Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

SELF-PERCEPTION OF COMPETENCE: CONCEPT, CHANGES IN CHILDHOOD, AND GENDER AND AGE-GROUP DIFFERENCES

AUTOPERCEPÇÃO DE COMPETÊNCIA: CONCEITO, MUDANÇAS CARACTERÍSTICAS NA INFÂNCIA E DIFERENÇAS ENTRE GÊNERO E FAIXAS ETÁRIAS

ABSTRACT

The objectives of this narrative review were to describe: 1) the concept of self-perception of competence and the relationship with the concept of the self; 2) the characteristic changes in self-perceived competence that occur during childhood using a developmental theoretical perspective as the main reference; 3) the results of studies on the self-perceived competence of boys and girls of different ages. Methods: Studies published in electronic databases, studies identified by manually searching the references of the published studies, and the main theoretical bases of the self-concept in childhood were reviewed. Considering the theoretical basis adopted, self-perception is the judgment of individuals about their own competencies to satisfy the requirements of a task. In childhood, self-perception is an important indicator of the self, since children naturally describe themselves taking the judgment about their own competencies as a parameter. Several cognitive changes and diversity in experiences are observed between 5 and 10 years of age that guide children to move from internal and general parameters in their judgments to external sources of information in the different domains of competence with increasing age. The results of the studies suggest a plateau in the self-perceptions of competence in relation to age and higher self-perception in boys, which result respectively from the lack of experiences to optimize development and from the beliefs of gender-specific roles.

Keywords:
Child Development; Self-Concept; Perceived Competence

RESUMO

Esta revisão narrativa tem como objetivos descrever: 1) o conceito de auto-percepção de competência e a relação com o conceito de self; 2) as mudanças características na auto-percepção de competência na infância tendo como principal referência uma perspectiva teórica desenvolvimentista; 3) os resultados de pesquisa sobre autopercepçōes de competência de meninos e meninas de diferentes idades. Método: Foram revisadas pesquisas em bases de dados, busca manual nas referências de estudos publicados e as principais bases teóricas do auto-conceito na infância. Considerando a base teórica adotada, a auto-percepção é compreendida como o julgamento dos indivíduos sobre suas competências para satisfazer exigências de uma tarefa. Na infância, a auto-percepção é um importante indicador do self, pois as crianças naturalmente descrevem a si mesmas tendo como parâmetros o julgamento sobre as suas próprias competências. Entre os 5 e 10 anos de idade, ocorrem várias mudanças cognitivas e as experiências se diversificam guiando as crianças a transitarem de parâmetros mais internos e generalistas nos seus julgamentos para, com o avanço da idade, usar fontes externas de informações nos diferentes domínios de competência. Os resultados das pesquisas sugerem platô nas percepções de competência em relação as idades e percepções mais elevadas em meninos decorrentes, respectivamente, de poucas experiências potencializadoras de desenvolvimento e de crenças de papéis específicos para os gêneros.

Palavras-chave:
Desenvolvimento Infantil; Autoconceito; Competência Percebida

Introduction

The self-perception of competence is a psychological variable that reflects the judgment of people about their own abilities to mobilize resources in order to achieve a particular goal. This perceived competence is a valuable indicator of the self, especially in childhood, as children naturally describe themselves taking the judgment about their own abilities as a parameter11 Harter S. Development Process in the Construction of the Self. In: Yawkey TD, Johnson JE, editors. Integrative process and socialization: early to middle childhood. New Jersey: Child Psychology; 1988, p. 278.,22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.. Harter22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p. proposes a concept of the self in terms of attributes and characteristics that are recognized and verbalized by the individual him/herself and that change according to cognitive maturity and experiences of success and failure acquired during human development.

Considering this perspective, the self-perception of competence is an important parameter to understand the motivation and development of the self-concept in childhood. This self-judgment is a mediator for children’s achievements since, when children experience success in their attempts, they tend to perceive themselves to be more competent and motivated and continue this pursuit, which generates a continuous cycle of pursuit and engagement to improve performance. This cycle may strengthen the development of other psychological constructs, such as autonomy, self-confidence and self-esteem. Furthermore, the feeling of competence enhances the child’s intrinsic motivation to engage in challenging activities, with he/she persisting in the task and striving for mastery. On the other hand, when children have negative perceptions about their competencies, they tend to avoid challenging experiences and to protect themselves from situations that could cause shame because of the expected and unrealized performance33 Harter S. Competence considered: Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. In: Sternberg RJ, Kolligian JJ, editors. Competence considered: Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420.,44 Harter S. Causes, correlates, and the functional role of global self-worth: A life-span perspective. In: Sternberg R, Kolligian J, editors. Competence considered. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420.. In these two cases, the perceived competence will have a significant impact on the actual competence and on the components of this system2.

Several internal and external factors influence the construction and strengthening of self-perceived competence. Parents, teachers, close friends and classmates are significant external agents that provide information for the construction of a self-evaluation mechanism. This mechanism influences the construction of the child’s self-concept and depends on the value and importance given by the child to the external agent and experience22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.,66 Amorose AJ, Chanal JP, Marsh HW, Sarrazin RG, Bois JE, Weiss MR, et al. Reflected appraisals and perceived importance of significant others' appraisals as predictors of college athletes' self-perceptions of competence. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2005;27(1):416-430.

7 Weiss MR, Amorose AJ. Children's self-perceptions in the physical domain: Between- and within-age variability in level, accuracy, and sources of perceived competence. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2005;27:226-244.
-88 Valentini NC. Competência Percebida: Considerações para promover a aprendizagem. In: Krebs RJ, Ferreira Neto CA, editores. Tópicos em desenvolvimento motor na infância e adolescência. Rio de Janeiro: LECSU; 2007, p. 137-157.. Internal factors include the developmental stage of the child, motivational orientation, affective reactions to the performance results, and persistence in response to difficulties. Younger children (2 to 4 years of age) tend to have less realistic perceptions and overestimate their abilities. In general, these children have little experience to make effective judgments and fail to distinguish between their desire to be (i.e., idealized self-concept) and what they really are. Consequently, they tend not to judge their competencies realistically. Furthermore, these children base the judgment about their abilities simply on the success in completing the task and/or on the feedback of adults that are important to them66 Amorose AJ, Chanal JP, Marsh HW, Sarrazin RG, Bois JE, Weiss MR, et al. Reflected appraisals and perceived importance of significant others' appraisals as predictors of college athletes' self-perceptions of competence. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2005;27(1):416-430.,77 Weiss MR, Amorose AJ. Children's self-perceptions in the physical domain: Between- and within-age variability in level, accuracy, and sources of perceived competence. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2005;27:226-244.. As children advance in the development process, they develop an internal set of performance criteria that can be used in different situations. They begin to prioritize the comparison with peers to enhance self-evaluation and tend to make more realistic judgments about their own performance in different domains22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.,66 Amorose AJ, Chanal JP, Marsh HW, Sarrazin RG, Bois JE, Weiss MR, et al. Reflected appraisals and perceived importance of significant others' appraisals as predictors of college athletes' self-perceptions of competence. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2005;27(1):416-430.,77 Weiss MR, Amorose AJ. Children's self-perceptions in the physical domain: Between- and within-age variability in level, accuracy, and sources of perceived competence. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2005;27:226-244.,99 Harter S. Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. In: Sternberg RJ, Kolligian JJ, editors. Competence considered. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420-445..

Different external mediators affect these perceptions and childhood experiences are influenced by culture. Children of similar ages may have different cognitive abilities affect they way they judge their own abilities. Therefore, several studies have shown divergent results regarding self-perceived competence in children of different ages and between boys and girls. Similar self-perception in young and older children, as well as higher perceptions for younger children have been reported1010 Nobre GC, Bandeira PFR, Ramalho MHS, Nobre FSS, Valentini NC. Self-perception of competence in children from socially vulnerable contexts, assisted and unassisted by social sports projects. J Hum Growth Dev 2015;25(3):271. DOI: 10.7322/jhgd.97673
https://doi.org/10.7322/jhgd.97673...

11 Villwock G, Valentini NC. Percepção de competência atlética, orientação motivacional e competência motora em crianças de escolas públicas: estudo desenvolvimentista e correlacional. Rev Bras Educ Fís Esporte 2007;21(4):245-257.
-1212 Valentini NC. Percepções de competência e desenvolvimento motor de meninos e meninas: um estudo transversal. Movimento 2002;8(2):51-62..Higher perception of competence for boys is another aspect observed in studies. Specifically regarding athletic competence, boys frequently show higher perception than girls, influenced by cultural agents that more strongly value these experiences in boys1111 Villwock G, Valentini NC. Percepção de competência atlética, orientação motivacional e competência motora em crianças de escolas públicas: estudo desenvolvimentista e correlacional. Rev Bras Educ Fís Esporte 2007;21(4):245-257.,1313 Ridgers ND, Fazey DMA, Fairclough SJ. Perceptions of athletic competence and fear of negative evaluation during physical education. Br J Educ Psychol 2007;77(2):339-49.

14 Raustorp A, Stahle A, Gudasic H, Kinnunen A, Mattsson E. Physical activity and self-perception in school children assessed with the Children and Youth - Physical Self-Perception Profile. Scand J Med Sci Sports 2005;15(1):126-134.

15 Goodway J, Robinson L, Crowe H. Gender differences in motor skill development in disadvantaged preschoolers from two geographical regions. Res Q Exerc Sport 2010;81(1):17-24. DOI: 10.5641/027013610X13352775119510
https://doi.org/10.5641/027013610X133527...

16 Jacobs JE, Lanza S, Osgood DW, Eccles JS, Wigfield A. Changes in children's self-competence and values: gender and domain differences across grades one through twelve. Child Dev 2002;73(2):509-527.

17 Nobre GC, Valentini NC, Nobre FSS. Fundamental motor skills, nutritional status, perceived competence, and school performance of Brazilian children in social vulnerability: Gender comparison. Child Abuse Negl 2018;80:335-45. DOI: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.04.007
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.04...
-1818 Nobre GC, Bandeira P, Valentini NC. The relationship between general perceived motor competence, perceived competence relative to motor skill and actual motor competence in children. J Phys Educ 2016;27(1):2744. DOI: 10.4025/jphyseduc.v27i1.2744
https://doi.org/10.4025/jphyseduc.v27i1....
. However, other studies found no such differences1919 Almeida G, Valentini NC, Berleze A. Percepções de competência?: um estudo com crianças e adolescentes do ensino fundamental. Movimento 2009;15(1):71-79.

20 Feitoza AHP, Henrique R dos S, Barnett L, Re A, Lopes VP, Webster EK, et al. Perceived motor competence in childhood: comparative study among countries. J Mot Learn Dev 2017;10(7):1-22. DOI: 10.1123/jmld.2016-0079
https://doi.org/10.1123/jmld.2016-0079...

21 Valentini NC, Villwock G, Vieira LF, Vieira JLL, Levi M, Barbosa L. Brazilian validity of the Harter' Self-perception profile for children. Psicol Reflexão e Crítica 2010;23(3):411-419.
-2222 Valentini NC. The influence of a motor skill intervention on the motor performance and perceived competence of children with motor delays. Rev Paul Educação Física 2002;16(1):61-75., a fact reinforcing the importance of empirical studies on the cultural and developmental factors that can affect these perceptions in childhood.

Understanding the concept of self-perceived competence and how changes in this self-judgement occur during childhood, as well as the identification of the main factors associated with this construct, is fundamental for potentiating child development. The understanding of this construct can assist parents, teachers, school principals and professionals in promoting strategies that help children develop a positive perception about their own competence in different dimensions of life. As mediating agents, these individuals can promote learning experiences that enhance in the child feelings of self-determination and the pleasure of achievement, thus permitting the construction of a realistic perception of his/her competencies. Therefore, the objectives of this narrative review were to describe: 1) the concept of self-perception of competence and the relationship with the self; 2) the characteristic changes that occur in self-perceived competence during childhood using the developmental theoretical perspective proposed by Harter11 Harter S. Development Process in the Construction of the Self. In: Yawkey TD, Johnson JE, editors. Integrative process and socialization: early to middle childhood. New Jersey: Child Psychology; 1988, p. 278.

2 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.

3 Harter S. Competence considered: Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. In: Sternberg RJ, Kolligian JJ, editors. Competence considered: Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420.
-44 Harter S. Causes, correlates, and the functional role of global self-worth: A life-span perspective. In: Sternberg R, Kolligian J, editors. Competence considered. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420. as the main reference; 3) the results of studies on the perceptions of boys and girls of different ages.

Methods

In this study, we reviewed studies published in the SciELO, Sportdisscus, LILACS, PubMed, Google Scholar and APA PsycNET electronic databases, Capes journals, studies identified by manually searching the references of the published studies, and studies published in thesis and dissertation databases, conference proceedings, and books and book chapters by important authors on the topic. The references of descriptive, observational, experimental studies and systematic and literature reviews that contained information about the self-perception of competence and the self, about developmental and sociocultural aspects of self-perceived competence in childhood, and about factors associated with the self-perception of children were reviewed. In addition, the major works of Susan Harter on the development of the self-concept in childhood were reviewed.

Development

This section covers three subtopics: 1) self-perception of competence and the self; 2) self-perception of competence in childhood considering developmental and sociocultural aspects; 3) the results of studies on the self-perceived competence of boys and girls of different ages. In the first subtopic, we describe the self-perception of competence based on the developmental model proposed by Susan Harter and its relationship with the self-concept. In the second subtopic, we describe the main changes in the self and consequently in the self-perception of competence that occur during childhood, focusing on the period of greatest changes according to the developmental model of Harter, that is from 5 to 10 years of age. In the last subtopic, we compare the results of self-perceived competence between children of different age groups and between boys and girls, focusing on studies that address the athletic dimension of self-perception.

Self-perception of competence and the self

The understanding of the development of self-perceived competence and of related aspects requires above all a reflection on the self. This construct and its relationship with aspects of human behavior have been the subject of interest of researchers in recent decades. The self is a multifaceted construct and its understanding is therefore complex; however, in general, it expresses a feature that distinguishes humans from other animals, i.e., self-reflection1414 Raustorp A, Stahle A, Gudasic H, Kinnunen A, Mattsson E. Physical activity and self-perception in school children assessed with the Children and Youth - Physical Self-Perception Profile. Scand J Med Sci Sports 2005;15(1):126-134.. As Harter22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p. points out, self-esteem, self-concept, self-image, self-worth, self-evaluation, self-appraisal and self-perception are only some terms used to refer to the self. These terms, which are often used as synonyms, point to different constructs depending on the theoretical orientation assumed.

Harter2,9,24 proposes a self-concept in terms of attributes and characteristics that are consciously recognized by the individual him/herself and that can be verbalized. It is expressed in the way one consciously thinks about one’s own characteristic evaluated. Thus, according to this concept, we have ourselves as the object of reflection and especially as the object of evaluation that can be expressed by language. The words self-representation, self-description and self-perception are used by Harter22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.,99 Harter S. Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. In: Sternberg RJ, Kolligian JJ, editors. Competence considered. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420-445. for general characterization of the self. It is evident that the position of this researcher on this phenomenon is that it is an aspect subject to self-description explicitly referenced in terms of classifiable attributes. Harter22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p. reinforces that the self-evaluation of an individual for any characteristic occurs within a continuum, for example, how much the individual evaluates him/herself over a spectrum that ranges from athletic to non-athletic, attractive to unattractive, popular to unpopular. Furthermore, the self is not only a description of a set of characteristics, roles or assumed behaviors (man, friend, or student), but such description is likely to be judged in valuative terms such as “a good man”, a “devoted friend” and an “intelligent student”.

These adjectives express the knowledge that the individual constructs about him/herself not only from a single perspective, but also in different dimensions22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.. This knowledge about oneself cannot be represented by a single measure, as individuals naturally describe and evaluate themselves in the different domains of their lives22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.. Hence, the self as a complex psychological construct is also multidimensional22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.,99 Harter S. Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. In: Sternberg RJ, Kolligian JJ, editors. Competence considered. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420-445.. People may judge their own attributes or characteristics in specific areas, such as in the academic or school environment, work, friendships, romantic relationships, maternal acceptance, the relationship with parents, athletic activities, satisfaction with life, and health status22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.,44 Harter S. Causes, correlates, and the functional role of global self-worth: A life-span perspective. In: Sternberg R, Kolligian J, editors. Competence considered. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420.,99 Harter S. Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. In: Sternberg RJ, Kolligian JJ, editors. Competence considered. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420-445.. Likewise, the self-evaluated domains are directly related to the stage of life and change with the individual’s experiences and development. In addition, the self-evaluations of these modifiable attributes can also change.

Finally, the self conceived from a developmental and sociocultural perspective is considered a cognitive and social construction of the individual22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.. The self is the product of processes that involve the interaction between the individual’s resources and the characteristics of the environment in which he/she participates actively across the life cycle. Different perceptions of the self can be observed during the stages of life since cognitive maturity and experiences in a certain context alter the way individuals describe and evaluate themselves22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.. As a result of these processes, researchers have made efforts to understand how changes in the self throughout life can be explained by cognitive processes of change through the development, and by the children’ relationships and experiences with others. Issues related to how individuals perceive and ask questions about who they are, about their value as a human being and about their competencies in different domains are the focus of studies22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.,33 Harter S. Competence considered: Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. In: Sternberg RJ, Kolligian JJ, editors. Competence considered: Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420.,55 Gabbard C. Lifelong Motor Development. 7th ed. Austin: Wolters Kluwer Health; 2016.

6 Amorose AJ, Chanal JP, Marsh HW, Sarrazin RG, Bois JE, Weiss MR, et al. Reflected appraisals and perceived importance of significant others' appraisals as predictors of college athletes' self-perceptions of competence. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2005;27(1):416-430.

7 Weiss MR, Amorose AJ. Children's self-perceptions in the physical domain: Between- and within-age variability in level, accuracy, and sources of perceived competence. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2005;27:226-244.
-88 Valentini NC. Competência Percebida: Considerações para promover a aprendizagem. In: Krebs RJ, Ferreira Neto CA, editores. Tópicos em desenvolvimento motor na infância e adolescência. Rio de Janeiro: LECSU; 2007, p. 137-157.. The following subtopic describes the characteristic changes in perceived competence as an important indicator of the self in early (2 to 4 years), middle (5 to 7 years) and late childhood (8 to 10 years) using the theoretical perspective proposed by Harter11 Harter S. Development Process in the Construction of the Self. In: Yawkey TD, Johnson JE, editors. Integrative process and socialization: early to middle childhood. New Jersey: Child Psychology; 1988, p. 278.,22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.,44 Harter S. Causes, correlates, and the functional role of global self-worth: A life-span perspective. In: Sternberg R, Kolligian J, editors. Competence considered. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420.,99 Harter S. Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. In: Sternberg RJ, Kolligian JJ, editors. Competence considered. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420-445.,2525 Harter S. Developmental and dynamic changes in the nature of the self-concept implications for child psychotherapy. In: Shirk SR, editor. Cognitive development and child psychotherapy. Denver: Springer Science; 1988, p. 110. as the main reference.

Self-perception of competence in childhood: developmental and sociocultural aspects

The self-perception of competence is a valuable indicator of the self, especially in childhood, as children naturally describe themselves in terms of their competencies22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.. Competence can be understood in this context as the ability of the child to meet the specific requirements of a situation or task in one or several domains of development, which can range from a low to high level11 Harter S. Development Process in the Construction of the Self. In: Yawkey TD, Johnson JE, editors. Integrative process and socialization: early to middle childhood. New Jersey: Child Psychology; 1988, p. 278.,22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p..

According to Harter22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.,99 Harter S. Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. In: Sternberg RJ, Kolligian JJ, editors. Competence considered. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420-445., younger children (2 to 4 years of age) tend to make self-judgments based on aspects that appear to be concrete and observable in their lives. Consequently, children describe their attributes in terms of their possessions (objects, pets) or based on what they think they can do (recite the entire alphabet, climb stairs, count from 1 to 100) or what they feel in a certain situation (I am smart because I can climb a ladder). In this age group, children are even able to verbalize how much they think they have improved their performance in recent periods and naturally tend to have unrealistic perceptions, overestimating their capabilities. This is due to the inability of children to distinguish between their desire to be and reality itself, i.e., by formulating an idealized self-concept, it tends to be different from what is actually observed.

Cognitive limitations that impair children to significantly utilize social comparison information for building their perceived competencies justify the overestimated self-evaluation in early childhood. Specifically, these factors of influence as established by Harter2,9 on the unrealistic perceptions of children are related to the lack of cognitive maturity to understand that they possess opposing characteristics (good and bad). During this phase, children naturally exhibit an “all-or-none” thinking and still do not understand that they can be good at some attributes and bad at others. A second factor of influence is the inability to comprehend and incorporate into the self the criticisms of significant people such as parents and teachers. Furthermore, the significant people in the life of the child, the socializing agents, are benevolent, reinforcing the positive self-attributes of children in an unrealistic manner22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.,99 Harter S. Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. In: Sternberg RJ, Kolligian JJ, editors. Competence considered. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420-445..

However, having unrealistic positive perceptions during this phase does not compromise the construction of a child’s self. On the contrary, these positive perceptions can serve as a kind of motivational and emotional protection strategy, helping the child to perceive himself effective in the tasks, even if he is not, and thus to persist and develop new abilities11 Harter S. Development Process in the Construction of the Self. In: Yawkey TD, Johnson JE, editors. Integrative process and socialization: early to middle childhood. New Jersey: Child Psychology; 1988, p. 278.. According to Harter22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.,99 Harter S. Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. In: Sternberg RJ, Kolligian JJ, editors. Competence considered. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420-445., the positive and unrealistic perceived competence at this stage may propel the child towards growth-building mastery attempts, introducing a sense of confidence and minimizing perceptions of inadequacy. Taken together, these factors contribute to a positive development. Thus, this trend of positive perceptions has an adaptive and self-reinforcement function in children of this age. This self-reinforcement can serve to prevent feelings of fragility in the face of more challenging situations that are naturally present during the acquisition of new skills, such as learning how to throw a ball, read, and write 22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.. Thus, children remain motivated to attempt a broad range of new tasks and these typically positive characteristics tend to persist into subsequent ages.

In middle childhood (5 to 7 years of age), children still exhibit several characteristics of the previous period in the development of self-perception. They evaluate themselves in terms of their various competencies (social, athletic, and/or cognitive ability). In addition, gender differences are more evident, as well as cultural influences, especially in relation to cultural roles, beliefs and values22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.,99 Harter S. Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. In: Sternberg RJ, Kolligian JJ, editors. Competence considered. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420-445.. During this period, the self-description of children already tends to reflect established gender stereotypes, for example, in professional roles. Male-dominated professions such as firefighter, doctor and professional athlete and those in which women predominate such as nurse and teacher are used in the self-description of children at this stage22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.. Behavior norms related to gender roles are also salient during this period. Gender roles do not only dictate the content of stereotypes, but demonstrate the importance of adhering to one of the socially established directions (male or female)22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.,99 Harter S. Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. In: Sternberg RJ, Kolligian JJ, editors. Competence considered. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420-445..

Cognitive advances are evident in middle childhood. Children begin to show the ability, even if rudimentary, to interrelate concepts that were previously compartmentalized. Moreover, children are able to organize, even though in a rudimentary way, categories that link a number of self-described allusive competencies to the level of judgment presented for a given competence, e.g., “I am good at running, and jumping, and swimming, and writing”. This does not mean that children have already acquired the capacity of coordinating thoughts about self-judgment since the “all-or-none” thinking still prevails. According to Harter22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p., this period emerges from new cognitive acquisitions that allow the child to relate sets in order to map overlapping representations. The representational map in the form of opposites (all-or-none thinking) is still present at this stage and children are able to differentiate attributes such as “bad” and “good”, although they cannot coordinate these representations to understand and accept that one characteristic or representational map can coexist. In other words, children still do not understand that they can evaluate themselves as good at some skills and bad at others.

Like the cognitive advances described above, the context of socialization also plays a fundamental role in the construction of children’s self-perception in middle childhood. Children become more aware of their self-representation and how others construct an image about them. Thus, the increasing cognitive perception for the perspective of the other influences the development of the self. This relational process allows the child to understand that socializing agents have a particular point of view about their behavior. Increasing discussions and comparisons of typical abilities during this period allows the child to realize that others are actively evaluating them. The self-evaluation of the child is therefore now influenced by the view of others (socializing agents), with this view serving as a guide that helps the child regulate his/her behavior, and children begin to identify what socializing agents expect from them. However, children do not yet have the capacity to internalize these evaluations sufficiently so that they can make independent or autonomous judgments about their own attributes and competencies, and thus show little interest in exploring the self22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p..

As children continue to develop, the perceived competence becomes more realistic and is gradually more influenced by socializing agents, especially peers66 Amorose AJ, Chanal JP, Marsh HW, Sarrazin RG, Bois JE, Weiss MR, et al. Reflected appraisals and perceived importance of significant others' appraisals as predictors of college athletes' self-perceptions of competence. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2005;27(1):416-430.,77 Weiss MR, Amorose AJ. Children's self-perceptions in the physical domain: Between- and within-age variability in level, accuracy, and sources of perceived competence. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2005;27:226-244.. Specifically children in the late childhood period, which ranges from 8 to 10 years of age, describe their competencies by focusing on the comparison of their own abilities with the characteristics of others and, certainly, this mechanism becomes much more relevant for their self-evaluation. These social comparisons that before served to adjust behavior and were thus temporary are now intended to contribute to the construction of self-perception. In this respect, the opinion and values of socializing agents are internalized by the child and, at some extension, guide children’ options, choices and development. In other words, the external agents, especially peers, provide parameters that are incorporated and support the construction of a self-evaluation mechanism maintained by the feedback of these agents about the adequacy of the child’s performance in a given task or experience22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.,99 Harter S. Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. In: Sternberg RJ, Kolligian JJ, editors. Competence considered. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420-445.. This mechanism influences the construction of the child’s self-concept and depends on the value and importance it gives to the external agent and on the characteristics of the contexts in which the child actively participates22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.,33 Harter S. Competence considered: Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. In: Sternberg RJ, Kolligian JJ, editors. Competence considered: Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420..

Another important advance at this age, as Harter22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p. explains, is the ability of children to link representational sets and thus to realize that they can evaluate themselves as good at some skills and bad in others. This allows the child to make more realistic and potentially more negative judgements about his/her attributes when compared to previous ages. Some cognitive acquisitions that emerge during this period can support the construction of these realistic self-evaluations. First, the cognitive ability of children to understand that they possess not only positive but also negative attributes is a factor of strong influence that allows the child to make more realistic and accurate self-evaluations. Another important cognitive advance is the use of social comparisons for self-evaluation that renders the child’s self-perception more mature and, consequently, more competent. Furthermore, cognitive abilities such as the differentiation between actual and desired self-perception, the increased ability to understand what people think about him/her and the capacity to put oneself in someone’s place are part of the child’s cognitive changes that aid in the development of perceived competence during this period22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.,99 Harter S. Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. In: Sternberg RJ, Kolligian JJ, editors. Competence considered. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420-445.. Another important change in late childhood results from the child’s cognitive ability to distinguish between the concept of ability (the capacity to execute the task with mastery) and the concept of effort (the energy needed to develop mastery), to establish relationships between his/her perceptions and the difficulty of the task, and to alter his/her self-perceptions according to lived experiences22 Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.

3 Harter S. Competence considered: Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. In: Sternberg RJ, Kolligian JJ, editors. Competence considered: Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420.
-44 Harter S. Causes, correlates, and the functional role of global self-worth: A life-span perspective. In: Sternberg R, Kolligian J, editors. Competence considered. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420..

Self-perception of competence in different age groups and in boys and girls: the contributions of current studies to the understanding of this construct

Recent studies on the self-perceived competence of children have demonstrated similar perception between children of different ages1010 Nobre GC, Bandeira PFR, Ramalho MHS, Nobre FSS, Valentini NC. Self-perception of competence in children from socially vulnerable contexts, assisted and unassisted by social sports projects. J Hum Growth Dev 2015;25(3):271. DOI: 10.7322/jhgd.97673
https://doi.org/10.7322/jhgd.97673...
,1111 Villwock G, Valentini NC. Percepção de competência atlética, orientação motivacional e competência motora em crianças de escolas públicas: estudo desenvolvimentista e correlacional. Rev Bras Educ Fís Esporte 2007;21(4):245-257.,1818 Nobre GC, Bandeira P, Valentini NC. The relationship between general perceived motor competence, perceived competence relative to motor skill and actual motor competence in children. J Phys Educ 2016;27(1):2744. DOI: 10.4025/jphyseduc.v27i1.2744
https://doi.org/10.4025/jphyseduc.v27i1....
,2626 Lopes VP, Saraiva L, Gonçalves C, Rodrigues LP. Association between perceived and actual motor competence in Portuguese children. J Mot Learn Dev 2017;10(7):1-22. DOI: 10.1123/jmld.2016-0059
https://doi.org/10.1123/jmld.2016-0059...
,2727 Spessato BC, Gabbard C, Robinson L, Valentini NC. Body mass index, perceived and actual physical competence: The relationship among young children. Child Care Health Dev 2013;39(6):845-850.. The results showed that older children continue to report unrealistic perceptions, over- or underestimating their own capability2828 Masci I, Schmidt M, Marchetti R, Vannozzi G, Pesce C. When children's perceived and actual motor competence mismatch: sport participation and gender differences. J Mot Learn Dev 2017;11(5):1-33. DOI: 10.1123/jmld.2016-0081
https://doi.org/10.1123/jmld.2016-0081...
. This plateau in the self-perception of competence, specifically athletic competence, between 5 and 10 years of age1010 Nobre GC, Bandeira PFR, Ramalho MHS, Nobre FSS, Valentini NC. Self-perception of competence in children from socially vulnerable contexts, assisted and unassisted by social sports projects. J Hum Growth Dev 2015;25(3):271. DOI: 10.7322/jhgd.97673
https://doi.org/10.7322/jhgd.97673...
,1212 Valentini NC. Percepções de competência e desenvolvimento motor de meninos e meninas: um estudo transversal. Movimento 2002;8(2):51-62. is related to the little experience and lack of internal criteria of these children to make more autonomous and accurate judgments. Theoretically, older children should gradually adopt more intrinsic parameter for self-evaluation and should thus begin to make more efficient judgments about their own performance. However, these cognitive changes depend on lived experiences. If the experiences do not permit the construction of internal parameters for self-judgment, it is possible that children over- or underestimate their own competencies even with advancing age. Valentini1212 Valentini NC. Percepções de competência e desenvolvimento motor de meninos e meninas: um estudo transversal. Movimento 2002;8(2):51-62. emphasizes that the unrealistic and therefore inaccurate perception of children in late childhood is the result of the absence of parameters to judge their own competence and may be due in part to their participation in contexts that do not contribute to the construction of internal criteria for judging their capabilities.

These results are a matter of concern since the construction of a realistic perception is fundamental for motivation and persistence on activities. Children that overestimate their competence tend to build unrealistic performance expectations and the experience of failure in the task, if not recognized as difficult, may lead to the abandonment of tasks as a form of self-protection. On the other hand, when competencies are overestimated, children may develop poor performance expectations in relation to future attempts and, consequently, do not persist on activities88 Valentini NC. Competência Percebida: Considerações para promover a aprendizagem. In: Krebs RJ, Ferreira Neto CA, editores. Tópicos em desenvolvimento motor na infância e adolescência. Rio de Janeiro: LECSU; 2007, p. 137-157..

Another aspect observed in the studies is the higher perception of boys, especially regarding the athletic dimension of self-perceived competence. Several national1111 Villwock G, Valentini NC. Percepção de competência atlética, orientação motivacional e competência motora em crianças de escolas públicas: estudo desenvolvimentista e correlacional. Rev Bras Educ Fís Esporte 2007;21(4):245-257.,1717 Nobre GC, Valentini NC, Nobre FSS. Fundamental motor skills, nutritional status, perceived competence, and school performance of Brazilian children in social vulnerability: Gender comparison. Child Abuse Negl 2018;80:335-45. DOI: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.04.007
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.04...
,2020 Feitoza AHP, Henrique R dos S, Barnett L, Re A, Lopes VP, Webster EK, et al. Perceived motor competence in childhood: comparative study among countries. J Mot Learn Dev 2017;10(7):1-22. DOI: 10.1123/jmld.2016-0079
https://doi.org/10.1123/jmld.2016-0079...
,2929 Spessato BC, Gabbard C, Valentini NC. The role of motor competence and body mass index in children's activity levels in physical education classes. J Teach Phys Educ 2013;32(1):118-130.

30 Nobre GC, Bandeira PFR, Ramalho MHS, Nobre FSS, Valentini NC. Self-perception of competence in children from socially vulnerable contexts, assisted and unassisted by social sports projects. J Hum Growth Dev 2015;25(3):276-281. DOI: 10.7322/jhgd.97673
https://doi.org/10.7322/jhgd.97673...

31 Pansera SM. Crianças que se percebem competentes e são intrinsecamente motivadas são motoramente mais competentes? Um estudo associativo entre desenvolvimento motor e aspectos psicossociais de escolares. [Dissertação de Mestrado em Ciência do movimento Humano]. Porto Alegre: Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul. Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ciência do Movimento Humano; 2012.

32 Bandeira PFR, De Souza MS, Zanella LW, Nobre GC, Valentini NC. Percepção de competência motora e nível de atividade física: estudo comparativo entre sexos. Cinergis 2014;15(4):191-194.

33 Nobre G, Bandeira P, Valentini N. Relação entre a competência motora percebida geral, o gênero, a competência motora percebida específica à tarefa e a competência motora real de crianças. J Phys Educ 2016;27(1):e2744.
-3434 Carroll B, Loumidis J. Children's perceived competence and enjoyment in physical education and physical activity outside school. Eur Phys Educ Rev 2001;7(1):24-43. and international2626 Lopes VP, Saraiva L, Gonçalves C, Rodrigues LP. Association between perceived and actual motor competence in Portuguese children. J Mot Learn Dev 2017;10(7):1-22. DOI: 10.1123/jmld.2016-0059
https://doi.org/10.1123/jmld.2016-0059...
,3535 Brian A, Bardid F, Barnett L, Deconinck F, Lenoir M, Goodway J. Actual and perceived motor competence levels of Belgian and US preschool children. J Mot Learn Dev 2017:S320-S336. DOI: 10.1123/jmld.2016-0071
https://doi.org/10.1123/jmld.2016-0071...

36 Barnett L, Lubans D, Salmon J, Timperio A, Ridgers ND. What is the contribution of actual motor skill, fitness, and physical activity to children's self-perception of motor competence? J Mot Learn Dev 2017;6(2):S461-S473. Doi:10.1123/jmld.2016-0076
https://doi.org/10.1123/jmld.2016-0076...

37 Duncan MJ, Jones V, O'Brien W, Barnett LM, Eyre ELJ. Self-Perceived and actual motor competence in young British children. Percept Mot Skills 2018;125(2):251-264. DOI: 10.1177/0031512517752833
https://doi.org/10.1177/0031512517752833...
-3838 Liong GHE, Ridgers ND, Barnett LM. Associations between skill perceptions and young children's actual fundamental movement skills. Percept Mot Skills 2015;120(2):591-603 DOI: 10.2466/10.25.PMS.120v18x2
https://doi.org/10.2466/10.25.PMS.120v18...
studies confirm this trend. Cultural beliefs and values, especially in terms of the attribution of social roles, also influence the construction of perceived competence. In many cultures, gender stereotypes are reinforced with respect to particular roles in which boys should adopt more independent behaviors, for example, being encouraged to participate in physical and sports activities, while girls should engage in more passive and dependent behaviors3939 Bois JE, Sarrazin PG, Brustad RJ, Trouilloud DO, Cury F. Elementary schoolchildren's perceived competence and physical activity involvement: the influence of parents' role modelling behaviours and perceptions of their child's competence. Psychol Sport Exerc 2005;6(4):381-397.. These more passive behaviors may cause girls to build a limited and often negative self-perception of competence, affecting their motivation to perform and consequently their pursuit of challenges and achievements.

Other hypotheses regarding gender differences in self-perceived competence can be raised, especially for the self-perception of athletic competence. One possible explanation might be the frequent social reinforcement of boys to engage in different motor activities and to participate in sports2222 Valentini NC. The influence of a motor skill intervention on the motor performance and perceived competence of children with motor delays. Rev Paul Educação Física 2002;16(1):61-75.,4040 Lentillon V. Les stéréotypes sexués relatifs à la pratique des activités physiques et sportives chez les adolescents français et leurs conséquences discriminatoires. Bull Psychol 2009;499(1):15.. Consequently, they may use as parameters previous experience and feedback from parents and teachers to report their perceptions of competence in these activities. Bois et al.3939 Bois JE, Sarrazin PG, Brustad RJ, Trouilloud DO, Cury F. Elementary schoolchildren's perceived competence and physical activity involvement: the influence of parents' role modelling behaviours and perceptions of their child's competence. Psychol Sport Exerc 2005;6(4):381-397. argue that parents as socializing agents in childhood and early adolescence are very influential in modeling the beliefs of their children about their capabilities. The authors further clarify that the mother’s perception is particularly important in this modeling process because she is generally more active in raising her children, reinforcing the masculine stereotype model that is characteristic of sport. Other socializing agents such as teachers and coaches also influence this process, especially when they are important to the child77 Weiss MR, Amorose AJ. Children's self-perceptions in the physical domain: Between- and within-age variability in level, accuracy, and sources of perceived competence. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2005;27:226-244.,4141 Horn TS, Weiss MR. A developmental analysis of children's self-ability judgments in the physical domain. Pediatr Exerc Sci 1991;3(4):310-326.,4242 Horn TS, Amorose AJ. Sources of competence information. In: Duda JL, editor. Advancements in sport and exercise psychology measurement. Morgantown: Fitness Information Technology; 1998, p. 49-64..

On the other hand, girls frequently receive less social reinforcement to participate in motor activities and therefore have potentially lower motor competence. Girls tend to build a lower and often negative self-perception of their motor competence and thus avoid engagement and the pursuit of challenges and achievements in sports activities1313 Ridgers ND, Fazey DMA, Fairclough SJ. Perceptions of athletic competence and fear of negative evaluation during physical education. Br J Educ Psychol 2007;77(2):339-49.,3939 Bois JE, Sarrazin PG, Brustad RJ, Trouilloud DO, Cury F. Elementary schoolchildren's perceived competence and physical activity involvement: the influence of parents' role modelling behaviours and perceptions of their child's competence. Psychol Sport Exerc 2005;6(4):381-397.. Another important factor is that girls are more affected by the phenomenon of social anxiety and the fear of negative evaluation during physical education classes. The judgments of girls about their own motor competence tend to be weaker1313 Ridgers ND, Fazey DMA, Fairclough SJ. Perceptions of athletic competence and fear of negative evaluation during physical education. Br J Educ Psychol 2007;77(2):339-49.. Finally, possible differences in perceived motor competence between genders may also be related to the different reasons that lead boys and girls to participate in motor and sports activities. Girls are more motivated by the fun and sense of well-being that the activity can provide, while boys are more motivated by the pleasure of competition, the movement itself, the quest for learning, and improvement of performance4343 Krebs RJ, Santos JOL, Ramalho MHS, Nazario PF, Nobre GC, Almeida RT. Disposição de adolescentes para a prática de esportes: um estudo orientado pela Teoria Bioecológica de Bronfenbrenner. Motriz Rev Educ Física 2010;17(1):195-201.. Competition and the pursuit of the best performance can lead boys to have feelings of accomplishment and pleasure for the challenge, consequently affecting their own perception of athletic competence.

Contradictorily, other studies report a decrease in the self-perception of competence with age and similar perceptions in the athletic dimension between boys and girls4444 Barnett LM, Beurden E, Morgan PJ, Brooks LO, Beard JR. Gender differences in motor skill proficiency from childhood to adolescence. Res Q Exerc Sport 2010;81(2):162-170. DOI: 10.1080/02701367.2010.10599663
https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2010.10...
. Valentini8 suggests that appropriately provided development opportunities promote the construction of realistic and balanced perceptions of competence in both genders. Boys and girls with similar perceived competence may experience equivalent opportunities of interaction and social expectations of the environments in which they actively participate. These results reinforce the multidimensionality of this construct and the determinant role of family and cultural mediators. Thus, this phenomenon may exhibit different trends according to the context investigated.

Final Considerations

The self-perception of competence is an important mediating construct of individual development. Its construction is the result of an interactional process that involves individual internal characteristics and characteristics of the environments and culture in which children actively participate. To comprehend the processes related to the construction of self-perception in childhood, it is important to understand that the construction of the self requires self-reflection about one’s own capability to mobilize resources in order to achieve a certain goal and that the perceptions of self vary across the child’s developmental periods. These changes are the result of cognitive maturity and lived experiences, which change the way individuals describe and evaluate themselves. Younger children (5 and 6 years of age) naturally utilize more internal parameters and more frequently overestimate and generalize their own skills. Older children gradually tend to employ external sources of comparison, begin to better differentiate the domains of competence, and realize that they can have higher or lower competence depending on the domain evaluated.

The results of studies generally show a plateau in the perceptions of competence across ages and higher self-perception in boys in terms of athletic competencies. Possible factors that may explain these results are the lack of adequate parameters to evaluate their performance, for children in general. Regarding these differences, the low participation of girls in contexts that potentially enhance motor development and the low, social reinforcement to engage in broad motor activities and sports since the first years of life may explain the more fragile perceptions of the girls compared to the boys. Importantly, the opportunities and experience acquired in appropriate learning contexts, the cultural values and social support of significant others seems to be the desirable strategies to strengthen children’s perceptions of competence. Adequate opportunities are also necessary to promote the construction of realistic perceptions of competence, regardless of gender and age. Providing children with opportunities for interaction and equivalent social expectations of high performance are essential to promote positive and realistic perceptions of competence. Effective learning and educational experiences in an appropriate context, with mediating agents being aware of their role, are fundamental to promote the development of children.

Finally, the data reported here have important practical implications. As mediating agents, relatives in general and parents in particular, caregivers, teachers and coaches must be attentive to the organization of strategies that promote in the child feelings of pride and pleasure for the achievement, that equally motivate boys and girls for the task, and that permit the construction of a positive and realistic perception of their own competence.

References

  • 1
    Harter S. Development Process in the Construction of the Self. In: Yawkey TD, Johnson JE, editors. Integrative process and socialization: early to middle childhood. New Jersey: Child Psychology; 1988, p. 278.
  • 2
    Harter S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2012, 440 p.
  • 3
    Harter S. Competence considered: Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. In: Sternberg RJ, Kolligian JJ, editors. Competence considered: Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420.
  • 4
    Harter S. Causes, correlates, and the functional role of global self-worth: A life-span perspective. In: Sternberg R, Kolligian J, editors. Competence considered. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420.
  • 5
    Gabbard C. Lifelong Motor Development. 7th ed. Austin: Wolters Kluwer Health; 2016.
  • 6
    Amorose AJ, Chanal JP, Marsh HW, Sarrazin RG, Bois JE, Weiss MR, et al. Reflected appraisals and perceived importance of significant others' appraisals as predictors of college athletes' self-perceptions of competence. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2005;27(1):416-430.
  • 7
    Weiss MR, Amorose AJ. Children's self-perceptions in the physical domain: Between- and within-age variability in level, accuracy, and sources of perceived competence. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2005;27:226-244.
  • 8
    Valentini NC. Competência Percebida: Considerações para promover a aprendizagem. In: Krebs RJ, Ferreira Neto CA, editores. Tópicos em desenvolvimento motor na infância e adolescência. Rio de Janeiro: LECSU; 2007, p. 137-157.
  • 9
    Harter S. Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the life-span. In: Sternberg RJ, Kolligian JJ, editors. Competence considered. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1990, p. 420-445.
  • 10
    Nobre GC, Bandeira PFR, Ramalho MHS, Nobre FSS, Valentini NC. Self-perception of competence in children from socially vulnerable contexts, assisted and unassisted by social sports projects. J Hum Growth Dev 2015;25(3):271. DOI: 10.7322/jhgd.97673
    » https://doi.org/10.7322/jhgd.97673
  • 11
    Villwock G, Valentini NC. Percepção de competência atlética, orientação motivacional e competência motora em crianças de escolas públicas: estudo desenvolvimentista e correlacional. Rev Bras Educ Fís Esporte 2007;21(4):245-257.
  • 12
    Valentini NC. Percepções de competência e desenvolvimento motor de meninos e meninas: um estudo transversal. Movimento 2002;8(2):51-62.
  • 13
    Ridgers ND, Fazey DMA, Fairclough SJ. Perceptions of athletic competence and fear of negative evaluation during physical education. Br J Educ Psychol 2007;77(2):339-49.
  • 14
    Raustorp A, Stahle A, Gudasic H, Kinnunen A, Mattsson E. Physical activity and self-perception in school children assessed with the Children and Youth - Physical Self-Perception Profile. Scand J Med Sci Sports 2005;15(1):126-134.
  • 15
    Goodway J, Robinson L, Crowe H. Gender differences in motor skill development in disadvantaged preschoolers from two geographical regions. Res Q Exerc Sport 2010;81(1):17-24. DOI: 10.5641/027013610X13352775119510
    » https://doi.org/10.5641/027013610X13352775119510
  • 16
    Jacobs JE, Lanza S, Osgood DW, Eccles JS, Wigfield A. Changes in children's self-competence and values: gender and domain differences across grades one through twelve. Child Dev 2002;73(2):509-527.
  • 17
    Nobre GC, Valentini NC, Nobre FSS. Fundamental motor skills, nutritional status, perceived competence, and school performance of Brazilian children in social vulnerability: Gender comparison. Child Abuse Negl 2018;80:335-45. DOI: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.04.007
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.04.007
  • 18
    Nobre GC, Bandeira P, Valentini NC. The relationship between general perceived motor competence, perceived competence relative to motor skill and actual motor competence in children. J Phys Educ 2016;27(1):2744. DOI: 10.4025/jphyseduc.v27i1.2744
    » https://doi.org/10.4025/jphyseduc.v27i1.2744
  • 19
    Almeida G, Valentini NC, Berleze A. Percepções de competência?: um estudo com crianças e adolescentes do ensino fundamental. Movimento 2009;15(1):71-79.
  • 20
    Feitoza AHP, Henrique R dos S, Barnett L, Re A, Lopes VP, Webster EK, et al. Perceived motor competence in childhood: comparative study among countries. J Mot Learn Dev 2017;10(7):1-22. DOI: 10.1123/jmld.2016-0079
    » https://doi.org/10.1123/jmld.2016-0079
  • 21
    Valentini NC, Villwock G, Vieira LF, Vieira JLL, Levi M, Barbosa L. Brazilian validity of the Harter' Self-perception profile for children. Psicol Reflexão e Crítica 2010;23(3):411-419.
  • 22
    Valentini NC. The influence of a motor skill intervention on the motor performance and perceived competence of children with motor delays. Rev Paul Educação Física 2002;16(1):61-75.
  • 23
    Leary RM, Tangney JP. The Self as an organizing construct in behavioral and social sciences. In: Handbook of Self and Identity. 2nd ed. New York City: Guilford Press; 2003, p. 3-13.
  • 24
    Harter S. Effectance motivation reconsidered toward a developmental model. Hum Dev 1978;21(1):34-64.
  • 25
    Harter S. Developmental and dynamic changes in the nature of the self-concept implications for child psychotherapy. In: Shirk SR, editor. Cognitive development and child psychotherapy. Denver: Springer Science; 1988, p. 110.
  • 26
    Lopes VP, Saraiva L, Gonçalves C, Rodrigues LP. Association between perceived and actual motor competence in Portuguese children. J Mot Learn Dev 2017;10(7):1-22. DOI: 10.1123/jmld.2016-0059
    » https://doi.org/10.1123/jmld.2016-0059
  • 27
    Spessato BC, Gabbard C, Robinson L, Valentini NC. Body mass index, perceived and actual physical competence: The relationship among young children. Child Care Health Dev 2013;39(6):845-850.
  • 28
    Masci I, Schmidt M, Marchetti R, Vannozzi G, Pesce C. When children's perceived and actual motor competence mismatch: sport participation and gender differences. J Mot Learn Dev 2017;11(5):1-33. DOI: 10.1123/jmld.2016-0081
    » https://doi.org/10.1123/jmld.2016-0081
  • 29
    Spessato BC, Gabbard C, Valentini NC. The role of motor competence and body mass index in children's activity levels in physical education classes. J Teach Phys Educ 2013;32(1):118-130.
  • 30
    Nobre GC, Bandeira PFR, Ramalho MHS, Nobre FSS, Valentini NC. Self-perception of competence in children from socially vulnerable contexts, assisted and unassisted by social sports projects. J Hum Growth Dev 2015;25(3):276-281. DOI: 10.7322/jhgd.97673
    » https://doi.org/10.7322/jhgd.97673
  • 31
    Pansera SM. Crianças que se percebem competentes e são intrinsecamente motivadas são motoramente mais competentes? Um estudo associativo entre desenvolvimento motor e aspectos psicossociais de escolares. [Dissertação de Mestrado em Ciência do movimento Humano]. Porto Alegre: Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul. Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ciência do Movimento Humano; 2012.
  • 32
    Bandeira PFR, De Souza MS, Zanella LW, Nobre GC, Valentini NC. Percepção de competência motora e nível de atividade física: estudo comparativo entre sexos. Cinergis 2014;15(4):191-194.
  • 33
    Nobre G, Bandeira P, Valentini N. Relação entre a competência motora percebida geral, o gênero, a competência motora percebida específica à tarefa e a competência motora real de crianças. J Phys Educ 2016;27(1):e2744.
  • 34
    Carroll B, Loumidis J. Children's perceived competence and enjoyment in physical education and physical activity outside school. Eur Phys Educ Rev 2001;7(1):24-43.
  • 35
    Brian A, Bardid F, Barnett L, Deconinck F, Lenoir M, Goodway J. Actual and perceived motor competence levels of Belgian and US preschool children. J Mot Learn Dev 2017:S320-S336. DOI: 10.1123/jmld.2016-0071
    » https://doi.org/10.1123/jmld.2016-0071
  • 36
    Barnett L, Lubans D, Salmon J, Timperio A, Ridgers ND. What is the contribution of actual motor skill, fitness, and physical activity to children's self-perception of motor competence? J Mot Learn Dev 2017;6(2):S461-S473. Doi:10.1123/jmld.2016-0076
    » https://doi.org/10.1123/jmld.2016-0076
  • 37
    Duncan MJ, Jones V, O'Brien W, Barnett LM, Eyre ELJ. Self-Perceived and actual motor competence in young British children. Percept Mot Skills 2018;125(2):251-264. DOI: 10.1177/0031512517752833
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/0031512517752833
  • 38
    Liong GHE, Ridgers ND, Barnett LM. Associations between skill perceptions and young children's actual fundamental movement skills. Percept Mot Skills 2015;120(2):591-603 DOI: 10.2466/10.25.PMS.120v18x2
    » https://doi.org/10.2466/10.25.PMS.120v18x2
  • 39
    Bois JE, Sarrazin PG, Brustad RJ, Trouilloud DO, Cury F. Elementary schoolchildren's perceived competence and physical activity involvement: the influence of parents' role modelling behaviours and perceptions of their child's competence. Psychol Sport Exerc 2005;6(4):381-397.
  • 40
    Lentillon V. Les stéréotypes sexués relatifs à la pratique des activités physiques et sportives chez les adolescents français et leurs conséquences discriminatoires. Bull Psychol 2009;499(1):15.
  • 41
    Horn TS, Weiss MR. A developmental analysis of children's self-ability judgments in the physical domain. Pediatr Exerc Sci 1991;3(4):310-326.
  • 42
    Horn TS, Amorose AJ. Sources of competence information. In: Duda JL, editor. Advancements in sport and exercise psychology measurement. Morgantown: Fitness Information Technology; 1998, p. 49-64.
  • 43
    Krebs RJ, Santos JOL, Ramalho MHS, Nazario PF, Nobre GC, Almeida RT. Disposição de adolescentes para a prática de esportes: um estudo orientado pela Teoria Bioecológica de Bronfenbrenner. Motriz Rev Educ Física 2010;17(1):195-201.
  • 44
    Barnett LM, Beurden E, Morgan PJ, Brooks LO, Beard JR. Gender differences in motor skill proficiency from childhood to adolescence. Res Q Exerc Sport 2010;81(2):162-170. DOI: 10.1080/02701367.2010.10599663
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2010.10599663

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    29 Apr 2019
  • Date of issue
    2019

History

  • Received
    28 Feb 2017
  • Reviewed
    08 Aug 2018
  • Accepted
    21 Aug 2018
Universidade Estadual de Maringá Avenida Colombo, 5790 - cep: 87020-900 - tel: 44 3011 4315 - Maringá - PR - Brazil
E-mail: revdef@uem.br