Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Walking execution is not affected by divided attention in patients with multiple sclerosis with no disability, but there is a motor planning impairment

A execução da marcha não é afetada pela atenção dividida em pacientes com esclerose múltipla sem incapacidade, mas existe um comprometimento do planejamento motor

Abstracts

Purpose: We analysed the cognitive influence on walking in multiple sclerosis (MS) patients, in the absence of clinical disability. Method: A case-control study was conducted with 12 MS patients with no disability and 12 matched healthy controls. Subjects were referred for completion a timed walk test of 10 m and a 3D-kinematic analysis. Participants were instructed to walk at a comfortable speed in a dual-task (arithmetic task) condition, and motor planning was measured by mental chronometry. Results: Scores of walking speed and cadence showed no statistically significant differences between the groups in the three conditions. The dual-task condition showed an increase in the double support duration in both groups. Motor imagery analysis showed statistically significant differences between real and imagined walking in patients. Conclusion: MS patients with no disability did not show any influence of divided attention on walking execution. However, motor planning was overestimated as compared with real walking.

mild cognitive impairment; multiple sclerosis; walking


O objetivo do estudo foi analisar a influência cognitiva na caminhada de pacientes com esclerose múltipla (EM) sem incapacidade clínica. Foi conduzido um estudo caso-controle com 12 pacientes com EM sem incapacidade com 12 pessoas saudáveis como controles pareados. Os sujeitos fizeram um teste de caminhada de 10 metros , acompanhado de análise cinemática 3D, e foram orientados a caminhar em velocidade confortável, realizando dupla-tarefa (tarefa aritmética), e o planejamento motor foi medido pela cronometria mental. Os valores de velocidade da caminhada e da cadência não evidenciaram diferenças estatisticamente significativas entre os grupos nas três condições. A condição de dupla-tarefa demonstrou um aumento na duração do duplo apoio em ambos os grupos. A imagética motora evidenciou diferenças estatisticamente significativas entre a caminhada real e a imaginada nos pacientes com EM. Pacientes com EM sem incapacidade não apresentaram influência da atenção dividida na execução da caminhada. Entretanto, o planejamento motor esteve superestimado.

comprometimento cognitivo leve; esclerose múltipla; caminhada


Difficulty experienced while walking is the most visible sign of the functional impairments caused by multiple sclerosis (MS)11. Iezzoni LI, Rao SR, Kinkel RP. Patterns of mobility aid use among working-age persons with multiple sclerosis living in the community in the United States. Disabil Health J 2009;2:67-76.. Only 21% of the MS patients estimate their walking ability correctly22. Ringel I, Zettl UK. Estimates of the walking distance in multiple sclerosis patients and their effect on the EDSS. J Neurol 2006;253:666-667.. The need to concentrate on walking, caused by MS, was the most common problem found in a study involving 703 patients11. Iezzoni LI, Rao SR, Kinkel RP. Patterns of mobility aid use among working-age persons with multiple sclerosis living in the community in the United States. Disabil Health J 2009;2:67-76.. Walking is a complex sensorimotor task, requiring a dynamic interaction between the spinal locomotor pattern generators and the hierarchically organised supraspinal locomotion centres in the brainstem, cerebellum and forebrain33. Rossignol S, Dubuc R, Gossard JP. Dynamic sensorimotor interactions in locomotion. Physiol Rev 2006;86:89-154.. Walking has traditionally been considered as an automatic or reflex-controlled task; however, recent studies have suggested that there are significant attention requirements for postural and balance control44. Woollacott M, Shumway-Cook A. Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture 2002;16:1-14.. Epidemiological, cognitive and neuroimaging studies suggest that walking is influenced by higher order and cortical control mechanisms55. Holtzer R, Mahoney JR, Izzetoglu M, et al. fNIRS study of walking and walking while talking in young and old individuals. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2011;66:879-887..

Cognitive deficits can lead to gait instability and an increase in gait variability. Cognitive impairment in MS ranges from 40% to 65%, and cognitive dysfunction has been consistently demonstrated even in patients with clinically isolated syndromes, with early-stage disease and with low disability levels66. Amato MP, Zipoli V, Portaccio E. Multiple sclerosis-related cognitive changes: a review of cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. J Neurol Sci 2006;245:41-46.. MS pathophysiology involves spinal and supraspinal white matter lesions. Both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies have found that white matter changes are associated with gait disturbance and falls77. Xiong YY, Mok V. Age-related white matter changes. J Aging Res 2011;2011:617927..

MS patients have both motor and cognitive impairments, making them vulnerable to dual-tasking. Dual-tasks require an individual to have the ability to simultaneously perform two tasks. When two tasks are simultaneously performed, more than the total capacity of the individual is required and the performance of either or both tasks can deteriorate44. Woollacott M, Shumway-Cook A. Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture 2002;16:1-14.. Studies that have analysed attention and gait have used the dual-task paradigm, using gait as a primary task and a simultaneous secondary cognitive task. Gait analysis conducted while dual-tasking was usually based on visual observation in most prior studies, reporting various dual-task-related gait changes such as increase in walking time, number of steps and mediolateral deviation88. Beauchet O, Allali G, Annweiler C, et al. Does change in gait while counting backward predict the occurrence of a first fall in older adults? Gerontology 2008;54:217-223..

Cognitive aspects of the neural control of action and motor planning in the absence of sensorimotor feedback has been widely studied by motor imagery (MI)99. Bakker M, de Lange FP, Stevens JA, Toni I, Bloem BR. Motor imagery of gait: a quantitative approach. Exp Brain Res 2007;179:497-504.. MI can be defined as a dynamic state during which an individual mentally simulates a given action without any motor output1010. Decety J. The neurophysiological basis of motor imagery. Behav Brain Res 1996;77:45-52.. Brain activation during locomotion-imagined movement has the same pattern but with a lower amplitude as compared with active movement1111. Jahn K, Deutschlander A, Stephan T, Strupp M, Wiesmann M, Brandt T. Brain activation patterns during imagined stance and locomotion in functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 2004;22:1722-1731.. Clinically, mental chronometry can be reliably used for the screening of patients capable of MI or for measuring the temporal congruency between real and imagined movements1212. Malouin F, Richards CL, Durand A, Doyon J. Reliability of mental chronometry for assessing motor imagery ability after stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2008;89:311-319..

Early stages of MS show compensatory cortical activations, mainly located in regions involved in executive processing1313. Audoin B, Ibarrola D, Ranjeva JP, et al. Compensatory cortical activation observed by fMRI during a cognitive task at the earliest stage of MS. Hum Brain Mapp 2003;20:51-58.. The aim of this study was to analyse the cognitive influence on walking in patients with MS, in the absence of clinical disability. There is a lack of studies assessing the effect of dual-task on MS patients. To the best of our knowledge, in previously published literature, there is no description on the influence of MI or motor planning on walking in MS patients.

METHODS

Subjects

A case-control study was conducted with two groups of subjects (MS patients and control patients). Data were collected at the Physical Therapy clinic of Gaffrée and the Guinle University Hospital (GGUH), Rio de Janeiro, Brazil. Twelve subjects diagnosed with MS (average age: 30.6 years, average height: 168 cm, average weight: 67.17 kg, gender: 9 females and 3 males and average International Physical Activity Questionnaire score: 3221.75) and 12 healthy control subjects (average age: 33.2 years, average height: 169 cm, average weight: 68.17 kg, gender: 9 females and 3 males and average International Physical Activity Questionnaire score: 3874.62) participated in this study. No statistically significant differences were found between the study groups. The patients were recruited from the outpatient clinic of GGUH, and a control group of healthy subjects was recruited from the staff and student community of the physical therapy department. The inclusion criteria consisted of a diagnosis of MS according to the criteria established by McDonald et al.1414. McDonald WI, Compston A, Edan G, et al. Recommended diagnostic criteria for multiple sclerosis: guidelines from the International Panel on the diagnosis of multiple sclerosis. Ann Neurol 2001;50:121-127. and no disability on the Expanded Disability Status Scale (EDSS<1.5). The exclusion criteria included patients with other forms of idiopathic demyelinating disease, patients currently undergoing an MS attack and patients with another associated neurological disease. The control subjects were healthy adults without a clinical diagnosis of MS and no reports of neurological impairments. Subjects from the MS group were matched with subjects from the control group, based primarily on gender and age, followed by an agreement between the subject pairs in terms of height, weight and physical activity level. The study was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committees of GGUH, and all subjects provided informed consent prior to their participation in the study.

Procedures

After providing informed consent, the subjects underwent a clinical neurological evaluation to determine their disability level on the EDSS. The subjects were then referred for completion of the gait clinical trials and 3D-kinematic analysis. Gait was assessed by two tests: 10-m timed walk-test (10m-TWT) and 3D-kinematic analysis. The use of video analysis software is an efficient approach to improve the reliability of visual video assessments1515. Borel S, Schneider P, Newman CJ. Video analysis software increases the interrater reliability of video gait assessments in children with cerebral palsy. Gait Posture 2011;33:727-729.. Initially, participants were instructed to walk at a comfortable speed along a 14-m walkway, then, the subjects were similarly instructed to perform 10m-TWT while executing an arithmetic task; motor planning was measured by mental chronometry.

Instruments

Disability

A standardised, bedside neurological examination was performed by a neurologist. The scores obtained in this examination allowed neurological impairment and disability to be established using the EDSS. Clinicians determined a patient's EDSS level by first assigning a separate grade for the eight functional systems, including pyramidal, cerebellar, bowel and bladder, cerebral, brain stem, sensory, visual and other functions. A composite of grades was then used to determine an individual's EDSS score, ranging from 0 (normal neurologic exam) to 10.0 (death due to MS)1616. Kurtzke JF. Rating neurologic impairment in multiple sclerosis: an expanded disability status scale (EDSS). Neurology 1983;33:1444-1452.. The EDSS is the most widely used scale for MS disability and is a commonly used rating system for evaluating the degree of neurological impairment in MS based on neurological findings.

Gait clinical trial

Participants were instructed to walk barefoot at a self-selected, comfortable speed along a 14-m walkway. A 'dynamic start' was used, where the subject may accelerate 2 m before entering the timed 10 m distance and decelerate 2 m afterwards. As long as the subjects were capable of ambulating the required 14 m, they were allowed to participate in the test. Timing was marked when the lead foot crossed the starting line and was stopped when the lead foot crossed the finish line. Speed was only calculated for the 10 m distance between the starting and the finish line, to avoid measuring the acceleration and deceleration phases of gait. The second walking trial was recorded to minimise the learning effect. The walking time and the number of steps were registered. The gait speed, cadence, step and stride length were then estimated. The 10m-TWT is a valid and reliable measure for patients with neurological impairment1717. Rossier P, Wade DT. Validity and reliability comparison of 4 mobility measures in patients presenting with neurologic impairment. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2001;82:9-13..

3D-kinematic analysis

Video analyses were simultaneously performed in the same environment. A three-dimensional analysis was performed with four video camera recordings (Kodak Zi10 sampled at 60 frames per second). Data were collected across the central 4 m of the walkway to exclude the acceleration and deceleration phases of each trial. To evaluate the hip, knee and ankle kinematics during gait, 15 adhesive markers were attached to the subjects to define the thigh, shank and foot segments, according to the Helen Hayes protocol previously described for gait analysis1818. Vaughan C, Davis B, O'Connor J. Dynamics of human gait. 2nd ed. Cape Town: Kiboho Publishers,1999:p23-30.. The same examiner placed the markers. The Human software (HMA Technology) was used for video analysis. For three-dimensional analysis, the software accepts two-dimensional source digitised data and uses a direct linear transformation to produce the 3D coordinate file. A gait cycle was digitised and synchronised for each subject. The 3D-kinematic analysis was used to objectively measure the cadence (steps per minute), swing phase (% gait cycle), stance phase (% gait cycle), double support duration (% gait cycle) and step width (cm). The kinematic method is the most accurate method to measure the temporal-spatial gait characteristics.

Dual-task

Subjects were similarly instructed to perform the 10m-TWT while executing an arithmetic task, namely, counting aloud backwards from 100, subtracting by 3, to manipulate the attention demands of subjects during a motor task. One investigator walked beside the patients adjacent to the walkway to provide support if a loss of balance occurred. Gait speed and cadence were measured by 10m-TWT.

Motor planning

Motor planning was measured by mental chronometry. This strategy is based on the observation that the duration of mentally simulated and executed motor tasks are comparable. Thus, knowing the time length of the physical act, the investigator asked the patient to signal the beginning and termination of the imagined movement. A comparable time period of the imagery and physical performance of the task is considered to be an evidence of the engagement in MI practice of the required task. Subjects were instructed to imagine themselves (first-person perspective) walking along the walkway, then, kinaesthetic MI was used. Bakker et al.99. Bakker M, de Lange FP, Stevens JA, Toni I, Bloem BR. Motor imagery of gait: a quantitative approach. Exp Brain Res 2007;179:497-504. showed that kinaesthetic MI has higher correspondence with gait execution than visual MI. The motor planning results were obtained from the walking imagination time along the 10 m distance. The average walking speed in the physical act and the imagined movement (MI) were compared to analyse the motor planning congruence.

Data analysis

Normal probability plots were inspected for each variable. The data distribution of each variable was verified through the Shapiro-Wilk test. Comparison between the groups was performed using the paired and non-paired Student's t-test or the Mann-Whitney U test. The χ22. Ringel I, Zettl UK. Estimates of the walking distance in multiple sclerosis patients and their effect on the EDSS. J Neurol 2006;253:666-667. test was used to analyse categorical variables. Significance level was established at 5% (p<0.05). All data were analysed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, Illinois, US) version 17.0 software.

RESULTS

Twenty-four subjects participated in the study: 12 MS patients with no disability and 12 matched healthy controls. Most of the participants were young adults with female predominance and normal body fat. There were no differences in the falls history and education level between the groups. Patient demographic data are shown in Table 1.

Table 1
Demographic data of the multiple sclerosis patients and healthy control group.

The scores of walking speed and cadence showed no statistically significant differences between the groups in comfortable walking, dual-task condition and MI. The dual-task condition showed an increase in double support duration in both groups. However, step width did not show any significant differences between comfortable walking and the dual-task condition in both groups. The temporal-spatial walking values are described in Table 2. The comparison of double support duration in MS patients and healthy controls in normal walking and dual-task conditions is presented in Figure 1.

Table 2
Comparison of temporal-spatial characteristics in multiple sclerosis patients and healthy control patients in comfortable walking and dual-task conditions.

Fig 1
Comparison of double support duration in multiple sclerosis patients and healthy controls in normal walking and dual-task conditions.

MI analysis showed statistically significant differences between real and imagined walking in MS patients (MI=1.90 m/s, SD±0.93 vs. normal walking=1.27 m/s, SD±0.24; p=0.02), while healthy controls showed a tendency to higher the walking speed in imagined walking, but this result was not statistically significant (MI=1.55 m/s, SD±0.52 vs. normal walking=1.21 m/s, SD±0.11; p=0.05). MS patients overestimated walking speed by 33%, while matched healthy controls overestimated walking speed by 22%. Figure 2 shows the comparison in walking speed in MS patients and healthy controls in normal walking and MI conditions.


DISCUSSION

MS patients with no clinical disability did not show any influence of divided attention on gait execution; however, motor planning was overestimated compared to real walking. A matched healthy control group did not show overestimation of motor planning in the same task. MI-based exercises, as a ther apy tool, have been used for Parkinson's disease and post-stroke patients, with good results. However, for MS patients, it is important to con sider their ability to generate correct motor images. Cognitive impairments are highly prevalent in MS patients, even in early stages, and in patients with mild disability measured by the EDSS66. Amato MP, Zipoli V, Portaccio E. Multiple sclerosis-related cognitive changes: a review of cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. J Neurol Sci 2006;245:41-46..

The dual-task condition did not show any differences in walking speed, cadence and step width. However, double support duration significantly increased. A previous study showed that cadence does not differ with dual-task condition's clinically isolated syndromes, suggestive of MS1919. Kalron A, Dvir Z, Achiron A. Walking while talking-difficulties incurred during the initial stages of multiple sclerosis disease process. Gait Posture 2010;32:332-335.. The increase in double support duration was a compensatory strategy to maintain walking stability. Hamilton et al.2020. Hamilton F, Rochester L, Paul L, Rafferty D, O'Leary CP, Evans JJ. Walking and talking: an investigation of cognitive-motor dual tasking in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 2009;15:1215-1227. showed greater decrements in performance under dual-task conditions in cognitive task performance, walking speed and swing-time variability in MS patients. Kalron et al.1919. Kalron A, Dvir Z, Achiron A. Walking while talking-difficulties incurred during the initial stages of multiple sclerosis disease process. Gait Posture 2010;32:332-335. also showed that combined walking and cognitive tasks were expressed in prolonged double support duration, as shown by the present results. They also described a reduction in gait speed in the early stages of the disease. Our results showed an increase of 13% in double support duration in dual-task conditions, while Kalron et al.1919. Kalron A, Dvir Z, Achiron A. Walking while talking-difficulties incurred during the initial stages of multiple sclerosis disease process. Gait Posture 2010;32:332-335. showed an increase of 8% in double support duration. Despite the similarity of results, the differences found in both studies may be related to the methods used as Kalron et al.1919. Kalron A, Dvir Z, Achiron A. Walking while talking-difficulties incurred during the initial stages of multiple sclerosis disease process. Gait Posture 2010;32:332-335. used the Word List Generation Test as the executive function, while we used an arithmetic task. Stoquart-Elsankari et al.2121. Stoquart-Elsankari S, Bottin C, Roussel-Pieronne M, Godefroy O. Motor and cognitive slowing in multiple sclerosis: an attentional deficit? Clin Neurol Neurosurg 2010;112:226-232. showed that the action slowing of an MS patient was mainly related to the attention deficit, even in patients without motor deficit on clinical examinations, where divided attention and decisional processes were preserved. MS patients showed an interference of cognitive task on motor execution although the severity of clinical disability with more impairment, as compared with healthy subjects, still needs to be clarified.

The double support duration was increased in patients and matched healthy controls during the dual-task condition. Slower walking speed55. Holtzer R, Mahoney JR, Izzetoglu M, et al. fNIRS study of walking and walking while talking in young and old individuals. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2011;66:879-887.,2222. Doi T, Asai T, Hirata S, Ando H. Dual-task costs for whole trunk movement during gait. Gait Posture 2010;33:712-714. and an increase in stride-time variability are common findings, even in healthy adults performing dual-tasks2222. Doi T, Asai T, Hirata S, Ando H. Dual-task costs for whole trunk movement during gait. Gait Posture 2010;33:712-714.. Walking speed is the most common finding described in studies with dual-task condition and it is usually slower in many populations. The results of the present study reflect the increase of walking stability under cognitive demands. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study, with different types of dual-tasks, revealed that cortical areas along the inferior frontal sulcus, middle frontal gyrus and intraparietal sulcus are involved in dual-task performance, which highlights the role of cognitive function and the frontal lobe on mobility2323. Szameitat AJ, Schubert T, Muller K, Von Cramon DY. Localization of executive functions in dual-task performance with fMRI. J Cogn Neurosci 2002;14:1184-1199.. The question that should be asked is at which stage of the MS disease process do dual-task conditions affect gait performance; specifically, which gait parameters are affected and to what extent.

MS patients with no disability showed a reduction in motor planning accuracy compared with matched healthy controls. A recent study2424. Heremans E, D'Hooge AM, De Bondt S, Helsen W, Feys P. The relation between cognitive and motor dysfunction and motor imagery ability in patients with multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 2012;18:1303-1309., which was the first research that investigated MI in MS patients, focused on upper limb move ment capacity and described significant differences in temporal organisation. Heremans et al.2424. Heremans E, D'Hooge AM, De Bondt S, Helsen W, Feys P. The relation between cognitive and motor dysfunction and motor imagery ability in patients with multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 2012;18:1303-1309. used mental chronometry during the Box and Block Test in moderate disability (average EDSS=6.5) in MS patients. The present study showed that MS patients overestimated the imagined movement by 33%, while Heremans et al.2424. Heremans E, D'Hooge AM, De Bondt S, Helsen W, Feys P. The relation between cognitive and motor dysfunction and motor imagery ability in patients with multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 2012;18:1303-1309. showed an increase of 14% in the upper limb most affected side. The temporal invariance between executed and imagined movements, which was well documented in young adults, suggests that similar motor representations are shared between covert and overt stages of actions2525. Personnier P, Ballay Y, Papaxanthis C. Mentally represented motor actions in normal aging: III. Electromyographic features of imagined arm movements. Behav Brain Res 2010;206:184-191.. Young adults had their MI ability preserved irrespective of the width of the path, while the elderly group significantly overestimated the duration of imagined movements with respect to the executed movements2626. Personnier P, Kubicki A, Laroche D, Papaxanthis C. Temporal features of imagined locomotion in normal aging. Neurosci Lett 2010;476:146-149.. MI accuracy was significantly deteriorated in elderly adults; this could be attributed to functional changes in the brain that occur with ageing, influencing cognitive and motor abilities2525. Personnier P, Ballay Y, Papaxanthis C. Mentally represented motor actions in normal aging: III. Electromyographic features of imagined arm movements. Behav Brain Res 2010;206:184-191.. The temporal congruence of real and imagined movements in post-stroke patients remains similar to that of age-matched controls2727. Malouin F, Richards CL, Durand A, Doyon J. Clinical assessment of motor imagery after stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2008;22:330-340..

Neuroimaging studies have been described to have remarkable similarities between the real and imagined locomotion network. The major activated areas were the motor/premotor and multisensory cortices, parahippocampal gyri and midline cerebellum. There were deactivations in multisensory vestibular cortical areas in both conditions2828. la Fougere C, Zwergal A, Rominger A, et al. Real versus imagined locomotion: a [18F]-FDG PET-fMRI comparison. Neuroimage 2010;50:1589-1598.. In MS patients, it has been shown that an ipsilateral sensorimotor cortex deactivation with a simple motor task2929. Manson SC, Wegner C, Filippi M, et al. Impairment of movement-associated brain deactivation in multiple sclerosis: further evidence for a functional pathology of interhemispheric neuronal inhibition. Exp Brain Res 2008;187:25-31. and an existence of compensatory cortical activations at the earliest stage of MS were mainly located in regions involved in executive processing1313. Audoin B, Ibarrola D, Ranjeva JP, et al. Compensatory cortical activation observed by fMRI during a cognitive task at the earliest stage of MS. Hum Brain Mapp 2003;20:51-58.,3030. Au Duong MV, Audoin B, Boulanouar K, et al. Altered functional connectivity related to white matter changes inside the working memory network at the very early stage of MS. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 2005;25:1245-1253..

The sample size was a potential limitation of this study. Therefore, the possibility of making generalisations from our findings may still be fairly limited. The second possible limitation is that we did not find any gait research in MS patients with no disability (EDSS< 1.5) that analyses the influence of motor planning with which to compare our data. Despite these limitations, the originality of the research theme and intriguing results provide an impetus for future research. Studies with larger sample sizes that include participants with greater disability from the present study and different levels of cognitive impairments should be undertaken to confirm the present results.

In conclusion, MS patients with no disability did not show any influence of divided attention on walking execution. However, MI time was overestimated compared to real walking, revealing a motor planning impairment. A matched healthy control group did not show overestimation of motor planning to the same task.

References

  • 1
    Iezzoni LI, Rao SR, Kinkel RP. Patterns of mobility aid use among working-age persons with multiple sclerosis living in the community in the United States. Disabil Health J 2009;2:67-76.
  • 2
    Ringel I, Zettl UK. Estimates of the walking distance in multiple sclerosis patients and their effect on the EDSS. J Neurol 2006;253:666-667.
  • 3
    Rossignol S, Dubuc R, Gossard JP. Dynamic sensorimotor interactions in locomotion. Physiol Rev 2006;86:89-154.
  • 4
    Woollacott M, Shumway-Cook A. Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture 2002;16:1-14.
  • 5
    Holtzer R, Mahoney JR, Izzetoglu M, et al. fNIRS study of walking and walking while talking in young and old individuals. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2011;66:879-887.
  • 6
    Amato MP, Zipoli V, Portaccio E. Multiple sclerosis-related cognitive changes: a review of cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. J Neurol Sci 2006;245:41-46.
  • 7
    Xiong YY, Mok V. Age-related white matter changes. J Aging Res 2011;2011:617927.
  • 8
    Beauchet O, Allali G, Annweiler C, et al. Does change in gait while counting backward predict the occurrence of a first fall in older adults? Gerontology 2008;54:217-223.
  • 9
    Bakker M, de Lange FP, Stevens JA, Toni I, Bloem BR. Motor imagery of gait: a quantitative approach. Exp Brain Res 2007;179:497-504.
  • 10
    Decety J. The neurophysiological basis of motor imagery. Behav Brain Res 1996;77:45-52.
  • 11
    Jahn K, Deutschlander A, Stephan T, Strupp M, Wiesmann M, Brandt T. Brain activation patterns during imagined stance and locomotion in functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 2004;22:1722-1731.
  • 12
    Malouin F, Richards CL, Durand A, Doyon J. Reliability of mental chronometry for assessing motor imagery ability after stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2008;89:311-319.
  • 13
    Audoin B, Ibarrola D, Ranjeva JP, et al. Compensatory cortical activation observed by fMRI during a cognitive task at the earliest stage of MS. Hum Brain Mapp 2003;20:51-58.
  • 14
    McDonald WI, Compston A, Edan G, et al. Recommended diagnostic criteria for multiple sclerosis: guidelines from the International Panel on the diagnosis of multiple sclerosis. Ann Neurol 2001;50:121-127.
  • 15
    Borel S, Schneider P, Newman CJ. Video analysis software increases the interrater reliability of video gait assessments in children with cerebral palsy. Gait Posture 2011;33:727-729.
  • 16
    Kurtzke JF. Rating neurologic impairment in multiple sclerosis: an expanded disability status scale (EDSS). Neurology 1983;33:1444-1452.
  • 17
    Rossier P, Wade DT. Validity and reliability comparison of 4 mobility measures in patients presenting with neurologic impairment. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2001;82:9-13.
  • 18
    Vaughan C, Davis B, O'Connor J. Dynamics of human gait. 2nd ed. Cape Town: Kiboho Publishers,1999:p23-30.
  • 19
    Kalron A, Dvir Z, Achiron A. Walking while talking-difficulties incurred during the initial stages of multiple sclerosis disease process. Gait Posture 2010;32:332-335.
  • 20
    Hamilton F, Rochester L, Paul L, Rafferty D, O'Leary CP, Evans JJ. Walking and talking: an investigation of cognitive-motor dual tasking in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 2009;15:1215-1227.
  • 21
    Stoquart-Elsankari S, Bottin C, Roussel-Pieronne M, Godefroy O. Motor and cognitive slowing in multiple sclerosis: an attentional deficit? Clin Neurol Neurosurg 2010;112:226-232.
  • 22
    Doi T, Asai T, Hirata S, Ando H. Dual-task costs for whole trunk movement during gait. Gait Posture 2010;33:712-714.
  • 23
    Szameitat AJ, Schubert T, Muller K, Von Cramon DY. Localization of executive functions in dual-task performance with fMRI. J Cogn Neurosci 2002;14:1184-1199.
  • 24
    Heremans E, D'Hooge AM, De Bondt S, Helsen W, Feys P. The relation between cognitive and motor dysfunction and motor imagery ability in patients with multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 2012;18:1303-1309.
  • 25
    Personnier P, Ballay Y, Papaxanthis C. Mentally represented motor actions in normal aging: III. Electromyographic features of imagined arm movements. Behav Brain Res 2010;206:184-191.
  • 26
    Personnier P, Kubicki A, Laroche D, Papaxanthis C. Temporal features of imagined locomotion in normal aging. Neurosci Lett 2010;476:146-149.
  • 27
    Malouin F, Richards CL, Durand A, Doyon J. Clinical assessment of motor imagery after stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2008;22:330-340.
  • 28
    la Fougere C, Zwergal A, Rominger A, et al. Real versus imagined locomotion: a [18F]-FDG PET-fMRI comparison. Neuroimage 2010;50:1589-1598.
  • 29
    Manson SC, Wegner C, Filippi M, et al. Impairment of movement-associated brain deactivation in multiple sclerosis: further evidence for a functional pathology of interhemispheric neuronal inhibition. Exp Brain Res 2008;187:25-31.
  • 30
    Au Duong MV, Audoin B, Boulanouar K, et al. Altered functional connectivity related to white matter changes inside the working memory network at the very early stage of MS. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 2005;25:1245-1253.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Aug 2013

History

  • Received
    14 Aug 2012
  • Reviewed
    20 Mar 2013
  • Accepted
    27 Mar 2013
Academia Brasileira de Neurologia - ABNEURO R. Vergueiro, 1353 sl.1404 - Ed. Top Towers Offices Torre Norte, 04101-000 São Paulo SP Brazil, Tel.: +55 11 5084-9463 | +55 11 5083-3876 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista.arquivos@abneuro.org