Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

AUDIENCE PERCEPTIONS OF NEWS MEDIA ON THE COVID-19 PANDEMIC: relations among media credibility, subjective well-being and fear of missing out

PERCEPÇÕES DA AUDIÊNCIA SOBRE A MÍDIA NA PANDEMIA DE COVID-19: relações entre credibilidade da mídia, bem-estar subjetivo e fear of missing out

PERCEPCIONES DE LA AUDIENCIA SOBRE LOS MEDIOS EN LA PANDEMIA DEL COVID-19: relaciones de la credibilidad mediática, el bienestar subjetivo y el fear of missing out

ABSTRACT

This article pairs journalism studies and social psychology to investigate, with a quantitative method, audience perceptions of news media in the initial stages of the Covid-19 pandemic in Brazil by verifying the relations among media credibility, subjective well-being (SWB), and fear of missing out (FoMO). All told, 306 Brazilians took part in this study, answering a questionnaire to elicit demographic data, perceptions of media credibility, and behavioral characteristics. The findings revealed high averages of measures of news media credibility, with over 70% of respondents evaluating the work of the press as excellent or good. People with higher averages of negative affects tended to perceive the news media as more reliable. Furthermore, individuals who reported fear of missing out on the news during Covid-19 experienced more negative affect, and attributed greater credibility to news media. These findings show that in a time of fear and uncertainty, citizens seem to trust solid institutions more, accepting their reports less critically. We also found that unknown risks can attract attention more than regular events, tending to keep vigilance on specific news. Future studies can add additional measures of FoMO and use more diverse samples in different contexts.

Key words
Journalism; Audience perception; Media credibility; Subjective well-being; FoMO; Covid-19

RESUMO

Este artigo aproxima os estudos de jornalismo e a psicologia social para investigar, com um método quantitativo, as percepções da audiência sobre a mídia noticiosa nos estágios iniciais da pandemia Covid-19 no Brasil, verificando as relações entre credibilidade da mídia, bem-estar subjetivo (BES) e fear of missing out (FoMO). Participaram deste estudo 306 brasileiros, que responderam um questionário demográfico, uma escala de credibilidade da mídia e questões sobre características comportamentais. Os resultados revelam que as médias das medidas de credibilidade da mídia são altas, com mais de 70% dos entrevistados avaliando o trabalho da imprensa como excelente ou bom. Pessoas com médias mais altas de afetos negativos tendem a perceber a mídia como mais confiável. Além disso, os indivíduos que relatam medo de perder notícias durante a Covid-19 experienciam mais afetos negativos e também dão mais credibilidade para a mídia. Argumenta-se que, em uma época de medo e incerteza, os cidadãos parecem confiar mais em instituições sólidas, aceitando seus relatos de forma menos crítica. Também é discutido que eventos com riscos desconhecidos podem atrair mais atenção do que acontecimentos regulares e isso mantém a vigilância sobre notícias específicas. Estudos futuros podem adicionar medidas adicionais de FoMO e usar amostras mais diversas em diferentes contextos.

Palavras-chave
Jornalismo; Percepção da audiência; Credibilidade da mídia; Bem-estar subjetivo; FoMO; Covid-19

RESUMEN

Este artículo aborda los estudios de periodismo y psicología social para investigar, con un método cuantitativo, las percepciones de la audiencia sobre los medios de comunicación en las primeras etapas de la pandemia de Covid-19 en Brasil, verificando las relaciones entre la credibilidad de los medios, el bienestar subjetivo (BES) y el fear of missing out (FoMO). En este estudio participaron 306 brasileños, quienes respondieron un cuestionario demográfico, una escala de credibilidad mediática y preguntas sobre características de comportamiento. Los resultados revelan que los promedios de las medidas de credibilidad de los medios son altos, con más del 70% de los encuestados calificando el trabajo de prensa como excelente o bueno. Las personas con promedios más altos de afecto negativo tienden a percibir los medios como más confiables. Además, las personas que reportan miedo a perderse noticias durante el Covid-19 experimentan más efectos negativos y también dan más credibilidad a los medios. Se argumenta que, en una época de miedo e incertidumbre, los ciudadanos parecen confiar en instituciones más sólidas, aceptando sus cuentas de manera menos crítica. También se discute que los eventos con riesgos desconocidos pueden atraer más atención que los eventos regulares y esto mantiene al tanto de noticias específicas. Los estudios futuros pueden agregar medidas adicionales de FoMO y utilizar muestras más diversas en diferentes contextos.

Palabras clave
Periodismo; Percepción de la audienciaa; Credibilidad de los medios; Bienestar subjetivo (BES); FoMO; Covid-19

1 Introduction

An unprecedented event in modern world history, officially declared as a pandemic by the World Health Organization (WHO) in March 2020, coronavirus disease (Covid-19) has killed millions of people around the globe and changed many aspects of life1 1 In Brazil, from 3 January 2020 to 9 February 2022, there were 26.599.593 confirmed cases of Covid-19, with 632.621 deaths. Retrieved from https://Covid19.who.int/region/amro/country/br . The disease has been detected in more than 200 countries, with the US and Brazil experiencing the most widespread outbreaks outside of China. In order to avoid coronavirus spread, citizens were put under lockdown, with governments shutting down schools, bars, restaurants, and other non-essential service venues (Reynolds & Weiss, 2020Reynolds, M., & Weiss, S. (2020, May 27). How Coronavirus Started and What Happens Next, Explained. Wired. Retrieved from www.wired.co.uk/article/china-coronavirus
www.wired.co.uk/article/china-coronaviru...
).

Keeping informed about this crisis is almost imperative for survival, so much so that in the first months of 2020 several news media outlets saw a spike in website pageviews2 2 Brazilian online newspapers and TV channels also reported increased online audiences during February and March 2020. Retrieved from www1.folha.uol.com.br/ilustrada/2020/03/audiencia-de-telejor nalismo-explode-durante-crise-do-novo-coronavirus.shtml (Tameez, 2020Tameez, H. (2020, March 18). At Least Coronavirus Has Been Good For Online News Traffic (We’re Trying To Be Optimistic). Nieman Lab. Retrieved from www.niemanlab.org/2020/03/at-least-coronavirus-has-been-good-for-online-news-traffic-were-trying-to-be-optimistic/
www.niemanlab.org/2020/03/at-least-coron...
). In this context, news consumption during the Covid-19 pandemic has been a prominent object of study. Van Aelst et al. (2021)Van Aelst, P., Toth, F., Castro, L., Štětka, V., Vreese, C. de, Aalberg, T., Cardenal, A. S., Corbu, N., Esser, F., Hopmann, D. N., KocMichalska, K., Matthes, J., Schemer, C., Sheafer, T., Splendore, S., Stanyer, J., Sępińska, A., Strömböck, J., & Theocharis, Y. (2021). Does a Crisis Change News Habits? A Comparative Study of the Effects of COVID-19 on News Media Use in 17 European Countries. Digital Journalism, 9(9), 1–31. DOI: 10.1080/21670811.2021.1943481
https://doi.org/10.1080/21670811.2021.19...
found an overall rise in news use across 17 European countries, especially an increase in TV news consumption, and a higher reliance on social media and the internet for news and information. Investigating multi-platform news consumption in association with civic participation during the Covid-19 pandemic in Belgium, Waeterloos et al. (2021)Waeterloos, C., Walrave M., Ponnet, K. (2021). The Role of Multi-Platform News Consumption in Explaining Civic Participation during the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Communication Mediation Approach. New Media & Society, online first. DOI: 10.1177/14614448211058701
https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444821105870...
revealed that it stimulates two different types of participation, through civic talk with strong ties and civic attitudes.

But do a larger audience and more interest in news mean an elevated level of trust in the media? Using survey data collected in late March and early April 2020 in six countries, Nielsen et al. (2020)Nielsen, R. K., Fletcher, R., Newman, N., Brennen, J. S., & Howard, P. N. (2020, April 15). Navigating the ‘Infodemic’: How People in Six Countries Access and Rate News and Information about Coronavirus. Digital News Report. Retrieved from https://reutersinstitute.politics.ox.ac.uk/infodemic-how-people-six-countries-access-and-rate-news-and-information-about-coronavirus
https://reutersinstitute.politics.ox.ac....
found that news organizations were the most widely used source of information about Covid-19 in almost every country, and they were rated as relatively trustworthy by the majority of respondents. However, those with low levels of formal education were much less likely to say that they rely on news organizations. Trust and news consumption can also pose critical barriers to health literacy and foster negative prejudicial responses that further undermine public health efforts surrounding the Covid-19 pandemic (Dhanani & Franz, 2020Dhanani, LY, & Franz, B (2020). The Role of News Consumption and Trust in Public Health Leadership in Shaping COVID-19 Knowledge and Prejudice. Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 11, 1–13. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.560828
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.56082...
).

The unprecedented nature of this pandemic warrants a study about how people are connecting to news media and how this access impacts general life. In this vein, this study examines how people of the largest country in South America perceived the media in the early days of the Covid-19 pandemic in the country.

The way individuals interact with the media, considering confidence or skepticism, has the potential to generate emotions and influence states of mind, health, and happiness, and can affect emotional states and feelings of well-being (Bernardino et al., 2016Bernardino, C., Ferreira, H. A., & Chambel, T. (2016). Towards Media for Wellbeing. Proceedings of the Association for Computing Machinery International Conference on Interactive Experiences for TV and Online Video (171–177). NY, USA.). Studies have shown, for example, that the use of social media has a significant influence on well-being. Individuals who spend more time using social media suffer from more negative emotions and feelings (Baker & Algorta, 2016Baker, D. A., & Algorta, G. P. (2016). The Relationship Between Online Social Networking and Depression: A Systematic Review of Quantitative Studies. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 19(11), 638–648. DOI:10.1089/cyber.2016.0206); have less satisfaction with life (Elhai et al., 2016Elhai, J. D., Levine, J. C., Dvorak, R. D., & Hall, B. J. (2016). Fear of Missing Out, Need for Touch, Anxiety and Depression are Related to Problematic Smartphone Use. Computers in Human Behavior, vol. 63, 509-516. DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.079
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.07...
); experience more emotional tensions (James et al., 2017James, T. L., Lowry, P. B., Wallace, L., & Warkentin, M. (2017). The Effect of Belongingness on Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder in the Use of Online Social Networks. Journal of Management Information Systems, 34(2), 560–596. DOI: 10.1080/07421222.2017.1334496
https://doi.org/10.1080/07421222.2017.13...
; Lai et al., 2016Lai, C., Altavilla, D., Ronconi, A., & Aceto, P. (2016). Fear of Missing Out (FOMO) Is Associated with Activation of The Right Middle Temporal Gyrus During Inclusion Social Cue. Computers in Human Behavior, vol. 61, 516–521. DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.072
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.07...
); and suffer deterioration of physical and mental well-being (Alt, 2018Alt, D., & Boniel-Nissim, M. (2018). Links between Adolescents’ Deep and Surface Learning Approaches, Problematic Internet Use, and Fear of Missing Out (FoMO). Internet Interventions, vol. 13, 30–39. DOI: 10.1016/j.invent.2018.05.002
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.invent.2018.05...
). Drawing on a dataset of U.S. residents in mid-to-late March 2020, Stainback et al. (2020)Stainback, K., Hearne, B. N., & Trieu, M. M. (2020). COVID-19 and the 24/7 News Cycle: Does COVID-19 News Exposure Affect Mental Health? Socius, vol. 6, 1–15. DOI: 10.1177/2378023120969339
https://doi.org/10.1177/2378023120969339...
found that greater Covid-19 media consumption was associated with greater psychological distress. Also, regarding the feeling of well-being, Jain (2021)Jain, P. (2021). The COVID-19 Pandemic and Positive Psychology: The Role of News and Trust in News on Mental Health and Well-Being. Journal of Health Communication, 26(5), 317–327. DOI: 10.1080/10810730.2021.1946219
https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2021.19...
revealed that high levels of news exposure, combined with low levels of interest in Covid-19 news, led people to experience more stress and lower satisfaction, gratitude, and happiness.

Probably for this reason, media users deviate, temporarily or permanently, from the use of social media due to weariness caused by interactions and exposure. Compulsive media access triggers fatigue that can result in elevated anxiety and depression (Dhir et al., 2018Dhir, A., Yossatorn, Y., Kaur, P., & Chen, S. (2018). Online Social Media Fatigue and Psychological Wellbeing – A Study of Compulsive Use, Fear of Missing Out, Fatigue, Anxiety and Depression. International Journal of Information Management, vol. 40, 141–152. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2018.01.012
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2018...
), and the widespread apprehension that others may be having experiences from which someone is absent – the fear of missing out (FoMO) – favors dysfunctional use of media and contributes negatively to the subjective well-being (SWB) index.

Previous research has also investigated FoMO in the Covid-19 pandemic. Yu et al. (2020)Yu, S. C., Chen, H. R., Liu, A. C., & Lee, H. Y. (2020). Toward COVID-19 Information: Infodemic or Fear of Missing Out? Healthcare, 8(4), 1–8. Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute. DOI: 10.3390/healthcare8040550
https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare804055...
compiled the Covid-19 information fear of missing out scale (CIFS) and found that regarding social media usage, CIFS scores were associated with the frequency of active reposts rather than passive browsing. In other words, what causes an impact on Covid-19 FoMO is the active reposting and interpretation of information. Bruin et al. (2021)Bruin, K., Haan, Y., Vliegenthart, R., Kruikemeier, S., & Boukes, M. (2021). News Avoidance during the Covid-19 Crisis: Understanding Information Overload. Digital Journalism, 9(9), 1286–1302. DOI: 10.1080/21670811.2021.1957967
https://doi.org/10.1080/21670811.2021.19...
investigated the degree of news avoidance during the first months of the pandemic in the Netherlands and found that the increased presence of this behavior could be explained by negative emotions and feelings the news caused to citizens. “In a pandemic such as Covid-19, news consumers need to be informed, but avoiding news is sometimes necessary to stay mentally healthy” (Bruin et al., 2021Bruin, K., Haan, Y., Vliegenthart, R., Kruikemeier, S., & Boukes, M. (2021). News Avoidance during the Covid-19 Crisis: Understanding Information Overload. Digital Journalism, 9(9), 1286–1302. DOI: 10.1080/21670811.2021.1957967
https://doi.org/10.1080/21670811.2021.19...
, p. 1).

This article brings together two fields of knowledge – social psychology and journalism – to investigate the audience perception of news media on the Covid-19 pandemic in Brazil. The primary aim is to verify the relations among media credibility, subjective well-being (SWB), and fear of missing out (FoMO).

2 The credibility perceived by the audience

Media trust is a dialectical and live experience. It is not taken for granted; it does not remain forever – trust is dynamic and changes according to circumstances and individuals. To trust in news media is a matter of social orientation and is one of the most important mechanisms in helping people deal with the risk of a complex future (Kohring & Matthes, 2007Kohring, M., & Matthes, J. (2007). Trust in News Media: Development and Validation of a Multidimensional Scale. Communication Research, 34(2), 231–252. DOI : 10.1177/0093650206298071
https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650206298071...
).

From the perspective of communication research, trust is an important variable for media effects (Tsfati, 2003Tsfati, Y. (2003). Does Audience Skepticism of the Media Matter In Agenda Setting? Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 47(2), 157–176. DOI:10.1207/s15506878jobem4702_1.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem47...
) and it supplies information about how individuals perceive and evaluate news media. And here is a challenge: people have been relying less on the media over the years. According to the University of Oxford Digital News Report 2020, only 38% of the 80.000 participants in the sample said they trusted most news most of the time – a fall of four percentage points from 2019. In Brazil, the report highlighted that trust in news overall improved slightly after a significant contraction in 2019 amid an atmosphere of political polarization following the election of far-right President Jair Bolsonaro in 2018 (Newman et al., 2020Newman, N., Fletcher, R., Schulz, A., Andi, S., & Nielsen, R. (2020). Reuters Institute Digital News Report 2020. Reuters Institute: University of Oxford. Retrieved from https://reutersinstitute.politics.ox.ac.uk/sites/default/files/2020-06/DNR_2020_FINAL.pdf
https://reutersinstitute.politics.ox.ac....
).

Conceptually, it should be mentioned that trust in news is different from trust in news media. Citizens, non-professional journalists and other institutions can also produce news. Audience trust in news media is a general attitude toward the media as an institution (Tsfati & Cohen, 2013Tsfati, Y., & Cohen, J. (2013). Perceptions of Media and Media Effects. In R. Hobbs, A. N. Valdivia & P. Mihaiudis (Eds.), The International Encyclopedia of Media Studies (pp. 128–146). New York: Wiley-Blackwell. DOI: 10.1002/9781444361506.wbiems995
https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444361506.wb...
), and is rooted both in the journalistic text and in the individual characteristics and trends of the audience.

This intersection between reliability and credibility can upset concepts and cause the misconception that they are the same. However, there are significant epistemological differences to be considered (Santos & Grossi, 2018Santos, G. S., & Grossi, A. M. (2018). Jornalismo e credibilidade: Uma percepção do público [Journalism and Credibility: A Perception of the Public]. Ámbitos. Revista Internacional de Comunicación, 3(42), 40-54. Retrieved from https://revistascientificas.us.es/index.php/Ambitos/article/view/5415
https://revistascientificas.us.es/index....
). Trust can be understood as a psychological state in which an individual chooses to believe in something or that someone is aware of a vulnerable act since there are no forms of guarantee (Lewicki & Tomlinson, 2003Lewicki, R. J., & Tomlinson, E. C. (2003). Trust and Trust Building. Beyond Intractability. In G. Burgess & H. Burgess (Eds.), Conflict Information Consortium (305–315). University of Colorado, Boulder. Retrieved from www.beyondintractability.org/essay/trust_building
www.beyondintractability.org/essay/trust...
).

When it comes to credibility, however, the literature presents it as a multidimensional construct. Performance, accuracy, responsibility, and role of the news media make credibility a diversified concept, which can be defined as a characteristic anchored by practices and principles, along with consolidated values of journalism. In this sense, there is the credibility constituted by the speaker and the credibility perceived by the listener (Lisboa & Benetti, 2017Lisboa, S., & Benetti, M. (2017). Credibilidade no jornalismo: uma nova abordagem [Credibility in journalism: A new approach]. Estudos em Jornalismo e Mídia, 14(1), 51–62. DOI: 10.5007/1984-6924.2017v14n1p51
https://doi.org/10.5007/1984-6924.2017v1...
). This study focuses on the second one. Also, previous research indicates the existence of correlations between the decline in media credibility, the erosion of institutions, and scenarios of ideological polarization (Hanitzsch et al., 2018Hanitzsch, T., Van Dalen, A., & Steindl, N. (2018). Caught in the Nexus: A Comparative and Longitudinal Analysis of Public Trust in the Press. The International Journal of Press/Politics, 23(1), 3–23. DOI: 10.1177/1940161217740695
https://doi.org/10.1177/1940161217740695...
). Perceptions about the credibility of the news media can even be related to stability and confidence in democracy (Tsfati & Cohen, 2005Tsfati, Y., & Cohen, J. (2005). Democratic Consequences of Hostile Media Perceptions. Harvard International Journal of Press/Politics, 10(4), 28–51. DOI:10.1177/1081180x05280776
https://doi.org/10.1177/1081180x05280776...
).

Schudson (2019)Schudson, M. (2019, n.d.). The Fall, Rise, and Fall of Media Trust. Columbia Journalism Review. Retrieved from www.cjr.org/special_report/the-fall-rise-and-fall-of-media-trust.php
www.cjr.org/special_report/the-fall-rise...
identified that faith in institutions like Congress, medicine, education, the military, organized religion, major corporations, and journalism began to decline in the 1970s. Concerning journalism, this was due to at least three reasons: despite being a public service, the news media has been largely funded by the self-interest of the market (Waisbord, 2017Waisbord, S. (2017). Crisis? What crisis? In C. Peters C & M. Broersma (Eds.), Rethinking Journalism Again: Societal Role and Public Relevance in the Digital Age (pp. 205–215). Routledge.); the rise of new digital technologies has increased audience participation since they are more likely to identify political or business interests embedded in the news agenda; journalists have spent decades denying that they write from a set of values, not simply from a disinterested effort at truth (Schudson, 2019Schudson, M. (2019, n.d.). The Fall, Rise, and Fall of Media Trust. Columbia Journalism Review. Retrieved from www.cjr.org/special_report/the-fall-rise-and-fall-of-media-trust.php
www.cjr.org/special_report/the-fall-rise...
). In other words, they have sold impartiality and neutrality as values extremely close to trusting, instead of being transparent regarding their methods, angulations, and interests. Thus, the following research questions are raised:

RQ1: What is the perception of news media credibility by Brazilians during the Covid-19 press coverage?

RQ2: What are the most reliable types of news media?

3 Subjective well-being (SWB): the bottom-up aspect

Happiness is a component recognized as part of a healthy life, being a topic addressed by philosophers since ancient Greece. The hedonic perspective of well-being understands happiness in terms of obtaining pleasure and preventing pain, identifying it as a subjective experience, and considering individuals’ self-evaluation of their own life (Diener & Emmons, 1984Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1984). The Independence of Positive and Negative Affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(5), 1105–1117. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.47.5.1105
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.47.5.1...
). Even though there is a wide range of terminologies and concepts in literature (Ryan & Deci, 2001Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On Happiness and Human Potentials: A Review of Research on Hedonic and Eudaimonic Well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 141–166. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52...
), psychological science tests hypotheses with rigorous empirical methods and adopts the scientific expression given to people’s self-assessment of their own happiness: subjective well-being (SWB). Thus, the measurement of happiness has been workable with the usage of the concept of SWB, the scientific approach to happiness (Diener et al., 2018Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Tay, L. (2018). Advances in Subjective Well-Being Research. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(4), 253–260. DOI: 10.1038/s41562-018-0307-6
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0307-...
).

SWB is a multidimensional construct comprising three components: satisfaction with life; positive affect; and negative affect (Diener & Emmons, 1984Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1984). The Independence of Positive and Negative Affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(5), 1105–1117. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.47.5.1105
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.47.5.1...
; Diener et al., 1985Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction with Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71–75. DOI: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901...
). Satisfaction with life, the cognitive dimension of SWB, refers to the cognitive judgment about the degree of satisfaction that all people make about their lives in general (Diener & Emmons, 1984Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1984). The Independence of Positive and Negative Affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(5), 1105–1117. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.47.5.1105
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.47.5.1...
; Woyciekoski et al., 2012Woyciekoski, C., Stenert, F., & Hutz, C. S. (2012). Determinantes do bem-estar subjetivo. Psico, 43(3), 280–288. Retrieved from https://revistaseletronicas.pucrs.br/index.php/revistapsico/article/view/8263
https://revistaseletronicas.pucrs.br/ind...
). The affective dimension is composed of positive affect and negative affect and refers to the intensity and frequency with which individuals experience positive or pleasurable versus negative or unpleasant affects (Zanon & Hutz, 2014Zanon, C., & Hutz, C.S. (2014). Escala de afetos positivos e afetos negativos (PANAS). In C. S. Hutz (Ed.), Avaliação em psicologia positiva (pp. 63–67). Artmed.).

Many researchers have sought to understand the factors involved in human happiness – a very complex task considering the number of variables that play a role in explaining well-being (e.g., Deaton, 2008Deaton, A. (2008). Income, Health, and Well-Being Around the World: Evidence from the Gallup World Poll. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 22(2), 53–72. Retrieved from www.jstor.org/stable/27648241
www.jstor.org/stable/27648241...
; Diener et al., 2013Diener, E., Tay, L., & Oishi, S. (2013). Rising Income and the Subjective Well-being of Nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(2), 267–276. DOI:10.1037/a0030487
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030487...
). There is evidence that SWB varies not only due to individual factors, such as genetics and personality, but also for contextual and macro-social reasons (Diener et al., 2018Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Tay, L. (2018). Advances in Subjective Well-Being Research. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(4), 253–260. DOI: 10.1038/s41562-018-0307-6
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0307-...
). Thus, the society one lives in is a crucial influence on SWB. Conflicts, contingency or even feeling that one can or cannot trust other people are powerful predictors of happiness worldwide. These are called “bottom-up” aspects of subjective well-being. Situational factors external to persons influence their subjective well-being, such as good and adverse events, and circumstances like health and wealth (Diener, 2021Diener, E. (2021, n.d.). Happiness: The Science of Subjective Well-being. NOBA. Retrieved from www.nobaproject.com/modules/happiness-the-science-of-subjective-well-being
www.nobaproject.com/modules/happiness-th...
).

The literature suggests that SWB is highly desirable because several good outcomes flow from happiness. For example, happy people (those with long-term average positive emotions) live longer, have stronger immune systems and are better able to cope with tricky situations (Diener, 2021Diener, E. (2021, n.d.). Happiness: The Science of Subjective Well-being. NOBA. Retrieved from www.nobaproject.com/modules/happiness-the-science-of-subjective-well-being
www.nobaproject.com/modules/happiness-th...
). In contrast, people with low levels of SWB who are chronically stressed, depressed, or angry appear to be less healthy and function less effectively (Diener, 2021Diener, E. (2021, n.d.). Happiness: The Science of Subjective Well-being. NOBA. Retrieved from www.nobaproject.com/modules/happiness-the-science-of-subjective-well-being
www.nobaproject.com/modules/happiness-th...
). And this stress can be caused by exposure to news. Previous research has even shown that exposure to mass trauma events through the media can increase the initial rates of symptoms of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Brackbill et al., 2009Brackbill, R. M., Hadler, J. L., DiGrande, L., Ekenga, C. C., Farfel, M. R., Friedman, S., Perlman, S. E., Stellman, S. D., Walker, D. J., Wu, D., Yu, S., & Thorpe, L. E. (2009). Asthma and posttraumatic stress symptoms 5 to 6 years following exposure to the World Trade Center terrorist attack. JAMA, 302(5), 502–516. DOI: 10.1001/jama.2009.1121
https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2009.1121...
; Schlenger et al., 2002Schlenger, W. E., Caddell, J. M., Ebert, L., Jordan, B. K., Rourke, K. M., Wilson, D. Thalji, L., Dennis, J. M., Fairbank, J. A., Kulka, R. A. (2002). Psychological Reactions to Terrorist Attacks: Findings from the National Study of Americans’ Reactions to September 11. Jama, 288(5), 581–588. DOI: 10.1001/jama.288.5.581
https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.288.5.581...
).

Although it emphasizes the importance of citizens’ being well informed, aiming at protecting psychological health, the World Health Organization recommends reducing how many people watch, read, or hear the news that makes them feel anxious and depressed. In fact, compulsive access to the media triggers fatigue, and this can cause elevated anxiety and depression (Dhir et al., 2018Dhir, A., Yossatorn, Y., Kaur, P., & Chen, S. (2018). Online Social Media Fatigue and Psychological Wellbeing – A Study of Compulsive Use, Fear of Missing Out, Fatigue, Anxiety and Depression. International Journal of Information Management, vol. 40, 141–152. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2018.01.012
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2018...
). When people are aware of negative news, and at the same time are looking for more consistent information, the anxiety and stress caused during the pandemic (Yamada et al., 2021Yamada, Y., Ćepulić, DB., Coll-Martín, T., Debove, S., Gautreau, G., Han, H., Rasmussen, J., Tran, T. P., Travaglino, G. A., & Lieberoth, A. (2021). COVIDiSTRESS Global Survey Dataset Psychological and Behavioural Consequences of the COVID-19 Outbreak. Scientific Data, 8(1), 1–23. DOI: 10.1038/s41597-020-00784-9
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41597-020-00784...
), can lead to decreased well-being and impact media perception (Aquino & Vieira, 2020aAquino, S. D., & Vieira, L. S. (2020a). Bem-estar e consumo de notícias durante a pandemia de COVID-19 [Well-being and News Consumption During the COVID-19 Pandemic. Revista Fontes Documentais, vol. 3, 165 – 174. Retrieved from https://aplicacoes.ifs.edu.br/periodicos/index.php/fontesdocumentais/issue/view/52
https://aplicacoes.ifs.edu.br/periodicos...
). This leads to the following research question:

RQ3: Is there a correlation between news media credibility and subjective well-being?

4 The Fear of Missing Out (FoMO) in an environment of news overload

Fear of missing out (FoMO) is a feeling of foreboding that the person may miss some events that other people are going to enjoy (Przybylski et al., 2013Przybylski, A. K., Murayama, K., DeHaan, C. R., & Gladwell, V. (2013). Motivational, emotional, and Behavioral Correlates of Fear of Missing Out. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(4), 1841–1848. DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.014
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.01...
). It is also related to compulsively checking news feeds (e.g., Grohol, 2011Grohol, J. (2011). FOMO Addiction: The Fear of Missing Out. PsychCentral Retrieved from https://psychcentral.com/blog/fomo-addiction-the-fear-of-missing-out
https://psychcentral.com/blog/fomo-addic...
; Wortham, 2011Wortham, J. (2011, April 9). Feel Like a Wallflower? Maybe it’s your Facebook Wall. The New York Times. Retrieved from www.nytimes.com/2011/04/10/business/10ping.html
www.nytimes.com/2011/04/10/business/10pi...
). Although it is a term studied from different approaches, the consensus is that FoMO is generally conceptualized as a construct involving anxiety, rumination (Dempsey et al., 2019Dempsey, A., O’Brien, K., Tiamiyu, M., & Elhai, J. D. (2019). Fear of Missing Out (FoMO) and Rumination Mediate Relations Between Social Anxiety and Problematic Facebook Use. Addictive Behaviors Reports, vol. 9, 1–7. DOI: 10.1016/j.abrep.2018.100150
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.abrep.2018.100...
), and negative humor (Elhai et al., 2020Elhai, J. D., Rozgonjuk, D., Liu, T., & Yang, H. (2020). Fear of Missing out Predicts Repeated Measurements of Greater Negative Affect Using Experience Sampling Methodology. Journal of Affective Disorders, vol. 262, 298–303. DOI: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.11.026
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2019.11.02...
; Wolniewicz et al., 2018Wolniewicz, C. A., Tiamiyu, M. F., Weeks, J. W., & Elhai, J. D. (2018). Problematic Smartphone Use and Relations with Negative Affect, Fear of Missing Out, and Fear of Negative and Positive Evaluation. Psychiatry Research, vol. 262, 618–623. DOI: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.09.058
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017....
): Feelings and emotions which are important aspects of the negative affections that make up subjective well-being.

Such anxiety generated by FoMO tends to negatively impact SWB (Stead & Bibby, 2017Stead, H., & Bibby, P. A. (2017). Personality, Fear of Missing Out and Problematic Internet Use and Their Relationship to Subjective Well-Being. Computers in Human Behavior, vol. 76, 534–540. DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.08.016
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.08.01...
). This has been related to the perception of quality of life, both because it is inversely correlated with life satisfaction (Can & Satici, 2019Can, G., & Satici, S. A. (2019). Adaptation of Fear of Missing out Scale (FoMOs): Turkish Version Validity and Reliability Study. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 32(1), 1–7. DOI: 10.1186/s41155-019-0117-4
https://doi.org/10.1186/s41155-019-0117-...
; Sha et al., 2019Sha, P., Sariyska, R., Riedl, R., Lachmann, B., & Montag, C. (2019). Linking Internet Communication and Smartphone Use Disorder by Taking a Closer Look at the Facebook and WhatsApp Applications. Addictive Behaviors Reports, vol. 9, 1–9. DOI: 10.1016/j.abrep.2018.100148
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.abrep.2018.100...
) and because of the negative correlation with emotional well-being (Chai et al., 2019Chai, H. Y., Niu, G. F., Lian, S. L., Chu, X. W., & Sun, X. J. (2019). Why Social Network Site Use Fails to Promote Well-being? The Roles of Social Overload and Fear of Missing Out. Computers in Human Behavior, vol. 100, 85–92. DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.05.005
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.05.00...
; Roberts & David, 2019Roberts, J. A., & David, M. E. (2019). The Social Media Party: Fear of Missing Out (Fomo), Social Media Intensity, Connection, and Well-being. International Journal of Human–Computer Interaction, 36(4), 386–392. DOI: 10.1080/10447318.2019.1646517
https://doi.org/10.1080/10447318.2019.16...
). Increased exposure to critical social situations such as the pandemic leads to psychopathological consequences such as reduced quality of life and well-being (Norris et al., 2002Norris, F. H., Friedman, M. J., Watson, P. J., Byrne, C. M., Diaz, E., & Kaniasty, K. (2002). 60,000 Disaster Victims Speak: Part I. An Empirical Review of the Empirical Literature, 1981–2001. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 65(3), 207–239. DOI:10.1521/psyc.65.3.207.20173
https://doi.org/10.1521/psyc.65.3.207.20...
).

In a hyperconnected world, the FoMO on information can be related not to the lack of news, but to its opposite: news overload, which occurs when a person receives more information than s/he can process in a certain period (Ji et al., 2014Ji, Q., Ha, L., & Sypher, U. (2014). The role of news media use and demographic characteristics in the possibility of information overload prediction. International Journal of Communication, vol.8, 699–714. Retrieved from https://ijoc.org/index.php/ijoc/article/view/2419
https://ijoc.org/index.php/ijoc/article/...
). It is reasonable to assume that in a time of plentiful information available, people are experiencing negative feelings during the information consumption process.

Song et al. (2016)Song, H., Jung, J., & Kim, Y. (2016). Perceived news overload and its cognitive and attitudinal consequences for news usage in South Korea. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 94(4), 1172–1190. DOI: 10.1177/1077699016679975
https://doi.org/10.1177/1077699016679975...
identified that a flood of news from soaring numbers of sources and platforms is making news consumers feel overloaded, with negative consequences such as fatigue and indifference to the news. The authors found that perceived news overload-induced news avoidance by increasing news fatigue and news analysis paralysis.

Journalists from the New York Times (Wortham, 2011Wortham, J. (2011, April 9). Feel Like a Wallflower? Maybe it’s your Facebook Wall. The New York Times. Retrieved from www.nytimes.com/2011/04/10/business/10ping.html
www.nytimes.com/2011/04/10/business/10pi...
) and the San Francisco Chronicle (Morford, 2010Morford, M. (2010, August 4). Oh My God You Are So Missing Out. SFGate. Retrieved from www.sfgate.com/entertainment/morford/article/Oh-my-God-you-are-so-missing-out-2536241.php
www.sfgate.com/entertainment/morford/art...
) have already reported how the mix of media and FoMO can be linked to general unhappiness. Wortham (2011)Wortham, J. (2011, April 9). Feel Like a Wallflower? Maybe it’s your Facebook Wall. The New York Times. Retrieved from www.nytimes.com/2011/04/10/business/10ping.html
www.nytimes.com/2011/04/10/business/10pi...
proposed that FoMO can be a source of negative humor and depressed feelings. The research focused on the underlying motives why social media provide additional reasons to expect the FoMO to be associated with mood deficits and life satisfaction (Uram & Skalski, 2020Uram, P., & Skalski, S. (2020). Still Logged in? The Link Between Facebook Addiction, FoMO, Self-Esteem, Life Satisfaction and Loneliness in Social Media Users. Psychological Reports, 125(1), 218–231. DOI: 10.1177/0033294120980970
https://doi.org/10.1177/0033294120980970...
).

In an article for Vice, Dexter Thomas reported his observation in South Korea during the coronavirus crisis, in February 2020. He was interviewing someone on camera and heard the buzz of a phone notification. “At first, we thought someone had forgotten to put their phone on silent, but then we realized everyone’s phones were buzzing at the same time. The government was pushing out health alerts about nearby outbreaks” (Thomas, 2020Thomas, D. (2020, February 27) The Dark Side of FoMO: Living Through a Coronavirus Outbreak in South Korea. Vice News. Retrieved from www.vice.com/en_uk/article/7kz7pg/the-dark-side-of-FoMO-living-through-a-coronavirus-outbreak-in-south-korea
www.vice.com/en_uk/article/7kz7pg/the-da...
). According to him, that might be the most unsettling thing about an epidemic in the age of social media FoMO: the crawling dread that if you look away from your phone for even a moment, you’ll end up dying because you missed an important message.

Also referring to the Covid-19 pandemic, Rutledge (2020)Rutledge, P. (2020, March 23). Pandemic FoMO: How Much News Is Too Much? Psychology Today. Retrieved from www.psychologytoday.com/gb/blog/positively-media/202003/pandemic-FoMO-how-much-news-is-too-much
www.psychologytoday.com/gb/blog/positive...
argued that media content of all kinds influences people’s emotions. In a scenario of news overload, reading lots of angry or fearful tweets or worrisome news stories can increase agitation, raise stress levels, and create feelings of helplessness. “The outbreak of coronavirus has triggered our survival instinct and made us hypervigilant; we are on high-alert for signals of danger, amplifying the impact” (Rutledge, 2020Rutledge, P. (2020, March 23). Pandemic FoMO: How Much News Is Too Much? Psychology Today. Retrieved from www.psychologytoday.com/gb/blog/positively-media/202003/pandemic-FoMO-how-much-news-is-too-much
www.psychologytoday.com/gb/blog/positive...
). With this in mind, we propose the following question:

RQ4: Is it possible to verify fear of missing out (FoMO) on the news during the Covid-19 pandemic?

5 Method

This is an exploratory, descriptive, correlational, and transversal survey, utilizing quantitative analysis. We aimed to investigate the audience perceptions of news media in the initial stages of the Covid-19 pandemic in Brazil by verifying the relations among media credibility, subjective well-being (SWB), and fear of missing out (FoMO). In line with the combination of different methods and data sources, an online questionnaire was made available on the internet, and data collection took place for 15 days, between March 26th and April 9th, 2020.

5.1 Sample

There was a non-probabilistic convenience sample of 306 Brazilians over 18 years of age (Mage=36.83; SD=11.17); 66% were women. The sample contained people from over 15 Brazilian states, with 64.05% of respondents living in the Southeast region; 21.57% in the South region; 4.58% in the Northeast region; 3.27% in the Midwest region; and 0.65% in the North region. As for education, 3.6% had completed high school; 32.7% had higher education; 63.7% of the people had postgraduate degrees. Politically, 60.1% of the participants positioned themselves on the left, 22.2% in the center, and 17.6% on the right of the political spectrum.

5.2 Instruments

We used an online questionnaire made available on the internet, which contained sociodemographic questions (age, gender, place of residence, education level, political spectrum position), news consumption habits, and perceptions about media. In addition to these questions, there were the following psychometric instruments:

Media credibility scale (Gaziano & McGrath, 1986Gaziano, C., & McGrath, K. (1986). Measuring the Concept of Credibility. Journalism Quarterly, 63(3), 451–462. DOI: 10.1177/107769908606300301
https://doi.org/10.1177/1077699086063003...
; adapted for Brazil by Aquino & Vieira, 2020bAquino, S. D., & Vieira, L. S. (2020b). Credibilidade da Mídia – Adaptação de uma escala para o contexto brasileiro [Media Credibility – Adaptation of a Scale for the Brazilian Context]. Proceedings of the 50th Annual Meeting of the Brazilian Society of Psychology. Brasília, DF. DOI 10.13140/RG.2.2.23403.16160
https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.23403.16...
). This one-dimensional instrument is composed of four items that measure the degree of trust in the media (fair, accurate, tell the whole story, it can be trusted), ranging from 1-Strongly disagree to 5-Strongly agree ( α =.93).

Satisfaction with life scale (SWLS) (Diener et al., 1985Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction with Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71–75. DOI: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901...
; adapted for Brazil by Zanon et al., 2014Zanon, C., Bardagi, M. P., Layous, K., & Hutz, C. S. (2014). Validation of the Satisfaction with Life Scale to Brazilians: Evidences of measurement noninvariance across Brazil and US. Social Indicators Research, 119(1), 443–453. DOI:10.1007/s11205-013-0478-5.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-013-0478-...
). This instrument assesses the cognitive component of the SWB, through five self-responsive phrases about life satisfaction. Answers are provided on a seven-point scale, ranging from 1-Strongly disagree to 7-Strongly agree. ( α =.87).

Positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS) (Watson et al., 1988Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and Validation of Brief Measures of Positive and Negative Affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1...
; adapted for Brazil by Zanon et al., 2014Zanon, C., & Hutz, C.S. (2014). Escala de afetos positivos e afetos negativos (PANAS). In C. S. Hutz (Ed.), Avaliação em psicologia positiva (pp. 63–67). Artmed.). The affective dimension of the SWB was assessed using this two-dimensional scale, where each dimension is composed of 10 items. The subscale of positive impact (α=.88) measures the degree to which individuals experience positive affect, while the subscale of negative impact (α=.91) measures how much individuals experience momentary states of negative affect. In this instrument, the participants are given instructions to indicate “how much they feel this way lately” (1-Not at all; 5-Extremely).

Media perception (Tsfati, 2003Tsfati, Y., & Cappella, J. N. (2003). Do People Watch What They Do Not Trust? Communication Research, 30(5), 504–529. DOI:10.1177/0093650203253371.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650203253371...
; Tsfati & Cappella, 2003Tsfati, Y., & Cappella, J. N. (2003). Do People Watch What They Do Not Trust? Communication Research, 30(5), 504–529. DOI:10.1177/0093650203253371.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650203253371...
). Two questions related to perception about media trustworthiness were adapted and coded so that answers evaluating the media’s work in a more favorable and trustworthy manner had higher values, while answers indicating more skepticism were obtained with lower values. One item asked how often they rely on the media to report the news honestly (1-never, 2-sometimes, 3-most of the time, 4-almost always); and the other one scored the quality attributed to the work of the media when reporting on the pandemic (1-bad, 2-regular, 3-good, 4-excellent). Besides this, there was an open-response item in which respondents indicated which news programs were providing the best coverage of the pandemic.

FoMO. The questions regarding FoMO about the news were inserted twice at distinct moments. The first item of the questionnaire, in the beginning, was a general dichotomic question: “Do you usually feel fear of missing out on the news?”. At the end of the questionnaire, we specified the question to make a comparison: “Do you feel fear of missing out on the news about Covid-19?”.

5.3 Procedures

Data collection: participants were recruited by snowball sampling (Naderifar et al., 2017Naderifar, M., Goli, H., & Ghaljaie, F. (2017). Snowball Sampling: A Purposeful Method of sampling in Qualitative Research. Strides in Development of Medical Education, 14(3). DOI: 10.5812/SDME.67670.
https://doi.org/10.5812/SDME.67670...
) in response to invitations sent via email and made available on social networks. The invitations explained the survey and provided the link to access the questionnaire. On the first page of the website where the questionnaire was available, the Free and Informed Consent Term was presented, attesting to the anonymity of the participants, the confidentiality of the responses, and the free decision to participate in the survey, in compliance with the guidelines and regulatory standards for research involving human beings in Brazil (Resolution 466/2013 from the National Health Council). In addition, participants were informed that the survey was intended for Brazilians over 18 years of age, and we asked them to confirm whether they satisfied this criterion. The questionnaire was configured not to allow missing answers to the scales; however, the open questions were optional. Thus, no treatment of missing cases was necessary.

Analyses: initially, demographic data were analyzed using descriptive statistics (frequency/percentage, measures of central tendency, and dispersion). Then, Pearson’s correlation coefficients, and group differences using Student t-tests were also calculated with the IBM-Statistical Package for Social Science (SPSS).

6 Results

6.1 Media credibility

To answer RQ1, we analyzed the frequencies of responses to establish the perception of general news media trust. When asked how often they think they can trust the media to report the news honestly, 50% (n=153) of the sample answered “sometimes”, 36.9% (n=113) “most of the time”, 9.2% (n=28) “almost always” and 3.9% (n=12) “never”.

The question to elicit evaluation of the quality of press coverage of the Covid-19 pandemic obtained the following indices: 29.74% (n=91) evaluated the quality of coverage as “Excellent”, 42.48% (n=130) evaluated it as “Good”, 18.95% (n=58) rated it as “Regular” and 8.82% (n=27) rated it as “Bad”.

6.2 Perceptions of diverse types of media

RQ2 sought to explore the opinions of the most reliable types of media to report the pandemic. Participants indicated how they learned more about Covid-19 and chose their main source of news. The types of media to which respondents devoted more time to obtain information about the pandemic were: “News websites”, chosen by 38.9% (n=119), “TV”, indicated by 35% (n=107), “Social Networks”, chosen by 23.5% (n=72), “Radio” indicated by 2% (n=6), “Magazine” 0.3% (n=1) and “Printed Newspaper” also 0.3% (n=1).

Since the internet is a farthest reaching medium today, respondents also indicated which online service they used most to obtain information on the pandemic. First, “news websites (e.g., newspaper or magazine websites)” were indicated by 49.7% (n=152) of respondents, followed by “Twitter”, indicated by 10.1% (n=31), then “Influencer Channels” with 9.2% (n=28), “Facebook” with 8.2% (n=25) and “Instagram” 8.2% (n=25), both in fourth place. “WhatsApp” was indicated by 3.9% (n=12) of the respondents, and finally, “Podcasts” were chosen by 2.6% (n=8).

Additionally, we posed a qualitative question: “Think about the quality of TV and radio programs, magazines, blogs, printed newspapers, etc. Considering all the media, in your opinion, which news outlet has provided the best coverage of the Covid-19 pandemic? Cite only one product/program of a specific news outlet that you consider to best cover the pandemic”. Since this was an open/free question, we grouped the most cited answers according to some categories identified from the data. Some participants mentioned over one program/news outlet, despite our request. Some answers could not be categorized as a product, a program, or a news outlet (e.g., 36 participants cited “TV” as a type of media; and 28 cited Rede Globo, which is the largest TV company in Brazil). We considered the answers that mentioned other specific sources of information, such as experts and governments and responses that none of the news outlets have provided good coverage. The most mentioned and valid qualitative answers are in table 1.

Table 1
News outlets with the best coverage of the pandemic

6.3 SWB and media credibility

To discover the correlation between SWB and media credibility (RQ3), Pearson correlations were calculated. Negative affect was positively related to the average of credibility attributed to the media, r(306)=.20, p<.001, and positive affect was negatively related, r(306)=-.17, p<.001. These results indicate that people with a higher average of negative affect also had a higher average in credibility attributed to the media; while people with a higher average of positive affect had a lower average in credibility attributed to the media. Other variables, such as age and political orientation, also showed significant correlations with the variables, as seen in table 2.

Table 2
Correlations between media credibility, SWB, and other variables

The significance of the results found in Pearson’s analysis indicated the existence of correlation, but without indicating a causal direction. So, multiple linear regression analysis was performed (forward method) to investigate to what extent the affective dimension of SWB had an impact on levels of media credibility. The results showed there was a significant influence of the affects experienced by the sample on the degree of credibility they attributed to the news media (F(2.303)=8.046, p<.001; adjusted R2=0.044). The regression indicated that positive affect (β=-.12; p=.048), and negative affect (β=.16; p=.007) were statistically significant predictors of media credibility, explaining 4.4% of the variance in credibility they attributed to the news media (B=2.677, t=9.013, p<.001). VIF levels were acceptable, indicating excellent model adequacy. In contrast, according to multiple linear regression (enter method), the credibility attributed to the media explained 3.8% of negative affect (F(1,304)=12.018, p=.001; adjusted R2=0.035; B=.195, t=3.467, p=.001), while it explained 2.4% of positive affect (F(1.304)=8.577, p=.004; adjusted R2=0.024; B=-.166, t=-2.929, p=.004). Thus, there seemed to be a moderating effect of affects according to this regression model.

6.4 FoMO on Covid-19 news

The averages of FoMO were compared in two moments. The answers to the general question were ‘Yes’, indicated by 53.9% (n=165), and ‘No’, representing 46.1% (n=141). The mean differences between these groups according to the student t-test are reported in table 3.

Table 3
Mean differences between those who answered Yes and those who answered No to FoMO in general

The answers to the specific FoMO question about Covid were 61.8% (n=189) ‘Yes’ and 38.2% (n=117) ‘No’. Student t-tests were performed differentiating the group of individuals who declared FoMO in general and individuals who declared FoMO concerning Covid-19 (table 4). We highlight that higher averages of credibility attributed to the media were identified for individuals who said they are afraid of missing something important about the pandemic (M=2.94; SD=0.88) and those who declared they did not have this fear (M=2.43; SD=1.01), t(220,551)=4.44; p<.001; d=0.53. Negative affect averages were also higher in the group that declared FoMO on the news about Covid-19 (M=2.93; SD=0.86) than in the group of individuals who did not declare it (M=2.25; SD=0.79), t(304)=4.88; p<.001; d=0.58. All results of differences between these groups can be seen in table 4.

Table 4
Mean differences between those who answered Yes and those who answered No to FoMO on the news about Covid-19

To conclude the analysis regarding RQ4, we performed a chi-square test of independence to examine the association between FoMO in general and FoMO on the news about Covid-19. It showed there is a significant association between the two variables, confirming that FoMO was higher when referring to news about the pandemic. χ2(1, N=306) = 76.61, p<.001.

7 Discussion

During this survey, Brazil was in the early phases of the pandemic: on the last day of the data collection (April 9th, 2020), there were 18.176 people infected and 957 deaths in the country. That means Brazilians knew very little about how the disease would spread in the country. This study aimed to investigate the audience perceptions of news media in the initial stages of the Covid-19 pandemic in Brazil by verifying the relations among media credibility, subjective well-being (SWB), and fear of missing out (FoMO). The findings suggest that the pandemic crisis influenced the perceptions regarding the work of the press and media credibility, and this was correlated with the experienced levels of Brazilians’ well-being.

Initially, the responses obtained to establish the perceived trustworthiness of the news media in general, indicated that a large portion of the sample seemed to trust the honesty of the news reported by the media more often. In addition, the quality attributed to the media’s work in covering the pandemic was high. This result is similar to that of Nielsen et al. (2020)Nielsen, R. K., Fletcher, R., Newman, N., Brennen, J. S., & Howard, P. N. (2020, April 15). Navigating the ‘Infodemic’: How People in Six Countries Access and Rate News and Information about Coronavirus. Digital News Report. Retrieved from https://reutersinstitute.politics.ox.ac.uk/infodemic-how-people-six-countries-access-and-rate-news-and-information-about-coronavirus
https://reutersinstitute.politics.ox.ac....
, showing that the majority rated the media as reliable during the first stages of the pandemic. Thus, the perception of news media credibility by Brazilians during the Covid-19 press coverage was favorable, based on perceptions about reliability.

News websites and TV were indicated by the majority as the media to which people devoted the most time to get news about Covid-19. And when considering only the digital environment, news websites remained in the first place, despite Brazil being among the countries that most use social media in the world. In a way, at this time of intense social crisis, the internet still seemed to be a channel through which less reliable information circulates. This could endorse an argument about how much the global crisis context influences individuals to rely on traditional news sources. Even when choosing which online service they used most to obtain information on the pandemic, the reliance on traditional media was greater, considering that also most news sites are owned by legacy outlets.

This assumption became stronger when we asked the respondents to mention the outlet brand they trust the most in the pandemic coverage: two TV programs owned by the largest Brazilian media company were cited in first and second place. Official sources and experts were also named, highlighting the search for technical knowledge, in contrast to general media knowledge.

An important finding in this study is the correlation between SWB and media credibility: people with higher averages of negative affects were more confident in the media. The more a person is afflicted, frightened, distressed, angry, and disturbed, the more he/she thinks that news media are fair, accurate, tell the whole story, and can be trusted. On the other hand, people that feel more determination, enthusiasm, strength, pride, inspiration, and animation are more skeptical about the media. Considering that subjective well-being also varies according to contextual and macrosocial reasons (Diener et al., 2018Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Tay, L. (2018). Advances in Subjective Well-Being Research. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(4), 253–260. DOI: 10.1038/s41562-018-0307-6
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0307-...
), it is reasonable to assume that a disruptive context that brings profound social consequences will alter well-being. In a time of fear and uncertainty, even purchasing behavior can be impacted by perceived dangers (e.g., Billore & Anisimova, 2021Billore, S., & Anisimova, T. (2021). Panic buying research: A systematic literature review and future research agenda. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 45(4), 777–804. DOI: 10.1111/ijcs.12669
https://doi.org/10.1111/ijcs.12669...
; Lins & Aquino, 2020Lins, S., & Aquino, S. (2020). Development and Initial Psychometric Properties of a Panic Buying Scale during the COVID-19 pandemic. Heliyon, 6(9), 1–6. DOI: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2020.e04746
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2020.e...
). Thus, the impact of unwanted news can affect positive and negative emotions (e.g., Jain, 2021Jain, P. (2021). The COVID-19 Pandemic and Positive Psychology: The Role of News and Trust in News on Mental Health and Well-Being. Journal of Health Communication, 26(5), 317–327. DOI: 10.1080/10810730.2021.1946219
https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2021.19...
; Stainback et al., 2020Stainback, K., Hearne, B. N., & Trieu, M. M. (2020). COVID-19 and the 24/7 News Cycle: Does COVID-19 News Exposure Affect Mental Health? Socius, vol. 6, 1–15. DOI: 10.1177/2378023120969339
https://doi.org/10.1177/2378023120969339...
), also in case people trust solid institutions like the traditional media and accept their reports less critically. Conversely, it is assumed that people who wish to preserve some positive emotions and feelings during a disruptive event may tend to attribute less credibility to news to cope better with the tough times.

Despite the significance of the correlations found, they do not provide a guide to a causal direction, indicating only that the variables are mutually modified. Interestingly, the test of the predictive power of affections on credibility pointed out that they have direct impacts on the levels of credibility attributed to the media. Although degrees of trust in the media can be influenced by many contingencies, positive and negative emotions influence some credibility attributed to the media. If there is more experience of negative affections and feelings, they will predict higher credibility attributed to the media. Conversely, if there are more positive feelings and affections, this could explain the low credibility of the media, and here is a plausible reason: researchers suggest that happy individuals seem not to use diligent processing all the time (Diener, 2009Diener, E. (2009, n.d.). A Primer for Reporters and Newcomers. Retrieved from http://labs.psychology.illinois.edu/~ediener/faq.html#important
http://labs.psychology.illinois.edu/~edi...
), so avoiding giving credence to media can be effective in ameliorating the effects of negative life events and preserving positive affections. The coping avoidance skill cannot change the situation, but it can help by reducing psychological discomfort (Sun & Zhang, 2014Sun, L., & Zhang, J. (2014). Coping Skill as a Moderator Between Negative Life Events and Suicide Among Young People in Rural China. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 71(3), 258–266. DOI:10.1002/jclp.22140
https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22140...
). Therefore, in a challenging social context, there is a predictive power of anxiety or enthusiasm, fear or vigor on how much the audience gives credit to the work of the press.

Interestingly, the older people in the sample had lower perceptions about media credibility and honesty, along with feelings of uncertainty and nervousness, representing a negative affect. The only positive relationship with age was for positive affect, indicating that positive experiences such as vigor, dynamism, and enthusiasm increased with age. And at the other end of this correlation, younger people felt more fear and uncertainty. In parallel, the younger the age, the more they gave credit to the work of the media, evidencing that when listening to and reading news, people may feel more anguished.

As we pointed out, news media credibility can have a relation with the context. Brazilians have been experiencing an atmosphere of political polarization since the 2018 election (Nielsen et al., 2020Nielsen, R. K., Fletcher, R., Newman, N., Brennen, J. S., & Howard, P. N. (2020, April 15). Navigating the ‘Infodemic’: How People in Six Countries Access and Rate News and Information about Coronavirus. Digital News Report. Retrieved from https://reutersinstitute.politics.ox.ac.uk/infodemic-how-people-six-countries-access-and-rate-news-and-information-about-coronavirus
https://reutersinstitute.politics.ox.ac....
), and this can partly explain the increasing credibility attributed to the media by respondents on the left of the political spectrum. This perception has a practical example: in response to the Bolsonaro administration’s decision to restrict access to data on the Covid-19 pandemic, six Brazilian news outlets formed a partnership and worked collaboratively to gather appropriate data in the 26 states and the Federal District. Since then, the most reliable source of information on the pandemic in the country has been provided by the mainstream media, not by the extreme-right government (it is noteworthy that some of these news outlets were cited by the respondents as better sources of information during the pandemic). That shows how the press was actively helping society to keep informed in this moment of crisis, and maybe by doing so, achieving more credibility.

In this context, we also measured the FoMO on the news. It is assumed that unknown risks can attract attention more than regular events, and this maintains vigilance and awareness of specific news on the subject. That is the likely reason for the higher averages of FoMO when people answered questions about the pandemic than generically. It is worth highlighting the effect sizes found in the average differences in both measures of FoMO, indicating the magnitude of the results in this sample. These values constitute evidence (Cohen, 1977Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Routledge.) that the average differences found favor the interpretations discussed in this study.

In assessing the credibility of the media associated with FoMO, this study shows that individuals experiencing more FoMO and more negative affect believe more strongly in the media. Considering all analyses, those who said they trust the media tended to be more concerned about missing news, and this happened when they were experiencing negative affect such as fear or nervousness, e.g. Individuals that experience more positive affects like pride or joy tended to be more critical about the press and they experienced less fear of missing the news.

Regarding this point, mention should be made about a research limitation: the non-probabilistic sample, mostly consisting of women from the Southeast region (the country’s richest and most developed, with the highest average schooling) cannot be considered as representative of the Brazilian population. A sample with greater schooling diversity may bring different results. So, it is necessary to be cautious in interpreting the result because these extracted outcomes can reflect specific biases. Nevertheless, the pandemic has affected the entire country and the different Brazilian social strata. For example, people may have wanted to make a good impression – this commonly occurs with questionnaires with self-response scales – in saying they do not have any fear of missing news about the pandemic. This leads us to infer that the average of FoMO could be higher.

Although the results obtained are consistent and represent a significant contribution, it should be considered that it was not the purpose of this study to generalize, but rather to explore this pandemic context. As a broad result, the relation between media credibility and negative affect can be seen as a challenge. If well-being is also defined by the affective state experienced when positive affect prevails over negative affect, the positivity or negativity of an emotional state can influence judgments (Gilovich & Griffin, 2010Gilovich, T. D., & Griffin, D. W. (2010). Judgment and Decision Making. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology (pp. 542–588). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.), activate positive or negative information, and distort how objects or propositions are evaluated (Mayer et al., 1992Mayer, J. D., Gaschke, Y. N., Braverman, D. L., & Evans, T. W. (1992). Mood-congruent Judgment Is A General Effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(1), 119–132. DOI:10.1037/0022-3514.63.1.119
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.1.1...
). This helps to understand how attitudes about media can be influenced by affect.

8 Conclusion

This study reveals that people who are experiencing more negative affects demonstrate FoMO on the news, specifically during Covid-19, and these people are also the ones who give the media the most credibility. Here is a challenge to news outlets: how can journalism remain relevant to citizens who attach less credibility to the media and are comfortable being left out of the news – even when they are peremptory? If this same profile is also formed by people who hold the experience of positive emotions and feelings, supporting their subjective well-being, investigating whether disbelief in the media is a cause of this (beyond being related) is a relevant field for future audience-based research. It seems to be more difficult to make this type of audience less skeptical, but the effort may be worthwhile in a long-term strategy that seeks stronger connections with the audience.

Finally, this paper opens pathways for in-depth research on the FoMO phenomenon in relation to the news. Although the literature presents FoMO with several frameworks, specific tools for news reading can be developed to better cover the complexity of the construct. Still, the analysis of this topic during a disruptive event like the pandemic is a relevant contribution of this study, which can suggest a lengthy list of new investigative possibilities. Future studies can also explore, describe, and correlate SWB, media credibility, and FoMO in distinct stages of the pandemic worldwide, to verify outcomes of different audience perceptions of the news coverage in distinct samples.

Notes

  • FINAL REVISION: Guy Emmet Fulkerson

REFERENCES

  • Alt, D., & Boniel-Nissim, M. (2018). Links between Adolescents’ Deep and Surface Learning Approaches, Problematic Internet Use, and Fear of Missing Out (FoMO). Internet Interventions, vol. 13, 30–39. DOI: 10.1016/j.invent.2018.05.002
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.invent.2018.05.002
  • Aquino, S. D., & Vieira, L. S. (2020a). Bem-estar e consumo de notícias durante a pandemia de COVID-19 [Well-being and News Consumption During the COVID-19 Pandemic. Revista Fontes Documentais, vol. 3, 165 – 174. Retrieved from https://aplicacoes.ifs.edu.br/periodicos/index.php/fontesdocumentais/issue/view/52
    » https://aplicacoes.ifs.edu.br/periodicos/index.php/fontesdocumentais/issue/view/52
  • Aquino, S. D., & Vieira, L. S. (2020b). Credibilidade da Mídia – Adaptação de uma escala para o contexto brasileiro [Media Credibility – Adaptation of a Scale for the Brazilian Context]. Proceedings of the 50th Annual Meeting of the Brazilian Society of Psychology. Brasília, DF. DOI 10.13140/RG.2.2.23403.16160
    » https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.23403.16160
  • Baker, D. A., & Algorta, G. P. (2016). The Relationship Between Online Social Networking and Depression: A Systematic Review of Quantitative Studies. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 19(11), 638–648. DOI:10.1089/cyber.2016.0206
  • Bernardino, C., Ferreira, H. A., & Chambel, T. (2016). Towards Media for Wellbeing. Proceedings of the Association for Computing Machinery International Conference on Interactive Experiences for TV and Online Video (171–177). NY, USA.
  • Billore, S., & Anisimova, T. (2021). Panic buying research: A systematic literature review and future research agenda. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 45(4), 777–804. DOI: 10.1111/ijcs.12669
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/ijcs.12669
  • Brackbill, R. M., Hadler, J. L., DiGrande, L., Ekenga, C. C., Farfel, M. R., Friedman, S., Perlman, S. E., Stellman, S. D., Walker, D. J., Wu, D., Yu, S., & Thorpe, L. E. (2009). Asthma and posttraumatic stress symptoms 5 to 6 years following exposure to the World Trade Center terrorist attack. JAMA, 302(5), 502–516. DOI: 10.1001/jama.2009.1121
    » https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2009.1121
  • Bruin, K., Haan, Y., Vliegenthart, R., Kruikemeier, S., & Boukes, M. (2021). News Avoidance during the Covid-19 Crisis: Understanding Information Overload. Digital Journalism, 9(9), 1286–1302. DOI: 10.1080/21670811.2021.1957967
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/21670811.2021.1957967
  • Can, G., & Satici, S. A. (2019). Adaptation of Fear of Missing out Scale (FoMOs): Turkish Version Validity and Reliability Study. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 32(1), 1–7. DOI: 10.1186/s41155-019-0117-4
    » https://doi.org/10.1186/s41155-019-0117-4
  • Chai, H. Y., Niu, G. F., Lian, S. L., Chu, X. W., & Sun, X. J. (2019). Why Social Network Site Use Fails to Promote Well-being? The Roles of Social Overload and Fear of Missing Out. Computers in Human Behavior, vol. 100, 85–92. DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.05.005
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.05.005
  • Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences Routledge.
  • Dhanani, LY, & Franz, B (2020). The Role of News Consumption and Trust in Public Health Leadership in Shaping COVID-19 Knowledge and Prejudice. Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 11, 1–13. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.560828
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.560828
  • Deaton, A. (2008). Income, Health, and Well-Being Around the World: Evidence from the Gallup World Poll. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 22(2), 53–72. Retrieved from www.jstor.org/stable/27648241
    » www.jstor.org/stable/27648241
  • Dempsey, A., O’Brien, K., Tiamiyu, M., & Elhai, J. D. (2019). Fear of Missing Out (FoMO) and Rumination Mediate Relations Between Social Anxiety and Problematic Facebook Use. Addictive Behaviors Reports, vol. 9, 1–7. DOI: 10.1016/j.abrep.2018.100150
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.abrep.2018.100150
  • Dhir, A., Yossatorn, Y., Kaur, P., & Chen, S. (2018). Online Social Media Fatigue and Psychological Wellbeing – A Study of Compulsive Use, Fear of Missing Out, Fatigue, Anxiety and Depression. International Journal of Information Management, vol. 40, 141–152. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2018.01.012
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2018.01.012
  • Diener, E. (2009, n.d.). A Primer for Reporters and Newcomers Retrieved from http://labs.psychology.illinois.edu/~ediener/faq.html#important
    » http://labs.psychology.illinois.edu/~ediener/faq.html#important
  • Diener, E. (2021, n.d.). Happiness: The Science of Subjective Well-being. NOBA. Retrieved from www.nobaproject.com/modules/happiness-the-science-of-subjective-well-being
    » www.nobaproject.com/modules/happiness-the-science-of-subjective-well-being
  • Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1984). The Independence of Positive and Negative Affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(5), 1105–1117. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.47.5.1105
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.47.5.1105
  • Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction with Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71–75. DOI: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13
    » https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13
  • Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Tay, L. (2018). Advances in Subjective Well-Being Research. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(4), 253–260. DOI: 10.1038/s41562-018-0307-6
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0307-6
  • Diener, E., Tay, L., & Oishi, S. (2013). Rising Income and the Subjective Well-being of Nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(2), 267–276. DOI:10.1037/a0030487
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030487
  • Elhai, J. D., Levine, J. C., Dvorak, R. D., & Hall, B. J. (2016). Fear of Missing Out, Need for Touch, Anxiety and Depression are Related to Problematic Smartphone Use. Computers in Human Behavior, vol. 63, 509-516. DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.079
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.079
  • Elhai, J. D., Rozgonjuk, D., Liu, T., & Yang, H. (2020). Fear of Missing out Predicts Repeated Measurements of Greater Negative Affect Using Experience Sampling Methodology. Journal of Affective Disorders, vol. 262, 298–303. DOI: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.11.026
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2019.11.026
  • Gaziano, C., & McGrath, K. (1986). Measuring the Concept of Credibility. Journalism Quarterly, 63(3), 451–462. DOI: 10.1177/107769908606300301
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/107769908606300301
  • Gilovich, T. D., & Griffin, D. W. (2010). Judgment and Decision Making. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology (pp. 542–588). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
  • Grohol, J. (2011). FOMO Addiction: The Fear of Missing Out. PsychCentral Retrieved from https://psychcentral.com/blog/fomo-addiction-the-fear-of-missing-out
    » https://psychcentral.com/blog/fomo-addiction-the-fear-of-missing-out
  • Hanitzsch, T., Van Dalen, A., & Steindl, N. (2018). Caught in the Nexus: A Comparative and Longitudinal Analysis of Public Trust in the Press. The International Journal of Press/Politics, 23(1), 3–23. DOI: 10.1177/1940161217740695
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/1940161217740695
  • Jain, P. (2021). The COVID-19 Pandemic and Positive Psychology: The Role of News and Trust in News on Mental Health and Well-Being. Journal of Health Communication, 26(5), 317–327. DOI: 10.1080/10810730.2021.1946219
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2021.1946219
  • James, T. L., Lowry, P. B., Wallace, L., & Warkentin, M. (2017). The Effect of Belongingness on Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder in the Use of Online Social Networks. Journal of Management Information Systems, 34(2), 560–596. DOI: 10.1080/07421222.2017.1334496
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/07421222.2017.1334496
  • Ji, Q., Ha, L., & Sypher, U. (2014). The role of news media use and demographic characteristics in the possibility of information overload prediction. International Journal of Communication, vol.8, 699–714. Retrieved from https://ijoc.org/index.php/ijoc/article/view/2419
    » https://ijoc.org/index.php/ijoc/article/view/2419
  • Kohring, M., & Matthes, J. (2007). Trust in News Media: Development and Validation of a Multidimensional Scale. Communication Research, 34(2), 231–252 DOI : 10.1177/0093650206298071
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650206298071
  • Lai, C., Altavilla, D., Ronconi, A., & Aceto, P. (2016). Fear of Missing Out (FOMO) Is Associated with Activation of The Right Middle Temporal Gyrus During Inclusion Social Cue. Computers in Human Behavior, vol. 61, 516–521. DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.072
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.072
  • Lewicki, R. J., & Tomlinson, E. C. (2003). Trust and Trust Building. Beyond Intractability. In G. Burgess & H. Burgess (Eds.), Conflict Information Consortium (305–315). University of Colorado, Boulder. Retrieved from www.beyondintractability.org/essay/trust_building
    » www.beyondintractability.org/essay/trust_building
  • Lins, S., & Aquino, S. (2020). Development and Initial Psychometric Properties of a Panic Buying Scale during the COVID-19 pandemic. Heliyon, 6(9), 1–6. DOI: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2020.e04746
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2020.e04746
  • Lisboa, S., & Benetti, M. (2017). Credibilidade no jornalismo: uma nova abordagem [Credibility in journalism: A new approach]. Estudos em Jornalismo e Mídia, 14(1), 51–62. DOI: 10.5007/1984-6924.2017v14n1p51
    » https://doi.org/10.5007/1984-6924.2017v14n1p51
  • Mayer, J. D., Gaschke, Y. N., Braverman, D. L., & Evans, T. W. (1992). Mood-congruent Judgment Is A General Effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(1), 119–132. DOI:10.1037/0022-3514.63.1.119
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.1.119
  • Morford, M. (2010, August 4). Oh My God You Are So Missing Out SFGate. Retrieved from www.sfgate.com/entertainment/morford/article/Oh-my-God-you-are-so-missing-out-2536241.php
    » www.sfgate.com/entertainment/morford/article/Oh-my-God-you-are-so-missing-out-2536241.php
  • Naderifar, M., Goli, H., & Ghaljaie, F. (2017). Snowball Sampling: A Purposeful Method of sampling in Qualitative Research. Strides in Development of Medical Education, 14(3). DOI: 10.5812/SDME.67670.
    » https://doi.org/10.5812/SDME.67670
  • Newman, N., Fletcher, R., Schulz, A., Andi, S., & Nielsen, R. (2020). Reuters Institute Digital News Report 2020 Reuters Institute: University of Oxford. Retrieved from https://reutersinstitute.politics.ox.ac.uk/sites/default/files/2020-06/DNR_2020_FINAL.pdf
    » https://reutersinstitute.politics.ox.ac.uk/sites/default/files/2020-06/DNR_2020_FINAL.pdf
  • Nielsen, R. K., Fletcher, R., Newman, N., Brennen, J. S., & Howard, P. N. (2020, April 15). Navigating the ‘Infodemic’: How People in Six Countries Access and Rate News and Information about Coronavirus Digital News Report. Retrieved from https://reutersinstitute.politics.ox.ac.uk/infodemic-how-people-six-countries-access-and-rate-news-and-information-about-coronavirus
    » https://reutersinstitute.politics.ox.ac.uk/infodemic-how-people-six-countries-access-and-rate-news-and-information-about-coronavirus
  • Norris, F. H., Friedman, M. J., Watson, P. J., Byrne, C. M., Diaz, E., & Kaniasty, K. (2002). 60,000 Disaster Victims Speak: Part I. An Empirical Review of the Empirical Literature, 1981–2001. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 65(3), 207–239. DOI:10.1521/psyc.65.3.207.20173
    » https://doi.org/10.1521/psyc.65.3.207.20173
  • Przybylski, A. K., Murayama, K., DeHaan, C. R., & Gladwell, V. (2013). Motivational, emotional, and Behavioral Correlates of Fear of Missing Out. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(4), 1841–1848. DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.014
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.014
  • Reynolds, M., & Weiss, S. (2020, May 27). How Coronavirus Started and What Happens Next, Explained Wired. Retrieved from www.wired.co.uk/article/china-coronavirus
    » www.wired.co.uk/article/china-coronavirus
  • Roberts, J. A., & David, M. E. (2019). The Social Media Party: Fear of Missing Out (Fomo), Social Media Intensity, Connection, and Well-being. International Journal of Human–Computer Interaction, 36(4), 386–392. DOI: 10.1080/10447318.2019.1646517
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/10447318.2019.1646517
  • Rutledge, P. (2020, March 23). Pandemic FoMO: How Much News Is Too Much? Psychology Today. Retrieved from www.psychologytoday.com/gb/blog/positively-media/202003/pandemic-FoMO-how-much-news-is-too-much
    » www.psychologytoday.com/gb/blog/positively-media/202003/pandemic-FoMO-how-much-news-is-too-much
  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On Happiness and Human Potentials: A Review of Research on Hedonic and Eudaimonic Well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 141–166. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141
    » https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141
  • Santos, G. S., & Grossi, A. M. (2018). Jornalismo e credibilidade: Uma percepção do público [Journalism and Credibility: A Perception of the Public]. Ámbitos. Revista Internacional de Comunicación, 3(42), 40-54. Retrieved from https://revistascientificas.us.es/index.php/Ambitos/article/view/5415
    » https://revistascientificas.us.es/index.php/Ambitos/article/view/5415
  • Schlenger, W. E., Caddell, J. M., Ebert, L., Jordan, B. K., Rourke, K. M., Wilson, D. Thalji, L., Dennis, J. M., Fairbank, J. A., Kulka, R. A. (2002). Psychological Reactions to Terrorist Attacks: Findings from the National Study of Americans’ Reactions to September 11. Jama, 288(5), 581–588. DOI: 10.1001/jama.288.5.581
    » https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.288.5.581
  • Schudson, M. (2019, n.d.). The Fall, Rise, and Fall of Media Trust Columbia Journalism Review. Retrieved from www.cjr.org/special_report/the-fall-rise-and-fall-of-media-trust.php
    » www.cjr.org/special_report/the-fall-rise-and-fall-of-media-trust.php
  • Sha, P., Sariyska, R., Riedl, R., Lachmann, B., & Montag, C. (2019). Linking Internet Communication and Smartphone Use Disorder by Taking a Closer Look at the Facebook and WhatsApp Applications. Addictive Behaviors Reports, vol. 9, 1–9. DOI: 10.1016/j.abrep.2018.100148
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.abrep.2018.100148
  • Song, H., Jung, J., & Kim, Y. (2016). Perceived news overload and its cognitive and attitudinal consequences for news usage in South Korea. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 94(4), 1172–1190. DOI: 10.1177/1077699016679975
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/1077699016679975
  • Stainback, K., Hearne, B. N., & Trieu, M. M. (2020). COVID-19 and the 24/7 News Cycle: Does COVID-19 News Exposure Affect Mental Health? Socius, vol. 6, 1–15 DOI: 10.1177/2378023120969339
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/2378023120969339
  • Stead, H., & Bibby, P. A. (2017). Personality, Fear of Missing Out and Problematic Internet Use and Their Relationship to Subjective Well-Being. Computers in Human Behavior, vol. 76, 534–540. DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.08.016
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.08.016
  • Sun, L., & Zhang, J. (2014). Coping Skill as a Moderator Between Negative Life Events and Suicide Among Young People in Rural China. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 71(3), 258–266. DOI:10.1002/jclp.22140
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22140
  • Tameez, H. (2020, March 18). At Least Coronavirus Has Been Good For Online News Traffic (We’re Trying To Be Optimistic) Nieman Lab. Retrieved from www.niemanlab.org/2020/03/at-least-coronavirus-has-been-good-for-online-news-traffic-were-trying-to-be-optimistic/
    » www.niemanlab.org/2020/03/at-least-coronavirus-has-been-good-for-online-news-traffic-were-trying-to-be-optimistic/
  • Thomas, D. (2020, February 27) The Dark Side of FoMO: Living Through a Coronavirus Outbreak in South Korea Vice News. Retrieved from www.vice.com/en_uk/article/7kz7pg/the-dark-side-of-FoMO-living-through-a-coronavirus-outbreak-in-south-korea
    » www.vice.com/en_uk/article/7kz7pg/the-dark-side-of-FoMO-living-through-a-coronavirus-outbreak-in-south-korea
  • Tsfati, Y. (2003). Does Audience Skepticism of the Media Matter In Agenda Setting? Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 47(2), 157–176. DOI:10.1207/s15506878jobem4702_1.
    » https://doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem4702_1
  • Tsfati, Y., & Cappella, J. N. (2003). Do People Watch What They Do Not Trust? Communication Research, 30(5), 504–529. DOI:10.1177/0093650203253371.
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650203253371
  • Tsfati, Y., & Cohen, J. (2005). Democratic Consequences of Hostile Media Perceptions. Harvard International Journal of Press/Politics, 10(4), 28–51 DOI:10.1177/1081180x05280776
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/1081180x05280776
  • Tsfati, Y., & Cohen, J. (2013). Perceptions of Media and Media Effects. In R. Hobbs, A. N. Valdivia & P. Mihaiudis (Eds.), The International Encyclopedia of Media Studies (pp. 128–146). New York: Wiley-Blackwell. DOI: 10.1002/9781444361506.wbiems995
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444361506.wbiems995
  • Uram, P., & Skalski, S. (2020). Still Logged in? The Link Between Facebook Addiction, FoMO, Self-Esteem, Life Satisfaction and Loneliness in Social Media Users. Psychological Reports, 125(1), 218–231. DOI: 10.1177/0033294120980970
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/0033294120980970
  • Van Aelst, P., Toth, F., Castro, L., Štětka, V., Vreese, C. de, Aalberg, T., Cardenal, A. S., Corbu, N., Esser, F., Hopmann, D. N., KocMichalska, K., Matthes, J., Schemer, C., Sheafer, T., Splendore, S., Stanyer, J., Sępińska, A., Strömböck, J., & Theocharis, Y. (2021). Does a Crisis Change News Habits? A Comparative Study of the Effects of COVID-19 on News Media Use in 17 European Countries. Digital Journalism, 9(9), 1–31. DOI: 10.1080/21670811.2021.1943481
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/21670811.2021.1943481
  • Waeterloos, C., Walrave M., Ponnet, K. (2021). The Role of Multi-Platform News Consumption in Explaining Civic Participation during the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Communication Mediation Approach. New Media & Society, online first. DOI: 10.1177/14614448211058701
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/14614448211058701
  • Waisbord, S. (2017). Crisis? What crisis? In C. Peters C & M. Broersma (Eds.), Rethinking Journalism Again: Societal Role and Public Relevance in the Digital Age (pp. 205–215). Routledge.
  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and Validation of Brief Measures of Positive and Negative Affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
  • Wolniewicz, C. A., Tiamiyu, M. F., Weeks, J. W., & Elhai, J. D. (2018). Problematic Smartphone Use and Relations with Negative Affect, Fear of Missing Out, and Fear of Negative and Positive Evaluation. Psychiatry Research, vol. 262, 618–623. DOI: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.09.058
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.09.058
  • Wortham, J. (2011, April 9). Feel Like a Wallflower? Maybe it’s your Facebook Wall The New York Times. Retrieved from www.nytimes.com/2011/04/10/business/10ping.html
    » www.nytimes.com/2011/04/10/business/10ping.html
  • Woyciekoski, C., Stenert, F., & Hutz, C. S. (2012). Determinantes do bem-estar subjetivo. Psico, 43(3), 280–288. Retrieved from https://revistaseletronicas.pucrs.br/index.php/revistapsico/article/view/8263
    » https://revistaseletronicas.pucrs.br/index.php/revistapsico/article/view/8263
  • Yamada, Y., Ćepulić, DB., Coll-Martín, T., Debove, S., Gautreau, G., Han, H., Rasmussen, J., Tran, T. P., Travaglino, G. A., & Lieberoth, A. (2021). COVIDiSTRESS Global Survey Dataset Psychological and Behavioural Consequences of the COVID-19 Outbreak. Scientific Data, 8(1), 1–23. DOI: 10.1038/s41597-020-00784-9
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/s41597-020-00784-9
  • Yu, S. C., Chen, H. R., Liu, A. C., & Lee, H. Y. (2020). Toward COVID-19 Information: Infodemic or Fear of Missing Out? Healthcare, 8(4), 1–8. Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute. DOI: 10.3390/healthcare8040550
    » https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare8040550
  • Zanon, C., Bardagi, M. P., Layous, K., & Hutz, C. S. (2014). Validation of the Satisfaction with Life Scale to Brazilians: Evidences of measurement noninvariance across Brazil and US. Social Indicators Research, 119(1), 443–453. DOI:10.1007/s11205-013-0478-5.
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-013-0478-5
  • Zanon, C., & Hutz, C.S. (2014). Escala de afetos positivos e afetos negativos (PANAS). In C. S. Hutz (Ed.), Avaliação em psicologia positiva (pp. 63–67). Artmed.

Edited by

Desk Review Editor: Nelia Del Bianco

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    17 June 2022
  • Date of issue
    Jan-Apr 2022

History

  • Received
    02 Dec 2021
  • Reviewed
    19 Jan 2022
  • Reviewed
    10 Feb 2022
  • Accepted
    14 Feb 2022
Associação Brasileira de Pesquisadores em Jornalismo (SBPJor) Secretaria da SBPJor, Faculdade de Comunicação, Universidade de Brasília(UnB)., ICC Norte, Subsolo, Sala ASS 633 - cep: 70910-900, Brasília - DF / Brasil - Brasília - DF - Brazil
E-mail: sbpjor.dir.adm@gmail.com