Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Analysis of population recognition of pictures from the sentence production program for aphasia

ABSTRACT

Purpose

To analyze the recognition of the pictures and contexts familiarity from the Sentence Production Program for Aphasia (SPPA) to acquire data determining its potential use in Portuguese and/or need for adaption.

Methods

The stimuli for English speakers were first translated, back-translated and adjusted without changing the syntactic structures of the phrases. The material was then tested in 39 highly literate individuals. In the first stage of the study, the adults had to produce a phrase corresponding to each picture. In the second stage, participants were exposed to the original phrases associated with their respective contexts. Participants were given an answer sheet containing numbered responses for each picture and rated the familiarity of the contexts on an analogic scale ranging from 0 to 10.

Results

Based on the sentences produced by the participants, the overall mean agreement of stimuli with responses for the pictures from the SPPA was 35.5%. Familiarity of the contexts with pictures was greater in scenes involving sentences with a syntactic structure similar to that of Portuguese.

Conclusion

Given most stimuli had a low level of recognition, adaptations are required for use of the program in the Brazilian population. The study allowed identification of those pictures which need adapting before use in the rehabilitation of agrammatic patients in Brazil.

Keywords
Aphasia; Rehabilitation; Language; Language Disorders; Translation

RESUMO

Objetivo

Analisar o reconhecimento das figuras e a familiaridade dos contextos do Sentence Production Program for Aphasia (SPPA), para obter dados sobre a possibilidade de uso para a língua portuguesa e/ou da necessidade de sua adaptação.

Método

Inicialmente, foi realizada a tradução dos estímulos por falantes do inglês e, em seguida, a retrotradução para ajustes sem a alteração das estruturas sintáticas das frases. O material foi então submetido para a análise de 39 indivíduos adultos de alta escolaridade. Na primeira fase do estudo, os adultos deveriam criar uma frase de acordo com cada figura apresentada. Na fase seguinte do estudo, os participantes foram expostos às frases originais associadas aos seus respectivos contextos. Os participantes receberam uma folha de registro das respostas com numeração segundo as figuras e julgaram a familiaridade dos contextos a partir de uma escala analógica de 0 a 10.

Resultados

Observou-se que a média geral de concordância dos estímulos com as respostas para as figuras do SPPA foi de 35,5%. Em relação à familiaridade dos contextos com as figuras, essa foi maior para as cenas relativas às frases com estrutura sintática mais usadas em português.

Conclusão

Pelo fato de os estímulos terem apresentado baixo grau de reconhecimento, adaptações serão necessárias para que o programa possa vir a ser utilizado na população brasileira. O estudo também permitiu identificar quais as figuras que necessitam de adaptação antes de serem usadas para a reabilitação de pacientes agramáticos no Brasil.

Descritores
Afasia; Reabilitação; Linguagem; Transtornos de Linguagem; Tradução

INTRODUCTION

Aphasia is defined as an acquired language disorder that results from damage to the Central Nervous System (CNS), characterized by impairments in auditory comprehension and oral-expressive language and/or writing. Difficulty with comprehension, reading, speaking and/or writing can occur(11 McNeil MR, Pratt SR. Defining aphasia: some theoretical and clinical implications of operating from a formal definition. Aphasiology. 2001;15(10-11):901-11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02687040143000276.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0268704014300...
). Aphasia is caused by lesions to the left hemisphere, an area associated with language functions(22 Soares ECS, Ortiz KZ. Influence of brain lesion and educational background on language tests in aphasic subjects. Dement Neuropsychol. 2008;2(4):321-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN20400016. PMid:29213593.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-5764200...
).

Aphasia manifests in different forms in each individual, depending on the site and severity of the brain injury and language ability prior to lesion(33 Soares ECS, Ortiz KZ. Influence of schooling on language abilities of adults without linguistic disorders. Sao Paulo Med J. 2009;127(3):134-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-31802009000300005. PMid:19820873.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-3180200...
). Lesions can have different etiologies such as stroke, traumatic brain injury (TBI), infectious diseases, or tumors involving the CNS(44 Rodrigues TT, Jacuviske MV, Ortiz KZ. Perfil populacional de pacientes com distúrbios da comunicação humana decorrentes de lesão cerebral, assistidos em hospital terciário. Rev CEFAC. 2011;13:330-9. ).

The goal of speech-language therapy for aphasia-related deficits is to remediate patient communication abilities.

The therapy approaches available are designed to improve some of the more common language symptoms arising in this patient group. Therapy focused on language processing deficits allows control, at least in part, of the effectiveness of treatment(55 Basso A, Cattaneo S, Girelli L, Luzzatti C, Miozzo A, Modena L, et al. Treatment eficacy of language and calculation disorders and speech apraxia: a review of the literature. Eur J Phys Rehabil Med. 2011;47(1):101-21. PMid:21448123. ). No published Brazilian literature or commercial material exists reporting therapy methods for the specific language symptoms of aphasia. Therefore, a study of the methods published in other languages for rehabilitation of some of the problems classically found in aphasic patients can be of great use.

One of the most common symptoms of aphasia is agrammatism, a change in syntactic structure evidenced by the omission of sentence components(66 Cho-Reyes S, Mack JE, Thompson CK. Grammatical encoding and learning in agrammatic aphasia: evidence from structural priming. J Mem Lang. 2016;91:202-218. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2016.02.004. PMid:28924328.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2016.02...
). Grammatical words tend to be dropped in speech while lexical words are retained. In severe cases, utterances can be restricted to single words; sentence structure can be made up of only simplified forms. The core feature of grammatical production in aphasia is that utterances consist of words strung together in an ungrammatical sequence or, at best, simple canonical sentences (e.g., subject-verb-object)(77 Sang HK. Agrammatic aphasia verb and argument patterns in Kiswahili- English spontaneous language. S Afr J Commun Disord. 2015;62(1):1-10. http://dx.doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v62i1.89. PMid:26304215.
http://dx.doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v62i1.8...

8 Lise M, Obler K, Miceli G. A cross-language narrative sourcebook. 1st ed. Philadelphia: Jonh Benjamins Publishing Company; 1990.
-99 Cuetos-Vega F, Domínguez A, Baauw S, Berthier-Torres ML. Disociación entre paciente agramáticos y anómicos em laproducción de formas verbales. Rev Neurol. 2007;44(4):203-8. PMid:17311209. ). In agrammatism, sentences are often shorter and low grammatical complexity contributes to reduced fluency(1010 Faroqi-Shah Y, Friedman L. Production of verb tense in agrammatic afasia: a meta- analysis and further data. Behav Neurol. 2015;2015:983870. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2015/983870.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2015/983870 ...
). The speech of individuals with agrammatism is paused and characterized by omission and/or substitution of morphemes and verbs. Besides affecting spontaneous speech, agrammatism can be observed in repetition, reading and writing(1111 Silagi ML, Hirata FN, Mendonça LIZ. Sentence production in rehabilitation of agrammatism: a case study. Dement Neuropsychol. 2014;8(3):297-301. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642014DN83000015. PMid:29213917.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-5764201...
,1212 Andrade-Calderón P, Salvador-Cruz J, Sosa-Ortiz AL. Positive impact of speech therapy in progressive non-fluent aphasia. Acta Colomb Psicol. 2015;18(2):101-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.14718/ACP.2015.18.2.9.
http://dx.doi.org/10.14718/ACP.2015.18....
).

There is a lack of specific protocols for the treatment of agrammatism. In addition, the existence of syntactic differences across languages can hamper adaptation of available protocols. Three key therapeutic intervention protocols for agrammatism in the literature include: Linguistic Specific Treatment (LST), whose aim is to increase knowledge on the lexical representation of verbs and access of lexical and syntactical elements of sentences(1313 Thompson CK, Shapiro LP. A Linguistic-Specific Approach to Treatment of Sentence Production Deficits in Aphasia. Clinical Aphasiology. 1994;22:307-23. ); Reduced syntax therapy (REST), whose objective is to enable patients to produce more fluent speech and ensure that the formulation of everyday phrases takes place in an easier and simpler manner(1414 Springer L, Huber W, Schlenck K-J, Schlenck C. Agrammatism: defict or compensation? Consequences of aphasia therapy. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2010;10(3):279-309. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/096020100389165.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0960201003891...
); and the Sentence Production Program for Aphasia (SPPA) (1515 Helm-Estabrooks N, Nicholas M. Sentence production program for aphasia. 2nd ed. Austin: Pro-Ed; 2000. ).

The SPPA consists of 120 sentences grouped into 8 Blocks of 15 sentences according to 8 different types of grammatical structures. The sentences are presented together with a picture and introduced within a context read out by the therapist. The 8 types of grammatical structure have different inherent levels of complexity. Because the program was originally developed for use in the United States, some of the images reflect North-American culture, with references to baseball, to American football, and so forth. Moreover, it is believed that the complexity of the grammatical structures in English does not match that of the corresponding Portuguese sentences.

Before the SPPA can be used in Brazil, it is important to analyze whether recognition of the pictures and familiarity of the contexts in which the sentences appear in the program are similar to those of Brazilian culture or whether these require prior adaptation.

Therefore, the objective of the present study was to analyze the recognition of pictures and the familiarity of the contexts from the Sentence Production Program for Aphasia (SPPA) to acquire data determining its potential use in Portuguese and/or the need for adaption.

METHODS

This study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the institution (Permit 1475/2016). All participants were given information on the study and signed the Free and Informed Consent Form.

The present study was based on the SPPA program containing 120-pictorial stimuli and 120 sentences whose context was everyday situations of three families. The sentences are grouped into 8 blocks according to their syntactic structure: Imperative Intransitive; Imperative Transitive; Wh- Interrogative (What & Who); Wh- Interrogative (Where & When); Declarative Transitive; Declarative Intransitive; Comparative; and Yes-No Questions.

The study method is described in two sections: 1 and 2, with the latter is divided into subsections 2.1 and 2.2.

Part 1 Translation

The first step was to translate the SPPA sentences from their original English into Brazilian Portuguese. Two fluent speakers of both languages translated the instructions and stimuli into Portuguese. Another translator then performed the back-translation of this version. After translation and back-translation, minor changes were made to the stimuli in order to ensure equivalence of the structures at word and sentence level. In the event of doubts over items that differed between the translation and back-translation, a third expert judge fluent in Brazilian Portuguese and English was consulted to reach a consensus on the best way to translate the stimuli.

Part 1 of this study entailed translation of the instrument while part 2 involved the analysis of recognition of the pictures alone, and of the pictures together with their respective contexts.

Part 2: Analysis of pictures and of familiarity of contexts

Analysis of recognition of pictures from the SPPA

This step consisted of determining how well the pictures represented the context of the SPPA sentences, based on the recognition analysis performed by the non-expert judges.

In order to analyze the recognition of the context elicited by the pictures, group data collection was performed for 39 young adults with a high educational level. High-educated subjects were chosen because low educational level may have led to poor agreement of answers (22 Soares ECS, Ortiz KZ. Influence of brain lesion and educational background on language tests in aphasic subjects. Dement Neuropsychol. 2008;2(4):321-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN20400016. PMid:29213593.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-5764200...
,1616 Ishigaki EC, Lobrigate NL, Fonseca RP, Parente MA, Ortiz KZ. Análise do reconhecimento das figuras do teste MT Beta-86 modificado para uma adaptação: descrição e discussão de aspectos metodológicos. CoDAS. 2013;25(3):282-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2317-17822013000300015. PMid:24408341.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2317-1782201...
).

The SPPA pictures were first scanned and then organized by sentence group. The pictures associated with the sentences were presented to participants using the Microsoft Office Power Point application in a quiet room with appropriate lighting. The images were displayed by a projector connected to a microcomputer.

The participants were given an answer sheet numbered according to the slides. The following instruction was then given:

You are going to see a series of pictures depicting everyday scenes. You must imagine that a sentence is said in the context of each picture. The sentences may vary in terms of grammatical structure (imperatives, imperatives with complement, interrogatives, declaratives and comparatives). You must create a sentence that you believe best applies to the context shown by the picture and that obeys the predetermined grammatical structure.

The maximum display time for each picture was 20 seconds.

After writing the sentences for the pictures displayed on the slides, each participant handed in their answer sheet for analysis. The agreement between participants´ answers and the original stimuli for the SPPA was then analyzed. One of the researchers was responsible for assessing participants´ answers and checking whether participants had failed to provide an answer or had answered the questions differently to that requested. Answers which used a different grammatical structure to that requested were considered wrong.

The data were subjected to statistical analysis. The mean overall recognition of the pictures was calculated together with mean recognition per sentence block. Blocks were compared using the Kruskall-Wallis test. Tukey’s multiple comparison (pairwise) test was applied in cases exhibiting 5% significance to determine which sentence blocks were more recognized than others.

Analysis of rating of context familiarity together with picture recognition

For this stage, participants were shown all the material, i.e.: the original translated sentences of the SPPA were presented together with their respective contexts. The 39 volunteers from phase 2.1 took part in this step. This group only received the complete context after finishing the first step. The participants listened to the context read out loud, while reading the sentence. They then had to answer the following question: “How familiar is this context/scene to you?”. The participants were given an answer sheet containing numbered responses according to the slides/contexts and rated the familiarity of the contexts on an analogue scale from 0 to 10, where 0 indicated no familiarity and 10 indicated 100% familiarity. After collection of answers, the familiarity of the stimuli was analyzed statistically.

A separate statistical analysis was performed for the analysis of the familiarity of the pictures together with their respective context.

The result of the comparison between the two variables (pictures and context) was obtained using Friedman´s test with Sidak´s multiple comparisons (pairwise) for instances of > 5% significance. All of the items that reached the level of significance were submitted to multiple comparisons.

RESULTS

Phase 1: Translation

Phase 1 of the study involved the process of translating the original sentences into Portuguese. The Portuguese translation by the two translators and the back-translation were analyzed and found to agree. The stimuli from the SPPA essentially comprise simple sentences and thus the resultant translation obtained in this step proved consistent. Consequently, the 3rd judge only checked whether the final translation of the stimuli reflected the same syntactic structure as their corresponding stimuli in English.

Two participants discontinued during the group data collection process. The final analysis was thus performed for the answers of 37 individuals.

Phase 2: Analysis of pictures and of familiarity of contexts

Analysis of Recognition of pictures from SPPA

In phase 2.1 of the study, the extent to which pictures represented the context of the SPPA sentences was determined based on the overall mean agreement with expected answers, i.e. SPPA sentences versus sentences produced by the participants. This comparison revealed the degree of similarity between the two sets of sentences. The overall mean for expected answers for all program pictures is given in Table 1 .

Table 1
Overall Mean recognition of SPPA pictures

The mean recognition rate for each Block of Pictures was calculated to ascertain whether the low recognition was due to specific types of grammatical sentences distributed in the 8 blocks and which may have reduced the overall mean recognition of the picture contexts by the volunteers. The results are shown in Table 2 . For improved readability of the answers, the data are presented in increasing order for the Blocks of sentences (from lowest to highest mean recognition rate).

Table 2
Mean recognition of each sentence block for SPPA pictures

All sentence blocks were then statistically compared to determine whether the blocks differed significantly for picture recognition. The Kruskal-Wallis test was applied followed by Tukey´s multiple comparisons test to check where the differences occurred.

The results showed that the pictures for sentences from Blocks 1 and 8 had lower recognition than those for sentences from Block 6, where this difference was statistically significant. The other blocks were similar to one another in terms of recognition, with no statistically significant differences.

Analysis of rating of context familiarity together with picture recognition

The familiarity of the contexts together with pictures presented was assessed. To this end, mean recognition of each sentence in each of the blocks was checked. Subsequently, comparative analysis of sentences from each block was performed in a pairwise fashion. The results of these analyses are provided in the Appendix A Appendix A Analysis of the recognition of each one of the SSPA sentences Table A1 Mean recognition of figures of Block 1 Block 1 S4 S15 S7 S5 S12 S8 S2 S13 S11 S9 S14 S6 S10 S3 S1 Mean 3.0 4.4 5.2 5.5 6.1 6.2 6.3 6.9 7.1 7.8 7.9 8.2 9.1 9.1 9.4 Median 3.0 5.0 6.0 6.0 7.0 6.0 7.0 7.0 8.0 9.0 8.0 9.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 Standard deviation 3.1 3.4 4.0 3.8 3.1 3.1 3.0 2.6 3.1 2.9 2.2 2.0 1.6 2.0 .9 individuals 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 Chart A1 Multiple comparison of mean recognition of block 1 S1 S2 S3 S4 S5 S6 S7 S8 S9 S10 S11 S12 S13 S14 S2 .000 S3 1.000 .002 S4 .000 .001 .000 S5 .000 1.000 .007 .140 S6 .073 .044 .999 .000 .050 S7 .000 1.000 .002 .175 1.000 .004 S8 .000 1.000 .003 .003 1.000 .083 1.000 S9 .142 .793 .954 .000 .438 1.000 .05 .502 S10 1.000 .000 1.000 .000 .003 .750 .000 .000 .888 S11 .015 1.000 .312 .000 .859 1.000 .716 1.000 1.000 .248 S12 .000 1.000 .003 .001 1.000 .023 1.000 1.000 .782 .000 .999 S13 .000 1.000 .021 .000 .977 .453 .522 1.000 1.000 .014 1.000 1.000 S14 .025 .173 .920 .000 .063 1.000 .006 .737 1.000 .860 1.000 .034 .500 S15 .000 .691 .000 .983 1.000 .000 1.000 .925 .004 .000 .020 .443 .009 .000 Then: (S4=S15=S7=S5) < (S6=S10=S3=S1) Other comparisons showed similar recognition rates The differences in recognition between sentences are highlighted in yellow Table A2 Mean recognition of figures of Block 2 Block 2 S23 S16 S28 S29 S18 S20 S26 S21 S30 S22 S27 S25 S17 S24 S19 Mean 3.7 4.4 4.4 4.7 5.5 6.2 6.6 7.0 8.1 8.3 8.3 8.4 8.6 9.1 9.5 Median 4.0 5.0 4.0 5.0 5.0 7.0 7.0 8.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 9.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 Standard deviation 3.4 3.4 3.9 3.2 3.1 3.8 3.2 3.3 3.3 2.9 2.8 1.7 2.5 1.9 1.4 individuals 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 Chart A2 Multiple comparison of mean recognition of block 2 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 17 .000 18 1.000 .005 19 .000 .999 .000 20 .833 .368 1.000 .002 21 .041 .615 .999 .026 1.000 22 .000 1.000 .073 .924 .785 .988 23 1.000 .000 .300 .000 .096 .001 .000 24 .000 1.000 .001 1.000 .021 .042 1.000 .000 25 .000 1.000 .001 .102 .118 .876 1.000 .000 1.000 26 .318 .236 1.000 .001 1.000 1.000 .397 .021 .014 .303 27 .000 1.000 .012 .754 .631 1.000 1.000 .000 1.000 1.000 .559 28 1.000 .000 1.000 .000 .962 .108 .000 1.000 .000 .000 .287 .000 29 1.000 .000 1.000 .000 .999 .133 .001 .998 .000 .000 .437 .000 1.000 30 .000 1.000 .064 .917 .935 1.000 1.000 .000 1.000 1.000 .971 1.000 .000 .001 Then: (S23=S16=S28=S29=S18=S20) < (S30=S22=S27=S25=S17=S24=S19) Other comparisons showed similar recognition rates The differences in recognition between sentences are highlighted in yellow Table A3 Mean recognition of figures of Block 3 Block 3 S32 S44 S36 S39 S34 S38 S42 S37 S41 S40 S43 S35 S33 S45 S31 Mean 1.0 6.5 6.6 6.7 6.8 7.3 7.7 7.8 8.0 8.0 8.5 9.0 9.2 9.5 9.5 Median .0 7.0 7.0 7.0 7.0 8.0 9.0 9.0 10.0 9.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 Standard deviation 2.0 2.9 3.3 2.3 3.0 2.5 3.0 2.7 2.8 2.2 2.3 2.4 1.3 1.0 1.2 individuals 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 Chart A3 Multiple comparison of mean recognition of block 3 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 32 .000 33 1.000 .000 34 .000 .000 .002 35 1.000 .000 1.000 .222 36 .000 .000 .003 1.000 .003 37 .079 .000 .561 1.000 .936 .943 38 .000 .000 .010 1.000 .499 1.000 1.000 39 .000 .000 .000 1.000 .004 1.000 .976 1.000 40 .038 .000 .507 .926 .999 .819 1.000 1.000 .404 41 .080 .000 .730 1.000 .884 .551 1.000 1.000 .252 1.000 42 .194 .000 .584 1.000 .898 1.000 1.000 1.000 1.000 1.000 1.000 43 .868 .000 1.000 .601 1.000 .419 1.000 .979 .100 1.000 1.000 1.000 44 .000 .000 .000 1.000 .003 1.000 .382 1.000 1.000 .753 .243 .991 .040 45 1.000 .000 1.000 .001 1.000 .000 .031 .003 .000 .109 .207 .035 .547 .000 Then: S32 < (S42=S37=S41=S40=S43=S35=S33=S45=S31) Other comparisons showed similar recognition rates The differences in recognition between sentences are highlighted in yellow Table A4 Mean recognition of figures of Block 4 Block 4 S48 S51 S54 S58 S56 S46 S52 S59 S47 S53 S49 S57 S60 S50 S55 Mean 2.8 3.3 4.1 5.3 5.6 5.6 5.9 6.3 7.7 7.8 7.9 8.1 8.4 8.7 8.8 Median 2.0 3.0 4.0 5.0 6.0 7.0 7.0 6.0 9.0 10.0 9.0 10.0 10.0 9.0 10.0 Standard deviation 2.6 3.2 3.6 3.6 3.9 3.8 3.7 3.3 2.4 3.5 3.1 3.6 3.0 2.1 2.8 individuals 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 Chart A4 Multiple comparison of mean recognition of block 4 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 47 .408 48 .215 .000 49 .677 1.000 .000 50 .001 .998 .000 1.000 51 .328 .000 1.000 .000 .000 52 1.000 .571 .001 .190 .007 .035 53 .571 1.000 .000 1.000 1.000 .000 .885 54 .980 .001 1.000 .014 .000 1.000 .935 .000 55 .020 1.000 .000 1.000 1.000 .000 .012 1.000 .000 56 1.000 .383 .053 .458 .005 .191 1.000 .527 .975 .019 57 .463 1.000 .000 1.000 1.000 .000 .518 1.000 .000 1.000 .477 58 1.000 .142 .104 .658 .000 .302 1.000 .234 .999 .007 1.000 .056 59 1.000 .991 .001 .995 .008 .004 1.000 .896 .054 .014 1.000 .228 1.000 60 .166 1.000 .000 1.000 1.000 .000 .050 1.000 .000 1.000 .063 1.000 .025 .113 Then: (S48=S51=S54=S58=S56=S46) < (S47=S53=S49=S57=S60=S50=S55) Other comparisons showed similar recognition rates The differences in recognition between sentences are highlighted in yellow Table A5 Mean recognition of figures of Block 5 Block 5 S66 S74 S63 S61 S73 S69 S75 S71 S70 S64 S72 S67 S65 S62 S68 Mean 5.5 6.1 7.3 7.5 7.5 8.0 8.1 8.2 8.5 9.1 9.1 9.3 9.4 9.6 9.7 Median 5.0 6.0 8.0 9.0 8.0 8.0 9.0 9.0 9.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 Standard deviation 3.4 3.2 3.1 3.1 2.8 2.1 2.8 2.2 2.2 1.7 2.0 1.5 1.3 .7 .9 individuals 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 Chart A5 Multiple comparison of mean recognition of block 5 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 62 .027 63 1.000 .006 64 .791 .999 .089 65 .127 1.000 .021 .996 66 .657 .000 .333 .000 .000 67 .195 1.000 .113 1.000 1.000 .000 68 .024 1.000 .005 .824 1.000 .000 1.000 69 1.000 .007 1.000 .875 .177 .022 .385 .007 70 1.000 .312 .921 1.000 .823 .000 .997 .250 1.000 71 1.000 .141 .997 .997 .575 .000 .949 .104 1.000 1.000 72 .784 1.000 .179 1.000 1.000 .000 1.000 1.000 .850 1.000 .995 73 1.000 .013 1.000 .103 .033 .032 .247 .015 1.000 .917 .999 .021 74 .801 .000 .995 .000 .000 1.000 .000 .000 .122 .004 .101 .000 .516 75 1.000 .122 1.000 .992 .650 .095 .646 .172 1.000 1.000 1.000 .968 1.000 .600 Then: (S66=S74=S63=S61) < (S75=S71=S70=S64=S72=S67=S65=S62=S68) Other comparisons showed similar recognition rates The differences in recognition between sentences are highlighted in yellow Table A6 Mean recognition of figures of Block 6 Block 6 S88 S87 S86 S80 S77 S84 S81 S89 S82 S85 S76 S90 S78 S79 S83 Mean 6.0 7.7 7.9 8.0 8.1 8.5 8.6 8.8 9.3 9.4 9.6 9.6 9.6 9.6 9.6 Median 7.0 8.0 8.0 9.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 Standard deviation 3.8 2.6 2.0 2.8 3.4 2.3 2.3 2.4 1.7 1.8 1.7 1.0 .7 .8 .9 individuals 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 Chart A6 Multiple comparison of mean recognition of block 6 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 77 .957 78 1.000 .633 79 1.000 .702 1.000 80 .500 1.000 .146 .111 81 .130 1.000 .711 .655 1.000 82 1.000 .947 1.000 1.000 .909 1.000 83 1.000 .805 1.000 1.000 .163 .711 1.000 84 .970 1.000 .571 .454 1.000 1.000 1.000 .583 85 1.000 .922 1.000 1.000 .282 1.000 1.000 1.000 1.000 86 .068 1.000 .003 .002 1.000 1.000 .352 .003 1.000 .247 87 .116 1.000 .010 .014 1.000 1.000 .396 .014 .982 .397 1.000 88 .002 .242 .000 .000 .729 .091 .000 .000 .200 .003 .753 .988 89 .902 1.000 .995 .998 1.000 1.000 1.000 .992 1.000 1.000 1.000 1.000 .113 90 1.000 .890 1.000 1.000 .238 .571 1.000 1.000 .753 1.000 .003 .021 .001 .995 Then: (S88<S87=S86) < (S80=S77=S84=S81=S89=S82=S85=S76=S90=S78=S79=S83) Other comparisons showed similar recognition rates The differences in recognition between sentences are highlighted in yellow Table A7 Mean recognition of figures of Block 7 Block 7 S96 S98 S97 S94 S93 S104 S105 S103 S92 S101 S102 S95 S99 S91 S100 Mean 4.0 5.3 5.8 6.5 7.1 7.1 7.5 7.8 8.4 8.6 8.8 9.3 9.3 9.6 9.6 Median 5.0 5.0 6.0 7.0 8.0 8.0 7.0 9.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 Standard deviation 3.4 3.4 3.8 3.1 3.3 3.5 2.1 2.9 2.7 2.5 2.3 1.6 1.8 1.7 .8 individuals 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 Chart A7 Multiple comparison of mean recognition of block 7 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 92 .399 93 .039 .865 94 .000 .433 1.000 95 1.000 1.000 .048 .001 96 .000 .000 .001 .006 .000 97 .000 .065 .999 1.000 .001 .793 98 .000 .009 .283 1.000 .000 .972 1.000 99 1.000 1.000 .176 .012 1.000 .000 .001 .000 100 1.000 .438 .009 .000 1.000 .000 .000 .000 1.000 101 .999 1.000 .942 .067 1.000 .000 .086 .000 1.000 .886 102 .966 1.000 .654 .019 1.000 .000 .001 .000 1.000 .907 1.000 103 .285 1.000 1.000 .975 .678 .000 .618 .013 .488 .058 1.000 .979 104 .004 .675 1.000 1.000 .226 .000 1.000 .449 .267 .007 .880 .200 1.000 105 .005 1.000 1.000 1.000 .069 .000 .986 .030 .040 .000 .958 .833 1.000 1.000 Then: (96=S98=S97) < (S103=S92=S101=S102=S95=S99=S91=S100) Other comparisons showed similar recognition rates The differences in recognition between sentences are highlighted in yellow Table A8 Mean recognition of figures of Block 8 Block 8 S115 S109 S110 S113 S119 S111 S106 S118 S108 S120 S116 S114 S112 S107 S117 Mean 3.9 5.0 5.2 5.4 5.7 6.6 6.9 7.6 7.8 8.2 8.6 8.6 9.1 9.1 9.5 Median 5.0 5.0 5.0 6.0 7.0 6.0 7.0 8.0 9.0 9.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 Standard deviation 3.4 3.2 3.4 3.5 3.7 2.5 2.7 2.5 2.9 2.2 2.6 2.6 2.1 2.0 1.7 individuals 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 37 Chart A8 Multiple comparison of mean recognition of block 8 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 107 .001 108 1.000 .992 109 .479 .000 .000 110 .982 .001 .003 1.000 111 1.000 .004 .999 .738 .938 112 .030 1.000 .933 .000 .000 .006 113 .998 .001 .003 1.000 1.000 .996 .000 114 .747 1.000 1.000 .000 .006 .118 1.000 .001 115 .045 .000 .000 .999 .997 .075 .000 .518 .000 116 .685 1.000 1.000 .000 .003 .144 1.000 .005 1.000 .000 117 .001 1.000 .480 .000 .000 .000 1.000 .000 .982 .000 .930 118 1.000 .758 1.000 .000 .021 .997 .301 .028 1.000 .000 .999 .077 119 1.000 .004 .533 1.000 1.000 1.000 .001 1.000 .037 .904 .005 .000 .443 120 .955 1.000 1.000 .000 .004 .851 1.000 .003 1.000 .000 1.000 .629 1.000 .127 Then: (S115=S109=S110=S113=S119=S11) < (S118=S108=S120=S116=S114=S112=Ss07=S117) Other comparisons showed similar recognition rates The differences in recognition between sentences are highlighted in yellow for reference.

The analysis of all the tables revealed which sentences had lower recognition rates. These sentences are shown in Chart 1 .

Chart 1
Comparison of recognition of sentences from the SPPA for each Block

DISCUSSION

The aim of the present study was to analyze recognition of pictures and familiarity of contexts of a North American program for rehabilitation of agrammatism for potential use in Brazil. The key finding of this study was that recognition of the pictures was low, even when used together with their respective contexts. These findings will be discussed in the ensuing text.

With regard to the translation of the stimuli, a consensus on the versions produced between the translators and researcher was reached, where the professionals involved were versed in the English language. Some adaptations were made to maintain the original syntactic structure of the sentences, and the names of the characters of the three families were changed so as not to sound strange to the Brazilian volunteers. These changes were minor and agreed by the 3 judges.

It is widely acknowledged that visual elements do not have pre-established meanings. These elements only take on meaning when associated with a formal context(1515 Helm-Estabrooks N, Nicholas M. Sentence production program for aphasia. 2nd ed. Austin: Pro-Ed; 2000. ). In order for an image to be understood and duly processed, there is a hierarchy of recognition that ensures the image can be recognized accurately. The types of signs featured in the image are first identified: icon signs (drawing), plastic signs (characteristics such as color, shape and texture) and language sign (related to verbal language)(1616 Ishigaki EC, Lobrigate NL, Fonseca RP, Parente MA, Ortiz KZ. Análise do reconhecimento das figuras do teste MT Beta-86 modificado para uma adaptação: descrição e discussão de aspectos metodológicos. CoDAS. 2013;25(3):282-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2317-17822013000300015. PMid:24408341.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2317-1782201...
). The image is then processed using inferences, which are mental representations the individual has based on their world knowledge(1717 Ribeiro AF, Radanovic M. Inferential abilities based on pictorial stimuli in patients with right hemisphere damage. Dement Neuropsychol. 2014;8(3):243-8. PMid:29213910. ). We believe that the group data collection process with the volunteers elicited this processing: the image was presented to the individuals who then had to interpret it. This interpretation was possible (or not) based on the world knowledge held by the group of young high-educated adults. A high rate or percentage of image recognition was expected.

This world knowledge is directly related with the culture and experiences of the individual. If the representation of the image is compromised during any of these processes, particularly with regard to the mental representation of the stimulus, this inference is likely to be incomplete. This can lead to problems understanding the image, given that familiarity is an important predictor of this recognition(1818 Maldavsky D. Método de estudio de losdeseos y las defensas em el componente plástico del signo visual. Subj Procesos Cogn. 2015;19:149-73. ,1919 Pompéia S, Miranda MC, Bueno OF. A set of 400 pictures standardised for Portuguese: norms for name agreement, familiarity and visual complexity for children and adults. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2001;59(2-B):330-7. PMid:11460174. ). The generally low recognition of the pictures seen in Table 1 can be explained by 3 different hypotheses: the existence of cultural differences between the Brazilian and North-American populations; the fact that volunteers recognized the image, but this did not generate the expected context in the program; and difficulty interpreting pictures that represent actions, which are semantically more complex than nouns(2020 Yeatman JD, Kay KN. Bottom-up and top-down computations in word- and face- selective córtex. Computationaland Systems Biology. 2017;6:1-29. ). Regarding the overall mean recognition of pictures for each of the blocks, the statistical analysis showed that overall mean recognition of pictures from Blocks 1 and 8 was lower than that of pictures from Block 6. Comparisons among the other Blocks showed similar recognition rates. This finding might be explained by the type of syntactic structure used when participants have to write a sentence for the picture, allied with the cultural difference between the volunteers and North-American population. The first Block contains intransitive imperative sentences constituting commands with only one (intransitive) verb that does not require a complement. The use of intransitive imperative sentences is uncommon in Portuguese because, unlike in English, most verbs used for requests or commands require a complement. The pictures from Block 8 involve producing interrogative sentences which should be answered with a Yes or a No. Thus, from a picture, the individual should devise a sentence, e.g. “Do you eat meat?” This block had the lowest rates of answers matching those originally devised in the SPPA since, as outlined previously, the absence of a context widens the range of interpretations, especially if a picture is unclear. The pictures representing questions have broader and more open contexts that are harder to represent in a specific way. As a consequence, the range of answers increases and the recognition rate decreases. In this case, participants´ answers varied more when they had to devise questions, and the fact that questions are involved likely broadened interpretation and led to vaguer answers.

Block 6, whose pictures had higher recognition by the volunteers, correspond to declarative intransitive sentences (subject + verb without complement). The pictures in this block were clear and objective, i.e., elicited the target sentence. Also, the simplicity of the syntactic structure of this block was a facilitating factor.

In phase 2.1 of the study, in which pictures were presented to participants without their respective contexts ( Table 2 ), removal of context increased the number of different interpretations of the images. The pictures tended to be identified based on participants´ world knowledge and most interpretations failed to match the target phrases from the SPPA. This result confirmed that the pictures alone proved insufficient to elicit the target sentence of the program.

These images depict everyday scenes of three North-American families, but some activities performed by this population differ to those found in Brazil(2121 Campos TF, Carvalho SM, Melo LP, Lima AA. Figuras de atividades funcionais: concordância de nomeação e familiaridade. Psicol, Teor Pesqui. 2008;24(2):323-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-37722008000300008.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-3772200...
).

In phase 2.2 of the study, pictures together with their contexts were presented to the participants, who rated how familiar these were on an analog scale.

The statistical analysis revealed, in increasing order of recognition, which images plus contexts were least familiar to the participants for each of the Blocks of sentences. The results of comparisons showed that for all blocks, certain pictures when presented together with their respective sentences, were rated as having low familiarity by study participants. Individual analysis of each Block revealed that in Block 3, for example, only one stimuli differed in familiarity to the rest. By contrast, eight stimuli in Block 8 showed a statistically significant difference for familiarity. On average, at least 4 stimuli (sentences+pictures) per block were rated as less familiar relative to the other stimuli. This result indicates that, even when the pictures were combined with sentences, there were cultural barriers to identifying the stimuli. This finding strongly suggests that the SPPA requires extensive reformulation of some situations depicted in the pictures and others in the sentences, so that they may better reflect Brazilian culture. Chart 1 shows which images with low recognition need reformulating for use in Brazil.

A message conveyed by an image predicts the textual message because the image is a form of universal understandable information for populations with different languages and cultures. The processing of these images is reliant on language inferences, defined as mental representations that allow the construction of new knowledge drawing on data previously held by the interlocutor (world knowledge). This memory is activated and applied to language information explicit in a message. The frequency and familiarity of the semantic representations aid recognition of what they infer(2222 Hofmann AR. The american turners: their past and present. Rev Bras Ciênc Esporte. 2015;37(2):119-27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.rbce.2014.11.020.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.rbce.2014.1...
).

However, the degree of imageability and concreteness(2323 Rossi NF, Lindau TA, Gillam RB, Giacheti CM. Adaptação cultural do Test of Narrative Language (TNL) para o Português Brasileiro. Codas. 2016;28(5):507-16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20162016018. PMid:27683830.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/201...
) of the SPPA images failed to promote the recognition of the contexts these images are supposed to depict. Major cultural differences and comprehension difficulties for the stimuli presented were evident, where lower than expected ratings were given even for pictures representing everyday scenes of Brazilian culture. Images incorporated world knowledge of the population and were therefore expected to have a high level of recognition, but this was not the case for many of the stimuli. A number of pictures were poorly recognized when presented alone, but subsequently interpreted correctly when presented together with their contexts. This shows low recognition for pictures alone and highlights the importance of presenting images within a context to facilitate their comprehension, as occurs in the SPPA. However, determining the degree of recognition of the pictures and the familiarity of the situation produced by the contexts allows estimation of their role in processing the stimuli from the program. Not all pictures with low recognition rates proved easier to recognize when presented together with the context.

This study had some limitations including a difficulty inherent to group data collection of excluding participants and the large number of pictures presented, which may have led to participants losing interest in providing the best possible answer during the course of the procedure.

CONCLUSION

The pictures from the SPPA had a low degree of recognition and familiarity for the contexts, where adaptations are required for use of the program in the Brazilian population. The study also allowed identification of those pictures which need adapting before use in the rehabilitation of agrammatic patients in Brazil.

Appendix A Analysis of the recognition of each one of the SSPA sentences

Table A1
Mean recognition of figures of Block 1
Chart A1
Multiple comparison of mean recognition of block 1
Table A2
Mean recognition of figures of Block 2
Chart A2
Multiple comparison of mean recognition of block 2
Table A3
Mean recognition of figures of Block 3
Chart A3
Multiple comparison of mean recognition of block 3
Table A4
Mean recognition of figures of Block 4
Chart A4
Multiple comparison of mean recognition of block 4
Table A5
Mean recognition of figures of Block 5
Chart A5
Multiple comparison of mean recognition of block 5
Table A6
Mean recognition of figures of Block 6
Chart A6
Multiple comparison of mean recognition of block 6
Table A7
Mean recognition of figures of Block 7
Chart A7
Multiple comparison of mean recognition of block 7
Table A8
Mean recognition of figures of Block 8
Chart A8
Multiple comparison of mean recognition of block 8

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We particularly thank CNPq for the financial support.

  • Study conducted at Departamento de Fonoaudiologia, Universidade Federal de São Paulo -UNIFESP- São Paulo (SP), Brazil.
  • Financial support: The National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq).

REFERÊNCIAS

  • 1
    McNeil MR, Pratt SR. Defining aphasia: some theoretical and clinical implications of operating from a formal definition. Aphasiology. 2001;15(10-11):901-11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02687040143000276.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02687040143000276
  • 2
    Soares ECS, Ortiz KZ. Influence of brain lesion and educational background on language tests in aphasic subjects. Dement Neuropsychol. 2008;2(4):321-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN20400016. PMid:29213593.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN20400016
  • 3
    Soares ECS, Ortiz KZ. Influence of schooling on language abilities of adults without linguistic disorders. Sao Paulo Med J. 2009;127(3):134-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-31802009000300005. PMid:19820873.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-31802009000300005
  • 4
    Rodrigues TT, Jacuviske MV, Ortiz KZ. Perfil populacional de pacientes com distúrbios da comunicação humana decorrentes de lesão cerebral, assistidos em hospital terciário. Rev CEFAC. 2011;13:330-9.
  • 5
    Basso A, Cattaneo S, Girelli L, Luzzatti C, Miozzo A, Modena L, et al. Treatment eficacy of language and calculation disorders and speech apraxia: a review of the literature. Eur J Phys Rehabil Med. 2011;47(1):101-21. PMid:21448123.
  • 6
    Cho-Reyes S, Mack JE, Thompson CK. Grammatical encoding and learning in agrammatic aphasia: evidence from structural priming. J Mem Lang. 2016;91:202-218. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2016.02.004. PMid:28924328.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2016.02.004
  • 7
    Sang HK. Agrammatic aphasia verb and argument patterns in Kiswahili- English spontaneous language. S Afr J Commun Disord. 2015;62(1):1-10. http://dx.doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v62i1.89. PMid:26304215.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v62i1.89
  • 8
    Lise M, Obler K, Miceli G. A cross-language narrative sourcebook. 1st ed. Philadelphia: Jonh Benjamins Publishing Company; 1990.
  • 9
    Cuetos-Vega F, Domínguez A, Baauw S, Berthier-Torres ML. Disociación entre paciente agramáticos y anómicos em laproducción de formas verbales. Rev Neurol. 2007;44(4):203-8. PMid:17311209.
  • 10
    Faroqi-Shah Y, Friedman L. Production of verb tense in agrammatic afasia: a meta- analysis and further data. Behav Neurol. 2015;2015:983870. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2015/983870.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2015/983870
  • 11
    Silagi ML, Hirata FN, Mendonça LIZ. Sentence production in rehabilitation of agrammatism: a case study. Dement Neuropsychol. 2014;8(3):297-301. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642014DN83000015. PMid:29213917.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642014DN83000015
  • 12
    Andrade-Calderón P, Salvador-Cruz J, Sosa-Ortiz AL. Positive impact of speech therapy in progressive non-fluent aphasia. Acta Colomb Psicol. 2015;18(2):101-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.14718/ACP.2015.18.2.9.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.14718/ACP.2015.18.2.9
  • 13
    Thompson CK, Shapiro LP. A Linguistic-Specific Approach to Treatment of Sentence Production Deficits in Aphasia. Clinical Aphasiology. 1994;22:307-23.
  • 14
    Springer L, Huber W, Schlenck K-J, Schlenck C. Agrammatism: defict or compensation? Consequences of aphasia therapy. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2010;10(3):279-309. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/096020100389165.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/096020100389165
  • 15
    Helm-Estabrooks N, Nicholas M. Sentence production program for aphasia. 2nd ed. Austin: Pro-Ed; 2000.
  • 16
    Ishigaki EC, Lobrigate NL, Fonseca RP, Parente MA, Ortiz KZ. Análise do reconhecimento das figuras do teste MT Beta-86 modificado para uma adaptação: descrição e discussão de aspectos metodológicos. CoDAS. 2013;25(3):282-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2317-17822013000300015. PMid:24408341.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2317-17822013000300015
  • 17
    Ribeiro AF, Radanovic M. Inferential abilities based on pictorial stimuli in patients with right hemisphere damage. Dement Neuropsychol. 2014;8(3):243-8. PMid:29213910.
  • 18
    Maldavsky D. Método de estudio de losdeseos y las defensas em el componente plástico del signo visual. Subj Procesos Cogn. 2015;19:149-73.
  • 19
    Pompéia S, Miranda MC, Bueno OF. A set of 400 pictures standardised for Portuguese: norms for name agreement, familiarity and visual complexity for children and adults. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2001;59(2-B):330-7. PMid:11460174.
  • 20
    Yeatman JD, Kay KN. Bottom-up and top-down computations in word- and face- selective córtex. Computationaland Systems Biology. 2017;6:1-29.
  • 21
    Campos TF, Carvalho SM, Melo LP, Lima AA. Figuras de atividades funcionais: concordância de nomeação e familiaridade. Psicol, Teor Pesqui. 2008;24(2):323-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-37722008000300008.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-37722008000300008
  • 22
    Hofmann AR. The american turners: their past and present. Rev Bras Ciênc Esporte. 2015;37(2):119-27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.rbce.2014.11.020.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.rbce.2014.11.020
  • 23
    Rossi NF, Lindau TA, Gillam RB, Giacheti CM. Adaptação cultural do Test of Narrative Language (TNL) para o Português Brasileiro. Codas. 2016;28(5):507-16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20162016018. PMid:27683830.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20162016018

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    08 Apr 2019
  • Date of issue
    2019

History

  • Received
    28 Nov 2017
  • Accepted
    26 Sept 2018
Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia Al. Jaú, 684, 7º andar, 01420-002 São Paulo - SP Brasil, Tel./Fax 55 11 - 3873-4211 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista@codas.org.br