Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

THE ANTICIPATORY AND REACTION TIME BEHAVIORS OF THE FUTSAL GOALKEEPER

OS COMPORTAMENTOS ANTECIPATÓRIO E DE TEMPO DE REAÇÃO DO GOLEIRO DO FUTSAL

ABSTRACT

This study aimed to investigate the goalkeeper’s anticipatory and reaction time behaviors. For this purpose, 50 kicks performed by male futsal players were analyzed. The time of goalkeeper’s response to a kick was analyzed in relation to the kick outcome, court zone and distance between the goalkeeper and player who performed the kick. The goalkeeper’s anticipation and reaction time behaviors were those below and above 200 ms as the time criterion, respectively, by considering the moment the player began the preparation for kicking to the moment the goalkeeper began to respond to it. Results showed that the goalkeeper performed anticipatory behaviors predominantly when (i) goals were attained and (ii) kicks were performed in the center and attack court’s zones (iii) at a distance ranging from 5.97 to 7.84 meters. In these conditions, the goalkeepers’ anticipatory behaviors implied ineffective performances.

Keywords:
Futsal kick; Goalkeeper; Anticipation; Reaction time

RESUMO

Este estudo teve como objetivo investigar os comportamentos antecipatório e de tempo de reação do goleiro. Para este fim foram analisados 50 chutes executados por jogaores de futsal. O tempo de resposta do goleiro a um chute foi analisado em relação ao resultado do chute, à zona da quadra e à distância entre o goleiro e o jogador que executou o chute. Os comportamentos de antecipação e tempo de reação do goleiro foram considerados, respectivamente, como aqueles executados abaixo e acima de 200 ms considerando o início do movimento do chutador e o início do movimento do goleiro. Os resultados mostraram que o goleiro executou comportamentos antecipatórios predominantemente quando (i) ocorreram gols, (ii) os chutes foram executados das zonas central e de ataque, (iii) de distâncias entre 5,97 a 7,84 metros. Nestas condições, os comportamentos antecipatórios do goleiro implicaram em performances ineficazes.

Palavras-chave:
Chute de futsal; Goleiro; Antecipação; Tempo de Reação

Introduction

As a countless human motor skills, those of futsal has the time as an essential variable for attaining their goals with successful. For instance, one could say that the temporal coincidence is the main purpose of the goalkeeper’s motor skills. This is because he/she has to intercept a moving ball by hiting or holding it in order to prevent it from entering the goal. If he/she arrives to the ball trajectory before or after it, the goal can occur and, therefore, his/her performance would be unsuccessful.

However, in order to get it a goalkeeper needs to deal with two other important temporal dimensions underlying the performance of motor skills: anticipation or reaction time11 Abernethy B, Neal R. Visual characteristics of clay target shooters. J Sci Med Sport 1999;2(1):1-19. Doi: 10.1016/s1440-2440(99)80180-7
https://doi.org/10.1016/s1440-2440(99)80...

2 Helson H. Design of equipment and optimal human operation. Am J Psychol 1949;62(4):473-479. Doi: 10.2307/1418555
https://doi.org/10.2307/1418555...

3 Kim HS, Petrakis E. Visuo perceptual speed of karate practitioners at three levels of skill. Percept Mot Skills 1998;87(1):96-98. Doi: 10.2466/pms.1998.87.1.96.
https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1998.87.1.96...

4 Kioumourtzoglou E, Derri V, Mertzanidou O, Tzetzis G. Experience with perceptual and motor skills in rhythmic gymnastics. Percept Mot Skills 1997;84(3):1363-1372. Doi: 10.2466/pms.1997.84.3c.1363.
https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1997.84.3c.1...

5 Kioumourtzoglou E, Kortessis T, Michalopoulou M, Derri V. Differences in several perceptual abilities between experts and novices in basketball, volleyball and water-polo. Percept Mot Skills 1998;86(3):899-912. Doi: 10.2466/pms.1998.86.3.899.
https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1998.86.3.89...

6 Poulton EC. On prediction in skilled movements. Psychol Bull 1957;54(6):467-478. Doi: 10.1037/h0045515
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0045515...

7 Schmidt RA. Anticipation and timing in human motor performance. Psychol Bull 1968;70(6):631-646. Doi.org/10.1037/h0026740
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0026740...

8 Tani G. Arte e ciência da habilidade de não ser fintado. In: Tani G, editor. Comportamento motor: Conceitos estudos e aplicações. Rio de Janeiro: Guanabara Koogan; 2016, p. 261-210.
-99 Schmidt RA, Wrisberg CA. Aprendizagem e performance motora: Uma aprendizagem baseada em situação. Brasil: Artmed; 2010.. To put it in another way, the temporal coincidence that goalkeeper seeks in relation to the moving ball in order to avoid the goal may be preceded by underlying prospective processing of information or processing of an already available target information, respectively.

Concerning to the first, one could say that anticipation involves a prediction about the event in the environment, as well as the location and time that it will occur77 Schmidt RA. Anticipation and timing in human motor performance. Psychol Bull 1968;70(6):631-646. Doi.org/10.1037/h0026740
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0026740...
,88 Tani G. Arte e ciência da habilidade de não ser fintado. In: Tani G, editor. Comportamento motor: Conceitos estudos e aplicações. Rio de Janeiro: Guanabara Koogan; 2016, p. 261-210.. In this case, perception of environmental cues (e.g. spatiotemporal tendencies of players displacement) would allow the response selection (decision-making) and programming in advance to existence of the target stimulus itself66 Poulton EC. On prediction in skilled movements. Psychol Bull 1957;54(6):467-478. Doi: 10.1037/h0045515
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0045515...
,99 Schmidt RA, Wrisberg CA. Aprendizagem e performance motora: Uma aprendizagem baseada em situação. Brasil: Artmed; 2010.. For instance, the fact an attacker without the ball possession is positioned without a marking defender could lead the goalkeeper to perceive a high risk of a kick if he/she received a pass. Based on this, he/she could anticipate his/her preparation for defending the kick.

Importantly, anticipation is a hallmark of skillful behavior and as such is acquired from practice and experience1010 Edwards WH. Motor Learning and control: From theory to practice. Belmont: Wadsworth; 2010.

11 Magill RA. Aprendizagem motora: Conceitos e aplicações. São Paulo: Edgard Blucher; 2000.
-1212 Singer RN. Motor learning and human performance: An application to motor skills and movement behavior. 3.ed. New York: MacMillan Publishing; 1980.. In fact, anticipatory behavior has been related to the experienced players’ perceptual ability in a way they are able to detect information for anticipation from the environment more quickly and effectively than unexperienced players1313 French KE, Nevett ME, Spurgeon JH, Graham KC, Rink JE, McPherson SL. Knowledge representation and problem solution in expert and novice youth baseball players. Res Q Exer Sport 1996;67(4):386-395. Doi: 10.1080/02701367.1996.10607970
https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.1996.10...
. For example, a study by Savelsbergh et al.1414 Savelsbergh GJP, Williams AM, Der Kamp JV, Ward P. Visual search, anticipation and expertise in soccer goalkeepers. J Sports Sc 2002;20(3):279-287. Doi:10.1080/026404102317284826
https://doi.org/10.1080/0264041023172848...
examined the differences in anticipation and visual search behaviors between experts and novices goalkeepers in a soccer penalty kick situation. Experts and novices goalkeepers were required to move a joystick in response to penalty kicks presented on film. The proportions of penalties saved were assessed, as well as the frequency and time of initiation of joystick corrections. Visual search behaviors were examined using an eye movement registration system. In comparing to novices, experts goalkeepers (i) were more accurate in predicting the direction of the penalty kick, (ii) waited longer before initiating a response and (iii) made fewer corrective movements with the joystick.

On the other hand, reaction time (RT) refers to the time between the beginning of the stimulus and the beginning of response. RT has been used as an approximate estimate measure which allows inferring the time required for interpreting a stimulus, selecting and programming a response99 Schmidt RA, Wrisberg CA. Aprendizagem e performance motora: Uma aprendizagem baseada em situação. Brasil: Artmed; 2010.,1515 Oxendine JB. Psychology of motor learning. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1968.

16 Schmidt RA, Lee T. Motor control and learning: a behavioral emphasis. 3rd.ed. Champaign: Human Kinetics; 2005.
-1717 Henry FM, Rogers DE. Increased response latency for complicated movements and a "memory drum »,» ®,® §,§ ­,­ ¹,¹ ²,² ³,³ ß,ß Þ,Þ þ,þ ×,× Ú,Ú ú,ú Û,Û û,û Ù,Ù ù,ù ¨,¨ Ü,Ü ü,ü Ý,Ý ý,ý ¥,¥ ÿ,ÿ ¶,¶ theory of neuromotor reaction. Research Quarterly 1960;31:448-458. Doi: 10.1080/10671188.1960.10762052
https://doi.org/10.1080/10671188.1960.10...
. Importantly to be clear that such inference is closely related to the tasks in which to respond as quickly as possible to the not foreseen stimulus is essential for successful performance.

Interestingly to note that RT manifests differently for each sensory system (e.g. tactile RT ≈ 110 ms; auditory RT ≈ 150 ms; visual RT ≈ 200 ms)1111 Magill RA. Aprendizagem motora: Conceitos e aplicações. São Paulo: Edgard Blucher; 2000.,1818 Brebner JT, Welford AT. Introduction: An historical background sketch. In: Welford AT, editor. Reaction Times. New York: Academic Press; 1980, p. 1-23.

19 Welford AT. Choice reaction time: Basic concepts. In: Welford AT, editor. Reaction Times. New York: Academic Press; 1980, p. 73-128.

20 Scott SH. Optimal feedback control and the neural basis of volitional motor control. Nat Rev Neurosci 2004;5:532-546. Doi: 10.1038/nrn1427
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1427...

21 Marinovic W, Plooy AM, Tresilian JR. The utilisation of visual information in the control of rapid interceptive actions. Exp Psychol 2009;56(4):265-273. Doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.4.265
https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.56.4.2...

22 Yang L, Michaels JA, Andrew J, Scott SH. Rapid motor responses quickly integrate visuospatial task constraints. Exp Brain Res 2011;211:231-242. Doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2674-3
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2674-...
-2323 Scott SH. A functional taxonomy of bottom-up sensory feedback processing for motor actions. Trends in Neurosciences 2016;39(8):512-526. Doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2016.06.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2016.06.0...
. Furthermore, it can differ from some other variables including age, player category (e.g. amateur or professional), field position (e.g. defense, midfield and attack) and type of physical activity/sport (e.g. soccer, basketball, surf and gym)2424 Montés- Micó R, Bueno I, Candel J, Pons AM.Eye-hand and eye-foot visual reaction times of young soccer players. Optometry 2000;71(12):775-780.

25 Ruschel C, Haupenthal A, Hubert M, Fontana HB, Pereira SM, Roesler H. Tempo de reação simples de jogadores de futebol de diferentes categorias e posições. Rev Motri 2011;7(4):73-82. Doi: 10.6063/motricidade.7(4).90
https://doi.org/10.6063/motricidade.7(4)...

26 Vaghetti CAO, Roesler H, Andrade, A. Tempo de reação simples auditivo e visual em surfistas com diferentes níveis de habilidade: Comparação entre atletas profissionais, amadores e praticantes. Rev Bras Med Esporte 2007;13(2):81-85. Doi: 10.1590/S1517-86922007000200003.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1517-8692200700...
-2727 Bruzi AT, Fialho JVAP, Fonseca FS, Ugrinowitsch H. Comparação do tempo de reação entre atletas de basquetebol, ginástica artística e não atletas. Rev Bras Ciênc Esporte 2013;35(2):469-480. Doi.org/10.1590/S0101-32892013000200015
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0101-3289201300...
. It is important to highlight that most of these studies were conducted in experimental laboratory environment, which have pointed out that visual RT involves values ranging from 150 to 301 ms, whose average value still accepted is about 200 ms1111 Magill RA. Aprendizagem motora: Conceitos e aplicações. São Paulo: Edgard Blucher; 2000.,2828 Cordo PJ, Flanders M. Sensory control of target acquisition. Trends in Neurosciences 1989;12(2):110-117. Doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(89)90167-7
https://doi.org/rg/10.1016/0166-2236(89)...
,2929 Senel Ö, Eroglu H. Correlation between reaction time and speed in elite soccer players. J Exerc Sci Fit 2006;4(2):126-130.

RT can be simple when it involves one stimulus and one response, and of choice when it involves several stimuli and responses. This latter tends to increase as the number of stimulus-response alternatives increases99 Schmidt RA, Wrisberg CA. Aprendizagem e performance motora: Uma aprendizagem baseada em situação. Brasil: Artmed; 2010.,1111 Magill RA. Aprendizagem motora: Conceitos e aplicações. São Paulo: Edgard Blucher; 2000.. For example, the goalkeeper is confronted with the choice RT when the attacker player uses a feint composed by two simultaneous (or almost) movements to kick the ball, being one to induce the goalkeeper's error.

Similarly to the anticipation, RT is influenced by the players’ experience level5,15,26,30,31,. For example, Ruschel25 analyzed the visual and auditory simple RT of soccer players from differing categories (amateurs and professionals) and field positions (goalkeeper, center defender, right and left defenders, halfback, midfielder and forward). Results showed that visual RT did not differ between categories. However, professionals showed faster auditory RT than amateurs. Regarding field positions, goalkeepers showed faster visual RT than midfield players.

In sum, the fact that futsal game is a dynamic system, i.e. the interactions between players change in a non-linear way over time1010 Edwards WH. Motor Learning and control: From theory to practice. Belmont: Wadsworth; 2010.,3232 Corrêa UC, Alegre F, Freudenheim AM, Santos S, Tani G. The game of futsal as an adaptive process. Nonlinear Dynamics, Psychol Life Sci 2012;16(2):185-204.

33 Corrêa UC, Davids K, Silva SL, Denardi RA, Tani G. The influence of a goalkeeper as an outfield player on defensive subsystems in futsal. Adv Phys Educ 2014;4(2):84-92. Doi: 10.4236/ape.2014.42012
https://doi.org/10.4236/ape.2014.42012...
-3434 Silva SL, Travassos B, Davids K, Moreira A, Silva Filho AS, Correa UC. Effects of experience, knowledge and skill on regulationg the performance of futsal passing actions. Int J Sport Psychol 2017;48(1):37-49. Doi: 10.7352/IJSP2017.48.037
https://doi.org/10.7352/IJSP2017.48.037...
, allowed hypothesize that the anticipatory and reaction time behaviors would also function in a dynamic way. For instance, due to the game dynamic, not always the emerging patterns would imply enough cues for the anticipation of the goalkeeper. Therefore, we asked: what would influence the anticipatory and reaction time behaviors of the goalkeeper? In order to investigate this question the goalkeeper’s responses to a kick were analyzed in relation to the kick outcome, court location and distance between the goalkeeper and player who performed the kick.

Methods

Participants

Twenty-four male amateur players, men, who took part in two qualifying round games in the adult category (over 20 years old) of Futsal League of Carapicuíba in 2018 participated. This is an annual championship held in Carapicuíba, São Paulo, Brazil, which involves more than 1,000 players in various categories. All the players were experienced, since they had participated at least in two futsal official championships. The protocol of this study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the University of São Paulo - USP under the number CAAE: 10850513.0.0000.5391.

Procedures

Concerning the data collection, sample consisted of 50 sequences of play involving kicks performed by outfield players or goalkeeper, which were randomly selected from digital video footage of the aforementioned games. The sequences of play ranged from 16 to 494 ms of duration. They were recorded by a digital camera (GoPro HEROS3; frequency = 120 Hz) located above and behind the short axis of a futsal court, which had dimensions of 17 m x 26 m (Figure 1).

The selected sequences of kicks were digitized by KINOVEA software 0.8.15 in a slow motion video image using a computer mouse, from the moment the player began the preparation for kicking (taking the foot off the floor and flexing the knee by lifting the leg back and up), which characterized the start of the stimulus, to the moment the goalkeeper began to respond to it (start of the response). This involved any goalkeeper's movement in response to the kick. This procedure made it possible to obtain the goalkeeper’s response time in milliseconds on the basis of which anticipation and reaction were defined. Specifically, the goalkeeper’s anticipation and reaction behaviors were those below and above 200 ms as the time criterion, respectively24-27.

As previously described, the goalkeeper’s behaviors were analyzed in relation to following variables:

  • (a) kick outcome. It was based on the kick successful by considering if a goal occurred or there was a goalkeeper’s defense, kick out or an outfield player’s interception.;

  • (b) lateral (right, center and left) and longitudinal court zones (attack, midfield and defense). For this purpose, the court was divided into six equal parts (zone);

  • (c) distance between the goalkeeper and player who performed the kick, which was considered in terms of quartiles1313 French KE, Nevett ME, Spurgeon JH, Graham KC, Rink JE, McPherson SL. Knowledge representation and problem solution in expert and novice youth baseball players. Res Q Exer Sport 1996;67(4):386-395. Doi: 10.1080/02701367.1996.10607970
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.1996.10...
    : 1st quartile involved distances between 2.68 and 5.92 meters; 2nd quartile were those kicks performed between 5.97 and 7.84 meters; 3rd quartile referred to the kicks performed between 7.88 and 13.79 meters; and, 4th quartile were involved kicks performed between 13.97 and 25.03 meters.

Figure 1
Illustration of the data collection experimental environment

Data analysis

As the Shapiro-Wilk W test of normality and Bartlett's test of homogeneity of variance revealed that the assumptions for parametric analysis were not met, a Chi-square test of one criterion was used. This procedure allowed verifying if the observed occurrences of anticipatory and reaction behaviors differed from a balanced distribution. For this purpose, we considered as expected behaviors a balanced distribution of both behaviors. That is, from the total observed behaviors, 50% would be expected from anticipatory behaviors and 50% from reaction time behaviors. It was run by the following equation: χ² = Σ(of - ef)² / ef, where of referred to observed frequency and the ef was the expected frequency. In order to obtain the values of the Chi-square table, the degrees of freedom was determined by df = k - 1, where df referred to the degree of freedom, k was the number of categories in the observed frequency distribution table. Significant difference was considered when the value of the acquired Chi-square was higher than the tabulated Chi-square value3535 Levin J, Fox JA. Estatística para ciências humanas. 9.ed. São Paulo, Brazil: Pearson Prentice Hall; 2004.. The level of significance of p < 0.05 was considered.

Results

Regarding the kick outcome, the Chi-square test revealed difference in the goal variable (Table 1). In this case, anticipation was more observed than reaction time in situation in which occurred goals.

Table 1
Frequencies of goalkeepers' behaviors of anticipation and reaction time and results of the Chi-square test for kick outcome variable

In relation to the court zone, the Chi-square test revealed differences for center and attack zones (Table 2). In both cases, anticipation occurred more frequently than reaction time.

Table 2
Frequencies of goalkeepers' behaviors of anticipation and reaction time and results of the Chi-square test for court zones variable

Concerning the distance between the goalkeeper and player who performed the kick, the Chi-square test revealed differences for the 2nd quartile (Table 3). The observed behavior was greater than that expected in relation to the anticipation. For other distances, no significant differences were found in relation to the observed behavior of the expected.

Table 3
Frequencies of goalkeepers' behaviors of anticipation and reaction time and results of the Chi-square test for kick distance variable

Discussion

This study aimed to investigate the anticipatory and reaction time behaviors of the goalkeeper based on the kick outcome, court zone and distance between the goalkeeper and player who performed the kick. Results showed that anticipation as the goalkeeper’s predominantly behavior when (i) goals were attained and (ii) kicks were performed in the center and attack court’s zones (iii) at a distance ranging from 5.97 to 7.84 meters.

Anticipation requires an understanding of the dynamics of the game. An important feature of anticipation is the impact of looking to the future about current behavior. An individual not only predicts the future, but he/she also modifies behavior according to such prediction3636 Butz MV, Sigaud O, Pezzulo G, Baldassarre, G. Anticipations, brains, individual and social behavior: An introduction to anticipatory systems. In: Butz MV, Sigaud O, Pezzulo G, Baldassarre G, editors. Anticipatory behavior in adaptive learning systems. Berlin: Springer-Verlag; 2007, p. 1-18.,3737 Fajen BR, Riley MA, Turvey MT. Information, affordances, and the control of action in sport. Int J Sport Psychol 2009;40(1):79-107.. Dealing with future information in the present is always accompanied by uncertainties, which implies in increasing the risk of unsuccessuful performances3838 Tani G. Hierarchical organisation of human motor behaviour. Sheffield: Unpublished Technical Report; 1995..

It is possible that goalkeeper had tried to anticipate because the kicks performed at the center and attack court zones at a distance ranging from 5.97 to 7.84 meters implied a high risk of a goal occurs. In this case, anticipation would function as a kind of resource for diminishing the attacker advantage. It can be said that when an attacker is facing and close to the goal he has more options to choose how, when and where to kick the ball.

However, our results allow inferring the goalkeeper’s decisions to anticipate were not successful because the kicks resulted in goals. Thus, anticipation may not have been anadequated decision, since he was unable to avoid the goal. To put it in another way, anticipation had an irreparable cost99 Schmidt RA, Wrisberg CA. Aprendizagem e performance motora: Uma aprendizagem baseada em situação. Brasil: Artmed; 2010.. Ineffective anticipation causes the player to lose time and compromise their performance3939 Travassos B, Araujo A, Davids K, Vilar L, Esteves PT, Correia V. Informational constraints shape emergent functional behaviours during performance of interceptive actions in team sports. Psychol Sport Exerc 2012;13(2):216-223. Doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.11.009
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.201...
.

It is important to note that the futsal players who participated in this research were experienced. However, the competence in anticipatory behavior which is evident in experienced players88 Tani G. Arte e ciência da habilidade de não ser fintado. In: Tani G, editor. Comportamento motor: Conceitos estudos e aplicações. Rio de Janeiro: Guanabara Koogan; 2016, p. 261-210.,1010 Edwards WH. Motor Learning and control: From theory to practice. Belmont: Wadsworth; 2010.,1111 Magill RA. Aprendizagem motora: Conceitos e aplicações. São Paulo: Edgard Blucher; 2000.,2626 Vaghetti CAO, Roesler H, Andrade, A. Tempo de reação simples auditivo e visual em surfistas com diferentes níveis de habilidade: Comparação entre atletas profissionais, amadores e praticantes. Rev Bras Med Esporte 2007;13(2):81-85. Doi: 10.1590/S1517-86922007000200003.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1517-8692200700...
,3030 Miyamoto RJ, Meira JR. Tempo de reação e tempo das provas de 50 e 100 metros rasos do atletismo em federados e não federados. Rev Port Cienc Desporto 2004;4(3):42-48. Doi:10.5628/rpcd.04.03.42
https://doi.org/10.5628/rpcd.04.03.42...
,3434 Silva SL, Travassos B, Davids K, Moreira A, Silva Filho AS, Correa UC. Effects of experience, knowledge and skill on regulationg the performance of futsal passing actions. Int J Sport Psychol 2017;48(1):37-49. Doi: 10.7352/IJSP2017.48.037
https://doi.org/10.7352/IJSP2017.48.037...
was not enough for the goalkeepers to deal with the risks involved in the kicks situations.

Conclusion

The findings of this study allowed us to conclude that the goalkeeper performed anticipatory behavior predominantly when (i) goals were attained and (ii) kicks were performed in the center and attack court’s zones (iii) at a distance ranging from 5.97 to 7.84 meters. In these conditions, the goalkeepers’ anticipatory behaviors here considered as those responses below 200 ms implied ineffective performances. Although 200 ms be a well accept measure for inferring anticipation/reaction2424 Montés- Micó R, Bueno I, Candel J, Pons AM.Eye-hand and eye-foot visual reaction times of young soccer players. Optometry 2000;71(12):775-780.

25 Ruschel C, Haupenthal A, Hubert M, Fontana HB, Pereira SM, Roesler H. Tempo de reação simples de jogadores de futebol de diferentes categorias e posições. Rev Motri 2011;7(4):73-82. Doi: 10.6063/motricidade.7(4).90
https://doi.org/10.6063/motricidade.7(4)...

26 Vaghetti CAO, Roesler H, Andrade, A. Tempo de reação simples auditivo e visual em surfistas com diferentes níveis de habilidade: Comparação entre atletas profissionais, amadores e praticantes. Rev Bras Med Esporte 2007;13(2):81-85. Doi: 10.1590/S1517-86922007000200003.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1517-8692200700...
-2727 Bruzi AT, Fialho JVAP, Fonseca FS, Ugrinowitsch H. Comparação do tempo de reação entre atletas de basquetebol, ginástica artística e não atletas. Rev Bras Ciênc Esporte 2013;35(2):469-480. Doi.org/10.1590/S0101-32892013000200015
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0101-3289201300...
, most of the evidence supporting it comes from studies conducted in artificial laboratory environments. This aspect should be focused on further studies.

References

  • 1
    Abernethy B, Neal R. Visual characteristics of clay target shooters. J Sci Med Sport 1999;2(1):1-19. Doi: 10.1016/s1440-2440(99)80180-7
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/s1440-2440(99)80180-7
  • 2
    Helson H. Design of equipment and optimal human operation. Am J Psychol 1949;62(4):473-479. Doi: 10.2307/1418555
    » https://doi.org/10.2307/1418555
  • 3
    Kim HS, Petrakis E. Visuo perceptual speed of karate practitioners at three levels of skill. Percept Mot Skills 1998;87(1):96-98. Doi: 10.2466/pms.1998.87.1.96.
    » https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1998.87.1.96
  • 4
    Kioumourtzoglou E, Derri V, Mertzanidou O, Tzetzis G. Experience with perceptual and motor skills in rhythmic gymnastics. Percept Mot Skills 1997;84(3):1363-1372. Doi: 10.2466/pms.1997.84.3c.1363.
    » https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1997.84.3c.1363
  • 5
    Kioumourtzoglou E, Kortessis T, Michalopoulou M, Derri V. Differences in several perceptual abilities between experts and novices in basketball, volleyball and water-polo. Percept Mot Skills 1998;86(3):899-912. Doi: 10.2466/pms.1998.86.3.899.
    » https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1998.86.3.899
  • 6
    Poulton EC. On prediction in skilled movements. Psychol Bull 1957;54(6):467-478. Doi: 10.1037/h0045515
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/h0045515
  • 7
    Schmidt RA. Anticipation and timing in human motor performance. Psychol Bull 1968;70(6):631-646. Doi.org/10.1037/h0026740
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/h0026740
  • 8
    Tani G. Arte e ciência da habilidade de não ser fintado. In: Tani G, editor. Comportamento motor: Conceitos estudos e aplicações. Rio de Janeiro: Guanabara Koogan; 2016, p. 261-210.
  • 9
    Schmidt RA, Wrisberg CA. Aprendizagem e performance motora: Uma aprendizagem baseada em situação. Brasil: Artmed; 2010.
  • 10
    Edwards WH. Motor Learning and control: From theory to practice. Belmont: Wadsworth; 2010.
  • 11
    Magill RA. Aprendizagem motora: Conceitos e aplicações. São Paulo: Edgard Blucher; 2000.
  • 12
    Singer RN. Motor learning and human performance: An application to motor skills and movement behavior. 3.ed. New York: MacMillan Publishing; 1980.
  • 13
    French KE, Nevett ME, Spurgeon JH, Graham KC, Rink JE, McPherson SL. Knowledge representation and problem solution in expert and novice youth baseball players. Res Q Exer Sport 1996;67(4):386-395. Doi: 10.1080/02701367.1996.10607970
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.1996.10607970
  • 14
    Savelsbergh GJP, Williams AM, Der Kamp JV, Ward P. Visual search, anticipation and expertise in soccer goalkeepers. J Sports Sc 2002;20(3):279-287. Doi:10.1080/026404102317284826
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/026404102317284826
  • 15
    Oxendine JB. Psychology of motor learning. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1968.
  • 16
    Schmidt RA, Lee T. Motor control and learning: a behavioral emphasis. 3rd.ed. Champaign: Human Kinetics; 2005.
  • 17
    Henry FM, Rogers DE. Increased response latency for complicated movements and a "memory drum »,» ®,® §,§ ­,­ ¹,¹ ²,² ³,³ ß,ß Þ,Þ þ,þ ×,× Ú,Ú ú,ú Û,Û û,û Ù,Ù ù,ù ¨,¨ Ü,Ü ü,ü Ý,Ý ý,ý ¥,¥ ÿ,ÿ ¶,¶ theory of neuromotor reaction. Research Quarterly 1960;31:448-458. Doi: 10.1080/10671188.1960.10762052
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/10671188.1960.10762052
  • 18
    Brebner JT, Welford AT. Introduction: An historical background sketch. In: Welford AT, editor. Reaction Times. New York: Academic Press; 1980, p. 1-23.
  • 19
    Welford AT. Choice reaction time: Basic concepts. In: Welford AT, editor. Reaction Times. New York: Academic Press; 1980, p. 73-128.
  • 20
    Scott SH. Optimal feedback control and the neural basis of volitional motor control. Nat Rev Neurosci 2004;5:532-546. Doi: 10.1038/nrn1427
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1427
  • 21
    Marinovic W, Plooy AM, Tresilian JR. The utilisation of visual information in the control of rapid interceptive actions. Exp Psychol 2009;56(4):265-273. Doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.4.265
    » https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.56.4.265
  • 22
    Yang L, Michaels JA, Andrew J, Scott SH. Rapid motor responses quickly integrate visuospatial task constraints. Exp Brain Res 2011;211:231-242. Doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2674-3
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2674-3
  • 23
    Scott SH. A functional taxonomy of bottom-up sensory feedback processing for motor actions. Trends in Neurosciences 2016;39(8):512-526. Doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2016.06.001
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2016.06.001
  • 24
    Montés- Micó R, Bueno I, Candel J, Pons AM.Eye-hand and eye-foot visual reaction times of young soccer players. Optometry 2000;71(12):775-780.
  • 25
    Ruschel C, Haupenthal A, Hubert M, Fontana HB, Pereira SM, Roesler H. Tempo de reação simples de jogadores de futebol de diferentes categorias e posições. Rev Motri 2011;7(4):73-82. Doi: 10.6063/motricidade.7(4).90
    » https://doi.org/10.6063/motricidade.7(4).90
  • 26
    Vaghetti CAO, Roesler H, Andrade, A. Tempo de reação simples auditivo e visual em surfistas com diferentes níveis de habilidade: Comparação entre atletas profissionais, amadores e praticantes. Rev Bras Med Esporte 2007;13(2):81-85. Doi: 10.1590/S1517-86922007000200003.
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S1517-86922007000200003
  • 27
    Bruzi AT, Fialho JVAP, Fonseca FS, Ugrinowitsch H. Comparação do tempo de reação entre atletas de basquetebol, ginástica artística e não atletas. Rev Bras Ciênc Esporte 2013;35(2):469-480. Doi.org/10.1590/S0101-32892013000200015
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0101-32892013000200015
  • 28
    Cordo PJ, Flanders M. Sensory control of target acquisition. Trends in Neurosciences 1989;12(2):110-117. Doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(89)90167-7
    » https://doi.org/rg/10.1016/0166-2236(89)90167-7
  • 29
    Senel Ö, Eroglu H. Correlation between reaction time and speed in elite soccer players. J Exerc Sci Fit 2006;4(2):126-130
  • 30
    Miyamoto RJ, Meira JR. Tempo de reação e tempo das provas de 50 e 100 metros rasos do atletismo em federados e não federados. Rev Port Cienc Desporto 2004;4(3):42-48. Doi:10.5628/rpcd.04.03.42
    » https://doi.org/10.5628/rpcd.04.03.42
  • 31
    Mori S, Ohtani Y, Imanaka K. Reaction time and anticipatory skills of karate athletes. Hum Mov Sc 2002;21(2):213-230. Doi: 10.1016/S0167-9457(02)00103-3
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-9457(02)00103-3
  • 32
    Corrêa UC, Alegre F, Freudenheim AM, Santos S, Tani G. The game of futsal as an adaptive process. Nonlinear Dynamics, Psychol Life Sci 2012;16(2):185-204.
  • 33
    Corrêa UC, Davids K, Silva SL, Denardi RA, Tani G. The influence of a goalkeeper as an outfield player on defensive subsystems in futsal. Adv Phys Educ 2014;4(2):84-92. Doi: 10.4236/ape.2014.42012
    » https://doi.org/10.4236/ape.2014.42012
  • 34
    Silva SL, Travassos B, Davids K, Moreira A, Silva Filho AS, Correa UC. Effects of experience, knowledge and skill on regulationg the performance of futsal passing actions. Int J Sport Psychol 2017;48(1):37-49. Doi: 10.7352/IJSP2017.48.037
    » https://doi.org/10.7352/IJSP2017.48.037
  • 35
    Levin J, Fox JA. Estatística para ciências humanas. 9.ed. São Paulo, Brazil: Pearson Prentice Hall; 2004.
  • 36
    Butz MV, Sigaud O, Pezzulo G, Baldassarre, G. Anticipations, brains, individual and social behavior: An introduction to anticipatory systems. In: Butz MV, Sigaud O, Pezzulo G, Baldassarre G, editors. Anticipatory behavior in adaptive learning systems. Berlin: Springer-Verlag; 2007, p. 1-18.
  • 37
    Fajen BR, Riley MA, Turvey MT. Information, affordances, and the control of action in sport. Int J Sport Psychol 2009;40(1):79-107.
  • 38
    Tani G. Hierarchical organisation of human motor behaviour. Sheffield: Unpublished Technical Report; 1995.
  • 39
    Travassos B, Araujo A, Davids K, Vilar L, Esteves PT, Correia V. Informational constraints shape emergent functional behaviours during performance of interceptive actions in team sports. Psychol Sport Exerc 2012;13(2):216-223. Doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.11.009
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.11.009

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    05 May 2021
  • Date of issue
    2021

History

  • Received
    23 May 2019
  • Reviewed
    10 Feb 2020
  • Accepted
    01 May 2020
Universidade Estadual de Maringá Avenida Colombo, 5790 - cep: 87020-900 - tel: 44 3011 4315 - Maringá - PR - Brazil
E-mail: revdef@uem.br