Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Phonological remediation program in students with learning difficulties

Abstracts

PURPOSE: To verify the effectiveness of a phonological remediation program in students with learning difficulties. METHODS: Forty students from 2nd to 4th grades, from both genders, ranging from eight years and one month to 12 years old, were divided into two groups: Group 1 (G1), composed by 20 students with learning disabilities; and Group 2 (G2), composed by 20 students without learning disabilities. The Cognitive-Linguistic Performance Test and the oral reading and comprehension were applied in pre-test and post-test situations in all students. After the pre-test, only the students from Group 1 were submitted to the phonological remediation program. RESULTS: After the phonological remediation program, Group 1 presented differences for reading abilities, auditory and visual processing, and processing speed, evidenced by superior performance in the post-testing situation. CONCLUSION: The phonological remediation program was efficient to the development of cognitive-linguistic skills and reading and text comprehension of students with learning difficulties

Learning disorders; Students; Evaluation; Rehabilitation; Reading


OBJETIVO: Verificar a eficácia do programa de remediação fonológica em escolares com dificuldades de aprendizagem. MÉTODOS: Participaram 40 escolares, de 2ª a 4ª séries do município de Marília (SP), de ambos os gêneros, faixa etária de oito anos e um mês a 12 anos, que foram distribuídos em dois grupos: Grupo 1 (G1), composto por 20 escolares com dificuldades de aprendizagem; e Grupo 2 (G2), composto por 20 escolares sem dificuldades de aprendizagem. Foram aplicados o Teste de Desempenho Cognitivo-Linguístico e a prova de leitura oral e compreensão de textos em todos os escolares, pré e pós testagem. Os participantes do Grupo 1 foram submetidos ao programa de remediação fonológica após a pré-testagem. RESULTADOS: Ao final do programa de remediação fonológica o Grupo 1 apresentou diferença para as habilidades de leitura, processamento auditivo, visual e velocidade de processamento, evidenciada por desempenho superior na pós-testagem. CONCLUSÃO: Houve eficácia do programa de remediação fonológica para o desenvolvimento de habilidades cognitivo-linguísticas e de leitura e compreensão de texto para escolares com dificuldade de aprendizagem

Transtornos de aprendizagem; Estudantes; Avaliação; Reabilitação; Leitura


ORIGINAL ARTICLE ARTIGO ORIGINAL

IClinical speech-language pathologist - Marília (SP), Brazil

IIDepartment of Speech, Language and Hearing Therapy, Post-graduation Program in Education - Faculty of Sciences and Phylosophy - Universidade Estadual Paulista "Julio de Mesquita Filho" - UNESP - Marília (SP), Brazil

Correspondence address

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: To verify the effectiveness of a phonological remediation program in students with learning difficulties.

METHODS: Forty students from 2nd to 4th grades, from both genders, ranging from eight years and one month to 12 years old, were divided into two groups: Group 1 (G1), composed by 20 students with learning disabilities; and Group 2 (G2), composed by 20 students without learning disabilities. The Cognitive-Linguistic Performance Test and the oral reading and comprehension were applied in pre-test and post-test situations in all students. After the pre-test, only the students from Group 1 were submitted to the phonological remediation program.

RESULTS: After the phonological remediation program, Group 1 presented differences for reading abilities, auditory and visual processing, and processing speed, evidenced by superior performance in the post-testing situation.

CONCLUSION: The phonological remediation program was efficient to the development of cognitive-linguistic skills and reading and text comprehension of students with learning difficulties.

Keywords: Learning disorders, Students, Evaluation, Rehabilitation/methods, Reading

INTRODUCTION

There is no consensus on the definition of learning disabilities, nor is it agreed upon how, why or when it manifests itself. According to the literature, learning disabilities are characterized by a heterogeneous group of events that leads to low academic performance on tasks of reading, writing and mathematical calculus. They can be categorized as transient and occur at any time during the teaching-learning process(1,2).

Thus, the difficulty of learning can also be regarded as a specific difficulty in school activities and concerns the discrepancy between what is assumed that the child is potentially able to learn in a given situation in the classroom and what he/she actually performs(3).

In Brazil, there is no estimate on the prevalence of learning disabilities, because this diagnostic category is not within the educational system, however, the inability to read affects from 2 to 8% of elementary school students in Brazil(4).

There is a consensus among researchers that phonological awareness is important for reading acquisition and that most individuals with delayed reading skills present disorders on it(5,6). The phonological deficit hypothesis has been supported by numerous studies which have identified delays for the sensitivity to rhyme, alliteration, and phonemic segmentation during the development of reading(7-9). However, studies(6,8) indicate the existence of deficits in addition to problems with phonological processing, such as deficits in working memory, in perceptual-motor automaticity and in rapid naming, which may be present not only in students with learning disabilities, but also in students who have some difficulty in learning the mechanism of phoneme-grapheme conversion during literacy.

Thus, reading difficulties may originate from three kinds of deficits: phonological, naming and double deficit (phonological and naming). The existence of a double deficit causes the most severe reading difficulties resulting in difficulty in decoding words. The difficulty in decoding has implications such as a limitation on the reading of texts of increasing complexity, decreasing reader's exposure to new words, limiting the acquisition of vocabulary and hindering the development of expertise in reading comprehension(5).

There is scientific evidence that highlights the importance of using interventions in early school years(10) for children with learning difficulties, as there are indications that the initial problems with the acquisition of reading mechanisms may not completely disappear after the first school years if the child is not subjected to an intervention program(11).

Although there are international and national studies which describe the use of remediation programs(12-15), there are few national studies on children with learning difficulties, highlighting the need for research to adjust or develop remediation programs for the specific treatment of cognitive-linguistic change in this population.

Due to the above, the aim of this study was to verify the effectiveness of a phonological remediation program in students with learning difficulties.

METHODS

This study is characterized as experimental and longitudinal realized with students after the approval of the Ethics in Research Committee of the Universidade Estadual Paulista (UNESP), under protocol number 2576/2007.

Participants were 40 students from both genders aged between 8 years and 1 month old to 12 years old, from 2nd to 4th grade of elementary education in a public school in Marília - São Paulo - Brazil, divided in the following groups: Group 1 (G1): composed by 20 students, being twelve females and eight males with learning difficulties submitted to a remediation program; Group 2 (G2): composed by 20 students, being ten females and ten males with learning difficulties not submitted to a remediation program.

The students were selected by teachers based on academic performance in two consecutive periods. Those with poor performance are considered students with learning difficulties, and those who had sufficient performance on tests of Portuguese and Mathematics in two consecutive periods are considered students without learning difficulties. From this first selection, the researcher analyzed the student's files following the inclusion criteria: parent signing the Instrument of Consent, no family history of development dyslexia or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), lack of diagnosis of hearing, visual, motor or cognitive disorders, and exclusion criteria: no signing of the Instrument of Consent by parents.

The students were submitted to the following procedures in pre and post-testing:

- Cognitive-Linguistic Performance Test - Collective Version(16): This version is composed of five subtests: Recognition of the alphabet in sequence, copying forms; Arithmetics (addition, subtraction, multiplication and division); Dictation of 30 real words and ten nonwords, Short-term memory (writing of 16 digits sequence, which can contain from two to nine digits). The results of the subtests of the collective version were registered in the form of scoring, one point being assigned to each correct mark of the student.

- Cognitive-Linguistic Performance Test - Individual Version(16): This version is composed of ten subtests: Reading words and nonwords (reading of words, reading of correct words in one minute and reading of nonwords), phonological awareness (alliteration and rhyme); mathematical ability (arithmetic and counting down), auditory processing (discrimination of sounds, repetition of numbers, repetition of inverse numbers, rhythmic beats, word repetition and nonword repetition), writing (word spelling, nonword spelling, total spelling) visual processing (visual memory for shapes), processing speed (rapid naming of pictures and rapid naming of numbers); sequencing (alphabet and strings); motor skill (motor skills in right, left and both hands); reversals (reversals of numbers). The results of the subtests of the individual version were registered in the form of scoring, which varied from one to 40 points, one point being assigned to each correct mark of the student.

- Assessment of oral reading and text comprehension: three texts were selected out of the suggestions of 14 teachers from 2nd to 4th grade of Municipal Schools of Marília - SP. The texts with higher approval ratings from teachers were selected to each grade. Each student was individually evaluated. The texts and questions were presented on A4 paper, typed in Arial, size 16, black, double-spaced. The analysis of the texts was done by counting the errors of words read (reading accuracy), total time of reading and reading speed, which was expressed in words per minute, as described in the literature(17,18). The number of words per minute was calculated by multiplying the number of words per sixty seconds, and this value was divided by the total time of reading in seconds. The comprehension was analyzed by four questions presented sequentially in the text, to which the students wrote their answers. The questions of the texts were analyzed for comprehension from the count of correct answers in written form, with four correct answers = 100% accuracy (full comprehension), three correct answers = 75% correct, two correct answers = 50% correct, one correct answer = 25% accuracy (partial comprehension), no correct answer = no reading comprehension.

Upon completion of the pre-testing, students with learning difficulties were subjected to a remediation program(19). Strategies were drawn up for the program from the description of the original research(19), based on psycholinguistic criteria of adaptation for Portuguese(20,21). The program had a total of 18 cumulative sessions, i.e., every session crafted a new activity associated with the activities from the previous session. The order of activities within the program is described below:

1) Identification of sound and letter: every letter of the alphabet was presented on A4 paper and the students should name them and identify the sounds of the alphabet.

2) Words identification in a sentence: seven statements were spoken and the students were supposed to divide each one into words, marking them by clapping.

3) Syllable identification and manipulation in the word: two words were spoken for the students to identify their syllables and then manipulate the syllables to form new words in initial, medial and final position. In each session, six words were provided to students.

4) Phonemic synthesis: seven words were spoken separated by sounds and the students should recognize them.

5) Rhyme: students were solicited to say words that ended with the same sound and identify pictures that presented rhymes when named.

6) Phonemes identification and discrimination: a phoneme was spoken and the students were asked to say a word that began with this sound. Then, seven words were spoken to the students and they were asked if the target phoneme was in each word or not. The phonemes were presented by following the order of development of speech and language.

7) Phoneme segmentation: a word was presented orally and the students were asked to speak all of the phonemes on it. In this phase of the program colored cards were used to help the students segment the sounds of words spoken. In each session seven words were provided.

8) Phoneme subtraction: six words were presented to the students and they were asked to withdraw the final phoneme, and then six more words for them to withdraw the initial phoneme.

9) Phoneme substitution: a word was presented orally and the students were asked to withdraw the initial phoneme and substitute it with another, forming thus a new word. In each session students were provided with seven words.

10) Phoneme transposition: the students were asked to speak the words in reverse order to form new words.

All students in this study were subjected to evaluations of pre-testing and post-testing, and only the students from G1 were submitted to a phonological remediation program. All data collection was performed at the school by the researcher in a room designated by the director so that the collection could be accomplished individually. The application of the procedures in this study began in February 2008 and ended in April of the same year. Each student was submitted to two sessions for the implementation of the procedures of pre-testing and post-testing, lasting 40 minutes each, and attended the program twice a week during regular class. Each session lasted 40 minutes, totaling 18 therapy sessions.

Statistical analysis was performed based on the number of correct marks between the two groups. The Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test was applied with the objective of verifying possible differences between the situations of pre-and post-testing in the groups. The results were analyzed statistically at a significance level of 5%, with an asterisk when there was a statistically significant difference.

RESULTS

To better present the results of this study, data will be presented from the grouping of the skills assessed in the Cognitive-Linguistic Performance Test. Thus, the data are presented according to the following skills: reading, phonological awareness, auditory processing, processing speed and visual processing.

Table 1 shows the description of the mean, standard deviation and p-value comparison of performance of students from G1 and G2 in pre- and post-testing in the tests of reading skills and phonological awareness of the Cognitive-Linguistic Performance Test.

It was observed that G1 presented a statistically significant difference between pre- and post-testing in the reading skills tests, and there was an increase in the average score in reading words and nonwords and a decrease of reading time, showing that students submitted to the remediation program presented improvement in this skill.

No statistically significant difference was found between the scores obtained by students from G1 in pre- and post-testing on the skill of phonological awareness (rhyme and alliteration).

Table 2 shows the description of the mean, standard deviation and p-value comparison of performance of students from G1 and G2 in the pre- and post-testing in the tests of auditory processing and processing speed of the Cognitive-Linguistic Performance Test.

After applying the Wilcoxon Test, a statistically significant difference was verified, when pre-testing was compared to post-testing, only on the direct memory test for G1. As for processing speed, there was statistically significant difference in pre-testing and post-testing between the scores obtained by G1 for rapid automatized naming of pictures and rapid automatized naming of digits with the reduction of naming time.

Table 3 is the description of the mean, standard deviation and p-value of the performance of students in pre-testing and post-testing regarding visual processing skills of the Cognitive-Linguistic Performance Test.

After applying the Wilcoxon Test, it was observed that there was a statistically significant difference between pre-testing and post-testing of the scores obtained by G1 on the tests of access to visual processing, copying forms, visual memory for two cards, visual memory for three cards, visual memory for four cards, visual memory for five cards, and there was an increase in the number of correct answers in these tests.

Table 4 shows the description of the mean, standard deviation and p-value of the performance of students in G1 and G2 in pre- and post-testing on reading and reading comprehension.

After applying the Wilcoxon Test, it was observed in relation to G1 statistically significant difference between pre- and post-testing, with an increased number of words read correctly, less time and increased speed in reading and enhanced skills in reading comprehension. As for G2, we found statistically significant difference in reading accuracy, speed and total time of reading, however, no statistically significant difference in reading comprehension. These results demonstrate that the improvement in the reading comprehension of G1 is due to the remediation program.

DISCUSSION

The analysis revealed the difference in performance of participants from G1 in relation to G2 in reading skills, phonological awareness, auditory processing, visual processing speed, assessed by the Cognitive-linguistic Performance Test. These results corroborate studies(5,22,23) showing that the difficulties of poor readers compared to good readers are primarily related to phonological processing, including phonological awareness, auditory memory and discrimination, access to the recovery of phonological information and phonological working memory. In this study, the participants from G1 presented lower performances when compared to G2 in the skills of phonological awareness, auditory processing (words and nonwords repetition), direct and indirect memory for digits, indicating the presence of phonological deficits.

The ability to recognize a pattern of letters within words as a unit is important for fluent reading(23). This skill is based on previously established memory representations of common letter patterns and can be impaired if there is a deficit in naming speed. These deficits are related to the difficulty in accessing and retaining phonological information in long-term memory(24).

These results demonstrate that the students from G1 presented inferior performance in relation to G2 in pre-testing in tests of rapid automatized naming of digits and figures, as shown the literature(25), which indicates the presence of alteration in auditory memory in children with learning difficulties. These results also corroborate a study(26) which described inferior results in phonological awareness and rapid naming in children with learning difficulties when compared to children without difficulty.

By analyzing results of G1 on reading skills in pre- and post-testing, statistically significant difference was found for word reading, nonwords reading and words read correctly in one minute, and there was an increase in the average of correct answers and reduction of reading time in post-testing. These results are in line with a study(27) which describes the increase in performance of children in words and nonwords reading after the application of a therapeutic procedure that included explicit teaching of phonological awareness and grapheme-phoneme correspondence. In this study, the influence of the remediation program in the progress of students with reading difficulties was also evidenced by a statistically significant difference between pre-testing and post-testing for accuracy and speed of reading, but only G1 presented statistically significant difference between the pre- and post-testing for reading comprehension. These results are consistent with a study(28) which described that after participation in interventions that focused on phonological awareness the children demonstrated superior skills in the post-testing of nonwords reading and reading comprehension tasks.

These results are in accordance with research(27) that linked the improvement in word recognition with the improvement in reading comprehension. Participants in the G1 had a statistically significant difference between pre-testing and post-testing on word reading tests and number of words read correctly in one minute, showing an improvement in decoding skills after the intervention program, as well as in the text comprehension test.

These results are consistent with the literature(29) which noted that efficient decoding relieves the cognitive load, allowing more attention to be directed to comprehension. Literature(30) has indicated that reading comprehension is based on working memory. In this study, children with reading difficulties (G1) showed a statistically significant difference in the situation of pre-and post-testing among the scores for rapid automatized naming of figures and rapid automatized naming of digits, indicating an improvement in working memory after the program intervention, and improved skills in reading comprehension.

However, it is noteworthy that students from G2, who were not submitted to a phonological remediation program, showed improvement in post-testing compared to pre-testing, highlighting that the work of teachers in the classroom favored the acquisition and development of the alphabetic principle of the writing system in Portuguese, however, if not carried out systematically and instructionally it can cause delays in the development of cognitive-linguistic skills for reading acquisition. What proves this assertion is the fact that students in G1, even after being submitted to a phonological remediation program, could not reach the average performance of students from G2.

CONCLUSION

It can be concluded that the remediation program was effective for students with learning disabilities to obtain improved performance with regard to reading skills, auditory processing, visual processing speed and reading comprehension.

The results of this study indicate the necessity of using instruments of intervention with children with learning disabilities which have the basic structure of the writing system in Brazilian Portuguese. Only through the explicit teaching of rules associated with the phoneme-grapheme conversion and reading and text comprehension practices in the classroom can we minimize the number of students who have learning problems due to lack of systematic education and focus on the alphabetic principle of Portuguese, as the students from G1 could only parallel in the performance of reading skills, auditory processing, visual processing speed and reading comprehension with G2 after they took part in the intervention proposed.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We are grateful for the National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq) for the support given to this research, under process number 135549/2007-8.

REFERENCES

  • 1. Capellini SA. Distúrbios de aprendizagem versus dislexia. In: Ferreira LP, Befi-Lopes DM, Limongi SCO, organizadores. Tratado de fonoaudiologia. São Paulo: Roca; 2004. p. 862-76.
  • 2. Capellini AS, Silva APC, Silva C, Pinheiro FH. Avaliação e diagnóstico fonoaudiológico nos distúrbios de aprendizagem e dislexias. In: Zorzi JL, Capellini SA. Dislexia e outros distúrbios da leitura-escrita. 2a- ed. São José dos Campos: Pulso Editorial; 2009. p. 95-111.
  • 3. Rolfsen AB, Martinez CMS. Programa de intervenção para pais de crianças com dificuldades de aprendizagem: um estudo preliminar. Paidéia (Ribeirão Preto).2008;18(39):175-88.
  • 4. Ciasca SM, Capellini SA, Tonelloto JMF. Distúrbios específicos de aprendizagem. In: Ciasca SM. Distúrbios de aprendizagem: proposta de avaliação interdisciplinar. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2003. p. 55-65.
  • 5. Chard DJ, Stoolmiller M, Harn BA, Wanzek J, Vaughn S, Linan-Thompson S, Kame'enui EJ. Predicting reading success in a multilevel schoolwide reading model: a retrospective analysis. J Learn Disabil. 2008;41(2):174-88.
  • 6. Savage RS, Frederickson N. Beyond phonology: What else is needed to describe the problems of below-average readers and spellers? J Learn Disabil. 2006;39(5):399-413.
  • 7. Vukovic RK, Wilson AM, Nash KK. Naming speed deficits in adults with reading disabilities: a test of the double-deficit hypothesis. J Learn Disabil. 2004;37(5):440-50.
  • 8. Savage RS, Frederickson N, Goodwin R, Patni U, Smith N, Tuersley L. Relationships among rapid digit naming, phonological processing, motor automaticity, and speech perception in poor, average, and good readers and spellers. J Learn Disabil. 2005;38(1):12-28.
  • 9. Swanson HL, Howard CB, Sáez L. Do different components of working memory underlie different subgroups of reading disabilities? J Learn Disabil. 2006;39(3):252-69.
  • 10. Hay I, Elias G, Fielding-Barnsley R, Homel R, Freiberg K. Language delays, reading delays, and learning difficulties: interactive elements requiring multidimensional programming. J Learn Disabil. 2007;40(5):400-9.
  • 11. Denton CA, Fletcher JM, Anthony JL, Francis DJ. An evaluation of intensive intervention for students with persistent reading difficulties. J Learn Disabil. 2006;39(5):447-66.
  • 12. Vaughn S, Fletcher JM, Francis DJ, Denton CA, Wanzek J, Wexler J, et al. Response to intervention with older students with reading difficulties.Learn Individ Differ.2008;18(3):338-45.
  • 13. Ziolkowska R. Early intervention for students with reading and writhing difficulties. Read Improv. 2007;44(2):76-86.
  • 14. Capellini SA, Padula NAMR, Ciasca SM. Desempenho de escolares com distúrbio específico de leitura em programa de remediação. Pró-Fono. 2004;16(3):261-74.
  • 15. Germano GD, Capellini SA. Eficácia do programa de remediação auditivo-visual computadorizado em escolares com dislexia. Pró-Fono. 2008;20(4):237-42.
  • 16. Capellini SA, Silva C, Gonzaga J, Galhardo MT, Cruvinel P, Smythe I. Desempenho cognitivo-linguístico de escolares de 1Ş a 4Ş séries do ensino público municipal. Rev Psicopedagogia. 2007;24(73):30-44.
  • 17. Condemarin M, Blomquist M. Dislexia: manual de leitura corretiva. 3a ed. Porto Alegre: Artes Médicas; 1989.
  • 18. Ramos CS. Avaliação da leitura em escolares com indicação de dificuldade de leitura e escrita [dissertação]. São Paulo: Universidade Federal de São Paulo. Escola Paulista de Medicina; 2005.
  • 19. Hatcher PJ, Hulme C, Ellis AW. Ameliorating early reading failure by integrating the teaching of reading and phonological skills: the phonological linkage hypothesis. Child Dev. 1994;65(1):41-57.
  • 20. Silva C, Capellini SA. Eficácia do programa de remediação fonológica e leitura no distúrbio de aprendizagem. Pró-Fono. 2010;22(2):131-9.
  • 21. Capellini SA. Eficácia terapêutica do programa de remediação fonológica, programa de remediação com leitura e programa de remediação fonológica e leitura em crianças com dislexia do desenvolvimento: estudo comparativo (FAPESP - Processo 06/5752-8). 2009. [Relatório de pesquisa]
  • 22. Giangiacomo MCPB, Navas ALGP. A influência da memória operacional nas habilidades de compreensão de leitura em escolares de 4Ş série. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2008;13(1):69-74.
  • 23. Conrad N, Levy B. Letter processing and the formation of memory representations in children with naming speed deficits. Read Writ. 2007;20(3):201-23.
  • 24. Bretherton L, Holmes VM. The relationship between auditory temporal processing, phonemic awareness, and reading disability. J Exp Child Psychol. 2003;84(3):218-43.
  • 25. Billard C, Fluss J, Ducot B, Warszawski J, Ecalle J, Magnan A, et al. Étude des facteurs liés aux difficultés d'apprentissage de la lecture. À partir d'un échantillon de 1062 enfants de seconde année d'école élementaire. Arch Pediatr.2008;15(6):1058-67.
  • 26. Furbeta TC, Felippe AC. Avaliação simplificada do processamento auditivo e dificuldades de leitura-escrita. Pró-Fono. 2005;17(1):11-8.
  • 27. Aaron PG, Joshi RM, Gooden R, Bentum KE. Diagnosis and treatment of reading disabilities based on the component model of reading: an alternative to the discrepancy model of LD. J Learn Disabil. 2008;41(1):67-84.
  • 28. Landry SH, Swank PR, Smith KE, Assel MA, Gunnewig SB. Enhancing early literacy skills for preschool children: bringing a professional development model to scale. J Learn Disabil. 2006;39(4):306-24.
  • 29. Shapiro ES, Solari E, Petscher Y. Use of a measure of reading comprehension to enhance prediction on the state high stakes assessment. Learn Individ Differ. 2008;18(3):316-28.
  • 30. Gindri G, Keske-Soares M, Mota HB. Memória de trabalho, consciência fonológica e hipótese de escrita. Pró-Fono. 2007;19(3):313-22.
  • Phonological remediation program in students with learning difficulties

    Ana Paula de Castro SilvaI; Simone Aparecida CapelliniII
  • Publication Dates

    • Publication in this collection
      02 May 2011
    • Date of issue
      Mar 2011

    History

    • Accepted
      28 Oct 2010
    • Received
      16 July 2009
    Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia Alameda Jaú, 684, 7ºandar, 01420-001 São Paulo/SP Brasil, Tel/Fax: (55 11) 3873-4211 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
    E-mail: jornal@sbfa.org.br