the expansion (or reduction) of the scene increases (or decreases) the size of the image at the retina and the optic flow in the periphery of the visual field (Paulus et al., 1989Paulus W, Straube A, Krafczyk S, Brandt T. Differential effects of retinal target displacement, changing size and changing disparity in the control of anterior/posterior and lateral body-sway. Experimental Brain Research. 1989; 78(2):243-52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00228896. PMid:2599035.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00228896...
; Seno et al., 2009Seno T, Ito H, Sunaga S. The object and background hypothesis for vection. Vision Research. 2009; 49(24):2973-82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.09.017. PMid:19782099.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009....
; Stoffregen, 1985Stoffregen TA. Flow structure versus retinal location in the optical control of stance. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 1985; 11(5):554-65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.11.5.554. PMid:2932530.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.11.5...
). Thus, it evokes an illusion that something moves towards (or backwards) the subject, as pointed out by Berthoz et al. (1975Berthoz A, Pavard B, Young LR. Perception of linear horizontal self-motion induced by peripheral vision (linearvection): basic characteristics and visual-vestibular interactions. Experimental Brain Research. 1975; 23(5):471-89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916. PMid:1081949.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916...
; 1979Berthoz A, Lacour M, Soechting JF, Vidal PP. The role of vision in the control of posture during linear motion. Progress in Brain Research. 1979; 50:197-209. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60820-1. PMid:551426.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(08)...
), O’Connor et al. (2008)O’Connor KW, Loughlin PJ, Redfern MS, Sparto PJ. Postural adaptations to repeated optic flow stimulation in older adults. Gait & Posture. 2008; 28(3):385-91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.01.010. PMid:18329878.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.200...
, Pretto et al. (2009)Pretto P, Ogier M, Bülthoff HH, Bresciani JP. Influence of size of the field of view on motion perception. Computer Graphics. 2009; 33(2):139-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01.003.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01....
, Reed-Jones et al. (2008)Reed-Jones RJ, Vallis LA, Reed-Jones JG, Trick LM. The relationship between postural stability and virtual environment adaptation. Neuroscience Letters. 2008; 435(3):204-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.02.047. PMid:18359162.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008....
and Wang et al. (2010)Wang Y, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Identifying the control of physically and perceptually evoked sway responses with coincident visual scene velocities and tilt of the base of support. Experimental Brain Research. 2010; 201(4):663-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-2082-0. PMid:19924408.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-208...
. This finding suggests that linear egomotion and the direction of the A/P COP displacement during distinct dynamic scenes are directly associated, as reported by Fushiki et al. (2005)Fushiki H, Kobayashi K, Asai M, Watanabe Y. Influence of visually induced self-motion on postural stability. Acta Oto-Laryngologica. 2005; 125(1):60-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00016480410015794. PMid:15799576.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00016480410015...
and Wang et al. (2010)Wang Y, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Identifying the control of physically and perceptually evoked sway responses with coincident visual scene velocities and tilt of the base of support. Experimental Brain Research. 2010; 201(4):663-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-2082-0. PMid:19924408.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-208...
. Therefore, the virtual dynamic visually evoked postural responses during stabilometric test cannot be elicited without an illusion of egomotion.

Hence, the dynamic property of the proposed protocol generates visual optic flow stimulation as a tunnel pattern similar to the natural vision during orthostatic sway. The virtual stimulation induces COP displacements response due to dynamic layout available to a stationary observer. The effects of linear egomotion are dependent on the direction of the dynamic visual stimulation. This finding indicates the potential application of the proposed virtual dynamic visual stimulation protocol to investigate the cortical visual evoked response in postural control studies.

Acknowledgements

Special thanks to our Professor Antonio Fernando Catelli Infantosi (in memorian) for dedication and expertise in Biomedical Engineering. Professor Antonio Infantosi will always be our mentor. Thanks to our best friend, João Gama (in memorian), for his dedication and for the books provided, enabling our improvement in the art of biological signal processing and neural engineering. The authors are thankful to all participants for investing their time and effort. This research was financially supported by FAPERJ and the Brazilian Research Council (CNPq).

References

  • Akizuki H, Uno A, Arai K, Morioka S, Ohyama S, Nishiike S, Tamura K, Takeda N. Effects of immersion in virtual reality on postural control. Neuroscience Letters. 2005; 379(1):23-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2004.12.041 PMid:15814192.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2004.12.041
  • Andersen GJ, Dyre BP. Spatial orientation from optic flow in the central visual field. Perception & Psychophysics. 1989; 45(5):453-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03210719 PMid:2726408.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03210719
  • Berthoz A, Lacour M, Soechting JF, Vidal PP. The role of vision in the control of posture during linear motion. Progress in Brain Research. 1979; 50:197-209. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60820-1 PMid:551426.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60820-1
  • Berthoz A, Pavard B, Young LR. Perception of linear horizontal self-motion induced by peripheral vision (linearvection): basic characteristics and visual-vestibular interactions. Experimental Brain Research. 1975; 23(5):471-89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916 PMid:1081949.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00234916
  • Da Silva PJG, Nadal J, Infantosi AFC. Investigating the center of pressure velocity Romberg’s quotient for assessing the visual role on the body sway. Revista Brasileira de Engenharia Biomédica. 2012; 28(4):319-26. http://dx.doi.org/10.4322/rbeb.2012.039
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.4322/rbeb.2012.039
  • Darekar A, McFadyen BJ, Lamontagne A, Fung J. Efficacy of virtual reality-based intervention on balance and mobility disorders post-stroke: a scoping review. Journal of Neuroengineering and Rehabilitation. 2015; 12(1):46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12984-015-0035-3 PMid:25957577.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12984-015-0035-3
  • Da-Silva PJG, Cagy M, Infantosi AFC. A new dynamic virtual stimulation protocol to induce vection. IFMBE Proceedings, 2015; 45:497-500. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-11128-5_124
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-11128-5_124
  • Day BL, Guerraz M. Feedforward versus feedback modulation of human vestibular-evoked balance responses by visual self-motion information. The Journal of Physiology. 2007; 582(1):153-61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2007.132092 PMid:17446222.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2007.132092
  • Dijkstra TMH, Gielen CCAM, Melis BJM. Postural responses to stationary and moving scenes as a function of distance to the scene. Human Movement Science. 1992; 11(1-2):195-203. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-9457(92)90060-O
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-9457(92)90060-O
  • Dijkstra TMH, Schöner G, Gielen CCAM. Temporal stability of the action-perception cycle for postural control in a moving visual environment. Experimental Brain Research. 1994; 97(3):477-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241542 PMid:8187859.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241542
  • Dokka K, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA, Kording KP. Self versus environment motion in postural control. PLoS Computational Biology. 2010; 6(2):1-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000680 PMid:20174552.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000680
  • Dokka K, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Influence of visual scene velocity on segmental kinematics during stance. Gait & Posture. 2009; 30(2):211-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.05.001 PMid:19505827.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.05.001
  • Fushiki H, Kobayashi K, Asai M, Watanabe Y. Influence of visually induced self-motion on postural stability. Acta Oto-Laryngologica. 2005; 125(1):60-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00016480410015794 PMid:15799576.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00016480410015794
  • Gibson JJ. The perception of the visual world. Oxford: Houghton Mifflin; 1950.
  • Gibson JJ. The visual perception of objective motion and subjective movement. Psychological Review. 1954; 61(5):304-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0061885 PMid:13204493.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0061885
  • Guerraz M, Bronstein AM. Mechanisms underlying visually induced body sway. Neuroscience Letters. 2008; 443(1):12-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.07.053 PMid:18672020.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.07.053
  • Guerraz M, Gianna CC, Burchill PM, Gresty MA, Bronstein AM. Effect of visual surrounding motion on body sway in a three-dimensional environment. Perception & Psychophysics. 2001a; 63(1):47-58. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03200502 PMid:11304016.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03200502
  • Guerraz M, Thilo KV, Bronstein AM, Gresty MA. Influence of action and expectation on visual control of posture. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research. 2001b; 11(2):259-66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00080-X PMid:11275487.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00080-X
  • Haibach PS, Slobounov SM, Newell KM. The potential applications of a virtual moving environment for assessing falls in elderly adults. Gait & Posture. 2008; 27(2):303-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.04.004 PMid:17524647.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.04.004
  • Kandel ER. Perception of motion, depth, and form. In: Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessel TM, editors. Principles of neurological science. New York: Elsevier; 1991. p. 440-66.
  • Keshner EA, Dokka K, Kenyon RV. Influences of the perception of self-motion on postural parameters. Cyberpsychology & Behavior. 2006; 9(2):163-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2006.9.163 PMid:16640471.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2006.9.163
  • Keshner EA, Kenyon RV, Langston J. Postural responses exhibit multisensory dependencies with discordant visual and support surface motion. Journal of Vestibular Research. 2004; 14(4):307-19. PMid:15328445.
  • Keshner EA, Kenyon RV. The influence of an immersive virtual environment on the segmental organization of postural stabilizing responses. Journal of Vestibular Research. 2000; 10(4-5):201-19.
  • Keshner EA, Kenyon RV. Using immersive technology for postural research and rehabilitation. Assistive Technology. 2004; 16(1):54-62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400435.2004.10132074 PMid:15357148.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400435.2004.10132074
  • Kuba M, Kubová Z, Kremlácek J, Langrová J. Motion-onset VEPs: Characteristics, methods, and diagnostic use. Vision Research. 2007; 47(2):189-202. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2006.09.020 PMid:17129593.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2006.09.020
  • Kuno S, Kawakita T, Kawakami O, Miyake Y, Watanabe S. Postural adjustment response to depth direction moving patterns produced by virtual reality graphics. Japanese Journal of Physiology. 1999; 49(5):417-24. http://dx.doi.org/10.2170/jjphysiol.49.417 PMid:10603425.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.2170/jjphysiol.49.417
  • Lestienne F, Soechting J, Berthoz A. Postural readjustments induced by linear motion of visual scenes. Experimental Brain Research. 1977; 28(3-4):363-84. PMid:885185.
  • Lishman JR, Lee DN. The autonomy of visual kinaesthesis. Perception. 1973; 2(3):287-94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p020287 PMid:4546578.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p020287
  • Masson G, Mestre DR, Blin O, Pailhous J. Low luminance contrast sensitivity: effect of training on psychophysical and optokinetic nystagmus thresholds in man. Vision Research. 1994; 34(14):1893-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(94)90313-1 PMid:7941391.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(94)90313-1
  • Masson G, Mestre DR, Pailhous J. Effects of the spatio-temporal structure of optical flow on postural readjustments in man. Experimental Brain Research. 1995; 103(1):137-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241971 PMid:7615029.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00241971
  • Mestre DR. Visual perception of self-motion. In: Proteau L, Elliott D, editors. Vision and motor control. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers B.V.; 1992. p. 421-38.
  • O’Connor KW, Loughlin PJ, Redfern MS, Sparto PJ. Postural adaptations to repeated optic flow stimulation in older adults. Gait & Posture. 2008; 28(3):385-91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.01.010 PMid:18329878.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.01.010
  • Paulus W, Straube A, Brandt T. Visual stabilization of posture: physiological stimulus characteristics and clinical aspects. Brain. 1984; 107(Pt 4):1143-63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/107.4.1143 PMid:6509312.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/107.4.1143
  • Paulus W, Straube A, Krafczyk S, Brandt T. Differential effects of retinal target displacement, changing size and changing disparity in the control of anterior/posterior and lateral body-sway. Experimental Brain Research. 1989; 78(2):243-52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00228896 PMid:2599035.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00228896
  • Pretto P, Ogier M, Bülthoff HH, Bresciani JP. Influence of size of the field of view on motion perception. Computer Graphics. 2009; 33(2):139-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01.003
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cag.2009.01.003
  • Reed-Jones RJ, Vallis LA, Reed-Jones JG, Trick LM. The relationship between postural stability and virtual environment adaptation. Neuroscience Letters. 2008; 435(3):204-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.02.047 PMid:18359162.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.02.047
  • Seno T, Ito H, Sunaga S. The object and background hypothesis for vection. Vision Research. 2009; 49(24):2973-82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.09.017 PMid:19782099.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.09.017
  • Seno T, Nakamura S, Ito H, Sunaga S. Static visual components without depth modulation alter the strength of vection. Vision Research. 2010; 50(19):1972-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010.07.004 PMid:20633575.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010.07.004
  • Slobounov S, Wu T, Hallett M, Shibasaki H, Slobounov E, Newell K. Neural underpinning of postural responses to visual field motion. Biological Psychology. 2006; 72(2):188-97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.10.005 PMid:16338048.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.10.005
  • Stoffregen TA. Flow structure versus retinal location in the optical control of stance. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 1985; 11(5):554-65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.11.5.554 PMid:2932530.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.11.5.554
  • Stoffregen TA. The role of optical velocity in the control of stance. Perception & Psychophysics. 1986; 39(5):355-60. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03203004 PMid:3737368.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03203004
  • Streepey JW, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Field of view and base of support width influence postural responses to visual stimuli during quiet stance. Gait & Posture. 2007; 25(1):49-55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.12.013 PMid:16464594.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.12.013
  • Tossavainen T, Juhola M, Pyykkö I, Aalto H, Toppila E. Development of virtual reality stimuli for force platform posturography. International Journal of Medical Informatics. 2003; 70(2-3):277-83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1386-5056(03)00034-0 PMid:12909179.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1386-5056(03)00034-0
  • Van Asten WNJC, Gielen CCAM, Van der Gon JJD. Postural adjustments induced by simulated motion of differently structured environments. Experimental Brain Research. 1988; 73(2):371-83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00248230 PMid:3215313.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00248230
  • Wang Y, Kenyon RV, Keshner EA. Identifying the control of physically and perceptually evoked sway responses with coincident visual scene velocities and tilt of the base of support. Experimental Brain Research. 2010; 201(4):663-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-2082-0 PMid:19924408.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-2082-0
  • Warren WH Jr, Hannon DJ. Direction of self-motion is perceived from optical flow. Nature. 1988; 336(6195):162-3. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/336162a0
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/336162a0
  • Warren WH. Self-motion: visual perception and visual control. In: Epstein W, Rogers S, editors. Perception of space and motion. London: Academic Press; 1995. p. 263-325.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    10 Oct 2016
  • Date of issue
    Jul-Sep 2016

History

  • Received
    26 Feb 2016
  • Accepted
    17 Sept 2016
Sociedade Brasileira de Engenharia Biomédica Centro de Tecnologia, bloco H, sala 327 - Cidade Universitária, 21941-914 Rio de Janeiro RJ Brasil, Tel./Fax: (55 21)2562-8591 - Rio de Janeiro - RJ - Brazil
E-mail: rbe@rbejournal.org