Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Transcending the problem: perceptions of mothers and children about the impact of developmental coordination disorder in everyday life

Abstract

Introduction

Most of the population can carry out daily activities with competence. The profile of some children who have difficulty doing typical activities of childhood is well documented in the literature. Children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) demonstrate a marked effort in the development of motor skills, with possible repercussions on mental health.

Objective

To investigate the perceptions of mothers and their children about the impacts caused by DCD on everyday life; to explore the relations between the occupations, skills, and the environment; to investigate potential influences of the disorder on children’s self-concept and self-efficacy.

Method

This is an exploratory cross-sectional study with a qualitative approach involving three mothers and three children. Data were analyzed using content analysis.

Results

Three categories emerged: play and doing opportunities and impacts in school activities. Mothers perceived difficulties on occupational performance of their children, especially in school activities. Social context's characteristics are related to the offer of play opportunities for the children. The children understand that they have some difficulties, and one child identifies peer problems.

Conclusion

This study can highlight the importance of the recognition of DCD and its co-occurrence with learning disability as a persistent problem that affects children’s and their families’ everyday life, and the role of environmental factors on the opportunities children have to play.

Keywords:
Motor Skill Disorders; Activities of Daily Living; Child; Mothers; Perception; Social Environment

Resumo

Introdução

A maior parte da população é capaz de executar movimentos e atividades cotidianas com relativa competência. É bem documentado na literatura o perfil de algumas crianças que têm muita dificuldade para realizar atividades consideradas típicas da infância. Crianças com transtorno do desenvolvimento da coordenação (TDC) demonstram prejuízo acentuado no desenvolvimento das habilidades motoras, com possíveis repercussões na saúde física e mental.

Objetivo

Investigar as percepções da díade mãe-criança sobre os prováveis impactos do TDC no desempenho ocupacional das crianças e no dia a dia da família; explorar as relações existentes entre as ocupações, habilidades e ambiente; investigar a influência do transtorno no autoconceito e autoeficácia das crianças.

Método

Trata-se de um estudo exploratório de corte transversal, de abordagem qualitativa que contou com a participação de 3 mães e 3 crianças. A análise dos dados foi feita por análise de conteúdo.

Resultados

Duas categorias emergiram: oportunidades de brincar e de fazer e impactos na escola. As mães percebem as dificuldades relacionadas ao desempenho das ocupações das crianças, principalmente nas atividades escolares. Algumas questões do contexto social foram identificadas pelas mães como balizadoras das oportunidades de brincar de suas crianças. As crianças compreendem que têm algumas dificuldades e uma delas identifica problemas na relação com os colegas.

Conclusão

Acredita-se que o estudo possa contribuir para reforçar a necessidade de reconhecimento do TDC e sua co-ocorrência com dificuldades de aprendizagem como um problema persistente e impactante no cotidiano de crianças e suas famílias, e a importância de fatores ambientais como mediadores da oferta de oportunidades de brincar das crianças.

Palavras-chave:
Transtornos das Habilidades Motoras; Atividades Cotidianas; Criança; Mães; Percepção; Meio Social

1 Introduction

Since the middle of the twentieth-century human development has been understood by many theorists as a multidimensional process (SPENCER; PERONE; BUSS, 2011SPENCER, J. P.; PERONE, S.; BUSS, A. T. Twenty years and going strong: A dynamic systems revolution in motor and cognitive development. Child Development Perspectives, Malden, v. 5, n. 4, p. 260-266, 2011.). From the perspective of the contemporary theories, the child development has been studied from an ecological and dynamic perspective and, rather than characterizing what changes with the development, there is a tendency to understand how developmental changes occur (BRONFENBRENNER; CECI, 1994BRONFENBRENNER, U.; CECI, S. J. Nature-nuture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychological review, Washington, v. 101, n. 4, p. 568-586, 1994.; THELEN, 1995THELEN, E. Motor development: A new synthesis. American psychologist, Washington, v. 50, n. 2, p. 79-95, 1995.; SPENCER; PERONE; BUSS, 2011SPENCER, J. P.; PERONE, S.; BUSS, A. T. Twenty years and going strong: A dynamic systems revolution in motor and cognitive development. Child Development Perspectives, Malden, v. 5, n. 4, p. 260-266, 2011.). There is also a valorization in the relationships between the child development and the cultural traditions, practices, places and historical context in which children's activities are experienced (HEDEGAARD, 2012HEDEGAARD, M. The dynamic aspects in children’s learning and development. In: HEDEGAARD, M.; EDWARDS A.; FLEER, M. (Ed.). Motives in children’s development: cultural-historical approaches. New York: Cambridge University Press, 2012. p. 9-27.).

The care given to children is also influenced by cultural, socioeconomic, and family structure factors (FERREIRA et al., 2018FRANCA, A. S.; CARDOSO, A. A.; ARAÚJO, C. R. S. Problemas de coordenação motora e de atenção em crianças em idade escolar. Revista de Terapia Ocupacional da Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, v. 28, n. 1, p. 86-92, 2017.; FREITAS et al., 2013FREITAS, T. C. B. et al. Family socioeconomic status and the provision of motor affordances in the home. Brazilian Journal of Physical Therapy, São Carlos, v. 17, n. 4, p. 319-327, 2013.). The development occurs from the interaction of relationships experienced in the individual's daily life mediated by culture and socio-historical context (NELSON; IWAMA, 2010NELSON, A.; IWAMA, M. Cultural influences and occupation-centred practice with children and families. In: RODGER, S. (Ed.). Occupation-centred practice with children: a practical guide for occupational therapists. Oxford: John Wiley & Sons, 2010. p. 75-96.; HEDEGAARD, 2012HEDEGAARD, M. The dynamic aspects in children’s learning and development. In: HEDEGAARD, M.; EDWARDS A.; FLEER, M. (Ed.). Motives in children’s development: cultural-historical approaches. New York: Cambridge University Press, 2012. p. 9-27.). Within these perspectives, Bronfenbrenner's ecological approach assists in understanding that the individual is in constant interaction with their relationships and social contexts, that is, genetic material does not produce finished traits, but interacts with environmental experience in determining the results of the development (BRONFENBRENNER; CECI, 1994BRONFENBRENNER, U.; CECI, S. J. Nature-nuture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychological review, Washington, v. 101, n. 4, p. 568-586, 1994.). Bronfenbrenner (1979)BRONFENBRENNER, U. Ecological models of human development. Readings on the development of children, New York, v. 2, n. 1, p. 37-43, 1979. characterizes the context as an event or condition outside the organism that can influence or be influenced by the developing person. The bio-ecological approach understands the context as influential potentializing opportunities in the development of the individual and that all relationships (child, family, society) are interconnected (BENETTI et al., 2013BENETTI, C. I. et al. Fundamentos de teoria bioecológica de Urie Bronfenbrenner. Pensando Psicología, Bogotá, v. 9, n. 16, p. 89-99, 2013.; BRONFENBRENNER, 1979BRONFENBRENNER, U. Ecological models of human development. Readings on the development of children, New York, v. 2, n. 1, p. 37-43, 1979.).

The development of human movement happens similarly anywhere on the planet since most of the population can perform everyday movements and activities with relative competence: dressing, eating, playing, walking are some of the activities we can do with little or no formal instruction (DANTAS; MANOEL, 2009DANTAS, L. E. B. P. T.; MANOEL, E. J. Crianças com dificuldades motoras: questões para a conceituação do transtorno do desenvolvimento da coordenação. Movimento, Porto Alegre, v. 15, n. 3, p. 293-313, 2009.). However, some children have a lot of difficulties to perform these activities, and their motor skills are different from their peers but do not have classic neurological signs that can explain the problem (MAGALHÃES; CARDOSO; MISSIUNA, 2011MAGALHÃES, L.; CARDOSO, A.; MISSIUNA, C. Activities and participation in children with developmental coordination disorder: A systematic review. Research in Developmental Disabilities, New York, v. 32, n. 4, p. 1309-1316, 2011.). Currently, the term developmental coordination disorder (BDD) is considered the most appropriate to refer to these children (MAGALHÃES; CARDOSO; MISSIUNA, 2011MAGALHÃES, L.; CARDOSO, A.; MISSIUNA, C. Activities and participation in children with developmental coordination disorder: A systematic review. Research in Developmental Disabilities, New York, v. 32, n. 4, p. 1309-1316, 2011.). The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) establishes four criteria for diagnosing DCD considering children who at the beginning of development have substantially lower acquisition and performance of coordinated motor skills than expected considering their chronological age and level of intelligence and the opportunity to learn and use the skill in the absence of neurological or physical problems (ASSOCIAÇÃO..., 2014).

The prevalence of DCD commonly reported in the literature is 5% to 6% of school-age children (BLANK et al., 2019BLANK, R. et al. International clinical practice recommendations on the definition, diagnosis, assessment, intervention, and psychosocial aspects of developmental coordination disorder. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, London, v. 61, n. 3, p. 242-285, 2019.). Brazilian studies reported rates ranging from 3% to 43%, depending on the region of the country, assessments used and cutoff point established depending on the motor test used (BARBA et al., 2017BARBA, P. C. S. D. et al. Prevalence of Developmental Coordination Disorder signs in children 5 to 14 years in São Carlos. Motricidade, Ribeira de Pena, v. 13, n. 3, p. 22-30, 2017.; BELTRAME et al., 2017BELTRAME, T. S. et al. Prevalência do Transtorno do Desenvolvimento da Coordenação em uma amostra de crianças brasileiras. Cadernos de Terapia Ocupacional da UFSCar, São Carlos, v. 25, n. 1, p. 105-113, 2017.; FRANCA; CARDOSO; ARAÚJO, 2017FERREIRA, L. et al. Motor development in school‐age children is associated with the home environment including socioeconomic status. Child: Care, Health and Development, Oxford, v. 44, n. 6, p. 801-806, 2018.). The possible causes of DCD are still poorly understood, although it is considered as a neurodevelopmental disorder that leads to central nervous system problems, implying functional consequences (ASSOCIAÇÃO..., 2014; ZWICKER; HARRIS; KLASSEN, 2013ZWICKER, J.; HARRIS, S.; KLASSEN, A. Quality of life domains affected in children with developmental coordination disorder: a systematic review. Child: Care, Health and Development, Oxford, v. 39, n. 4, p. 562-580, 2013.). Boys and children with a history of prematurity and very low birth weight are more likely to develop DCD (ASSOCIAÇÃO..., 2014; ZWICKER et al., 2013ZWICKER, J. et al. Perinatal and neonatal predictors of developmental coordination disorder in very low birthweight children. Archives of Disease in Childhood, London, v. 98, n. 2, p. 118-22, 2013.). Also, motor problems are often associated with learning disabilities and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (GOULARDINS et al., 2015GOULARDINS, J. B. et al. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and developmental coordination disorder: Two separate disorders or do they share a common etiology. Behavioural brain research, Amsterdam, v. 292, p. 484-492, 2015.; OLIVEIRA; NETO; PALHARES, 2018OLIVEIRA, C. C.; NETO, J. L. C.; PALHARES, M. S. Características motoras de escolares com Transtorno de Déficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade. Cadernos de Terapia Ocupacional da UFSCar, São Carlos, v. 26, n. 3, p. 590-600, 2018.).

These children are sometimes called as “lazy,” “sloppy,” “slow,” and they often fail to complete tasks at home and at school (SEGAL et al., 2002SEGAL, R.; MANDICH, A.; POLATAJKO, H. Stigma and its management: A framework for understanding social isolation of children with developmental coordination disorder. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, New York, v. 56, n. 4, p. 422-428, 2002.; MISSIUNA et al., 2006MISSIUNA, C. et al. Mysteries and mazes: parents’ experiences of children with developmental coordination disorder. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, Ottawa, v. 73, n. 1, p. 7-17, 2006.; MAGALHÃES; CARDOSO; MISSIUNA, 2011MAGALHÃES, L.; CARDOSO, A.; MISSIUNA, C. Activities and participation in children with developmental coordination disorder: A systematic review. Research in Developmental Disabilities, New York, v. 32, n. 4, p. 1309-1316, 2011.). Children with DCD tend to avoid activities that require more body effort and more difficult to do because the repeated failure to perform them (CAIRNEY et al., 2012CAIRNEY, J. et al. Developmental coordination disorder, gender, and body weight: examining the impact of participation in active play. Research in Developmental Disabilities, New York, v. 33, n. 5, p. 1566-1573, 2012.). Children with DCD can avoid participating in more active games, physical education classes, and may lead to repercussions on physical and mental health, such as low self-esteem, poor motivation, social isolation and depression (CAIRNEY et al., 2012CAIRNEY, J. et al. Developmental coordination disorder, gender, and body weight: examining the impact of participation in active play. Research in Developmental Disabilities, New York, v. 33, n. 5, p. 1566-1573, 2012.; MISSIUNA; CAMPBELL, 2014MISSIUNA, C.; CAMPBELL, W. N. Psychological aspects of developmental coordination disorder: can we establish causality? Current Developmental Disorders Reports, Toronto, v. 1, n. 2, p. 125-131, 2014.; CAPISTRANO et al., 2015CAPISTRANO, R. et al. Transtorno do desenvolvimento da coordenação e nível de atividade física em crianças: revisão sistemática da literatura. Cadernos de Terapia Ocupacional da UFSCar, São Carlos, v. 23, n. 3, p. 633-646, 2015.).

As a key point in recognizing DCD, there are difficulties in performing activities that the child is expected to do or want to do, but he cannot do them (SMITS-ENGELSMAN et al., 2015SMITS-ENGELSMAN, B. et al. Diagnostic criteria for DCD: Past and future. Human Movement Science, Amsterdam, v. 42, p. 293-306, 2015.). In this sense, children with DCD represent a considerable portion of the total referrals for occupational therapy (BLANK et al., 2019BLANK, R. et al. International clinical practice recommendations on the definition, diagnosis, assessment, intervention, and psychosocial aspects of developmental coordination disorder. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, London, v. 61, n. 3, p. 242-285, 2019.). From the perspective of the Canadian Model of Occupational Performance and Engagement (CMOP-E), occupational therapy is concerned with occupational performance and with transformation, meaning, identity, engagement, balance, and occupational justice (TOWNSEND; POLATAJKO, 2013TOWNSEND, E. A.; POLATAJKO, H. J. Enabling occupation II: Advancing an occupational therapy vision for health, well-being, and justice through occupation. Ottawa: CAOT Publications ACE, 2013.). Occupational performance is defined as the actual execution of occupation, while occupational competence is the adequacy or sufficiency of an occupational skill satisfying the requirements of the environment (TOWNSEND; POLATAJKO, 2013TOWNSEND, E. A.; POLATAJKO, H. J. Enabling occupation II: Advancing an occupational therapy vision for health, well-being, and justice through occupation. Ottawa: CAOT Publications ACE, 2013.). Engagement is defined as getting involved in a situation, participating (TOWNSEND; POLATAJKO, 2013TOWNSEND, E. A.; POLATAJKO, H. J. Enabling occupation II: Advancing an occupational therapy vision for health, well-being, and justice through occupation. Ottawa: CAOT Publications ACE, 2013.). Engagement in occupation involves dynamic interdependence between the person, environment, and occupation.

DCD compromises the child's engagement in occupations, and these problems transcend the diagnosis of a motor disorder and may lead to social exclusion, low self-esteem, delayed learning, and problems in the family context (MISSIUNA; CAMPBELL, 2014MISSIUNA, C.; CAMPBELL, W. N. Psychological aspects of developmental coordination disorder: can we establish causality? Current Developmental Disorders Reports, Toronto, v. 1, n. 2, p. 125-131, 2014.). Children with DCD do not participate in the same activities as other children and may fail to explore their skills and may not be motivated to participate in new opportunities offered to them (ENGEL-YEGER; HANNA-KASIS, 2010ENGEL-YEGER, B.; HANNA-KASIS, A. The relationship between developmental coordination disordes, child’s perceived self-efficacy and preference to participate in daily activities. Child: Care, Health and Development, Oxford, v. 36, n. 5, p. 670-677, 2010.; WAGNER et al., 2012WAGNER, M. O. et al. Peer problems mediate the relationship between developmental coordination disorder and behavioral problems in school-aged children. Research in developmental disabilities, New York, v. 33, n. 6, p. 2072-2079, 2012.; MISSIUNA; CAMPBELL, 2014MISSIUNA, C.; CAMPBELL, W. N. Psychological aspects of developmental coordination disorder: can we establish causality? Current Developmental Disorders Reports, Toronto, v. 1, n. 2, p. 125-131, 2014.). These issues are independent of socioeconomic and cultural conditions (ASSOCIAÇÃO..., 2014). Brazilian research groups have been investigating the impact of contextual factors such as environment and socioeconomic level on motor and cognitive development and performance, and the impacts on the perception of skills and motivation to learn and on the self-concept formation of children with DCD (BOBBIO et al., 2010BOBBIO, T. G. et al. Interlimb coordination differentiates Brazilian children from two socioeconomic settings. Pediatrics international, Tokyo, v. 52, n. 3, p. 353-357, 2010.; VALENTINI et al., 2016VALENTINI, N. C. et al. Fundamental motor skills across childhood: Age, sex, and competence outcomes of Brazilian children. Journal of Motor Learning and Development, Tennessee, v. 4, n. 1, p. 16-36, 2016.; COUTINHO et al., 2017COUTINHO, M. T.; SOUZA, M. S.; VALENTINI, N. C. Crianças com desordem coordenativa desenvolvimental percebem-se menos competente e evidenciam autoconceito fragilizado. Revista Brasileira de Ciência e Movimento, Brasília, v. 24, n. 4, p. 5-15, 2017.; NOBRE et al., 2018NOBRE, G. C.; VALENTINI, N. C.; NOBRE, F. S. S. Fundamental motor skills, nutritional status, perceived competence, and school performance of Brazilian children in social vulnerability: Gender comparison. Child Abuse & Neglect, Oxford, v. 80, p. 335-345, 2018.).

The self-efficacy or perceived competence construct reflects the ability to assess their ability to perform a particular task (MISSIUNA et al., 2006MISSIUNA, C. et al. Mysteries and mazes: parents’ experiences of children with developmental coordination disorder. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, Ottawa, v. 73, n. 1, p. 7-17, 2006.). The perception of self-efficacy determines the activities they participate, the energy they use, and the degree of perseverance in the adversity and also predicts goals that individuals can set for themselves in their personal growth (BANDURA, 1997BANDURA, A. Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W.H. Freeman and Company, 1997.). In general, a strong sense of self-efficacy can be critical to achieving success they want to achieve (MISSIUNA et al., 2006MISSIUNA, C. et al. Mysteries and mazes: parents’ experiences of children with developmental coordination disorder. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, Ottawa, v. 73, n. 1, p. 7-17, 2006.). Besides being important to self-evaluate and identify therapy goals (MISSIUNA et al., 2006MISSIUNA, C. et al. Mysteries and mazes: parents’ experiences of children with developmental coordination disorder. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, Ottawa, v. 73, n. 1, p. 7-17, 2006.), the perception of self-efficacy of children with DCD can influence choices and preferences for certain activities (ENGEL-YEGER; HANNA-KASIS, 2010ENGEL-YEGER, B.; HANNA-KASIS, A. The relationship between developmental coordination disordes, child’s perceived self-efficacy and preference to participate in daily activities. Child: Care, Health and Development, Oxford, v. 36, n. 5, p. 670-677, 2010.). The lower the self-efficacy, the worse the motor performance and the more restricted the activities in which children with DCD prefer to participate (ENGEL-YEGER; HANNA-KASIS, 2010ENGEL-YEGER, B.; HANNA-KASIS, A. The relationship between developmental coordination disordes, child’s perceived self-efficacy and preference to participate in daily activities. Child: Care, Health and Development, Oxford, v. 36, n. 5, p. 670-677, 2010.). Thus, children with DCD can avoid participating in activities not only because of motor difficulties but also because of their low sense of self-efficacy (ENGEL-YEGER; HANNA-KASIS, 2010ENGEL-YEGER, B.; HANNA-KASIS, A. The relationship between developmental coordination disordes, child’s perceived self-efficacy and preference to participate in daily activities. Child: Care, Health and Development, Oxford, v. 36, n. 5, p. 670-677, 2010.).

The self-concept and self-perception of the child are characterized as a set of cognitive attributions, such as personal and behavioral characteristics in everyday situations (OLIVEIRA; MATSUKURA; FONTAINE, 2017OLIVEIRA, A. K. C.; MATSUKURA, T. S.; FONTAINE, A. M. Autoconceito e autoeficácia em crianças com deficiência física: revisão sistemática da literatura. Revista Brasileira de Educação Especial, Bauru, v. 23, n. 1, p. 145-160, 2017.). This view varies according to the considerations of the individual's perceptions of himself and, as a consequence, of the judgment that the others make of him, mainly influenced by the interactions established in the family, social, and school contexts (OLIVEIRA; MATSUKURA; FONTAINE, 2017OLIVEIRA, A. K. C.; MATSUKURA, T. S.; FONTAINE, A. M. Autoconceito e autoeficácia em crianças com deficiência física: revisão sistemática da literatura. Revista Brasileira de Educação Especial, Bauru, v. 23, n. 1, p. 145-160, 2017.). Galvão et al. (2014a)GALVÃO, B. A. P. et al. Perspectiva dos pais sobre as consequências funcionais do Transtorno do Desenvolvimento da Coordenação (TDC): revisão da literatura. Cadernos de Terapia Ocupacional da UFSCar, São Carlos, v. 22, n. 1, p. 187-193, 2014a. reviewed studies on parents' perception of the routine especially at home and at school of their children with signs or already diagnosed with DCD. These parents talked about differences in their children's motor behavior and reported restrictions on participation in typical childhood activities. The socialization process and the lack of preparedness of the education system to deal with children's difficulties were among the concerns of theses respondents (GALVÃO et al., 2014aGALVÃO, B. A. P. et al. Perspectiva dos pais sobre as consequências funcionais do Transtorno do Desenvolvimento da Coordenação (TDC): revisão da literatura. Cadernos de Terapia Ocupacional da UFSCar, São Carlos, v. 22, n. 1, p. 187-193, 2014a.).

As all the reviewed studies were international, the authors warn of the importance of researching the perception of mothers of Brazilian children with DCD (GALVÃO et al., 2014aGALVÃO, B. A. P. et al. Perspectiva dos pais sobre as consequências funcionais do Transtorno do Desenvolvimento da Coordenação (TDC): revisão da literatura. Cadernos de Terapia Ocupacional da UFSCar, São Carlos, v. 22, n. 1, p. 187-193, 2014a.). Thus, they interviewed five caregivers of children identified with the disorder, and the results showed that the problems of motor coordination are still poorly recognized, and the problems resulting from academic difficulties are more impactful (GALVÃO et al., 2014bGALVÃO, B. A. P. et al. Percepção materna do desempenho de crianças com transtorno do desenvolvimento da coordenação. Psicologia em Estudo, Maringá, v. 19, n. 3, p. 527-538, 2014b.).

Seeking to contribute to the literature in this area and moving towards understanding the point of view of mothers and children about motor difficulties, we start from the following research questions: what is the perception of mothers and their children about the possible impacts of DCD in daily life? Does the disorder influence children's self-concept and self-efficacy? This study aimed to investigate the perceptions of the mother-child dyad about the likely impacts of DCD on children's occupational performance and family daily life; and, as specific objectives: (1) to explore the relationships between children's occupations, skills and environment; (2) to investigate the possible impacts of the disorder on children's self-concept and self-efficacy formation.

2 Method

2.1 Design

This is an exploratory cross-sectional study that investigated the perception of children and their mothers on the possible impacts of DCD in the family's daily life. We chose the qualitative methodology because it is a more appropriate approach to explore people's interpretations of the way they live, think, and perceive reality (MINAYO et al., 2002MINAYO, M. C. S. et al. Pesquisa social: teoria, método e criatividade. Petrópolis: Editora Vozes Limitada, 2002.). This research is part of a larger project that investigated the relationship between central auditory processing, sensory processing, and motor coordination in school-age children in the process of finalization, approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Medical School of the Federal University of São Paulo. (CAAE 61403916.1.0000.0065; Opinion 1.856.907).

2.2 Participants

The largest study included 72 children, seven to ten years old, from two public schools in the city of João Pessoa, Paraíba. This study involved two municipal public elementary schools that agreed to participate. The age range of children refers to a stage of increased school demand, contributing to the possible recognition of academic problems and identification of their causes.

Children were initially evaluated with: (1) the - MABC-2 Movement Assessment Battery for Children 2 (HENDERSON et al., 2007HENDERSON, S.; SUGDEN, D.; BARNETT, A. Movement assessment battery for children-2: Examiner's manual. São Paulo: Pearson, 2007.) to assess their motor performance through direct observation. Children with percentile less or equal to 15 presented motor problems, and the percentile equal to 5 was considered the most severe condition of DCD; (2) Raven's Color Progressive Matrices, standardized for Brazilian children (ANGELINI et al., 1999ANGELINI, A. L. et al. Manual matrizes progressiva coloridas de Raven: escala especial. São Paulo: CETEPP, 1999.) to assess their intellectual development. It classifies the cognitive level as intellectually superior, definitely above average, intellectually average, definitely below average, intellectually deficient; (3) Swanson, Nolan and Phelham (SNAP-IV) Brazilian version (MATTOS et al., 2006MATTOS, P. et al. Apresentação de uma versão em português para uso no Brasil do instrumento MTA-SNAP-IV de avaliação de sintomas de transtorno do déficit de atenção/hiperatividade e sintomas de transtorno desafiador e de oposição. Revista de Psiquiatria do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, v. 28, n. 3, p. 290-297, 2006.), to assess signs of ADHD, filled by the caregivers; (4) Sensory Profile 2, version in the process of translation and cross-cultural adaptation (DUNN, 2014DUNN, W. Perfil sensorial 2. São Paulo: Pearson, 2014.), to characterize sensory processing; (5) data record form developed for the study of the child's growth and development history and data on socioeconomic and environmental conditions.

The caregivers were informed about the nature of the research and the possibility of the child presenting motor performance below the expected for his age, and in these circumstances, he would be called for interview and guidance. After agreeing to participate, the caregivers signed the informed consent form, and the children signed the consent form. After this battery of evaluations for the larger study, children invited to this study were eligible according to the following inclusion criteria: a) percentile equal and lower than 15 in the MABC-2 test; b) category equal or greater than III (intellectually average) in RAVEN. The exclusion criteria were: diagnosis of clinical conditions or neurological diseases; delay of more than one school year which could indicate other problems.

We identified seven children with motor performance below the expected for their age, but without a confirmed diagnosis, who, together with their mothers, were invited to participate in this study. Four mother-child dyads were excluded due to non-attendance despite two attempts at new appointments or, due to the impossibility of contact, all sources were exhausted. In the end, three mothers and three children agreed to participate in this study (Figure 1).

Figure 1
Flowchart of participant selection.

2.3 Data collection procedures

Eligible mothers and children were contacted by telephone, informed of their children's performance on the tests, and invited to attend the Clinical School of Occupational Therapy at the Federal University of Paraíba in July and August 2018 to be interviewed for the impacts of the child's difficulties in daily life and to receive guidance on how to improve their children's development and performance.

For the sample characterization, the mothers answered the Brazil Economic Classification Criterion - CCEB of the Brazilian Association of Research Companies (ABEP) (BRASIL, 2014BRASIL. Critério de Classificação Econômica Brasil: cortes do critério. São Paulo: Associação Brasileira de Empresas de Pesquisa, 2014.) to estimate the economic level of the family. This questionnaire estimates the purchasing power of urban people and families, providing the classification of the population into economic classes. The CCEB scoring system is divided into two categories: item ownership and education of the head of family and access to public services. The sum of the total score defines which of the six classes the family is (A - around R$ 20,272.56; B1 - around R$ 8,695.88; B2 - around R$ 4,427.36; C1 - around R$ 2,409.01; C2 - around R$ 1,446.24; DE - around R$ 639.78).

The mothers answered the Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire Brazilian version (DCDQ-Brazil) to confirm the difficulties in the child's performance in activities of daily living, school, play, and leisure (Criterion B suggestive of DCD according to DSM-V) (PRADO; MAGALHÃES; WILSON, 2009PRADO, M.; MAGALHÃES, L.; WILSON, B. Cross-cultural adaptation of the Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire for Brazilian children. Revista Brasileira de Fisioterapia, São Carlos, v. 13, n. 3, p. 236-243, 2009.). The DCDQ-Brazil has 15 items distributed in the following sessions: control during movement, fine motor skills, and writing, gross motor skills, and planning, global motor coordination. The items are scored on a five-point scale, with a maximum score of 75. The higher the score, the better the child's performance in the activity according to the respondent. Although there are no Brazilian standards for the DCDQ-Brazil, the proposed cutoff points for Canadian children have been used, as the performance averages are similar in both countries (PRADO; MAGALHÃES; WILSON, 2009PRADO, M.; MAGALHÃES, L.; WILSON, B. Cross-cultural adaptation of the Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire for Brazilian children. Revista Brasileira de Fisioterapia, São Carlos, v. 13, n. 3, p. 236-243, 2009.). The child has motor problems that are reflecting on the performance of activities if: from five to seven years and 11 months old has a total score of 0-46; from eight to nine years and 11 months old, obtained a total score of 0-55; from 10 to 15 years and six months old, obtain a total score of 0-57.

Mothers and children were interviewed at the same time in different rooms by two researchers (CL and CA). A guiding script was elaborated for the semi-structured interview so that mothers and children could talk about their perceptions about the impacts of DCD on the daily lives of children and their families. This script was built according to data obtained in the literature (GALVÃO et al., 2014bGALVÃO, B. A. P. et al. Percepção materna do desempenho de crianças com transtorno do desenvolvimento da coordenação. Psicologia em Estudo, Maringá, v. 19, n. 3, p. 527-538, 2014b.) and based on the interviewer's experience (CA) with children with DCD and their families (ARAÚJO; MAGALHÃES; CARDOSO, 2011; ARAÚJO; CARDOSO; MAGALHÃES, 2017ARAÚJO, C. R. S.; CARDOSO, A. A.; MAGALHÃES, L. C. Efficacy of the cognitive orientation to daily occupational performance with Brazilian children with developmental coordination disorder. Scandinavian Journal of Occupational Therapy, Abingdon, v. 20, p. 1-9, 2017.). Initially, mothers were encouraged to talk about how the child did the homework, how he get organized, get dressed, take a shower, help with housework, and then about the child at school, playing, and relating to friends. The questions were triggered as the mother talked to the interviewer so that they could talk freely about the topic.

To investigate whether motor difficulties were identified and perceived by children, the methodological technique was different: a free-drawing activity was proposed to the child, while the researcher (CL) conducted an open interview to make the child comfortable to talk about it. Oliveira (2012)OLIVEIRA, M. M. Como fazer pesquisa qualitativa. Petrópolis: Vozes, 2012. understands research as a creative act and that the researcher can use instruments appropriate to his or her object of study without using a way to label realities (OLIVEIRA, 2012OLIVEIRA, M. M. Como fazer pesquisa qualitativa. Petrópolis: Vozes, 2012.). Also, interviewing children is a challenge, as it is necessary to think of innovative ways for the child to interact with the researcher and express their ideas. This allows the child to contribute to the researcher's understanding of their ways of seeing and thinking about their life contexts (FOLQUE, 2010FOLQUE, M. A. Interviewing young children. In: NAUGHTON, G. M.; ROLFE, S. A.; SIRAJ-BLATCHFORD, I. (Ed.). Doing early childhood research. International Perspectives on Theory and Practice. Berkshire England: Open University Press McGraw-Hill Education, 2010. p. 239-260.).

The interviews were recorded on audio equipment (Samsung Galaxy Tab 4 Tablet, via Voice Recorder application) with the permission of the participants. The interview time with both mothers and children ranged from 30 to 40 minutes (total of 126 hours and 31 minutes of recording). Then, the interviews were fully transcribed by the researcher (CL).

2.4 Data analysis

We opted for content analysis to allow the researcher to find answers to the questions asked, confirming or not the hypotheses established before the investigation, and find out what is behind the manifested content (GOMES, 2002GOMES, R. A análise de dados em pesquisa qualitativa. In: MINAYO, M. C. S. et al. Pesquisa social: teoria, método e criatividade. Petrópolis: Editora Vozes Limitada, 2002. p. 67-80.). The analysis process was conducted in stages: pre-analysis, coding (raw data transformation, clipping, aggregation, and enumeration for analysis organization) and definitions of different categories (GOMES, 2002GOMES, R. A análise de dados em pesquisa qualitativa. In: MINAYO, M. C. S. et al. Pesquisa social: teoria, método e criatividade. Petrópolis: Editora Vozes Limitada, 2002. p. 67-80.; MINAYO et al., 2002MINAYO, M. C. S. et al. Pesquisa social: teoria, método e criatividade. Petrópolis: Editora Vozes Limitada, 2002.; BARDIN, 2010BARDIN, L. Análise de Conteúdo. Lisboa: Edições 70, 2010.).

The theoretical references used were the bio-ecological theory of Bronfenbrenner (BRONFENBRENNER, 1979BRONFENBRENNER, U. Ecological models of human development. Readings on the development of children, New York, v. 2, n. 1, p. 37-43, 1979.; BRONFENBRENNER; CECI, 1994BRONFENBRENNER, U.; CECI, S. J. Nature-nuture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychological review, Washington, v. 101, n. 4, p. 568-586, 1994.) and the Canadian model of Occupational Performance and Engagement (CMOP-E) (TOWNSEND; POLATAJKO, 2013TOWNSEND, E. A.; POLATAJKO, H. J. Enabling occupation II: Advancing an occupational therapy vision for health, well-being, and justice through occupation. Ottawa: CAOT Publications ACE, 2013.) to understand the coping processes that the child with DCD and his family experience.

Bronfenbrenner's (1979)BRONFENBRENNER, U. Ecological models of human development. Readings on the development of children, New York, v. 2, n. 1, p. 37-43, 1979. bio-ecological approach explains that lack of opportunity can come from two central spheres: macrosystem and microsystem - by the impact of the cultural context, subculture, poverty, wealth, and by the situations the child has direct personal experience, such as family relationships. In the exosystem layer, situations not directly experienced by the child in daily life, but that affect the microsystem layer are considered; the family. According to this approach, the child's development occurs from the interaction of the concentric layers, which are situations experienced by the individual, in which all the influences of the environment are related to each other (BRONFENBRENNER, 1979BRONFENBRENNER, U. Ecological models of human development. Readings on the development of children, New York, v. 2, n. 1, p. 37-43, 1979.).

The CMOP-E contributed to the understanding that there is a dynamic interaction between the individual and the different perspectives of his life characterized as the triad: environment - occupation - person. Every individual (with its cognitive, affective, physical, and other components) is inserted in an environment - physical, cultural, social, or institutional - engaged in occupations (TOWNSEND; POLATAJKO, 2013TOWNSEND, E. A.; POLATAJKO, H. J. Enabling occupation II: Advancing an occupational therapy vision for health, well-being, and justice through occupation. Ottawa: CAOT Publications ACE, 2013.). Environmental factors - social and economic - may represent barriers or facilitators for children's engagement in activities meaningful to them. The CMOP-E understands that the environment influences human occupation and the person can adapt and acquire new skills for the new life contexts (TOWNSEND; POLATAJKO, 2013TOWNSEND, E. A.; POLATAJKO, H. J. Enabling occupation II: Advancing an occupational therapy vision for health, well-being, and justice through occupation. Ottawa: CAOT Publications ACE, 2013.).

3 Results and Discussion

The results will be presented along with the discussions from the interviews with mothers and children. The characterization of the participants was summarized in tables 1 and 2.

Table 1
Children's profile.
Table 2
Informant Profile.

From the analysis of the interviews, the themes that stood out among the speeches and guided the construction of the following categories emerged: offering opportunities to play and do, the impacts on the school, and, within this, the subcategory relationship with others.

3.1 Offering opportunities to play and do

The opportunities experienced by the children identified with DCD were a recurring theme. In general, all mothers perceived the influences of their children's difficulties and factors in the contexts in which they lived when offering opportunities to learn and practice daily skills.

No, I let him play at home, in my house, there is a video game there he plays with the boys, who come to play with him (M1).

No, she plays, she watches TV more and plays with her, she has one, she walks, it is what she is playing with now, the doll so much, she rips her head, rips arm and goes straight to the trash. Then now she is only with the walks, only plays with what she has (M2).

She plays with the girls, I just don't let her play with a boy anymore, I used to, but now I don't let her anymore. When she's playing with a boy, I put her inside. There are only two boys that I let her play who are my neighbors where I live because I live there for rent I live in a village, but in the middle of it is in the middle of the street playing with a boy I do not leave anymore. She plays with a girl, or she does not play with anyone; she stays indoors and plays alone (M2).

Given the factors that influence the opportunity for interviewed children to learn and develop skills, we obtained two parameters: 1) few opportunities - children 1 and 2 lived in a different context, with a high rate of socioeconomic vulnerability, violence, and hazardousness; 2) increased opportunities - child 3 lived in a context of restricted opportunities due to the physical space in which he was inserted and started to have opportunities with changing environment, that is, the child had an increased opportunity to experience situations not common before in his daily lives.

Until last year, we lived in my parents' apartment, which has no space; it has this detail... And as I spend the day working, I'm very careful that she doesn't fall, for her not ... to happen ... she has a bicycle, but I never let her use it, fearing because there is no space there, it's a garage, then it was very dangerous, or it was on the street or where the cars are. Then when it was from last year here, I got it, we managed to live in our apartment, it's not a mansion, but it's ours. Then there is a space, a better area, she has her room, she doesn't sleep in the room with me... And there is a space for children, a space to play, so she freer. Even though she is having a period like this, with my parents, a period I'm working on, but she plays with the other children at night, socializing better (M3).

Maior and Cândido (2014)MAIOR, M. M. S.; CÂNDIDO, G. A. Vulnerabilidade socioeconômica: um estudo transversal para o município de João Pessoa - PB. Revista Principia: divulgação científica e tecnológica do IFPB, João Pessoa, n. 24, p. 72-87, 2014. used the criteria (economic, social, and environmental) proposed by other Brazilian researchers to research on vulnerability and analyzed different places in the city where this study took place. The context in which children 1 and 2 lived was considered a community with high socioeconomic vulnerability, something that is reinforced in mothers M1 and M2 about the place, reporting and recognizing the place as violent and dangerous, being one of the barriers attributed by them to the participation of children in the community (MAIOR; CÂNDIDO, 2014MAIOR, M. M. S.; CÂNDIDO, G. A. Vulnerabilidade socioeconômica: um estudo transversal para o município de João Pessoa - PB. Revista Principia: divulgação científica e tecnológica do IFPB, João Pessoa, n. 24, p. 72-87, 2014.).

However, M3 lived in a gated community in a middle-class neighborhood, where child 3 began to experience opportunities from the change of context, including school. M3 referred to this change. Although this contextual issue has emerged, it is interesting to note that both M2, and children 2 and 3 stated that they play indoors. That is, in general, mothers and children stated on offering opportunities that their children were subjected - restriction and increase - are possibly mediated by the context factor, but it is noticeable that issues inherent to motor coordination problems and preferences for less active activities also influence the activities in which children choose to participate.

She improved on the activities a lot […] this year she made a very big leap, don't you know? (M3).

[…] she plays at home, now there are some girls there that she befriended, then she is playing more at home now, isn't she? When she gets home from school, but that's all (M2).

Playing in the small house, it's... Playing in the dollhouse (Child 2).

Barbie, at Barbie's house with my friends, I watch TV (Child 3).

Because sometimes I'm playing, then I get stumped at home, and I accidentally knock things over, my mom doesn't like it… (Child 2).

The relationship between physical and social context or environment and development seemed to be linked to issues related to DCD. In some situations, the children have the opportunity to experience playing in their daily lives, but the activities are more restricted to video games, dolls, and what is available to them more often at home (CAIRNEY et al., 2010CAIRNEY, J.; VELDHUIZEN, S.; SZATMARI, P. Motor coordination and emotional-behavioral problems in children. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, Philadelphia, v. 23, n. 4, p. 324-329, 2010.; ENGEL-YEGER; HANNA-KASIS, 2010ENGEL-YEGER, B.; HANNA-KASIS, A. The relationship between developmental coordination disordes, child’s perceived self-efficacy and preference to participate in daily activities. Child: Care, Health and Development, Oxford, v. 36, n. 5, p. 670-677, 2010.). These results showed the interrelationship between the microsystem and the mesosystem according to the ecological theory of Bronfenbrenner (1979)BRONFENBRENNER, U. Ecological models of human development. Readings on the development of children, New York, v. 2, n. 1, p. 37-43, 1979.. The child in a given context (micro) - his activities, roles, relationships, and physical characteristics - may or may not interact with another context - such as the neighborhood or community (meso-system) in which he/she is inserted to participate actively. The essential idea of ​​ecological theory is that human development is influenced by changes that occur in the cultural, social, economic environment and by life histories, experiences, beliefs, and behaviors (HADDAD, 1997HADDAD, L. A Ecologia da Educação Infantil: construindo um modelo de sistema unificado de cuidado e educação. 1997. 336 f. Tese (Doutorado em Educação) - Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, 1997.).

Children 1 and 2 recognized their motor difficulties and avoided games that require their motor skills and agility to run.

You don't like to run, don't you? No, miss. (laughs) Because it's boring, miss. Very! I fall too much.” He added: “No. I'm not much in the street; I play more with my friend in PlayStation 2, Mario. Maaaaaario ... I don't like to play on the street very much. Because it's boring, the person keeps falling, you get hurt. My mother said it's normal. I stay home, me and my friend (Child 1).

Because sometimes I'm playing, then I get stumped at home, and I inadvertently knock things over, my mother doesn't like it [] (Child 2).

The speech of child 1 reflects his behavior and situations that occurred even during the interview. In one of the episodes, the child got up to throw a paper in the trash and even fell by slightly lowering his head and legs to reach it. At this moment, the child fixed her eyes on the interviewer, lowered her head with a look of sadness, and said, “It's always like that, miss”.

The Canadian Occupational Performance and Engagement Model (CMOP-E) is a dynamic model that allows the occupational therapist to think about the interaction between the person, environment, and occupation, as well as occupational engagement, beyond competence in performing the tasks that make up human beings occupations (TOWNSEND; POLATAJKO, 2013TOWNSEND, E. A.; POLATAJKO, H. J. Enabling occupation II: Advancing an occupational therapy vision for health, well-being, and justice through occupation. Ottawa: CAOT Publications ACE, 2013.). Understanding how a person engages and participates in life situations is central. Children with DCD may have limitations in occupational engagement not only by physical motor coordination problems but by how the dynamic interaction of their characteristics (physical, affective, cognitive, beliefs) with the environment in which they live occurs. The cultural and social contexts such as family, neighborhood, and school also offer meanings to occupations and the way children perceived themselves in this interaction.

The child who has a perception of positive self-efficacy - affective domain in CMOP-E maintains the belief that he can achieve the desired results, able to make plans, and set goals related to the activities they want to perform. On the other hand, the perception of negative self-efficacy can lead the child to present problem abandonment behaviors, impacting on health and well-being in their life, influencing motivation and learning for the demands of activities and participation (OLIVEIRA; MATSUKURA; FONTAINE, 2017OLIVEIRA, A. K. C.; MATSUKURA, T. S.; FONTAINE, A. M. Autoconceito e autoeficácia em crianças com deficiência física: revisão sistemática da literatura. Revista Brasileira de Educação Especial, Bauru, v. 23, n. 1, p. 145-160, 2017.; ENGEL-YEGER; HANNA-KASIS, 2010ENGEL-YEGER, B.; HANNA-KASIS, A. The relationship between developmental coordination disordes, child’s perceived self-efficacy and preference to participate in daily activities. Child: Care, Health and Development, Oxford, v. 36, n. 5, p. 670-677, 2010.).

Besides the perception of self-efficacy, their self-concept can also affect the child's self-confidence system, thoughts, feelings, and actions, and limiting occupational engagement. Assuming that self-concept is also based on judgments that others make about the child, it is understood that caregivers, teachers, and others can also influence and impact on the child's perception of themselves and their abilities (OLIVEIRA; MATSUKURA; FONTAINE, 2017OLIVEIRA, A. K. C.; MATSUKURA, T. S.; FONTAINE, A. M. Autoconceito e autoeficácia em crianças com deficiência física: revisão sistemática da literatura. Revista Brasileira de Educação Especial, Bauru, v. 23, n. 1, p. 145-160, 2017.).

Children with DCD avoid participating in activities that require these skills and tend to show little or no interest in sports and games that require finer coarse or fine motor coordination (CAIRNEY et al., 2007CAIRNEY, J. et al. Developmental coordination disorder and reported enjoyment of physical education in children. European Physical Education Review, Chester, v. 13, n. 1, p. 81-98, 2007.). Therefore, child 1 suggested in his speech the interest in games that do not require more than he can, that is, the effort to perform motor skills in games that cause suffering and distress, maintaining the preference for video games and superhero games.

Ah, we play a lot of things […] Ah, we play a superhero, like ... It's easy, imagine that you're a […] We play things of heroes, like ... Naruto, do you know what naruto is? […] We play dragon bool, naruto, we play, of ... We play a loto f things, it is ... I almost don't know (Child 1).

However, children 2 and 3 preferred games that challenge their motor skills and have opportunities to experience them in their daily lives, both in the school context and in the community. According to Cairney et al. (2010)CAIRNEY, J.; VELDHUIZEN, S.; SZATMARI, P. Motor coordination and emotional-behavioral problems in children. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, Philadelphia, v. 23, n. 4, p. 324-329, 2010., deficits in the participation in organized activities and free games of boys with DCD tend to decrease over time, while in girls with DCD, participation tends to increase slightly (CAIRNEY et al., 2010CAIRNEY, J.; VELDHUIZEN, S.; SZATMARI, P. Motor coordination and emotional-behavioral problems in children. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, Philadelphia, v. 23, n. 4, p. 324-329, 2010.). However, in this study, child 1 (boy) reported a decreased participation in more active activities and perceived the interference of their difficulties, while children 2 and 3 (girls) tried to engage in organized activities and play freely at least in the school and community context.

Anything, hide and seek, touch game, touch ice ... […] I'm not very close to the ball (Child 2).

Hide and seek, it's ... Boy catches girl, it's ... Chess, we play with dolls at school […] soccer, I like to dodgeball too (Child 3).

The interrelationship between the opportunity and the routine also stood out in this category since to optimize time, avoid frustrations and hassles, mothers usually complete activities for their children. Children are also rated lazy about their performance on ADL. Initially, when asked about how their children performed their activities of daily living (ADL), the mother of child 1 was exhaustive:

It takes time... I put the clothes on him. […] Taking a shower, he takes like this, goes to the bathroom, gets wet, at the time of the shower, right? That he goes, pretty fast. Then I do it, let´s take a shower “N,” then sometimes he does not take a shower, then I go there and bath him. To wear the clothes sometimes it's the same way, he doesn't want to put the clothes on and get wrapped with the towel, so I go there and put the clothes on him, but sometimes I think it's his laziness, I don't know. I keep thinking like that, sometimes I think (M1).

[…] sometimes he puts it wrong […] it's not every time, but sometimes he puts it, then I say, it's wrong, take it, it's not like that, then he goes there and puts it right […] the clothes sometimes he worn, the shirt. Sometimes the shorts from back to front, front to back (M1).

As I lived at my parents' house, they woke up, they had to wake up very early, because the van was too early, I organized. I got her dressed […] (M3).

The change to child 3 happens when the mother stops doing the activities for her, offering the child the opportunity:

[…] Oh, as we were in our house, then I said, look, my daughter, your mother needs to rest, and you're about to do your own thing. Then she is right... like this, dressing alone, getting ready, choosing her clothes only […] when she got the shoes, I have to... tie them […] (M3).

In contrast, M2 reported that her daughter does her ADLs independently: “She puts on sneakers on her own, she ties them, she doesn't do well, but she does” (M2). The relationship between doing and the opportunities experienced in everyday life says a lot about the possibilities of using and experimenting with children's abilities and ability to perform them. In general, the care and time offered to children with DCD in day-to-day activities are greater than the time provided for typically developing children of the same age (SUMMERS; LARKIN; DEWEY, 2008SUMMERS, J.; LARKIN, D.; DEWEY, D. What impact does developmental coordination disorder have on daily routines? International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, Rio Claro, v. 55, n. 2, p. 131-141, 2008.). There were also reports of recognition of the problem due to not performing ADLs:

For me, I think it's… I don't know, right. Because for his age, right, this was not the time to do that, right (M1).

The difficulties when performing daily activities are not always related to the child's motor coordination problems. DCD is still poorly recognized and considered by parents and teachers when trying to explain children's performance. This is reinforced in the words of mothers: children are rated as lazy, disinterested, and stunned for not being able to perform the day-to-day activities that a child of the same age performs with mastery. By leaving this issue aside, problems can increase. Studies indicated that motor coordination problems are not brief and typical of childhood and may impact on adulthood (TAL-SABAN et al., 2012TAL-SABAN, M. et al. The functional profile of young adults with suspected Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD). Research in Developmental Disabilities, New York, v. 33, n. 6, p. 2193-2202, 2012.), leading to a higher rate of depression, social and emotional problems in individuals with DCD (CAIRNEY et al., 2010CAIRNEY, J.; VELDHUIZEN, S.; SZATMARI, P. Motor coordination and emotional-behavioral problems in children. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, Philadelphia, v. 23, n. 4, p. 324-329, 2010.; MISSIUNA; CAMPBELL, 2014MISSIUNA, C.; CAMPBELL, W. N. Psychological aspects of developmental coordination disorder: can we establish causality? Current Developmental Disorders Reports, Toronto, v. 1, n. 2, p. 125-131, 2014.).

The early identification of DCD is a means to predict the levels of activity participation of children diagnosed with the disorder, as they have barriers to participation and involvement in several activities such as self-care, academic tasks, play, leisure and social involvement (MAZER; DELLA BARBA, 2010MAZER, E. P.; DELLA BARBA, P. C. S. Identificação de sinais de Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Coordenação em crianças de três a seis anos e possibilidades de atuação da Terapia Ocupacional. Revista de Terapia Ocupacional da Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, v. 21, n. 1, p. 74-82, 2010.; ROSENBLUM; ENGEL-YEGER, 2014ROSENBLUM, S.; ENGEL-YEGER, B. Predicting participation in Children with DCD. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, Toronto, v. 1, n. 2, p. 109-117, 2014.). Parents often perceived some problems in performing common tasks; however, when reporting these concerns to healthcare professionals, DCD is disregarded for lack of awareness of the disorder (ROSENBLUM; ENGEL-YEGER, 2014ROSENBLUM, S.; ENGEL-YEGER, B. Predicting participation in Children with DCD. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, Toronto, v. 1, n. 2, p. 109-117, 2014.). Early identification is critical to preventing more serious emotional issues related to decreased success and participation in different environments.

Therefore, parents, caregivers, and teachers need to know about DCD and its impacts to early identify problems and act immediately (MAZER; DELLA BARBA, 2010MAZER, E. P.; DELLA BARBA, P. C. S. Identificação de sinais de Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Coordenação em crianças de três a seis anos e possibilidades de atuação da Terapia Ocupacional. Revista de Terapia Ocupacional da Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, v. 21, n. 1, p. 74-82, 2010.; OKUDA, 2015OKUDA, P. M. M. Intervenção e identificação precoce do transtorno do desenvolvimento da coordenação em escolares no início da alfabetização. 2015. 132 f. Dissertação (Mestrado em Fonoaudiologia) – Universidade Estadual Paulista, Marília, 2015.; ROSENBLUM; ENGEL-YEGER, 2014ROSENBLUM, S.; ENGEL-YEGER, B. Predicting participation in Children with DCD. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, Toronto, v. 1, n. 2, p. 109-117, 2014.).

3.2 Impacts on school

One of the criteria for DCD diagnosis is the significant interference with the performance or participation in activities of family, social, school, or community life. The difficulties of their children in performing school tasks, arousing their eyes, and pointing to some problems were the issue that most stood out in the interviews of mothers. The three mothers recognized the difficulties of their children but believed that is more for laziness and lack of interest in school activities.

The mothers of children 1 and 2 in their speeches shared the same anguish and concern about their children's behavior and learning at school.

I don't know if it is lazy. I don't know what it is. I don't know either, maybe the teacher, but also she says she talked to me yesterday and said she's not talking, but she said it's ... he's developing more because now she put her name there she goes there and starts writing very slowly, it takes only that (M1).

So far for me, like, she hasn't learned anything. Because she doesn't know the alphabet yet, she doesn't ... she's is not interested in that part. I find her disinterested because the teacher... from the beginning when I put her to study, she always joked, talked, she doesn't pay attention to what the teacher is teaching, she is disconnected. Her thing is giving attention to others. I was often called from the beginning when I put her to study on that because she stopped doing her things, to go help her classmates, so conversation, she got distracted, most of the time I was called at school, it was more so because she is so distracted (M2).

Because… she… the first year she went to private school, the principal wanted to fail. The private school, to price and such, also wanted her to repeat the first year. She didn't read. She had extra classes, but still she couldn't read. My mother is a teacher but had no patience, is already retired; she is elderly. When it was the sophomore year, she went to public school, all that adaptation, but she also left without being able to read. It greatly improved writing. When she entered the third year she stayed in school A she went to another school B, which she got, before the middle of the year was literate. That's why I say that was the leap. For math she has a... (M3).

All three children obtained a percentile 5 in the motor performance test, which indicated a more pronounced presence of motor problems, which interfere with activities at home and school, and participation in games and playing. Besides being inserted in a different context than child 3, children 1 and 2 showed clinical signs of lack of attention and/or impulsivity/hyperactivity problems. About 50% of children with DCD have learning difficulties and ADHD (GOULARDINS et al., 2015GOULARDINS, J. B. et al. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and developmental coordination disorder: Two separate disorders or do they share a common etiology. Behavioural brain research, Amsterdam, v. 292, p. 484-492, 2015.; OLIVEIRA; NETO; PALHARES, 2018OLIVEIRA, C. C.; NETO, J. L. C.; PALHARES, M. S. Características motoras de escolares com Transtorno de Déficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade. Cadernos de Terapia Ocupacional da UFSCar, São Carlos, v. 26, n. 3, p. 590-600, 2018.). In most cases, at the beginning of elementary school the child begins to have the first complaints of difficulties at school and often has to do with motor difficulties, although they are not always identified as a problem (MAGALHÃES; CARDOSO; MISSIUNA, 2011MAGALHÃES, L.; CARDOSO, A.; MISSIUNA, C. Activities and participation in children with developmental coordination disorder: A systematic review. Research in Developmental Disabilities, New York, v. 32, n. 4, p. 1309-1316, 2011.). For the children in this study, learning disabilities may also be present and should be further investigated.

Given that school activity requires several motor skills to be performed together, which for children with DCD is more difficult to do, children are rated as lazy, disinterested and suffer from failing to complete or to do the tasks of the school:

Yeah, sometimes he says, okay, take my hand right away because you don't do the task in my place. Then I say: no, you have to do it. I'm already helping you by taking your hand. Then, I help him; then he finishes the task, then he does it (M1).

It's because she's disinterested, you teach her now, when you ask, she doesn't know anymore […] A little to not say it's so much, right? (laughs) The person laughs, but the boy is serious, he is very... I don't know, he is very disconnected from life, not interested in things, to teach her work, there is the time that she gets stressed. Because you are explaining and she is laughing, she doesn't take anything seriously; she jokes, miss (M2).

To follow, she doesn't follow much. Yesterday, always the boys sometimes, who is more unrolled than she finishes fast and she is still doing the task […] is, because, so, she has, she has a little bit of difficulty getting out of the picture even (M2).

The tone of voice and gestures during the interviews with M2 and M1 were sad and worrying due to the difficulties and problems faced by children in the school context. Writing problems are present in the school routine and are closely related to DCD. Another point to be emphasized within the school environment, often undervalued by teachers, is the lack of involvement and poor performance in physical activity for their age, which has psychosocial implications to consider, such as social exclusion and low self-esteem, impacting school performance (CAIRNEY et al., 2012CAIRNEY, J. et al. Developmental coordination disorder, gender, and body weight: examining the impact of participation in active play. Research in Developmental Disabilities, New York, v. 33, n. 5, p. 1566-1573, 2012.; MISSIUNA; CAMPBELL, 2014MISSIUNA, C.; CAMPBELL, W. N. Psychological aspects of developmental coordination disorder: can we establish causality? Current Developmental Disorders Reports, Toronto, v. 1, n. 2, p. 125-131, 2014.; CAPISTRANO et al., 2015CAPISTRANO, R. et al. Transtorno do desenvolvimento da coordenação e nível de atividade física em crianças: revisão sistemática da literatura. Cadernos de Terapia Ocupacional da UFSCar, São Carlos, v. 23, n. 3, p. 633-646, 2015.).

3.2.1 Relationship with others

This subcategory was proposed because the theme emerged strongly in the speech of the dyad 1 when they talked about everyday life in school. As it is a relevant subject, it became important to highlight the reports of M1 and child 1 about the relationships experienced with peers in school and the community. Given that personal relationships impact the way we relate to the world, M1's speech pointed to the reality lived by her child in the school context and related to the impact of the child's relationship with peers, showing the child 1 suffering, who often prefer to distancing from activities with other colleagues to avoid conflicts.

He only talks about the playground when his colleagues hit him […] his colleagues hit him there, and he said you see, I didn't even want to leave the room, see mom, I didn't even want to leave the room, the boys they are complaining about me, so I stay inside the room (M1).

He says… that I'm boring… He hits me. I don't know; he's very annoying; he calls me a pipsqueak. I can't pass his side, he hits me (Child 1).

No, just him, he teases everyone. The boy down the street, he teases me too […] he won't let me ask. He is worse than the other; I only pass by his side that he gets irritated (Child 1).

The literature highlights that children with motor coordination problems tend to have fewer friends and not be invited to play games with others. Children often still become the object of teasing, avoiding participation in environments that have physical activities and play in groups involved, making even more scarce opportunities to practice their skills and experiences with each other (MISSIUNA; CAMPBELL, 2014MISSIUNA, C.; CAMPBELL, W. N. Psychological aspects of developmental coordination disorder: can we establish causality? Current Developmental Disorders Reports, Toronto, v. 1, n. 2, p. 125-131, 2014.; SEGAL et al., 2002SEGAL, R.; MANDICH, A.; POLATAJKO, H. Stigma and its management: A framework for understanding social isolation of children with developmental coordination disorder. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, New York, v. 56, n. 4, p. 422-428, 2002.; WAGNER et al., 2012WAGNER, M. O. et al. Peer problems mediate the relationship between developmental coordination disorder and behavioral problems in school-aged children. Research in developmental disabilities, New York, v. 33, n. 6, p. 2072-2079, 2012.).

Feelings related to the perception of competence and self-esteem of children with DCD are topics frequently discussed in the literature (ENGEL-YEGER; HANNA-KASIS, 2010ENGEL-YEGER, B.; HANNA-KASIS, A. The relationship between developmental coordination disordes, child’s perceived self-efficacy and preference to participate in daily activities. Child: Care, Health and Development, Oxford, v. 36, n. 5, p. 670-677, 2010.; MISSIUNA; CAMPBELL, 2014MISSIUNA, C.; CAMPBELL, W. N. Psychological aspects of developmental coordination disorder: can we establish causality? Current Developmental Disorders Reports, Toronto, v. 1, n. 2, p. 125-131, 2014.). When drawing during the interview, Child 1 expressed what other people say about him: (Crying expression)

Mimimi ... Everyone says I only scribble; I don't do anything right. […] A neighbor in my schoolroom, he is very annoying. Then he said I only scribble.

4 Final Considerations

The reports of mothers and children showed the perceptions about themselves and their children and explore the possible relationships between occupations, individuals, and the environment in which they operate, as well as the way these relationships and the perceptions of the child can interfere with activity engagement.

Although they perceived the problems their children face, mothers related them to learning disabilities. The clumsiness and slowness of children to do day-to-day activities such as dressing, undressing, eating, self-care, etc., may not be as relevant as the difficulties in the school context shared by all mothers, and which can also be linked to the DCD such as writing, board copies, clipping, organizing materials and performing school tasks on time and more independently. There is commitment of mothers to seek help, and understanding about the difficulties experienced by children and factors of context influence the offer of opportunities for motor and relational experiences from playing.

The mothers in this study also showed that they perceived their children as “lazy” and “clumsy,” reproducing a common speech for the children with DCD. Often mothers reported that they have to do certain activities for their children because they believed that the children are not able or do not do well. Personal and behavioral characteristics in everyday situations lead the child to form self-concept and perception of competence (self-efficacy). This may vary depending on the individual's perceptions of himself and the judgment that other relevant people make of him, especially the relationships he develops in the family, social and school contexts.

The results of this study showed that the problems of motor coordination are not identified and recognized, which points to the importance of clarifying mothers, educators and health professionals about the characteristics of DCD and the presence of co-occurrences, so the child and the family have adequate support to facilitate engagement and participation in their life contexts. Participants in this study received guidance on how to improve their children's development and performance in performing routine activities and think of solutions to minimize frustrations when facing everyday difficulties.

Some limitations can be described as the low number of participants, even if sufficient data have been obtained to discuss the realities experienced by children with DCD and their mothers. A single opportunity to interview the child may also be a limiting factor, understanding that if there were more interview sessions, the children might have been more comfortable talking about themselves.

We believe that the study may contribute to reinforce the need to report on the impacts of the motor problem potentially influencing performance at home and school activities and participation, and consider the role of the environment and providing opportunities for these children.

  • Funding Source Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior – CAPES.

Referências

  • ANGELINI, A. L. et al. Manual matrizes progressiva coloridas de Raven: escala especial. São Paulo: CETEPP, 1999.
  • ARAÚJO, C. R. S.; CARDOSO, A. A.; MAGALHÃES, L. C. Efficacy of the cognitive orientation to daily occupational performance with Brazilian children with developmental coordination disorder. Scandinavian Journal of Occupational Therapy, Abingdon, v. 20, p. 1-9, 2017.
  • ARAUJO, C. R. S.; MAGALHÃES, L. C.; CARDOSO, A. A. Uso da cognitive orientation to daily occupational performance (CO-OP) com crianças com transtorno do desenvolvimento da coordenação. Revista de Terapia Ocupacional da Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, v. 22, n. 3, p. 245-253, 2011.
  • ASSOCIAÇÃO DE PSIQUIATRIA AMERICA – APA. Manual Diagnóstico e Estatístico de Transtornos Mentais - DSM-5 Porto Alegre: American Psychiatric Association, 2014.
  • BANDURA, A. Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W.H. Freeman and Company, 1997.
  • BARBA, P. C. S. D. et al. Prevalence of Developmental Coordination Disorder signs in children 5 to 14 years in São Carlos. Motricidade, Ribeira de Pena, v. 13, n. 3, p. 22-30, 2017.
  • BARDIN, L. Análise de Conteúdo Lisboa: Edições 70, 2010.
  • BELTRAME, T. S. et al. Prevalência do Transtorno do Desenvolvimento da Coordenação em uma amostra de crianças brasileiras. Cadernos de Terapia Ocupacional da UFSCar, São Carlos, v. 25, n. 1, p. 105-113, 2017.
  • BENETTI, C. I. et al. Fundamentos de teoria bioecológica de Urie Bronfenbrenner. Pensando Psicología, Bogotá, v. 9, n. 16, p. 89-99, 2013.
  • BLANK, R. et al. International clinical practice recommendations on the definition, diagnosis, assessment, intervention, and psychosocial aspects of developmental coordination disorder. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, London, v. 61, n. 3, p. 242-285, 2019.
  • BOBBIO, T. G. et al. Interlimb coordination differentiates Brazilian children from two socioeconomic settings. Pediatrics international, Tokyo, v. 52, n. 3, p. 353-357, 2010.
  • BRASIL. Critério de Classificação Econômica Brasil: cortes do critério. São Paulo: Associação Brasileira de Empresas de Pesquisa, 2014.
  • BRONFENBRENNER, U. Ecological models of human development. Readings on the development of children, New York, v. 2, n. 1, p. 37-43, 1979.
  • BRONFENBRENNER, U.; CECI, S. J. Nature-nuture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychological review, Washington, v. 101, n. 4, p. 568-586, 1994.
  • CAIRNEY, J. et al. Developmental coordination disorder and reported enjoyment of physical education in children. European Physical Education Review, Chester, v. 13, n. 1, p. 81-98, 2007.
  • CAIRNEY, J.; VELDHUIZEN, S.; SZATMARI, P. Motor coordination and emotional-behavioral problems in children. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, Philadelphia, v. 23, n. 4, p. 324-329, 2010.
  • CAIRNEY, J. et al. Developmental coordination disorder, gender, and body weight: examining the impact of participation in active play. Research in Developmental Disabilities, New York, v. 33, n. 5, p. 1566-1573, 2012.
  • CAPISTRANO, R. et al. Transtorno do desenvolvimento da coordenação e nível de atividade física em crianças: revisão sistemática da literatura. Cadernos de Terapia Ocupacional da UFSCar, São Carlos, v. 23, n. 3, p. 633-646, 2015.
  • COUTINHO, M. T.; SOUZA, M. S.; VALENTINI, N. C. Crianças com desordem coordenativa desenvolvimental percebem-se menos competente e evidenciam autoconceito fragilizado. Revista Brasileira de Ciência e Movimento, Brasília, v. 24, n. 4, p. 5-15, 2017.
  • DANTAS, L. E. B. P. T.; MANOEL, E. J. Crianças com dificuldades motoras: questões para a conceituação do transtorno do desenvolvimento da coordenação. Movimento, Porto Alegre, v. 15, n. 3, p. 293-313, 2009.
  • DUNN, W. Perfil sensorial 2 São Paulo: Pearson, 2014.
  • ENGEL-YEGER, B.; HANNA-KASIS, A. The relationship between developmental coordination disordes, child’s perceived self-efficacy and preference to participate in daily activities. Child: Care, Health and Development, Oxford, v. 36, n. 5, p. 670-677, 2010.
  • FERREIRA, L. et al. Motor development in school‐age children is associated with the home environment including socioeconomic status. Child: Care, Health and Development, Oxford, v. 44, n. 6, p. 801-806, 2018.
  • FRANCA, A. S.; CARDOSO, A. A.; ARAÚJO, C. R. S. Problemas de coordenação motora e de atenção em crianças em idade escolar. Revista de Terapia Ocupacional da Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, v. 28, n. 1, p. 86-92, 2017.
  • FREITAS, T. C. B. et al. Family socioeconomic status and the provision of motor affordances in the home. Brazilian Journal of Physical Therapy, São Carlos, v. 17, n. 4, p. 319-327, 2013.
  • FOLQUE, M. A. Interviewing young children. In: NAUGHTON, G. M.; ROLFE, S. A.; SIRAJ-BLATCHFORD, I. (Ed.). Doing early childhood research International Perspectives on Theory and Practice. Berkshire England: Open University Press McGraw-Hill Education, 2010. p. 239-260.
  • GALVÃO, B. A. P. et al. Perspectiva dos pais sobre as consequências funcionais do Transtorno do Desenvolvimento da Coordenação (TDC): revisão da literatura. Cadernos de Terapia Ocupacional da UFSCar, São Carlos, v. 22, n. 1, p. 187-193, 2014a.
  • GALVÃO, B. A. P. et al. Percepção materna do desempenho de crianças com transtorno do desenvolvimento da coordenação. Psicologia em Estudo, Maringá, v. 19, n. 3, p. 527-538, 2014b.
  • GOMES, R. A análise de dados em pesquisa qualitativa. In: MINAYO, M. C. S. et al. Pesquisa social: teoria, método e criatividade. Petrópolis: Editora Vozes Limitada, 2002. p. 67-80.
  • GOULARDINS, J. B. et al. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and developmental coordination disorder: Two separate disorders or do they share a common etiology. Behavioural brain research, Amsterdam, v. 292, p. 484-492, 2015.
  • HADDAD, L. A Ecologia da Educação Infantil: construindo um modelo de sistema unificado de cuidado e educação. 1997. 336 f. Tese (Doutorado em Educação) - Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, 1997.
  • HEDEGAARD, M. The dynamic aspects in children’s learning and development. In: HEDEGAARD, M.; EDWARDS A.; FLEER, M. (Ed.). Motives in children’s development: cultural-historical approaches. New York: Cambridge University Press, 2012. p. 9-27.
  • HENDERSON, S.; SUGDEN, D.; BARNETT, A. Movement assessment battery for children-2: Examiner's manual. São Paulo: Pearson, 2007.
  • MAGALHÃES, L.; CARDOSO, A.; MISSIUNA, C. Activities and participation in children with developmental coordination disorder: A systematic review. Research in Developmental Disabilities, New York, v. 32, n. 4, p. 1309-1316, 2011.
  • MAIOR, M. M. S.; CÂNDIDO, G. A. Vulnerabilidade socioeconômica: um estudo transversal para o município de João Pessoa - PB. Revista Principia: divulgação científica e tecnológica do IFPB, João Pessoa, n. 24, p. 72-87, 2014.
  • MATTOS, P. et al. Apresentação de uma versão em português para uso no Brasil do instrumento MTA-SNAP-IV de avaliação de sintomas de transtorno do déficit de atenção/hiperatividade e sintomas de transtorno desafiador e de oposição. Revista de Psiquiatria do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, v. 28, n. 3, p. 290-297, 2006.
  • MAZER, E. P.; DELLA BARBA, P. C. S. Identificação de sinais de Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Coordenação em crianças de três a seis anos e possibilidades de atuação da Terapia Ocupacional. Revista de Terapia Ocupacional da Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, v. 21, n. 1, p. 74-82, 2010.
  • MINAYO, M. C. S. et al. Pesquisa social: teoria, método e criatividade. Petrópolis: Editora Vozes Limitada, 2002.
  • MISSIUNA, C.; CAMPBELL, W. N. Psychological aspects of developmental coordination disorder: can we establish causality? Current Developmental Disorders Reports, Toronto, v. 1, n. 2, p. 125-131, 2014.
  • MISSIUNA, C. et al. Mysteries and mazes: parents’ experiences of children with developmental coordination disorder. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, Ottawa, v. 73, n. 1, p. 7-17, 2006.
  • NELSON, A.; IWAMA, M. Cultural influences and occupation-centred practice with children and families. In: RODGER, S. (Ed.). Occupation-centred practice with children: a practical guide for occupational therapists. Oxford: John Wiley & Sons, 2010. p. 75-96.
  • NOBRE, G. C.; VALENTINI, N. C.; NOBRE, F. S. S. Fundamental motor skills, nutritional status, perceived competence, and school performance of Brazilian children in social vulnerability: Gender comparison. Child Abuse & Neglect, Oxford, v. 80, p. 335-345, 2018.
  • OKUDA, P. M. M. Intervenção e identificação precoce do transtorno do desenvolvimento da coordenação em escolares no início da alfabetização 2015. 132 f. Dissertação (Mestrado em Fonoaudiologia) – Universidade Estadual Paulista, Marília, 2015.
  • OLIVEIRA, A. K. C.; MATSUKURA, T. S.; FONTAINE, A. M. Autoconceito e autoeficácia em crianças com deficiência física: revisão sistemática da literatura. Revista Brasileira de Educação Especial, Bauru, v. 23, n. 1, p. 145-160, 2017.
  • OLIVEIRA, C. C.; NETO, J. L. C.; PALHARES, M. S. Características motoras de escolares com Transtorno de Déficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade. Cadernos de Terapia Ocupacional da UFSCar, São Carlos, v. 26, n. 3, p. 590-600, 2018.
  • OLIVEIRA, M. M. Como fazer pesquisa qualitativa Petrópolis: Vozes, 2012.
  • PRADO, M.; MAGALHÃES, L.; WILSON, B. Cross-cultural adaptation of the Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire for Brazilian children. Revista Brasileira de Fisioterapia, São Carlos, v. 13, n. 3, p. 236-243, 2009.
  • ROSENBLUM, S.; ENGEL-YEGER, B. Predicting participation in Children with DCD. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, Toronto, v. 1, n. 2, p. 109-117, 2014.
  • SEGAL, R.; MANDICH, A.; POLATAJKO, H. Stigma and its management: A framework for understanding social isolation of children with developmental coordination disorder. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, New York, v. 56, n. 4, p. 422-428, 2002.
  • SMITS-ENGELSMAN, B. et al. Diagnostic criteria for DCD: Past and future. Human Movement Science, Amsterdam, v. 42, p. 293-306, 2015.
  • SPENCER, J. P.; PERONE, S.; BUSS, A. T. Twenty years and going strong: A dynamic systems revolution in motor and cognitive development. Child Development Perspectives, Malden, v. 5, n. 4, p. 260-266, 2011.
  • SUMMERS, J.; LARKIN, D.; DEWEY, D. What impact does developmental coordination disorder have on daily routines? International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, Rio Claro, v. 55, n. 2, p. 131-141, 2008.
  • TAL-SABAN, M. et al. The functional profile of young adults with suspected Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD). Research in Developmental Disabilities, New York, v. 33, n. 6, p. 2193-2202, 2012.
  • THELEN, E. Motor development: A new synthesis. American psychologist, Washington, v. 50, n. 2, p. 79-95, 1995.
  • TOWNSEND, E. A.; POLATAJKO, H. J. Enabling occupation II: Advancing an occupational therapy vision for health, well-being, and justice through occupation. Ottawa: CAOT Publications ACE, 2013.
  • VALENTINI, N. C. et al. Fundamental motor skills across childhood: Age, sex, and competence outcomes of Brazilian children. Journal of Motor Learning and Development, Tennessee, v. 4, n. 1, p. 16-36, 2016.
  • WAGNER, M. O. et al. Peer problems mediate the relationship between developmental coordination disorder and behavioral problems in school-aged children. Research in developmental disabilities, New York, v. 33, n. 6, p. 2072-2079, 2012.
  • ZWICKER, J. et al. Perinatal and neonatal predictors of developmental coordination disorder in very low birthweight children. Archives of Disease in Childhood, London, v. 98, n. 2, p. 118-22, 2013.
  • ZWICKER, J.; HARRIS, S.; KLASSEN, A. Quality of life domains affected in children with developmental coordination disorder: a systematic review. Child: Care, Health and Development, Oxford, v. 39, n. 4, p. 562-580, 2013.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    25 Nov 2019
  • Date of issue
    Oct-Dec 2019

History

  • Received
    04 Nov 2018
  • Reviewed
    20 Mar 2019
  • Reviewed
    19 June 2019
  • Accepted
    26 Aug 2019
Universidade Federal de São Carlos, Departamento de Terapia Ocupacional Rodovia Washington Luis, Km 235, Caixa Postal 676, CEP: , 13565-905, São Carlos, SP - Brasil, Tel.: 55-16-3361-8749 - São Carlos - SP - Brazil
E-mail: cadto@ufscar.br