Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Proboscis Length and Resource Utilization in Two Uruguayan Bumblebees: Bombus atratus Franklin and Bombus bellicosus Smith (Hymenoptera: Apidae)

Abstract

Bumblebees (Bombus sp.) are eusocial insects with an annual life cycle whose use as pollinator of crops has gained great importance in modern agriculture. Several authors have found that resource use in Bombus species is usually based on the correlation between the proboscis length of the bumblebees and the corolla depth of the flowers. The aim of this study was to determine proboscis length of Bombus atratus and B. bellicosus, two Uruguayan bumblebees, and verify the resource exploitation testing two cultivated species, the red clover and the bird's foot trefoil. Bumblebee foraging activity was recorded in two culture conditions: in a red clover and bird's foot trefoil mixed meadow, and in contiguous plots of these legumes, and the proboscis length of collected foragers was determined. Both species of bumblebees exploited red clover and bird's foot trefoil although they did it in different proportions in all instances tested. The results indicated that the choice of the resources in B. atratus and B. bellicosus was influenced by their proboscis length. Bombus atratus has a longer proboscis and preferably visited red clover, possibly obtaining nectar easier and faster than B. bellicosus, which has a shorter proboscis. Bombus bellicosus used both resources without any clear preference.

Native bee; resource partitioning; tongue length


ECOLOGY, BEHAVIOR AND BIONOMICS

Proboscis Length and Resource Utilization in Two Uruguayan Bumblebees: Bombus atratus Franklin and Bombus bellicosus Smith (Hymenoptera: Apidae)

N Arbulo; E Santos; S Salvarrey; C Invernizzi

Sección Etología, Facultad de Ciencias, Universidad de la República, Montevideo, Uruguay

Correspondence Correspondence: Natalia Arbulo, Sección Etología, Facultad de Ciencias, Univ de la República, Iguá 4225 Esq. Mataojo, Piso 6 Norte, CP 11400, Montevideo, Uruguay; narbulo@fcien.edu.uy

ABSTRACT

Bumblebees (Bombus sp.) are eusocial insects with an annual life cycle whose use as pollinator of crops has gained great importance in modern agriculture. Several authors have found that resource use in Bombus species is usually based on the correlation between the proboscis length of the bumblebees and the corolla depth of the flowers. The aim of this study was to determine proboscis length of Bombus atratus and B. bellicosus, two Uruguayan bumblebees, and verify the resource exploitation testing two cultivated species, the red clover and the bird's foot trefoil. Bumblebee foraging activity was recorded in two culture conditions: in a red clover and bird's foot trefoil mixed meadow, and in contiguous plots of these legumes, and the proboscis length of collected foragers was determined. Both species of bumblebees exploited red clover and bird's foot trefoil although they did it in different proportions in all instances tested. The results indicated that the choice of the resources in B. atratus and B. bellicosus was influenced by their proboscis length. Bombus atratus has a longer proboscis and preferably visited red clover, possibly obtaining nectar easier and faster than B. bellicosus, which has a shorter proboscis. Bombus bellicosus used both resources without any clear preference.

Keywords: Native bee, resource partitioning, tongue length

Introduction

Bumblebees (Bombus sp) are univoltine eusocial insects (Michener 1974, 2007) whose use as crop pollinators has gained great importance in modern agriculture, especially in those cases where honeybees are not efficient (Velthuis 2002, Goulson 2003, Heinrich 2004, Velthuis & van Doorn 2006). They must be able to exploit a great diversity of plant species because colony life is generally longer than the flowering duration of a single species (Heinrich 1976a, 2004). Except by a few very specialized artic species, bumblebees are polilectic, i.e. they obtain food from a variety of plant species (Michener 1974, 2007, Heinrich 1976a, 2004, Goulson 2003). At any time and facing a varied floral offer, the choice of the plant species will be influenced by several factors, some inherent to the bumblebee and others to the plants. Among the former there are morphological characteristics such as weight, size, wing length and specially proboscis length, which will determine the ability of a bumblebee to use certain plant species (Harder 1983, 1985).

There is a number of reports on resource use in Bombus species based on the correlation between the proboscis length of the bumblebees and the corolla depth of the flowers (Heinrich 1976a,b, 2004, Inouye 1978, 1980, Pyke 1982, Graham & Jones 1996). This is explained by differences in the efficiency with which bumblebees can extract nectar from the different plant species. Nectar is the only source of energy for bumblebees, polen is the protein food and is essential to mantain the larval growth, but without nectar adult bumblebees perish in a few hours (Heinrich 2004). It is then expected that growth, survivorship and reproduction of bumblebee colonies depend on the rate of net energy gain of the colony (Pyke 1982, Heinrich 2004). Thus, foragers will seek to maximize their net profit working in those plant species in which they obtain the greatest amount of nectar per unit of time with the least energy expenditure (Heinrich 2004).

Proboscis length determines the depth at which a bumblebee can reach nectar in a flower as well as the handling time of each flower and therefore the number of flowers visited per unit of time (Holm 1966, Inouye 1980, Harder 1983, 1985, Graham & Jones 1996). Bumblebees probably learn through experience to visit flowers that are best suited to the length of their proboscis (Heinrich 2004). Variation between species in this morphological feature determines differences in floral preferences, with long proboscis species specializing on long corolla flowers and short proboscis species on short corolla flowers (Holm 1966, Heinrich 1976a,b, 1979, Inouye 1978, 1980, Harder 1985, Graham & Jones 1996). So, when different species of bumblebees coexist, the morphological differences between them as well as the great diversity of flower structures and sizes of the different plant species, provide an opportunity for differentiation in resource use. The resulting resource partitioning would diminish interspecific competition (Heinrich 1976b, Inouye 1978, 1980, Pyke 1982, Harder 1985, Graham & Jones 1996).

There are four species of bumblebees reported from Uruguay: Bombus atratus (Franklin), B. bellicosus (Smith), B. brasiliensis (Lepeletier) and B. morio (Swederus). The first two are widely distributed in the country, while the others have been recorded only in one location each (Abrahamovich & Díaz 2002, Abrahamovich et al 2004). The two most common species are easily distinguishable because of their different coloration. They also differ in mean worker body size, since B. atratus was larger in average than B. bellicosus (13.6 mm and 10.6 mm, respectively) (Moure & Sakagami 1962 in Cortopassi-Laurino et al 2003).

In Uruguay, red clover (Trifolium pratense L.) and bird's foot trefoil (Lotus corniculatus L.) are two of the most cultivated forage legumes for livestock feeding. These Fabaceae present a similar general floral structure in which sexual structures of flower are enclosed in the keel petals that form the corolla, at the bottom of which are found nectaries (Izaguirre & Beyhaut 1997). Despite this general similarity, these plants have large differences in several floral characters. The red clover inflorescence shows a head formed by numerous pink florets that have a deep tubular corolla 7.5 mm to 12.4 mm long and 1.6 mm to 2.5 mm wide (Carámbula 1981). On the other hand, the bird's foot trefoil presents a more open corolla of less than 10 mm long and form inflorescences in clusters of 4-6 yellow-orange flowers (Izaguirre & Beyhaut 1997). These morphological differences make a good scenario to explore possible differences in resource utilization by bumblebees.

The aim of this study was to determine proboscis length of Bombus atratus and B. bellicosus, two Uruguayan bumblebees, and verify the resource exploitation testing two cultivated species, the red clover and the bird's foot trefoil.

Material and Methods

Field work was carried out at the Centro Regional Sur (CRS) of Facultad de Agronomía (34º 36' S, 56º 13' W) in March 2005 and at La Estanzuela Experimental Station of the Instituto Nacional de Investigación Agropecuaria (INIA-LE) (34º 20' S, 57º 41' W) in March 2007. The only bumblebee species found in both locations were B. atratus and B. bellicosus, which were identified using the available literature (Abrahamovich et al 2005).

At the CRS, bumblebee visits to flowers were recorded in a 2 ha mixed meadow of red clover and bird's foot trefoil. Foraging activity of bumblebees was observed when both plant species were in full bloom. A transect was designed so as to allow covering the entire meadow surface without passing the same zone more than once. It was walked several times during the morning and afternoon of two days, totalizing 640 min of record. During the walk, the species of each bumblebee observed and the plant species in which it was foraging was recorded.

At INIA-LE, bumblebee visits were recorded in small contiguous plots of red clover and bird's foot trefoil located in two distant areas of the Station. Area 1 consisted of three plots: one red clover plot of 35 x 20 m, and one bird's foot trefoil plot at each side, one of 20 x 20 m at the ending of blooming and the other of 60 x 30 m which was starting a second bloom. The plots were walked simultaneously in the morning and the afternoon totalizing 135 min of record. In Area 2, foraging bumblebees were observed in a red clover plot of 45 x 25 m and in a bird's foot trefoil plot of 40 x 40 m, which were at approximately 3 km away from the previously described plots. These plots were walked at different times of the day, resulting in 180 min of record.

At the end of the data collection period, foraging bumblebees of both species were captured on bird's foot trefoil and red clover and were maintained at -20ºC. The proboscis of each individual was extended and its length was measured from the prementon to the tip of the glossa using a digital caliper under magnifying lens.

To test whether differences on distribution of B. atratus and B. bellicosus on red clover and bird's foot trefoil differ from the one expected by chance, a Chi square test in a 2 x 2 contingency table (Zar 1997) was used. To determine differences of bumblebee species proboscis length, and intraspecific differences according to the plant species in which they were foraging on, a t-test (α = 0.05) was used.

Results

Both species of bumblebees exploited red clover and bird's foot trefoil in the studied areas, although they did it in different proportions at each of the three instances tested.

In the mixed meadow at the CRS, a total of 400 visits of B. atratus and 49 visits of B. bellicosus were recorded. While B. atratus visited mainly red clover, B. bellicosus visited both resources in similar proportions (χ2 = 66.61, P < 0.001) (Fig 1).


At INIA-LE, 104 visits of B. atratus and 137 visits of B.bellicosus were recorded in Area 1. Both species visited mainly red clover, although the proportion of visits of B. bellicosus to bird's foot trefoil was significantly higher than that of B. atratus (χ2 = 28.06, P < 0001) (Fig 2a). Additionally, interspecific differences in resource utilization remained in the same in Area 2, with most of the visits of B. atratus to red clover, while most of the visits of B. bellicosus were to bird's foot trefoil (χ2 = 24.91; P < 0.001) (Fig 2b).


The proboscis length of B. atratus (8.4 ± 0.65 mm, N = 34) was longer than that of B. bellicosus (7.9 ± 0.79 mm, N = 25), providing significant interspecific differences (t = 2.90; P < 0.01) (Fig 3).


There were no differences in proboscis length between individuals of B. atratus collected on red clover and those collected on bird's foot trefoil (t = 0.86; P = 0.39) (Fig 4a). By contrast, the intraspecific analysis of B. bellicosus in the two legumes did show differences: the proboscis length of foragers caught on red clover was longer than those collected on bird's foot trefoil (t = 2.84; P < 0.01) (Fig 4b).


Discussion

The results showed that B. atratus and B. bellicosus visited red clover and bird's foot trefoil differentially and suggested that those differences could be associated with proboscis length: bumblebees with longer proboscis preferred red clover and those with shorter proboscis used bird's foot trefoil. This could indicate that differences in corolla depth between these plant species were enough to cause the observed pattern of resource utilization. In this scenario, B. atratus (with a longer proboscis) possibly obtained nectar easier and faster from deeper flowers than B. bellicosus, which has a shorter proboscis. The pattern observed in B. bellicosus represented an intraspecific resource partitioning that could also be explained by differences found in the length of the proboscis. Intraspecific resource partitioning related mainly to differences in proboscis length has been reported in several Bombus species (Inouye 1978, Morse 1978, Johnson 1986, Heinrich 2004). Proboscis length is highly correlated with body size (Morse 1978, Harder 1985, Heinrich 2004), so differences in worker body size within a Bombus species, and even within a colony, allow individuals to specialize in different plant species. This would enable bumblebee colonies to efficiently exploit the diverse range of resources available throughout the life cycle of the colony (Inouye 1978, Heinrich 1979, 2004, Kearns & Thomson 2001).

Several factors would indicate that during the analyzed periods, red clover was a more rewarding resource for bumblebees than bird's foot trefoil. First, red clover was the resource most frequently visited by bumblebees and, in general, the most visited resource in a given area is often the best source of nectar (Heinrich 1979). Red clover inflorescence is formed by numerous flowers so, as long as these contain nectar, a bumblebee can visit several flowers without incurring high costs of time and energy in flight (Heinrich 2004). Finally, due to the type of inflorescence, it is likely that the number of individual flowers of red clover was significantly greater than that of other resources, and the amount of potential reward of a particular plant species is also a function of the total number of available flowers (Heinrich 1979). However, the energetic advantage that red clover could provide would be diminished by the high rate of nectar consumption due to the large number of visits it receives.

A decrease in nectar level in the flowers will affect more severely short proboscis individuals since they will have to struggle to reach the nectar, increasing the time of manipulation and therefore the costs that could outweigh the benefits (Holm 1966, Inouye 1980, Harder 1983, 1985, Graham & Jones 1996). This would lead short proboscis individuals to visit other plant species in which they obtain a greater net energy gain than in those of long corolla, despite the lower daily production of nectar (Heinrich 1979). Thus, smaller B. bellicosus individuals would be displaced from red clover by B. atratus, and even by their larger conspecifics, with a higher foraging efficiency in this plant species.

Several authors agree that exploitative competition is the mechanism responsible for resource partitioning in bumblebees since direct competition is rarely observed (Inouye 1978, Heinrich 1979, 2004, Graham & Jones 1996), although there are a few documented cases (Morse 1978, Plowright & Laverty 1984). In this study, competition intensity between B. atratus and B. bellicosus did not seem to reach such a level to cause absolute resource partitioning. In this sense, Rodríguez-Gironés & Santamaría (2006) using a mathematical model to analyze foraging behavior of bumblebees predicted that when two coexisting bumblebee species differ in their proboscis length, differences in their ability to exploit two flower types that differ in the corolla depth will lead to resource partitioning. In such condition, at least one species would specialize in a single type of flower, while the other (no matter which one) would act as a specialist when nectar competition is intense or as a generalist when competition is weak. In the present study, B. atratus behavior would approach to that of a specialist whereas B. bellicosus behavior would do so to that of a generalist; nevertheless the allocation of these roles was not clear.

Although proboscis length is undoubtedly a very important factor in the selection of resources by bumblebees, it is certainly not the only one. Foraging efficiency of bumblebees and, therefore, the choice of resources are also affected by bumblebee body size (Harder 1983, 1985, Goulson 2003, Heinrich 2004), wing length (Harder 1985), learning capacity (Heinrich 1976a, 2004, Laverty 1994, Raine & Chittka 2007), daily volume of nectar produced per flower (Pyke 1982, Harder 1983, Goulson 2003, Heinrich 2004), communication between members of the colony and the state of the colony in relation to its nutritional requirements (Heinrich 1979, 2004, Goulson 2003), among other characteristics. There are several factors affecting distribution of bumblebees on the different resources available in a given area, making difficult explanations for the observed patterns. In the case studied, differences in the length of the proboscis would explain much of the general pattern of distribution of species on the evaluated resources, but further research on the discussed issues are required.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Pablo Cracco for access and support at the Centro Regional Sur of Facultad de Agronomía and the authorities and staff of La Estanzuela Experimental Station of the Instituto Nacional de Investigación Agropecuaria for permits, accommodation and support. We also thank Alejandro Arbulo for assistance in the field, Gloria Daners for providing her laboratory and Carolina Abud for her collaboration in the translation of the manuscript. Finally we thank two anonymous reviewers for their invaluable comments.

Received 09 October 2009 and accepted 12 May 2010

Edited by Kleber Del Claro - UFU

  • Abrahamovich AH, Díaz NB (2002) Bumble bees of the neotropical region (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Biota Colombiana 3: 199-214.
  • Abrahamovich AH, Díaz NB, Lucía M (2005) Las especies del género Bombus Latreille en Argentina (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Estudio taxonómico y claves para su identificación. Neotrop Entomol 34: 235-250.
  • Abrahamovich AH, Díaz NB, Morrone JJ (2004) Distributional patterns of the neotropical and andean species of the genus Bombus (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Acta Zool Mex Nueva Ser 20: 99-117.
  • Carámbula M (1981) Producción de semillas de plantas forrajeras. Montevideo, Editorial Hemisferio Sur, 518p.
  • Cortopassi-Laurino M, Knoll FRN, Imperatriz-Fonseca VL (2003) Nicho trófico e abundancia de Bombus morio e Bombus atratus em diferentes biomas brasileiros. In Melo GAR, Alves-dos-Santos I (eds) Apoidea Neotropica: homenagem a os 90 anos de Jesus Santiago Moure. Criciúma, Editora UNESC, 320p.
  • Goulson D (2003) Bumblebees: their behaviour and ecology. Oxford, Oxford University Press, 236p.
  • Graham L, Jones K (1996) Resource partitioning and per-flower foraging efficiency in two bumble bee species. Am Midl Nat 136: 401-406.
  • Harder LD (1983) Flower handling efficiency of bumble bees: morphological aspects of probing time. Oecología 57: 274-280.
  • Harder LD (1985) Morphology as a predictor of flower choice by bumble bees. Ecology 66: 198-209.
  • Heinrich B (1976a) The foraging specializations of individual bumblebees. Ecol Monogr 46: 105-128.
  • Heinrich B (1976b) Resource partitioning among some eusocial insects: bumblebees. Ecology 57: 874-889.
  • Heinrich B (1979) "Majoring" and "minoring" by foraging bumblebees, Bombus vagans: an experimental analysis. Ecology 60: 245-255.
  • Heinrich B (2004) Bumblebee economics. 2ş ed., Cambridge, Harvard University Press, 251p.
  • Holm N (1966) The utilization and management of bumble bees for red clover and alfalfa seed production. Annu Rev Entomol 11: 155-186.
  • Inouye DW (1978) Resource partitioning in bumblebees: experimental studies of foraging behavior. Ecology 59: 672-678.
  • Inouye DW (1980) The effect of proboscis and corolla tube lengths on patterns and rates of flower visitation by bumblebees. Oecologia 45: 197-201.
  • Izaguirre P, Beyhaut R (1997) Las leguminosas en Uruguay y regiones vecinas. Parte 1 - Papilionoideae (Faboideae). Montevideo, Editorial Hemisferio Sur, 549p.
  • Johnson RA (1986) Intraspecific resource partitioning in the bumble bees Bombus ternarius and B pennsylvanicus Ecology 67: 133-138.
  • Kearns CA, Thomson JD (2001) The natural history of bumblebees. A sourcebook for investigations. Boulder, University Press Colorado, 130p.
  • Laverty T M (1994) Bumblee bee learning and flower morphology. Anim Behav 47: 531-545.
  • Michener CD (1974) The social behavior of bees. A comparative study. Cambridge, Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, 404p.
  • Michener CD (2007) (2nd ed.) The bees of the world. Baltimore, The Johns Hopkins University Press, 953p.
  • Morse DH (1978) Size-related foraging differences of bumble bee workers. Ecol Entomol 3: 189-192.
  • Plowright RC, Laverty M (1984) The ecology and sociobiology of bumblebees. Annu Rev Entomol 29: 175-199.
  • Pyke GH (1982) Local geographic distributions of bumblebees near Crested Butte, Colorado: competition and community structure. Ecology 63: 555-573.
  • Raine NE, Chittka L (2007) Pollen foraging: learning a complex motor skill by bumblebees (Bombus terrestris). Naturwissenschaften 94: 459-464.
  • Rodríguez-Gironés MA, Santamaría L (2006) Models of optimal foraging and resource partitioning: deep corollas for long tongues. Behav Ecol 17: 905-910.
  • Velthuis HHW (2002) The historical background of the domestication of the bumble-bee, Bombus terrestris, and its intrduction to agriculture, p.177-184. In Kevan P, Imperatriz Fonseca VL (eds) Pollinating bees - the conservation link between agriculture and nature. Ministry of Environment, Brasilia, 319p.
  • Velthuis HHW, van Doorn A (2006) A century of advances in bumblebee domestication and the economic and environmental aspects of its commercialization for pollination. Apidologie 37: 421-451.
  • Zar JH (1997) (3rd ed) Biostatistical analysis. New Jersey, Prentice Hall, 662p.
  • Correspondence:

    Natalia Arbulo,
    Sección Etología, Facultad de Ciencias,
    Univ de la República, Iguá 4225 Esq. Mataojo, Piso 6 Norte,
    CP 11400, Montevideo, Uruguay;
  • Publication Dates

    • Publication in this collection
      14 Mar 2011
    • Date of issue
      Feb 2011

    History

    • Received
      09 Oct 2009
    • Accepted
      12 May 2010
    Sociedade Entomológica do Brasil Sociedade Entomológica do Brasil, R. Harry Prochet, 55, 86047-040 Londrina PR Brasil, Tel.: (55 43) 3342 3987 - Londrina - PR - Brazil
    E-mail: editor@seb.org.br