Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Vocabulary of preschool children with typical language development and socioeducational variables

ABSTRACT

Purpose

To investigate the correlation between age, socioeconomic status (SES), and performance on emissive and receptive vocabulary tests in children with typical language development.

Methods

The study sample was composed of 60 preschool children of both genders, aged 3 years to 5 years 11 months, with typical language development divided into three groups: G I (mean age=3 years 6 months), G II (mean age=4 years 4 months) and G III (mean age=5 years 9 months). The ABFW Child Language Test - Vocabulary and the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT) for emissive and receptive language were applied to the preschoolers. The socioeconomic classification questionnaire of the Brazilian Association of Survey Companies (ABEP) was applied to the preschoolers’ parents/legal guardians. Data were analyzed according to the criteria of the aforementioned instruments and were arranged in Excel spreadsheet for Windows XP®. A multiple linear regression model was used, adopting a statistical significance level of 5%, to analyze the correlation between age, SES, and performance on the receptive and emissive vocabulary tests.

Results

In the ABEP questionnaire, participants were classified mostly into social level C (63.3%), followed by levels B (26.6%) and D (10%). The preschoolers investigated presented emissive and receptive vocabulary adequate for the age groups. No statistically significant difference was found for the variables age and SES regarding emissive and receptive vocabulary. Higher test scores were observed with increased age and SES, for social levels “B” compared with “D” and for “C” with “D”.

Conclusion

The variables age and socioeconomic status influenced the performance on emissive and receptive vocabulary tests in the study group.

Keywords
Vocabulary; Child Development; Social Class; Preschool Children; Language

RESUMO

Objetivo

Verificar a associação entre idade, nível socioeconômico e o desempenho em prova de vocabulário emissivo e receptivo de crianças com desenvolvimento típico de linguagem.

Método

Participaram 60 estudantes da Educação Infantil, com idade entre 3 a 5:11 anos, de ambos os gêneros, com desenvolvimento típico de linguagem, divididos em Grupos: GI (ẋ 3:6 anos), GII (ẋ 4:4 anos) e GIII (ẋ 5:9 anos). Foram aplicados, individualmente, os testes de Linguagem Infantil ABFW- parte Vocabulário e o Peabody para vocabulário emissivo e receptivo, respectivamente, e o preenchimento do questionário de classificação socioeconômica da Associação Brasileira de Empresas de Pesquisa (ABEP) pelo responsável. Os dados foram analisados conforme critérios dos instrumentos e dispostos em planilha de Excel for Windows XP®. Foi utilizado modelo de Regressão Linear múltipla, adotou-se um nível de significância de 5% para estudar a relação entre a idade, o nível socioeconômico e o desempenho em prova de vocabulário receptivo e emissivo.

Resultados

No ABEP, os participantes foram classificados em sua maioria no nível social C (63,3%), seguido do B (26,6%) e D (10%). Apresentaram vocabulário emissivo e receptivo condizente com a faixa etária. No vocabulário emissivo e receptivo, houve diferença estatística para a variável idade e nível socioeconômico. Verificou-se maior pontuação nos testes, conforme o avanço da idade e do nível socioeconômico, do nível social “B” para o “D”, e do “C” para o “D”.

Conclusão

As variáveis idade e nível socioeconômico podem influenciar o desempenho nas provas de vocabulário emissivo e receptivo do grupo estudado.

Descritores
Vocabulário; Desenvolvimento Infantil; Classe Social; Pré-escolar; Linguagem

INTRODUCTION

Lexicon evolves with age, but the type of vocabulary can vary individually in children of the same age group(11 Cartmill EA, Armstrong BF 3rd, Gleitman LR, Goldin-Meadow S, Medina TN, Trueswell JC. Quality of early parente input predicts child vocabular 3 years later. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110(28):11278-83. PMid:23798423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1309518110.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.130951811...
,22 Medeiros V, Valença RK, Guimarães JA, Costa CC. Vocabulário expressivo e variáveis regionais em uma amostra de escolares em Maceió. ACR. 2013;18(2):71-7.) as a result of intrinsic (cognition, information processing, and personality) and/or extrinsic (socioeconomic status (SES) and cultural, family linguistic, and school contexts) aspects(11 Cartmill EA, Armstrong BF 3rd, Gleitman LR, Goldin-Meadow S, Medina TN, Trueswell JC. Quality of early parente input predicts child vocabular 3 years later. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110(28):11278-83. PMid:23798423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1309518110.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.130951811...

2 Medeiros V, Valença RK, Guimarães JA, Costa CC. Vocabulário expressivo e variáveis regionais em uma amostra de escolares em Maceió. ACR. 2013;18(2):71-7.
-33 Gurgel LG, Plentz RD, Joly MC, Reppold CT. Instrumentos de avaliação da compreensão de linguagem oral em crianças e adolescentes: uma revisão sistemática da literatura. Revista Neuropsicologia Latinoamericana. 2010;2(1):1-10.). Communicative experiences and family SES influence the formal development of language at early age(11 Cartmill EA, Armstrong BF 3rd, Gleitman LR, Goldin-Meadow S, Medina TN, Trueswell JC. Quality of early parente input predicts child vocabular 3 years later. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110(28):11278-83. PMid:23798423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1309518110.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.130951811...
,22 Medeiros V, Valença RK, Guimarães JA, Costa CC. Vocabulário expressivo e variáveis regionais em uma amostra de escolares em Maceió. ACR. 2013;18(2):71-7.,44 Ramos DD. Interação adulto criança em creches públicas: estilos linguísticos [dissertação]. João Pessoa (PB): Universidade Federal da Paraíba; 2010. 154 p.

5 Araújo MV, Marteleto MR, Schoen-Ferreira TH. Avaliação do vocabulário receptivo de crianças pré-escolares. Estud Psicol. 2010;27(2):169-76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0103-166X2010000200004.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0103-166X2010...

6 Brancalioni AR, Marini C, Cavalheiro LG, Keske-Soares M. Desempenho em prova de vocabulário de crianças com desvio fonológico e com desenvolvimento fonológico normal. CEFAC. 2011;13(3):428-36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462010005000011.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462010...

7 Scopel RR, Souza VC, Lemos SM. A influência do ambiente familiar e escolar na aquisição e no desenvolvimento da linguagem: revisão de literatura. CEFAC. 2012;14(4):732-41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462011005000139.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462011...

8 Sohr-Preston SL, Scaramella LV, Martin MJ, Neppl TK, Ontai L, Conger R. Parental socioeconomic status, communication, and children’s vocabular development: a third-generation test of the Family investimento model. Child Dev. 2013;84(3):1046-62. PMid:23199236. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12023.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12023...
-99 Derbie AY. A look into vocabular spunt in a typically developing child and a child with a developmental language disorder. JCLAD. 2014;2(6):24-9.). Children of different SES present heterogeneous performance in semantic evaluation tests(44 Ramos DD. Interação adulto criança em creches públicas: estilos linguísticos [dissertação]. João Pessoa (PB): Universidade Federal da Paraíba; 2010. 154 p. ,55 Araújo MV, Marteleto MR, Schoen-Ferreira TH. Avaliação do vocabulário receptivo de crianças pré-escolares. Estud Psicol. 2010;27(2):169-76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0103-166X2010000200004.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0103-166X2010...
). The vocabulary of preschool children is one of the predictors of future academic achievement(22 Medeiros V, Valença RK, Guimarães JA, Costa CC. Vocabulário expressivo e variáveis regionais em uma amostra de escolares em Maceió. ACR. 2013;18(2):71-7.).

Social, cultural, geographic, school and family (parents’ educational level, reading habits, and occupation) variables are risk or protective factors for overall and linguistic development(44 Ramos DD. Interação adulto criança em creches públicas: estilos linguísticos [dissertação]. João Pessoa (PB): Universidade Federal da Paraíba; 2010. 154 p. ,66 Brancalioni AR, Marini C, Cavalheiro LG, Keske-Soares M. Desempenho em prova de vocabulário de crianças com desvio fonológico e com desenvolvimento fonológico normal. CEFAC. 2011;13(3):428-36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462010005000011.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462010...
,77 Scopel RR, Souza VC, Lemos SM. A influência do ambiente familiar e escolar na aquisição e no desenvolvimento da linguagem: revisão de literatura. CEFAC. 2012;14(4):732-41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462011005000139.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462011...
,1010 Thomas MS, Forrester NA, Ronald A. Modeling socioeconomic status effects on language development. Dev Psychol. 2014;49(12):1325-2343. PMid:23544858.,1111 Palácios T, Oliveira LN, Chiossi JS, Soares AD, Chiari BM. Fatores biológicos e socioculturais na avaliação do vocabulário receptivo em português oral de deficientes auditivos pós linguais. Audiol Commun Res. 2014;19(4):360-66.) and should be considered during lexicon assessment(11 Cartmill EA, Armstrong BF 3rd, Gleitman LR, Goldin-Meadow S, Medina TN, Trueswell JC. Quality of early parente input predicts child vocabular 3 years later. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110(28):11278-83. PMid:23798423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1309518110.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.130951811...
). The purpose of the present study was to investigate the correlation between socioeconomic status (SES) and performance on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Tests for emissive (PPVT-E) and receptive (PPVT-R) language in a preliminary group of school children for subsequent collection using cluster (schools) sampling in different neighborhoods.

METHODS

Simple random sampling, following the age criterion (3 years to 5 years 11 months), was conducted with preschoolers of both genders enrolled in two schools selected by convenience of access, resulting in a sample of 60 participants. Exclusion criteria comprised language disorders after screening, signs of auditory and/or neurological alterations, previous or current speech-language pathology treatment, lack of authorization (by parents/legal guardians) or of consent (by the child).

This cross-sectional, analytical study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the aforementioned Institution under protocol no. 1488/2014. The participants’ parents and/or guardians signed an Informed Consent Form (ICF) and the children agreed to participate (Consent Form prepared based on Resolution 416/12) prior to study commencement.

The preschoolers’ parents/legal guardians were interviewed in the schools and responded to the socioeconomic classification questionnaire of the Brazilian Association of Survey Companies(1212 ABEP: Associação Nacional de Empresas e Pesquisas. Critério de classificação econômica do Brasil. São Paulo: ABEP; 2013.). After screening, the study participants were individually assessed in a room provided by the schools by means of expressive (ABFW - Vocabulary)(1313 Befi-Lopes DM. Vocabulário (Parte B). In: Andrade CR, Befi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FD, Wertzner HF. ABFW: Teste de Linguagem infantil e nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. Carapicuíba (SP): Pró-fono; 2000. p. 41-59.) and emissive/receptive (Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test - PPVT)(1414 Capovilla FC, Capovilla AG, Nunes LR, Araújo I, Nunes DR, Nogueira D, et al. Versão brasileira do Teste de Vocabulário por Imagens Peabody. Distúrbios da Comunicação. 1997;8(2):151-62.) vocabulary tests. After analysis, we obtained the mean for the absolute number of correct answers for the results of the PPVT(1414 Capovilla FC, Capovilla AG, Nunes LR, Araújo I, Nunes DR, Nogueira D, et al. Versão brasileira do Teste de Vocabulário por Imagens Peabody. Distúrbios da Comunicação. 1997;8(2):151-62.) and the means of occurrence for Usual Verbal Designation (UVD), No Designation (ND), and Substitution Process (SP) per conceptual field, of each participant, by means of the sum of the percentages obtained in each of these items divided by the total of the conceptual fields assessed.

Socioeconomic status (SES) was obtained by grouping the categories A, B, C, D, and E according to the intervals of the scores as follows: A (35 to 46 points), B (23 to 34 points), C (14 to 22 points), and D and E (0 to 13 points)(1212 ABEP: Associação Nacional de Empresas e Pesquisas. Critério de classificação econômica do Brasil. São Paulo: ABEP; 2013.).

After calculating the mean and standard deviation for the quantitative and frequency variables and the percentage for the qualitative variables, a multiple linear regression model was used to statistically analyze the correlation between the dependent variable (Peabody and ABFW tests) and the several other independent variables (groups and SES). This statistical model presupposes that residues present normal distribution with a mean of zero and constant variance. A significance level of 5% was adopted for all statistical analyses whose results were obtained using the SAS® 9.2 software.

RESULTS

Table 1 shows the classification of study participants according to socioeconomic status (SES).

Table 1
Mean age and socioeconomic status (SES)

Evidence of statistical difference was verified, with better performance in the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Tests for emissive (PPVT-E) (Figure 1) and receptive (PPVT-R) (Figure 2) language, for the groups with higher means of age and SES.

Figure 1
Correlation between age and performance on the emissive and receptive vocabulary tests
Figure 2
Correlation between socioeconomic status (SES) and performance on the emissive and receptive vocabulary tests

DISCUSSION

The performance of the preschoolers investigated on the PPVT-R and PPVT-E tests was influenced by the increased means of the variables age and family SES. School children of lower SES showed poor performance in the vocabulary tests(22 Medeiros V, Valença RK, Guimarães JA, Costa CC. Vocabulário expressivo e variáveis regionais em uma amostra de escolares em Maceió. ACR. 2013;18(2):71-7.). Children with higher mean age perform better during typical development(11 Cartmill EA, Armstrong BF 3rd, Gleitman LR, Goldin-Meadow S, Medina TN, Trueswell JC. Quality of early parente input predicts child vocabular 3 years later. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110(28):11278-83. PMid:23798423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1309518110.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.130951811...
,22 Medeiros V, Valença RK, Guimarães JA, Costa CC. Vocabulário expressivo e variáveis regionais em uma amostra de escolares em Maceió. ACR. 2013;18(2):71-7.), because they will develop and expand their cognitive skills to learn new words, organizing the information obtained through social and physical interaction, while interacting with adults and experiencing different situations(1515 Borges LC, Salomão NM. Aquisição da linguagem: considerações da perspectiva da interação social. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2003;16(2):327-36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722003000200013.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722003...
). Nevertheless(55 Araújo MV, Marteleto MR, Schoen-Ferreira TH. Avaliação do vocabulário receptivo de crianças pré-escolares. Estud Psicol. 2010;27(2):169-76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0103-166X2010000200004.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0103-166X2010...
), evidence of poorer performance of older children, aged four to seven years, on the PPVT-R was observed in preschoolers with typical development. This fact underscores the importance of quality teaching in promoting situations that can aid in the construction and expansion of the infantile lexicon(55 Araújo MV, Marteleto MR, Schoen-Ferreira TH. Avaliação do vocabulário receptivo de crianças pré-escolares. Estud Psicol. 2010;27(2):169-76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0103-166X2010000200004.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0103-166X2010...
).

There is an interrelation between the child's access to toys and cultural objects, activities shared with parents, and the sociolinguistic reality(77 Scopel RR, Souza VC, Lemos SM. A influência do ambiente familiar e escolar na aquisição e no desenvolvimento da linguagem: revisão de literatura. CEFAC. 2012;14(4):732-41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462011005000139.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462011...
) experienced in different social and education levels(22 Medeiros V, Valença RK, Guimarães JA, Costa CC. Vocabulário expressivo e variáveis regionais em uma amostra de escolares em Maceió. ACR. 2013;18(2):71-7.).

The way parents interact and produce their speech are determining factors for the lexical development and more complex language acquisitions of children(66 Brancalioni AR, Marini C, Cavalheiro LG, Keske-Soares M. Desempenho em prova de vocabulário de crianças com desvio fonológico e com desenvolvimento fonológico normal. CEFAC. 2011;13(3):428-36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462010005000011.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462010...
). A qualitative analysis of information on the communicative styles of adults and the level of child development, associated with the socioeconomic and educational classification of parents, are important for the identification of different developmental profiles. More importantly than the context, the speech addressed to children should consider their level of development, thus the emission of semantically and grammatically simpler linguistic structures during mutual attention activities is essential(44 Ramos DD. Interação adulto criança em creches públicas: estilos linguísticos [dissertação]. João Pessoa (PB): Universidade Federal da Paraíba; 2010. 154 p. ).

Classification into low or high SES may always be suggestive of physical and material resources that families may possess, such as access to a rich recreational and cultural context and leisure activities; however, it is necessary to jointly analyze other risk factors such as environmental characteristics, the communicative interactions experienced, and the communicative style used by parents.

CONCLUSION

The results indicate that the variables age and socioeconomic status influence the performance of preschoolers on emissive and receptive vocabulary tests. Further studies conducted with larger samples with stratification in different social classes are needed to confirm the evidence of action of this variable on semantic development.

  • Study carried out at Instituição de Educação Infantil da rede pública e privada, municipalities of Ribeirão Preto (SP) and Santa Rosa do Viterbo (SP), Brazil.
  • Financial support: Bolsa de Iniciação Científica, Pró Reitoria de Pesquisa da Universidade de São Paulo – RUSP.

REFERÊNCIAS

  • 1
    Cartmill EA, Armstrong BF 3rd, Gleitman LR, Goldin-Meadow S, Medina TN, Trueswell JC. Quality of early parente input predicts child vocabular 3 years later. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110(28):11278-83. PMid:23798423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1309518110
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1309518110
  • 2
    Medeiros V, Valença RK, Guimarães JA, Costa CC. Vocabulário expressivo e variáveis regionais em uma amostra de escolares em Maceió. ACR. 2013;18(2):71-7.
  • 3
    Gurgel LG, Plentz RD, Joly MC, Reppold CT. Instrumentos de avaliação da compreensão de linguagem oral em crianças e adolescentes: uma revisão sistemática da literatura. Revista Neuropsicologia Latinoamericana. 2010;2(1):1-10.
  • 4
    Ramos DD. Interação adulto criança em creches públicas: estilos linguísticos [dissertação]. João Pessoa (PB): Universidade Federal da Paraíba; 2010. 154 p.
  • 5
    Araújo MV, Marteleto MR, Schoen-Ferreira TH. Avaliação do vocabulário receptivo de crianças pré-escolares. Estud Psicol. 2010;27(2):169-76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0103-166X2010000200004
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0103-166X2010000200004
  • 6
    Brancalioni AR, Marini C, Cavalheiro LG, Keske-Soares M. Desempenho em prova de vocabulário de crianças com desvio fonológico e com desenvolvimento fonológico normal. CEFAC. 2011;13(3):428-36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462010005000011
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462010005000011
  • 7
    Scopel RR, Souza VC, Lemos SM. A influência do ambiente familiar e escolar na aquisição e no desenvolvimento da linguagem: revisão de literatura. CEFAC. 2012;14(4):732-41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462011005000139
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462011005000139
  • 8
    Sohr-Preston SL, Scaramella LV, Martin MJ, Neppl TK, Ontai L, Conger R. Parental socioeconomic status, communication, and children’s vocabular development: a third-generation test of the Family investimento model. Child Dev. 2013;84(3):1046-62. PMid:23199236. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12023
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12023
  • 9
    Derbie AY. A look into vocabular spunt in a typically developing child and a child with a developmental language disorder. JCLAD. 2014;2(6):24-9.
  • 10
    Thomas MS, Forrester NA, Ronald A. Modeling socioeconomic status effects on language development. Dev Psychol. 2014;49(12):1325-2343. PMid:23544858.
  • 11
    Palácios T, Oliveira LN, Chiossi JS, Soares AD, Chiari BM. Fatores biológicos e socioculturais na avaliação do vocabulário receptivo em português oral de deficientes auditivos pós linguais. Audiol Commun Res. 2014;19(4):360-66.
  • 12
    ABEP: Associação Nacional de Empresas e Pesquisas. Critério de classificação econômica do Brasil. São Paulo: ABEP; 2013.
  • 13
    Befi-Lopes DM. Vocabulário (Parte B). In: Andrade CR, Befi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FD, Wertzner HF. ABFW: Teste de Linguagem infantil e nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. Carapicuíba (SP): Pró-fono; 2000. p. 41-59.
  • 14
    Capovilla FC, Capovilla AG, Nunes LR, Araújo I, Nunes DR, Nogueira D, et al. Versão brasileira do Teste de Vocabulário por Imagens Peabody. Distúrbios da Comunicação. 1997;8(2):151-62.
  • 15
    Borges LC, Salomão NM. Aquisição da linguagem: considerações da perspectiva da interação social. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2003;16(2):327-36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722003000200013
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722003000200013

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    2017

History

  • Received
    02 Sept 2015
  • Accepted
    07 June 2016
Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia Al. Jaú, 684, 7º andar, 01420-002 São Paulo - SP Brasil, Tel./Fax 55 11 - 3873-4211 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista@codas.org.br