Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Linguistic performance in 2 years old preterm, considering chronological age and corrected age

ABSTRACT

Introduction

Preterm birth causes problems that are not restricted to perinatal mortality. Some premature, even in the absence of brain damage, have negative effects on various aspects of development, such as language difficulties.

Objective

This study aimed to verify the linguistic performance of preterm children at 2 years old, considering the chronological age and corrected age.

Methods

The study included 23 preterm children and applied the Test of Early Language Development- TELD-3 to assess the language skills.

Results

The premature children showed the linguistic performance alterations in Teld-3 in 39.13% of cases. They were also analyzed considering the delay to the chronological and corrected ages and there was no difference in performance for both receptive subtests (p = 0.250) and significant (p = 1.000).

Conclusion

The group of premature children at 2 years is a population at risk for language disorders that cannot be compensated with age correction.

Keywords:
Child Language; Language Development; Language Development Disorders; Premature

RESUMO

Introdução

O nascimento prematuro acarreta problemas que não se restringem à mortalidade perinatal. Alguns prematuros, mesmo na ausência de lesões cerebrais, apresentam consequências negativas em diversos aspectos do desenvolvimento, como dificuldades em adquirir linguagem.

Objetivo

O objetivo deste estudo foi verificar o desempenho linguístico de prematuros, na faixa etária de 2 anos, considerando a idade cronológica e a idade corrigida.

Métodos

Participaram do estudo 23 sujeitos prematuros e para verificar as habilidades linguísticas foi aplicado o Test of Early Language Development – TELD-3.

Resultados

Os sujeitos prematuros apresentaram desempenho total alterado no TELD-3 em 39,13% dos casos. Os prematuros também foram analisados considerando o atraso para a idade cronológica e a corrigida e não houve diferença no desempenho para os subtestes receptivo (p = 0,250) e expressivo (p = 1,000).

Conclusão

O grupo de prematuros aos 2 anos de idade constitui população de risco para alterações de linguagem que não podem ser compensadas com a correção da idade.

Descritores:
Linguagem Infantil; Desenvolvimento de Linguagem; Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem; Prematuro

INTRODUCTION

Premature birth is the main cause of perinatal mortality worldwide(11 Almeida PPM. Aspectos obstétricos da prematuridade. In: Pacchi P. O pré-termo: morbidade, diagnóstico e tratamento. São Paulo: Rocca; 2003. p. 1-22.,22 Graça LM. Parto pré-termo. In: Graça LM. Medicina materno-fetal. Lisboa: Lidel Edições Técnicas; 2010. p. 34-47.). The problems of prematurity are not restricted only to perinatal mortality, they also extend up to the difficulties in delivery care, the immediate care to the newborn, and to the risks of sequelae in various organs and systems(11 Almeida PPM. Aspectos obstétricos da prematuridade. In: Pacchi P. O pré-termo: morbidade, diagnóstico e tratamento. São Paulo: Rocca; 2003. p. 1-22.).

The advances in obstetrics and neonatology have resulted in better survival rates among children of premature birth(33 Msall ME. Developmental vulnerability and resilience in extremely preterm infants. JAMA. 2004;292(19):2399-401. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.292.19.2399. PMid:15547169.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.292.19.23...
), despite the difficulty in the rates of development remaining high(44 Marlow N. Neurocognitve outcome after very preterm birth. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2004;89(3):224-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/adc.2002.019752. PMid:15102725.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/adc.2002.01975...

5 Hack M, Costello DW. Trends in the rates of cerebral palsy associated with neonatal intensive care of preterm children. Clin Obstet Gynecol. 2008;51(4):763-74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/GRF.0b013e3181870922. PMid:18981801.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/GRF.0b013e3181...
-66 Anderson PJ. Neuropsychological outcomes of children born very preterm. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2014;19(2):90-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2013.11.012. PMid:24361279.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2013.11...
).

Some children that were born premature, in the absence of brain injuries, have negative consequences in several important aspects of development(77 Jansson-Verkasalo E, Ruusuvirta T, Huotilainen M, Alku P, Kushnerenko E, Suominen K, et al. Atypical perceptual narrowing in prematurely born infants is associated with compromised language acquisition at 2 years of age. BMC Neurosci. 2010;11(1):88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-11-88. PMid:20673357.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-11-8...
,88 Kallankari H, Kaukola T, Olsén P, Ojaniemi M, Hallman M. Very preterm birth and fetal growth restriction are associated with specific cognitive deficits in children attending mainstream school. Acta Paediatr. 2015;104(1):84-90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/apa.12811. PMid:25272976.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/apa.12811...
), such as difficulties in acquiring language(99 Bühler KEB, Limongi SCO, Diniz EMA. Language and cognition in very low birth weight preterm infants with PELCDO application. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2009;67(2A):242-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2009000200013. PMid:19547816.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2009...

10 Carlino FC, Lamônica DAC, Alvarenga KF. Avaliação da função auditiva receptive, expressive e visual em crianças prematuras. Pro Fono. 2012;22(1):19-24.
-1111 Silva GMD, Couto MIV, Molini-Avejonas DM. Risk factors identification in children with speech disorders: pilot study. CoDAS. 2013;25(5):456-62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2317-17822013000500010. PMid:24408550.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2317-17822013...
), poor cognitive ability(99 Bühler KEB, Limongi SCO, Diniz EMA. Language and cognition in very low birth weight preterm infants with PELCDO application. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2009;67(2A):242-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2009000200013. PMid:19547816.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2009...
,1212 Taylor HG, Minich NM, Klein N, Hack M. Longitudinal outcomes of very low birth weight: neuropsychological findings. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10(2):149-63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617704102038. PMid:15012835.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617704102...
), and linguistic and academic difficulties within the early school years(44 Marlow N. Neurocognitve outcome after very preterm birth. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2004;89(3):224-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/adc.2002.019752. PMid:15102725.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/adc.2002.01975...
,1313 Foster-Cohen SH, Friesen MD, Champion PR, Woodward LJ. High prevalence/low severity language delay in preschool children born very preterm. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2010;31(8):658-67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181e5ab7e. PMid:20613625.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181...

14 Sansavini A, Guarini A, Justice LM, Savini S, Broccoli S, Alessandroni R, et al. Does preterm birth increase a child’s risk for language impairment? Early Hum Dev. 2010;86(12):765-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2010.08.014. PMid:20846796.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2...

15 Pritchard VE, Bora S, Austin NC, Levin KJ, Woodward LJ. Identifying very preterm children at educational risk using a school readiness framework. Pediatrics. 2014;134(3):825-32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-3865. PMid:25113296.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-3865...

16 Maggiolo LM, Varela MV, Arancibia SC, Ruiz MF. Dificultades de lenguaje en niños preescolares con antecedente de prematuridad extrema. Rev Chil Pediatr. 2014;85(3):319-27. http://dx.doi.org/10.4067/S0370-41062014000300008. PMid:25697249.
http://dx.doi.org/10.4067/S0370-41062014...
-1717 Vohr B. Speech and language outcomes of very preterm infants. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2014;19(2):78-83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2013.10.007. PMid:24275068.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2013.10...
).

Premature children tend to present a lag in lexical development when compared with children born at term(1818 Isotani SM, Azevedo MF, Chiari BM, Perissinoto J. Expressive language of two year-old pre-term and full-term children (original title: Linguagem expressiva de crianças nascidas pré-termo e termo aos dois anos de idade). Pro Fono. 2009;21(2):155-60. PMid:19629327.). In receptive vocabulary tests, these children have lower mean score than that of the standardization of the applied test presented for the age(1919 Luu TM, Vohr BR, Schneider KC, Katz KH, Tucker R, Allan WC, et al. Trajectories of receptive language development from 3 to 12 years of age for very preterm children. Pediatrics. 2009;124(1):333-41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-2587. PMid:19564317.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-2587...
).

The grammatical development also seems to be affected in premature birth(1313 Foster-Cohen SH, Friesen MD, Champion PR, Woodward LJ. High prevalence/low severity language delay in preschool children born very preterm. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2010;31(8):658-67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181e5ab7e. PMid:20613625.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181...
) and these children present difficulties with complex functions of the language during their development(2020 van Noort-van der Spek IL, Franken MC, Weisglas-Kuperus N. Language functions in preterm-born children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2012;129(4):745-54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-1728. PMid:22430458.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-1728...
).

Premature children usually have slower cognitive progress than their peers born at term(99 Bühler KEB, Limongi SCO, Diniz EMA. Language and cognition in very low birth weight preterm infants with PELCDO application. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2009;67(2A):242-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2009000200013. PMid:19547816.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2009...
,1212 Taylor HG, Minich NM, Klein N, Hack M. Longitudinal outcomes of very low birth weight: neuropsychological findings. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10(2):149-63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617704102038. PMid:15012835.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617704102...
).

Most premature children will need support for the many changes they might present(33 Msall ME. Developmental vulnerability and resilience in extremely preterm infants. JAMA. 2004;292(19):2399-401. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.292.19.2399. PMid:15547169.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.292.19.23...
) and despite the risks seen in the growth and development of premature children, many factors may compensate these risks, such as the early intervention and support from these children’s families(2121 Spittle AJ, Ferrety C, Anderson PJ, Orton J, Eels A, Bates L, Boyd RN, Inder TE, Doyle LW. Improving the outcome of infants born at <30 weeks' gestation--a randomized controlled trial of preventative care at home. BMC Pediatr. 2009;9:73. PMID: 19954550.).

Most survivors of extremely low birth weight need a comprehensive and quality intervention and educational support during childhood(2222 Msall ME. Optimizing early development and understanding trajectories of resiliency after extreme prematurity. Pediatrics. 2009;124(1):387-90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2009-1149. PMid:19564325.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2009-1149...
,2323 Serenius F, Källén K, Blennow M, Ewald U, Fellman V, Holmström G, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcomes in extremely preterm infants at 2.5 years after active perinatal care in Sweden. JAMA. 2013;309(17):1810-20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.2013.3786. PMid:23632725.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.2013.3786...
).

This study is justified, as it will provide important information on the extension and kinds of linguistic deficit prevalent in the studied population. The identification of possible delays in linguistic development among premature children is necessary so that the professional team monitoring this population is able to perform an appropriate intervention, minimizing future deficits.

The objectives of this study were to verify the linguistic development of children with a history of prematurity and low weight at birth and at 2 years of age, and to compare the development considering the chronological and corrected age.

METHOD

This research was approved by the Ethics Committee for the Research Projects Analysis of the Hospital das Clínicas of the School of Medicine of the University of São Paulo under number 0719/11 and by the Research Ethics Committee of the School Hospital of the University of São Paulo under registration number 1240/12 – CAAE: 0713.0.015.000-11. Those responsible for the participants signed the informed consent.

The sample of the study consisted of children aged between 2 years and 2 years and 11 months, with a history of prematurity, who have follow-up monitoring at the clinics of two hospitals where they were prematurely born.

The subjects selected had absence of brain injury and/or any other pathology or alteration, which could justify a possible language alteration. The data of the births should include gestational age up to 36 weeks and birth weight under 2,500 g. They were invited to take part in the research at the day of their follow-up visits to the clinics. The children selected for the study did not have speech language rehabilitation before the study.

To carry out the study, we used a verification test for linguistic development: Test of Early Language Development – TELD-3(2424 Hersco WP, Reid DK, Hammil DD. Test of early language development. 3. ed. Austin: Pro-ed.; 1999.) translated and adapted into Brazilian Portuguese – BP(2525 Giusti E. Performance de crianças falantes do português brasileiro no test of early language development (Teld-3) [tese]. São Paulo (SP): Universidade de São Paulo; 2007. 211 p.). The TELD-3 is an early identification test for the alterations in the development process of the speech language of children aged between 2 years and 7 years and 11 months.

The adaptation of the TELD-3 into BP had results that allowed in stating that the performance of Brazilian children in the average development of language, in the referred study, is equivalent to the one of the original population (American) for the validation of the test, therefore, it being liable to be used without any other sociocultural or linguistic adaptation. The findings, after statistical analysis, indicated that the TELD-3 may be used as a measure of linguistic age of Brazilian children in language development. Thus, the Brazilian version of the test may be an excellent instrument not only for cross-cultural studies but also for verification, at the time of the diagnosis, of the deficit degree of the child, in case there is any, in addition to allowing the observation of the clinical evolution of children with communication disorders.

The test has two forms that, according to the authors, are equivalents (form A and form B). This research used only the form A of the test. The complete test kit consists of a set of toys, an album of pictures, and the protocol for the registration of the answers.

No national or international references were found related to the researches carried out with the TELD-3 among prematurely born children. Yet, the test was selected for performing this study, as it was an internationally known test, already translated, and adapted into BP.

After inclusion in the groups, the subjects were tested individually, in a silent and appropriate room of the clinic of premature follow-up. The application of the test was conducted in a single session of approximately 20 minutes.

For the TELD-3, the following procedures were conducted according to the application instructions in the guidebook of the test: the application of the test was always initiated by the receptive subtest and, later on, continued by the expressive subtest. For both subtests, the testing was initiated from the item indicated for the chronological age of the child, according to the registration protocol.

The first stage of the test consists of determining the base, which corresponds to correctly answering three items in the sequence. All items below the base will be considered as correct. The test is interrupted when the child makes mistakes in three items in the sequence determining the top and, therefore, all subsequent items will be considered as incorrect.

The answers of the subjects were registered in an appropriate and specific protocol. The child scores 1 point for each item of the subtest answered correctly and 0 (zero) points for each item answered incorrectly. There are scoring criteria for each and every item (which should be performed or answered by the child to obtain the points).

The answers of the subjects were analyzed separately for each subtest and the score of the subtests generated a total score for the TELD-3. This way, the subjects had a specific score for the receptive subtest and expressive subtest, in addition to the one for the total of the test.

After adding up the correct answers obtained, conversions of the gross scores of the subtests were done, using the conversion table of the test itself to determine the ratio (matched by age).

RESULTS

After the procedures of testing and tabulation of the data, the descriptive and inferential statistical analysis was initially implemented to explore the linguistic performance of children born prematurely.

The objective of this analysis was to verify the linguistic performance of the premature children at 2 years of age and to compare the performance considering both the chronological and corrected age of the subjects.

The groups of premature children consisted of 23 subjects, aged between 2 years and 2 years and 11 months, mean age of 28.69 months, that is, 2 years and 4 months of age.

To analyze the linguistic performance in the test, categories were created as provided by the TELD-3 test. The performance ratios classified as average or superior were categorized as appropriate, and the ratios classified as below average or inferior were categorized as altered.

Chart 1 shows the classifications proposed by the test and the category used for the statistical analysis.

Chart 1
Classification of the ratios provided by the TELD-3 and categories for the analysis

Table 1 presents the qualitative description of the group of subjects aged 2 years.

Table 1
Qualitative description of the premature group (n = 23)

We found that the sample of the study consists of 47.82% of male subjects and 52.18% of female subjects. There was an altered total performance in 39.13% of the premature subjects aged 2 years by TELD-3.

To explore the data, the delay in months in the receptive and expressive subtests was considered, as the total ratio of the test does not provide information on the equivalent age. The differences between the equivalent age provided by the test, the chronological age of the child, and their corrected age were calculated.

Table 2 presents the comparison of performance (with or without delay) considering the initial situation with the chronological age and, then, with the correction in the age.

Table 2
Comparison of the performance in the Receptive and Expressive subtests of the TELD-3 considering chronological age and corrected age (p < 0.05)

The results indicated that there was no significant difference between the situation of delay considering the chronological and the corrected age of subjects in both subtests: the receptive and the expressive one.

DISCUSSION

Initially we found that 39.13% of the premature subjects had altered total performance by the TELD-3, which corroborates the many researches(99 Bühler KEB, Limongi SCO, Diniz EMA. Language and cognition in very low birth weight preterm infants with PELCDO application. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2009;67(2A):242-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2009000200013. PMid:19547816.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2009...
,1111 Silva GMD, Couto MIV, Molini-Avejonas DM. Risk factors identification in children with speech disorders: pilot study. CoDAS. 2013;25(5):456-62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2317-17822013000500010. PMid:24408550.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2317-17822013...
,1818 Isotani SM, Azevedo MF, Chiari BM, Perissinoto J. Expressive language of two year-old pre-term and full-term children (original title: Linguagem expressiva de crianças nascidas pré-termo e termo aos dois anos de idade). Pro Fono. 2009;21(2):155-60. PMid:19629327.) pointing to premature birth as a risk for language difficulties.

The age range of 2 years in the premature subjects was selected to compose the research, as it offers the possibility of us studying an interesting aspect, when it comes to premature subjects, which is the correction of age. In clinical practice, it was used in discounting the gestational time that was not completed in the chronological gestational age.

In a study where premature subjects were tested with the Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ), it was verified that the ideal age to identify language difficulties in premature children was 24 months(2626 Boyer J, Flamant C, Boussicault G, Berlie I, Gascoin G, Branger B, et al. Characterizing early detection of language difficulties in children born preterm. Early Hum Dev. 2014;90(6):281-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.03.005. PMid:24726534.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2...
). This age is excellent for the reality of the services studied, as they correspond to the greater concentration of the population served.

Studies involving premature children and the development of the language have already been conducted considering the corrected age of the children(1313 Foster-Cohen SH, Friesen MD, Champion PR, Woodward LJ. High prevalence/low severity language delay in preschool children born very preterm. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2010;31(8):658-67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181e5ab7e. PMid:20613625.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181...
,2626 Boyer J, Flamant C, Boussicault G, Berlie I, Gascoin G, Branger B, et al. Characterizing early detection of language difficulties in children born preterm. Early Hum Dev. 2014;90(6):281-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.03.005. PMid:24726534.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2...

27 Stolt S, Korja R, Matomäki J, Lapinleimu H, Haataja L, Lehtonen L. Early relations between language development and the quality of mother–child interaction in very-low-birth-weight children. Early Hum Dev. 2014;90(5):219-25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.02.007. PMid:24636213.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2...
-2828 Bapat R, Narayana PA, Zhou Y, Parikh NA. Magnetic resonance spectroscopy at term-equivalent age in extremely preterm infants: association with cognitive and language development. Pediatr Neurol. 2014;51(1):53-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2014.03.011. PMid:24938140.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pediatrneuro...
).

Thus, we may question whether the delay in language development among premature children could correspond to this difference between the gestational age in which the child was born and the complete gestational time of 40 weeks.

Analyses were done to test the groups of 2-year-old premature children in two different situations: the existence of delay considering the linguistic age pointed out in the test and the chronological age of the subject and the existence of delay considering the linguistic age and the corrected age of the subjects. The results showed that there was no significant difference between the two situations.

Thus, we may observe that the delay in language development observed in 2-year-old premature children is not compensated by a correction of age. Therefore, in clinical practice, the results of this study should be considered in a way that premature children are always observed carefully regarding the risk of language alterations, as the correction of age does not compensate the occasionally presented alterations.

Although the results found are promising, one of the limitations of the study was the reduced number of subjects because of the shortage of suitable site for the conduction of the test; as it is a study carried out in high demand medical offices of premature follow-up monitoring, it reduces the space for concurrent activities.

Thus, as exposed, it is essential that children born premature are monitored for early identification of the ones who will eventually have language difficulties, as the early intervention in those cases may reduce or eliminate the impact of such deficits in the development of this population, as highlighted in several studies(2222 Msall ME. Optimizing early development and understanding trajectories of resiliency after extreme prematurity. Pediatrics. 2009;124(1):387-90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2009-1149. PMid:19564325.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2009-1149...
,2323 Serenius F, Källén K, Blennow M, Ewald U, Fellman V, Holmström G, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcomes in extremely preterm infants at 2.5 years after active perinatal care in Sweden. JAMA. 2013;309(17):1810-20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.2013.3786. PMid:23632725.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.2013.3786...
,2929 Wild KT, Betancourt LM, Brodsky NL, Hurt H. The effect of socioeconomic status on the language outcomes of preterm infants at toddler age. Early Hum Dev. 2013;89(9):743-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.05.008. PMid:23803578.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2...
).

CONCLUSIONS

We verified that the 2-year-old premature subjects had altered total performance, configuring a risk population for language alterations. The correction of age for the 2-year-old premature does not compensate the delay, if present.

The follow-up of premature children is essential, to identify early the ones who will have difficulties in language, so that the intervention begins with reducing or eliminating these deficits in the development of this population.

  • Study carried out at the Preterm Clinic of the Neonatology Unit of the Pediatric Department, School Hospital (SH), Universidade de São Paulo – USP – and in the Risk newborns Monitoring Clinic, Children’s Institute (Instituto da Criança – ICr), Hospital das Clínicas, School of Medicine, Universidade de São Paulo - USP.
  • Financial support: Higher Education Personnel Improvement Coordination (Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior – CAPES), in the form of a Doctorate´s scholarship.

REFERÊNCIAS

  • 1
    Almeida PPM. Aspectos obstétricos da prematuridade. In: Pacchi P. O pré-termo: morbidade, diagnóstico e tratamento. São Paulo: Rocca; 2003. p. 1-22.
  • 2
    Graça LM. Parto pré-termo. In: Graça LM. Medicina materno-fetal. Lisboa: Lidel Edições Técnicas; 2010. p. 34-47.
  • 3
    Msall ME. Developmental vulnerability and resilience in extremely preterm infants. JAMA. 2004;292(19):2399-401. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.292.19.2399 PMid:15547169.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.292.19.2399
  • 4
    Marlow N. Neurocognitve outcome after very preterm birth. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2004;89(3):224-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/adc.2002.019752 PMid:15102725.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/adc.2002.019752
  • 5
    Hack M, Costello DW. Trends in the rates of cerebral palsy associated with neonatal intensive care of preterm children. Clin Obstet Gynecol. 2008;51(4):763-74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/GRF.0b013e3181870922 PMid:18981801.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/GRF.0b013e3181870922
  • 6
    Anderson PJ. Neuropsychological outcomes of children born very preterm. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2014;19(2):90-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2013.11.012 PMid:24361279.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2013.11.012
  • 7
    Jansson-Verkasalo E, Ruusuvirta T, Huotilainen M, Alku P, Kushnerenko E, Suominen K, et al. Atypical perceptual narrowing in prematurely born infants is associated with compromised language acquisition at 2 years of age. BMC Neurosci. 2010;11(1):88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-11-88 PMid:20673357.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-11-88
  • 8
    Kallankari H, Kaukola T, Olsén P, Ojaniemi M, Hallman M. Very preterm birth and fetal growth restriction are associated with specific cognitive deficits in children attending mainstream school. Acta Paediatr. 2015;104(1):84-90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/apa.12811 PMid:25272976.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/apa.12811
  • 9
    Bühler KEB, Limongi SCO, Diniz EMA. Language and cognition in very low birth weight preterm infants with PELCDO application. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2009;67(2A):242-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2009000200013 PMid:19547816.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2009000200013
  • 10
    Carlino FC, Lamônica DAC, Alvarenga KF. Avaliação da função auditiva receptive, expressive e visual em crianças prematuras. Pro Fono. 2012;22(1):19-24.
  • 11
    Silva GMD, Couto MIV, Molini-Avejonas DM. Risk factors identification in children with speech disorders: pilot study. CoDAS. 2013;25(5):456-62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2317-17822013000500010 PMid:24408550.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S2317-17822013000500010
  • 12
    Taylor HG, Minich NM, Klein N, Hack M. Longitudinal outcomes of very low birth weight: neuropsychological findings. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10(2):149-63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617704102038 PMid:15012835.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617704102038
  • 13
    Foster-Cohen SH, Friesen MD, Champion PR, Woodward LJ. High prevalence/low severity language delay in preschool children born very preterm. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2010;31(8):658-67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181e5ab7e PMid:20613625.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181e5ab7e
  • 14
    Sansavini A, Guarini A, Justice LM, Savini S, Broccoli S, Alessandroni R, et al. Does preterm birth increase a child’s risk for language impairment? Early Hum Dev. 2010;86(12):765-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2010.08.014 PMid:20846796.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2010.08.014
  • 15
    Pritchard VE, Bora S, Austin NC, Levin KJ, Woodward LJ. Identifying very preterm children at educational risk using a school readiness framework. Pediatrics. 2014;134(3):825-32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-3865 PMid:25113296.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-3865
  • 16
    Maggiolo LM, Varela MV, Arancibia SC, Ruiz MF. Dificultades de lenguaje en niños preescolares con antecedente de prematuridad extrema. Rev Chil Pediatr. 2014;85(3):319-27. http://dx.doi.org/10.4067/S0370-41062014000300008 PMid:25697249.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.4067/S0370-41062014000300008
  • 17
    Vohr B. Speech and language outcomes of very preterm infants. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2014;19(2):78-83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2013.10.007 PMid:24275068.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2013.10.007
  • 18
    Isotani SM, Azevedo MF, Chiari BM, Perissinoto J. Expressive language of two year-old pre-term and full-term children (original title: Linguagem expressiva de crianças nascidas pré-termo e termo aos dois anos de idade). Pro Fono. 2009;21(2):155-60. PMid:19629327.
  • 19
    Luu TM, Vohr BR, Schneider KC, Katz KH, Tucker R, Allan WC, et al. Trajectories of receptive language development from 3 to 12 years of age for very preterm children. Pediatrics. 2009;124(1):333-41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-2587 PMid:19564317.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-2587
  • 20
    van Noort-van der Spek IL, Franken MC, Weisglas-Kuperus N. Language functions in preterm-born children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2012;129(4):745-54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-1728 PMid:22430458.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-1728
  • 21
    Spittle AJ, Ferrety C, Anderson PJ, Orton J, Eels A, Bates L, Boyd RN, Inder TE, Doyle LW. Improving the outcome of infants born at <30 weeks' gestation--a randomized controlled trial of preventative care at home. BMC Pediatr. 2009;9:73. PMID: 19954550.
  • 22
    Msall ME. Optimizing early development and understanding trajectories of resiliency after extreme prematurity. Pediatrics. 2009;124(1):387-90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2009-1149 PMid:19564325.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2009-1149
  • 23
    Serenius F, Källén K, Blennow M, Ewald U, Fellman V, Holmström G, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcomes in extremely preterm infants at 2.5 years after active perinatal care in Sweden. JAMA. 2013;309(17):1810-20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.2013.3786 PMid:23632725.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.2013.3786
  • 24
    Hersco WP, Reid DK, Hammil DD. Test of early language development. 3. ed. Austin: Pro-ed.; 1999.
  • 25
    Giusti E. Performance de crianças falantes do português brasileiro no test of early language development (Teld-3) [tese]. São Paulo (SP): Universidade de São Paulo; 2007. 211 p.
  • 26
    Boyer J, Flamant C, Boussicault G, Berlie I, Gascoin G, Branger B, et al. Characterizing early detection of language difficulties in children born preterm. Early Hum Dev. 2014;90(6):281-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.03.005 PMid:24726534.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.03.005
  • 27
    Stolt S, Korja R, Matomäki J, Lapinleimu H, Haataja L, Lehtonen L. Early relations between language development and the quality of mother–child interaction in very-low-birth-weight children. Early Hum Dev. 2014;90(5):219-25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.02.007 PMid:24636213.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.02.007
  • 28
    Bapat R, Narayana PA, Zhou Y, Parikh NA. Magnetic resonance spectroscopy at term-equivalent age in extremely preterm infants: association with cognitive and language development. Pediatr Neurol. 2014;51(1):53-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2014.03.011 PMid:24938140.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2014.03.011
  • 29
    Wild KT, Betancourt LM, Brodsky NL, Hurt H. The effect of socioeconomic status on the language outcomes of preterm infants at toddler age. Early Hum Dev. 2013;89(9):743-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.05.008 PMid:23803578.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.05.008

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Apr 2016

History

  • Received
    07 May 2015
  • Accepted
    11 July 2015
Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia Al. Jaú, 684, 7º andar, 01420-002 São Paulo - SP Brasil, Tel./Fax 55 11 - 3873-4211 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista@codas.org.br