Abstracts
Introduction
Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) has been used to modify cortical excitability and promote motor learning.
Objective
To systematically review published data to investigate the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on motor learning in healthy individuals.
Methods
Randomized or quasi-randomized studies that evaluated the tDCS effects on motor learning were included and the risk of bias was examined by Cochrane Collaboration’s tool. The following electronic databases were used: PubMed, Scopus, Web of Science, LILACS, CINAHL with no language restriction.
Results
It was found 160 studies; after reading the title and abstract, 17 of those were selected, but just 4 were included. All studies involved healthy, right-handed adults. All studies assessed motor learning by the Jebsen Taylor Test or by the Serial Finger Tapping Task (SFTT). Almost all studies were randomized and all were blinding for participants. Some studies presented differences at SFTT protocol.
Conclusion
The result is insufficient to draw conclusions if tDCS influences the motor learning. Furthermore, there was significant heterogeneity of the stimulation parameters used. Further researches are needed to investigate the parameters that are more important for motor learning improvement and measure whether the effects are long-lasting or limited in time.
Electric stimulation; Learning; Neuronal plasticity
Introdução
A estimulação transcraniana por corrente contínua (ETCC) tem sido usada para modificar a excitabilidade cortical e promover o aprendizado motor.
Objetivo
Revisar sistematicamente os dados publicados para investigar os efeitos da estimulação transcraniana por corrente contínua sobre o aprendizado motor em indivíduos saudáveis.
Métodos
Foram incluídos estudos randomizados ou quase randomizados que avaliaram os efeitos da ETCC sobre o aprendizado motor. O risco de viés foi avaliado por meio da ferramenta Cochrane Collaboration. As seguintes bases de dados eletrônicas foram utilizadas: PubMed, Scopus, Web of Science, LILACS, CINAHL, sem restrição de idioma.
Resultados
Foram encontrados 160 estudos. Depois de ler o título e o resumo, 17 deles foram selecionados, mas apenas 4 foram incluídos. Todos os estudos envolveram adultos saudáveis e destros e avaliaram o aprendizado motor por meio do Jebsen Taylor Test ou do Serial Finger Tapping Task (SFTT). Quase todos os estudos foram randomizados e todos foram cegos para os participantes. Alguns estudos apresentaram diferenças no protocolo do SFTT.
Conclusão
O resultado é insuficiente para tirar conclusões se a ETCC influencia o aprendizado motor. Além disso, houve uma significativa heterogeneidade dos parâmetros de estimulação utilizados nos estudos. Futuras pesquisas são necessárias para investigar quais são os parâmetros mais importantes para a melhoria do aprendizado motor e medir se os efeitos são duradouros ou limitados ao longo do tempo.
Estimulação elétrica; Aprendizado; Plasticidade neuronal
Introduction
Since transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) was introduced by Nitsche and
Paulus in 2000, it has been used to modify cortical excitability in a non-invasive
and painless way (11 Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Excitability changes induced in the human
motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J Physiol.
2000;527(Pt 3):633-9., 22 Zaehle T, Sandmann P, Thorne JD, Jancke L, Herrmann CS. Transcranial
direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex modulates working memory
performance: combined behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. BMC
Neurosci. 2011;12:2. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-12-2.). Furthermore, tDCS has been shown to be effective for
promoting motor learning in healthy subjects (33 Kantak SS, Mummidisetty CK, Stinear JW. Primary motor and premotor
cortex in implicit sequence learning–evidence for competition between implicit
and explicit human motor memory systems. Eur J Neurosci.
2012;36(5):2710-5.
4 Nitsche MA, Schauenburg A, Lang N, Liebetanz D, Exner C, Paulus W,
et al. Facilitation of implicit motor learning by weak transcranial direct
current stimulation of the primary motor cortex in the human. J Cogn Neurosci.
2003;15(4):619-26.-55 Marquez CMS, Zhang X, Swinnen SP, Meesen R, Wenderoth N.
Task-specific effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on motor
learning. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:333.) and patients with brain
disorders (66 Fregni F, Boggio PS, Santos MC, Lima M, Vieira AL, Rigonatti SP, et
al. Noninvasive cortical stimulation with transcranial direct current
stimulation in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord.
2006;21(10):1693-702.
7 Reis J, Fritsch B. Modulation of motor performance and motor
learning by transcranial direct current stimulation. Curr Opin Neurol.
2011;24(6):590-6.-88 Schambra HM, Abe M, Luckenbaugh DA, Reis J, Krakauer JW, Cohen LG.
Probing for hemispheric specialization for motor skill learning: a transcranial
direct current stimulation study. J Neurophysiol.
2011;106(2):652-61.).
Modulating externally the brain excitability with the proposal of understand the
mechanisms involved in motor learning has been largely employed in the last decade
(99 Vines BW, Cerruti C, Schlaug G. Dual-hemisphere tDCS facilitates
greater improvements for healthy subjects' non-dominant hand compared to
uni-hemisphere stimulation. BMC Neurosci. 2008;9:103.
10 Tecchio F, Zappasodi F, Assenza G, Tombini M, Vollaro S, Barbati G,
et al. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation enhances procedural
consolidation. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104(2):1134-40.
11 Nitsche MA, Doemkes S, Karakose T, Antal A, Liebetanz D, Lang N, et
al. Shaping the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation of the human
motor cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2007;97(4):3109-17.
12 Boggio PS, Castro LO, Savagim EA, Braite R, Cruz VC, Rocha RR, et
al. Enhancement of non-dominant hand motor function by anodal transcranial
direct current stimulation. Neurosci Lett. 2006;404(1-2):232-6.
13 Hummel FC, Heise K, Celnik P, Floel A, Gerloff C, Cohen LG.
Facilitating skilled right hand motor function in older subjects by anodal
polarization over the left primary motor cortex. Neurobiol Aging.
2010;31(12):2160-8.
14 Stagg CJ, Jayaram G, Pastor D, Kincses ZT, Matthews PM,
Johansen-Berg H. Polarity and timing-dependent effects of transcranial direct
current stimulation in explicit motor learning. Neuropsychologia.
2011;49(5):800-4.
15 Vines BW, Nair DG, Schlaug G. Contralateral and ipsilateral motor
effects after transcranial direct current stimulation. Neuroreport.
2006;17(6):671-4.-1616 Vines BW, Nair D, Schlaug G. Modulating activity in the motor cortex
affects performance for the two hands differently depending upon which
hemisphere is stimulated. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28(8):1667-73.). Electrophysiological data demonstrate that changes of neuronal
activity and excitability accompany the learning of new motor skill (1717 Clark VP, Coffman BA, Mayer AR, Weisend MP, Lane TD, Calhoun VD, et
al. TDCS guided using fMRI significantly accelerates learning to identify
concealed objects. Neuroimage. 2012;59(1):117-28.). As improving motor learning is the aim the
therapy of many neurological and musculoskeletal conditions, tDCS has been pointed
out as a therapeutic promise for enhancing clinical outcomes in these conditions
(1818 Schabrun SM, Chipchase LS. Priming the brain to learn: The future of
therapy? Man Ther. 2012;17(2):184-6.). However, it is known that tDCS
modulate the brain activity specific to the polarity, location of application and
other parameters of stimulation (e.g. duration, intensity, size of electrode) (11 Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Excitability changes induced in the human
motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J Physiol.
2000;527(Pt 3):633-9., 1414 Stagg CJ, Jayaram G, Pastor D, Kincses ZT, Matthews PM,
Johansen-Berg H. Polarity and timing-dependent effects of transcranial direct
current stimulation in explicit motor learning. Neuropsychologia.
2011;49(5):800-4.,
1919 Liebetanz D, Nitsche MA, Tergau F, Paulus W. Pharmacological
approach to the mechanisms of transcranial DC‐stimulation‐induced after‐effects
of human motor cortex excitability. Brain.
2002;125(10):2238-47.). Then, before using it in clinical
practice, it is crucial to determine the best stimulation parameters required to
increase motor learning, as well as to consider the effective ability of tDCS to
improve motor learning.
Here, the studies addressing the effects of tDCS on motor learning over the non-dominant upper limb motor function in healthy individuals were systematically reviewed. Furthermore, the purpose of the current review was to investigate the parameters of stimulation recommended in these studies.
Methods
Literature research and Selection criteria
A literature research was performed using the following databases: PubMed, Scopus, Web of Science, LILACS, CINAHL, from their inception to January 2014.
The following key words were used: ‘transcranial direct current stimulation’, ‘tDCS’ or ‘direct current stimulation’, ‘motor skill’ or ‘motor learning’, ‘upper extremity’ or ‘non-dominant upper extremity’, ‘healthy subjects’, and any other associated variation. These terms were used in various combinations to find relevant studies. In addition to searching the database, the reference lists of all retrieved papers were searched for any related publications unidentified by the initial search strategy.
Two reviewers (AF and SR) screened independently the title and abstracts identified from the database research to assess whether they met the predefined inclusion criteria. The inclusion and exclusion criteria are listed in Table 1. Full text articles from the potentially relevant studies were reviewed to determine the studies to be included in the review. Differences of opinion between reviewers were resolved by consulting the opinion of a third reviewer (KMS).
Outcome measures
Randomized and quasi-randomized controlled clinical trials that evaluated the effects of tDCS on motor learning as primary or secondary outcome measures were included. The motor learning had to be assessed by a performance motor test done with non dominant upper limbs before and after tDCS, i.e., “off-line” studies.
Risk of bias assessment
The Cochrane Collaboration’s tool (Reviewer’s Handbook version 5.1.0) was used to assess the risk of bias of the included studies. Through five items, this tool evaluates selection, execution, detection and publication bias. In each item the evaluator considers a low, unclear or high risk of bias. In this systematic review, for each methodological procedure the “low risk of bias” was considered when the authors cited the item above the text, “high risk of bias” when the authors report that did not perform it and “unclear risk of bias” when it was not clear whether it was done.
Data extraction
The following data relevant to the aims of this study were extracted: (11 Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J Physiol. 2000;527(Pt 3):633-9.) study design; (22 Zaehle T, Sandmann P, Thorne JD, Jancke L, Herrmann CS. Transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex modulates working memory performance: combined behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. BMC Neurosci. 2011;12:2. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-12-2.) characteristics of subjects; (33 Kantak SS, Mummidisetty CK, Stinear JW. Primary motor and premotor cortex in implicit sequence learning–evidence for competition between implicit and explicit human motor memory systems. Eur J Neurosci. 2012;36(5):2710-5.) outcome measures and tDCS parameters; and (44 Nitsche MA, Schauenburg A, Lang N, Liebetanz D, Exner C, Paulus W, et al. Facilitation of implicit motor learning by weak transcranial direct current stimulation of the primary motor cortex in the human. J Cogn Neurosci. 2003;15(4):619-26.) mean ± standard deviation (SD) of motor outcome before and immediately post intervention. Given the purpose of this review, only the data of non-dominant upper extremity were extracted.
Results
Identification and selection of studies
The literature research of on-line databases identified 160 studies. After removal of the duplicates, the research yielded 87 citations. After the exclusion based on title and abstract, 17 potentially relevant articles were obtained and evaluated by two independent reviews (AF and SR), and five papers that met our eligibility criteria were analyzed. Two papers (1515 Vines BW, Nair DG, Schlaug G. Contralateral and ipsilateral motor effects after transcranial direct current stimulation. Neuroreport. 2006;17(6):671-4.-1616 Vines BW, Nair D, Schlaug G. Modulating activity in the motor cortex affects performance for the two hands differently depending upon which hemisphere is stimulated. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28(8):1667-73.) resulted of the same study and the results obtained in one of them (1515 Vines BW, Nair DG, Schlaug G. Contralateral and ipsilateral motor effects after transcranial direct current stimulation. Neuroreport. 2006;17(6):671-4.) were shown in the other (1616 Vines BW, Nair D, Schlaug G. Modulating activity in the motor cortex affects performance for the two hands differently depending upon which hemisphere is stimulated. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28(8):1667-73.) with a larger sample, so four studies were considered and included (Figure 1).
Risk of bias
All studies showed more than one type of bias (Figure 2). Just one study did not perform randomization (1010 Tecchio F, Zappasodi F, Assenza G, Tombini M, Vollaro S, Barbati G, et al. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation enhances procedural consolidation. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104(2):1134-40.) and all of them failed in reporting the concealment of treatment allocation (99 Vines BW, Cerruti C, Schlaug G. Dual-hemisphere tDCS facilitates greater improvements for healthy subjects' non-dominant hand compared to uni-hemisphere stimulation. BMC Neurosci. 2008;9:103., 1010 Tecchio F, Zappasodi F, Assenza G, Tombini M, Vollaro S, Barbati G, et al. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation enhances procedural consolidation. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104(2):1134-40., 1212 Boggio PS, Castro LO, Savagim EA, Braite R, Cruz VC, Rocha RR, et al. Enhancement of non-dominant hand motor function by anodal transcranial direct current stimulation. Neurosci Lett. 2006;404(1-2):232-6., 1616 Vines BW, Nair D, Schlaug G. Modulating activity in the motor cortex affects performance for the two hands differently depending upon which hemisphere is stimulated. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28(8):1667-73.). Two studies did not mention if the evaluators were blinding (99 Vines BW, Cerruti C, Schlaug G. Dual-hemisphere tDCS facilitates greater improvements for healthy subjects' non-dominant hand compared to uni-hemisphere stimulation. BMC Neurosci. 2008;9:103., 1616 Vines BW, Nair D, Schlaug G. Modulating activity in the motor cortex affects performance for the two hands differently depending upon which hemisphere is stimulated. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28(8):1667-73.). Three studies failed in reporting if the outcome assessor was blinding (1010 Tecchio F, Zappasodi F, Assenza G, Tombini M, Vollaro S, Barbati G, et al. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation enhances procedural consolidation. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104(2):1134-40., 1212 Boggio PS, Castro LO, Savagim EA, Braite R, Cruz VC, Rocha RR, et al. Enhancement of non-dominant hand motor function by anodal transcranial direct current stimulation. Neurosci Lett. 2006;404(1-2):232-6., 1616 Vines BW, Nair D, Schlaug G. Modulating activity in the motor cortex affects performance for the two hands differently depending upon which hemisphere is stimulated. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28(8):1667-73.). All studies presented selective reporting of outcomes (99 Vines BW, Cerruti C, Schlaug G. Dual-hemisphere tDCS facilitates greater improvements for healthy subjects' non-dominant hand compared to uni-hemisphere stimulation. BMC Neurosci. 2008;9:103.-1010 Tecchio F, Zappasodi F, Assenza G, Tombini M, Vollaro S, Barbati G, et al. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation enhances procedural consolidation. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104(2):1134-40., 1212 Boggio PS, Castro LO, Savagim EA, Braite R, Cruz VC, Rocha RR, et al. Enhancement of non-dominant hand motor function by anodal transcranial direct current stimulation. Neurosci Lett. 2006;404(1-2):232-6., 1616 Vines BW, Nair D, Schlaug G. Modulating activity in the motor cortex affects performance for the two hands differently depending upon which hemisphere is stimulated. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28(8):1667-73.).
TDCS protocol
The stimulation parameters of tDCS varied among studies and are summarized in Table 2. All studies included used stimulation intensity of 1mA and time duration over 15 min. The parameters of electrode size and tDCS type were heterogeneous among the studies. The cortical area stimulated was the primary motor cortex (M1) in all studies.
Overview of included studies
Table 3 shows the main characteristics of the studies included in the systematic review. In sum, 85 healthy, right-handed adults were evaluated. Sham treatment was given to 63 patients and 63 patients were submitted to active tDCS. All studies verified improvement in motor performance of non-dominant hand and investigated the upper extremity dominance was by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory a sufficient means of assessment of the handedness aspect (2020 Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97-113.).
Only one study (1212 Boggio PS, Castro LO, Savagim EA, Braite R, Cruz VC, Rocha RR, et al. Enhancement of non-dominant hand motor function by anodal transcranial direct current stimulation. Neurosci Lett. 2006;404(1-2):232-6.) assessed the effects of tDCS on motor learning by Jebsen Taylor Hand Function Test (JTT). Three studies (99 Vines BW, Cerruti C, Schlaug G. Dual-hemisphere tDCS facilitates greater improvements for healthy subjects' non-dominant hand compared to uni-hemisphere stimulation. BMC Neurosci. 2008;9:103.-1010 Tecchio F, Zappasodi F, Assenza G, Tombini M, Vollaro S, Barbati G, et al. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation enhances procedural consolidation. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104(2):1134-40., 1616 Vines BW, Nair D, Schlaug G. Modulating activity in the motor cortex affects performance for the two hands differently depending upon which hemisphere is stimulated. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28(8):1667-73.) applied the serial finger tapping task (SFTT). The SFTT required subjects to press four numeric keys on a standard computer keyboard with the fingers, repeating a random or a sequential five element sequence “as quickly and as accurately as possible” for a period of 30s. The numeric sequence was displayed at the top of the screen at all times to exclude any working memory component to the task. Each key press produced a white dot on the screen, forming a row from left to right, rather than the number itself, so as not to provide accuracy feedback. The computer recorded the key press responses, and each 30s trial was automatically scored for the number of complete sequences achieved (speed) and the number of errors made (accuracy) a rest period of 30s between trials was applied (2121 Walker MP, Brakefield T, Morgan A, Hobson JA, Stickgold R. Practice with sleep makes perfect: sleep-dependent motor skill learning. Neuron. 2002;35(1):205-11.). One study (1010 Tecchio F, Zappasodi F, Assenza G, Tombini M, Vollaro S, Barbati G, et al. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation enhances procedural consolidation. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104(2):1134-40.) modified the SFTT and submitted subjects to random and sequential nine-element series and given an accuracy feedback to the subjects.
Discussion
This systematic review suggests that tDCS affects motor learning process of the non-dominant upper extremity in healthy adults, but it was not conclusive concerning the tDCS parameters (current intensity, electrode size, stimulation time and type) to be applied for this. All studies included presented risk of bias and failed in reveled the effect size of tDCS on motor learning.
The main objective of a systematic review is to assess the studies risk of bias, irrespectively of the anticipated variability in either the results. For instance, the results may be consistent among studies but all the studies may be flawed (2222 Higgins J, Altman DG. Assessing risk of bias in included studies. In: Higgins JPT, Green S. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. Chichester; Hoboken: Wiley-Blackwell; 2008. p. 187-241.).
Selection risk of bias were presented in all studies included, this type of bias refers to systematic differences between baseline characteristics of the groups that are compared. The only strength of randomization is that, if successfully accomplished, it prevents selection bias in allocating interventions to participants (2222 Higgins J, Altman DG. Assessing risk of bias in included studies. In: Higgins JPT, Green S. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. Chichester; Hoboken: Wiley-Blackwell; 2008. p. 187-241.). Two studies (99 Vines BW, Cerruti C, Schlaug G. Dual-hemisphere tDCS facilitates greater improvements for healthy subjects' non-dominant hand compared to uni-hemisphere stimulation. BMC Neurosci. 2008;9:103., 1616 Vines BW, Nair D, Schlaug G. Modulating activity in the motor cortex affects performance for the two hands differently depending upon which hemisphere is stimulated. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28(8):1667-73.) did not report if the evaluators were blinding, so, it presented execution risk of bias. In all studies the outcome assessor was not blinding, it is considering a detection risk of bias and could affect the outcome measurements, considering that detection bias refers to systematic differences between groups in how outcomes are determined. All studies presented selective reporting of outcomes, setting up a publication risk of bias this type of bias is one of the most substantial biases affecting results from individual studies (2323 Dickersin K. Publication bias: recognizing the problem, understanding its origins and scope, and preventing harm. In: Rothstein HR, Sutton AJ, Borenstein M, editors. Publication bias in meta-analysis: prevention, assessment and adjustments. Chichester; Hoboken: Wiley, 2005. p. 11-33.).
All studies were homogeneous regarding the population evaluated and assessed healthy,
right-handed adults. Motor function was assessed by JTT or SFTT, tools recognized in
the literature to be effective in measuring motor improvements (2121 Walker MP, Brakefield T, Morgan A, Hobson JA, Stickgold R. Practice
with sleep makes perfect: sleep-dependent motor skill learning. Neuron.
2002;35(1):205-11., 2424 Fregni F, Boggio PS, Mansur CG, Wagner T, Ferreira MJ, Lima MC, et
al. Transcranial direct current stimulation of the unaffected hemisphere in
stroke patients. Neuroreport. 2005;16(14):1551-5.
25 Jebsen RH, Taylor N, Trieschmann R, Trotter M, Howard L. An
objective and standardized test of hand function. Arch Phys Med Rehabil.
1969;50(6):311-9.-2626 Nissen MJ, Bullemer P. Attentional requirements of learning:
Evidence from performance measures. Cogn Psychol.
1987;19(1):1-32.).
Two studies modified the original SFTT (99 Vines BW, Cerruti C, Schlaug G. Dual-hemisphere tDCS facilitates greater improvements for healthy subjects' non-dominant hand compared to uni-hemisphere stimulation. BMC Neurosci. 2008;9:103., 1616 Vines BW, Nair D, Schlaug G. Modulating activity in the motor cortex affects performance for the two hands differently depending upon which hemisphere is stimulated. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28(8):1667-73.). In these studies an accuracy feedback was given for the subjects during the execution of the task. In general, concurrent augmented feedback has been shown to effectively enhance learning in complex motor tasks (2727 Sigrist R, Schellenberg J, Rauter G, Broggi S, Riener R, Wolf P. Visual and auditory augmented concurrent feedback in a complex motor task. Presence (Camb). 2011;20(1):15-32.). In musicians, the auditory feedback reinforced the serial reaction time task, a test similar of the SFTT, performance of the right hand (2828 Conde V, Altenmüller E, Villringer A, Ragert P. Task-irrelevant auditory feedback facilitates motor performance in musicians. Front Psychol. 2012;3:146.). The differences between the results showed in the studies which applied the SFTT could be explained for providing or not accuracy feedback.
Differences in the tDCS protocol applied were identified. The effect of tDCS over the motor learning process was presented when current intensity of 1 mA was applied over the M1 during at least 15 min. In this review the best tDCS type (uni or dual-hemisphere) and electrode size to be used cannot be pointed out. TDCS effects depend of the current density (electrode size/current intensity), so the different results obtained in the included could be explained for the density current applied for each one.
Considering the result of this review, studies that investigate all the types of tDCS and assess motor learning at the same time are necessary to determine the best protocol able to promote motor learning in healthy subjects. The selective reporting of outcomes presented in the studies and the impossibility to calculate the effect size of the tDCS making impossible to conduct a meta-analysis.
This review showed as limitation the fact of have done the search only in electronic databases, so that potential studies that have not been published on these data bases were not selected for analysis and possible inclusion.
Conclusion
This review suggests that tDCS may affect motor learning mechanisms of the non-dominant hand. However, at the moment, the studies are insufficient to draw conclusions. In addition, all studies presented risk of bias and did not provide necessary information to calculate the effect size of the tDCS. Furthermore, there was significant heterogeneity of the parameters of stimulation used. Therefore, further research is needed to investigate which type of motor learning (explicit or implicit) is most likely to influence, and which stimulation parameters are more important for motor learning improvement. This information will be valuable in guiding future use of tDCS in clinical practice.
References
-
1Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J Physiol. 2000;527(Pt 3):633-9.
-
2Zaehle T, Sandmann P, Thorne JD, Jancke L, Herrmann CS. Transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex modulates working memory performance: combined behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. BMC Neurosci. 2011;12:2. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-12-2.
-
3Kantak SS, Mummidisetty CK, Stinear JW. Primary motor and premotor cortex in implicit sequence learning–evidence for competition between implicit and explicit human motor memory systems. Eur J Neurosci. 2012;36(5):2710-5.
-
4Nitsche MA, Schauenburg A, Lang N, Liebetanz D, Exner C, Paulus W, et al. Facilitation of implicit motor learning by weak transcranial direct current stimulation of the primary motor cortex in the human. J Cogn Neurosci. 2003;15(4):619-26.
-
5Marquez CMS, Zhang X, Swinnen SP, Meesen R, Wenderoth N. Task-specific effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on motor learning. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:333.
-
6Fregni F, Boggio PS, Santos MC, Lima M, Vieira AL, Rigonatti SP, et al. Noninvasive cortical stimulation with transcranial direct current stimulation in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2006;21(10):1693-702.
-
7Reis J, Fritsch B. Modulation of motor performance and motor learning by transcranial direct current stimulation. Curr Opin Neurol. 2011;24(6):590-6.
-
8Schambra HM, Abe M, Luckenbaugh DA, Reis J, Krakauer JW, Cohen LG. Probing for hemispheric specialization for motor skill learning: a transcranial direct current stimulation study. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106(2):652-61.
-
9Vines BW, Cerruti C, Schlaug G. Dual-hemisphere tDCS facilitates greater improvements for healthy subjects' non-dominant hand compared to uni-hemisphere stimulation. BMC Neurosci. 2008;9:103.
-
10Tecchio F, Zappasodi F, Assenza G, Tombini M, Vollaro S, Barbati G, et al. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation enhances procedural consolidation. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104(2):1134-40.
-
11Nitsche MA, Doemkes S, Karakose T, Antal A, Liebetanz D, Lang N, et al. Shaping the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation of the human motor cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2007;97(4):3109-17.
-
12Boggio PS, Castro LO, Savagim EA, Braite R, Cruz VC, Rocha RR, et al. Enhancement of non-dominant hand motor function by anodal transcranial direct current stimulation. Neurosci Lett. 2006;404(1-2):232-6.
-
13Hummel FC, Heise K, Celnik P, Floel A, Gerloff C, Cohen LG. Facilitating skilled right hand motor function in older subjects by anodal polarization over the left primary motor cortex. Neurobiol Aging. 2010;31(12):2160-8.
-
14Stagg CJ, Jayaram G, Pastor D, Kincses ZT, Matthews PM, Johansen-Berg H. Polarity and timing-dependent effects of transcranial direct current stimulation in explicit motor learning. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(5):800-4.
-
15Vines BW, Nair DG, Schlaug G. Contralateral and ipsilateral motor effects after transcranial direct current stimulation. Neuroreport. 2006;17(6):671-4.
-
16Vines BW, Nair D, Schlaug G. Modulating activity in the motor cortex affects performance for the two hands differently depending upon which hemisphere is stimulated. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28(8):1667-73.
-
17Clark VP, Coffman BA, Mayer AR, Weisend MP, Lane TD, Calhoun VD, et al. TDCS guided using fMRI significantly accelerates learning to identify concealed objects. Neuroimage. 2012;59(1):117-28.
-
18Schabrun SM, Chipchase LS. Priming the brain to learn: The future of therapy? Man Ther. 2012;17(2):184-6.
-
19Liebetanz D, Nitsche MA, Tergau F, Paulus W. Pharmacological approach to the mechanisms of transcranial DC‐stimulation‐induced after‐effects of human motor cortex excitability. Brain. 2002;125(10):2238-47.
-
20Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97-113.
-
21Walker MP, Brakefield T, Morgan A, Hobson JA, Stickgold R. Practice with sleep makes perfect: sleep-dependent motor skill learning. Neuron. 2002;35(1):205-11.
-
22Higgins J, Altman DG. Assessing risk of bias in included studies. In: Higgins JPT, Green S. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. Chichester; Hoboken: Wiley-Blackwell; 2008. p. 187-241.
-
23Dickersin K. Publication bias: recognizing the problem, understanding its origins and scope, and preventing harm. In: Rothstein HR, Sutton AJ, Borenstein M, editors. Publication bias in meta-analysis: prevention, assessment and adjustments. Chichester; Hoboken: Wiley, 2005. p. 11-33.
-
24Fregni F, Boggio PS, Mansur CG, Wagner T, Ferreira MJ, Lima MC, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation of the unaffected hemisphere in stroke patients. Neuroreport. 2005;16(14):1551-5.
-
25Jebsen RH, Taylor N, Trieschmann R, Trotter M, Howard L. An objective and standardized test of hand function. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1969;50(6):311-9.
-
26Nissen MJ, Bullemer P. Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cogn Psychol. 1987;19(1):1-32.
-
27Sigrist R, Schellenberg J, Rauter G, Broggi S, Riener R, Wolf P. Visual and auditory augmented concurrent feedback in a complex motor task. Presence (Camb). 2011;20(1):15-32.
-
28Conde V, Altenmüller E, Villringer A, Ragert P. Task-irrelevant auditory feedback facilitates motor performance in musicians. Front Psychol. 2012;3:146.
Publication Dates
-
Publication in this collection
Mar 2015
History
-
Received
02 Apr 2014 -
Accepted
15 Oct 2014