Accessibility / Report Error

Phonemic and semantic verbal fluency tasks: normative data for elderly Brazilians

Fluência verbal fonêmica e semntica: dados normativos de idosos Brasileiros

Abstracts

This study aims to investigate the influence of sociodemographic characteristics on the performance of older people on two tasks of verbal fluency and provide normative data for a Brazilian population of healthy elderly individuals with different educational levels. The initial sample included 521 individuals aged from 60 years, participating in the Program Family Health Strategy. Participants who had scores suggestive of cognitive decline on the Mini-Mental State Examination, depressive symptoms in Geriatric Depression Scale and self-reported neurological or psychiatric disorders were excluded. The final sample consisted of 218 participants in phonemic verbal fluency task (letters F, A and S) and 265 participants for semantic verbal fluency task (animals). The performance in both tests was associated with age and education, but not with sex. Still, the education variable was shown to have a greater impact on scores in phonemic and semantic tests than age in both forms of evocation. The results of this study suggest the importance of providing normative data for elderly Brazilians appropriate to age and education on verbal fluency tasks.

Verbal fluency; normative data; elderly


Este estudo buscou investigar a influência de características sociodemográficas no desempenho de idosos em duas tarefas de fluência verbal, além de fornecer dados normativos de referência nessas tarefas, conforme níveis de educação, para a população brasileira de idosos saudáveis​​. A amostra inicial incluiu 521 idosos com idade a partir 60 anos, participantes do Programa Estratégia Saúde na Família. Foram excluídos os participantes que tinham pontuações sugestivas de declínio cognitivo no Mini Exame do Estado Mental, de sintomas depressivos na Escala de Depressão Geriátrica e com autorrelato de doenças neurológicas ou de distúrbios psiquiátricos. A amostra final foi composta por 218 participantes que responderam à tarefa de fluência verbal fonêmica (letras F, A e S) e 265 que responderam à tarefa semântica (animais). O desempenho em ambas as tarefas foi associado à idade e aos anos de ensino formal, mas não ao sexo. Ainda, a variável escolaridade mostrou ter um impacto maior no desempenho de fluência verbal do que a idade em ambas as modalidades de evocação. Os resultados desse estudo sugerem a importância de oferecer dados normativos de idosos brasileiros, adequados à idade e à escolaridade, em tarefas de fluência verbal.

Fluência verbal; dados normativos; idosos


The verbal fluency tasks are frequently used to evaluate cognitive changes, characterize the performance profiles in healthy aging and in neurodegenerative diseases (Di Biasio et al., 2012Di Biasio, F., Vanacore, N., Fasano, A., Modugno, N., Gandolfi, B., Lena, F., ...Ruggieri, S. (2012). Neuropsychology, neuroimaging or motor phenotype in diagnosis of Parkinson's disease-dementia: Which matters most? Journal of Neural Transmission, 119(5), 597-604. doi:10.1007/s00702-011-0733-3
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-011-0733-...
; Martyr et al., 2012Martyr, A., Clare, L., Nelis, S. M., Marková, I. S., Roth, I., Woods, R. T., ...Morris, R. G. (2012). Verbal fluency and awareness of functional deficits in early-stage dementia. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 26(3), 501-519. doi:10.1080/13854046.2012.665482
https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2012.66...
). Fast and easy to administer, these tasks can be managed individually or as being present in some assessment protocols (Dubois, Slachevsky, Litvan, & Pillon, 2000Dubois, B., Slachevsky, A., Litvan, I., & Pillon, B. (2000). The FAB: A Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside. Neurology, 55(11), 1621-1626. doi:10.1212/WNL.55.11.1621
https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.55.11.1621...
; Fillenbaum et al., 2008Fillenbaum, G. G., van Belle, G., Morris, J. C., Mohs, R. C., Mirra, S. S., Davis, P. C., ...Heyman, A. (2008). Consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer's disease (cerad): The first twenty years. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 4(2), 96-109. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2007.08.005
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2007.08.0...
). In general, the patient must evoke the greatest possible number of words in accordance with a predetermined rule. There are three types of verbal fluency tasks: free (no predetermined criterion to evoke words), phonemic (words must start with a certain letter) and semantic (words must correspond to a particular semantic field). Although the purpose of the activities is the same (evoking words), patients may present different performances according to the modality used and the type of stimulus chosen (Beausoleil, Monetta, Leblanc, & Joanette, 2001Beausoleil, N., Fortin, R., Le Blanc, B., & Joanette, Y. (2003). Unconstrained oral naming performance in right and left hemisphere damaged individuals: When education overrides the lesion. Aphasiology, 17(2), 143-158.), since there are less frequent words that begin with specific letter (letter B versus X), as well as more restricted semantic fields (food versus color). In addition, sociodemographic characteristics, such as age and education, have been reported as variables that influence performance in both modalities (Bolla, Gray, Resnick, Galante, & Kawas, 1998Bolla, K. I., Gray, S., Resnick, S. M., Galante, R., & Kawas, C. (1998). Category and letter fluency in highly educated older adults. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 2(3), 330-338.; Brucki & Rocha, 2004Brucki, S. M., & Rocha, M. S. G. (2004). Category fluency test: Effects of age, gender and education on total scores, clustering and switching in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking subjects. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 37(12), 1771-1777. doi:10.1590/S0100-879X2004001200002
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X200400...
; Carnero, Lendínez, Maestre, & Zunzunegui, 1999Carnero, C., Lendínez, A., Maestre, J., & Zunzunegui, M. V. (1999). Fluencia verbal semántica en pacientes neurológicos sin demencia y bajo nivel educativo. Revista de Neurología, 28(9), 858-862.; Kempler, Teng, Dick, Taussig, & Davis, 1998Kempler, D., Teng, E. L., Dick, M., Taussig, I. M., & Davis, D. S. (1998). The effects of age, education, and ethnicity on verbal fluency. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 4(6), 531-538. doi:10.1017/S1355617798466013
https://doi.org/10.1017/S135561779846601...
; Silva, Yassuda, Guimarães, & Florindo, 2011Silva, T. B. L., Yassuda, M. S., Guimarães, V. V., & Florindo, A. A. (2011). Fluência verbal e variáveis sociodemográficas no processo de envelhecimento: Um estudo epidemiológico. Psicologia: Reflexão & Crítica, 24(4), 739-746. doi:10.1590/S0102-79722011000400014
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7972201100...
). Other authors also point to the contribution of gender (Kempler et al., 1998; Silva et al., 2011).

Some studies suggest that the performance in verbal fluency modalities is a result of different cognitive functions that are activated during the task. In phonemic fluency were evaluated primarily production and language fluency skills, while in semantic verbal fluency observed semantic memory skills (Peña-Casanova et al., 2009). However, both forms are associated with executive functions (Schlindwein-Zanini, 2010Schlindwein-Zanini, R. (2010). Demência no idoso: Aspectos neuropsicológicos. Revista de Neurociências, 18, 220-226.), since they require self-regulation, working memory, processing speed and organization (Magila & Caramelli, 2001Magila, C., & Caramelli, P. (2001). Funções executivas no idoso. In O. V. Forlenza & P. Caramelli (Eds.), Neuropsiquiatria Geriátrica (pp. 517-525). São Paulo, SP: Atheneu.). Functional neuroimaging studies highlight an increased activation of the frontal cortex to the evocation of words with phonemic criteria, while the temporal cortex is related to the recall of words with semantic criteria. Moreover, in both modes there is greater involvement of the left hemisphere (Gourovitch et al., 2000Gourovitch, M. L., Kirkby, B. S., Goldberg, T. E., Weinberger, D. R., Gold, J. M., Esposito, G., ...Berman, K. F. (2000). A comparison of rCBF patterns during letter and semantic fluency. Neuropsychology, 14(3), 353-360. doi:10.1037//0894-4105.14.3.353
https://doi.org/10.1037//0894-4105.14.3....
).

During the aging process, many factors can influence the performance of cognitive skills. The verbal abilities (e.g., syntactic production), however, remain preserved with normal aging. Nevertheless, there is a perceived difficulty with advancing age in activities that relate language and memory (Parente, Saboskinsk, Ferreira, & Nespoulous, 1999Parente, M. A. M. P., Saboskinsk, A. P., Ferreira, E., & Nespoulous, J.-L. (1999). Memória e compreensão da linguagem no envelhecimento. Estudos Interdisciplinares sobre o Envelhecimento, 1, 57-76.). Thus verbal fluency tasks are used as identifiers of early cognitive decline (Chaves et al., 2011Chaves, M. L. F., Godinho, C. C., Porto, C. S., Mansur, L., Carthery-Goulart, M. T., Yassuda, M. S., & Beato, R. (2011). Cognitive, functional and behavioral assessment Alzheimer's disease. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 5(3), 153-166.; Silva et al., 2011). Verbal fluency tests are considered primarily as screening tools able to identify early cases of brain deterioration. The verbal fluency appears altered in multiple pathological processes such as Alzheimer's dementia or frontotemporal dementia, in the left or bilateral frontal lesions, and in psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia and depression (Rodrigues, Yamashita, & Chiappetta, 2008Rodrigues, A. B., Yamashita, E. T., & Chiappetta, A. L. M. L. (2008). Teste de fluência verbal no adulto e no idoso: Verificação da aprendizagem verbal. Revista CEFAC, 10(4), 443-451. doi:10.1590/S1516-18462008000400004
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-1846200800...
). Therefore this study aims to investigate the influence of sociodemographic characteristics on the performance of older people on two tasks of verbal fluency (phonemic and semantic) and provide normative data for a Brazilian population of healthy elderly individuals with different educational levels.

Method

Participants

The initial sample included 521 older adults, aged from 60 years, participants of the Family Health Strategy Program (FHSP; Gomes et al., 2013Gomes, I., Nogueira, E. L., Engroff, P., Ely, L. S., Schwanke, C. H. A., De Carli, G. A., & Resende T. L. (2013). The multidimensional study of the elderly in the family health strategy in Porto Alegre, Brazil (EMI-SUS). Pan American Journal of Aging Research, 1(1), 20-24. ) in Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil. The FHSP randomly selected elderly residents of East/Northeast and Parthenon/Lomba do Pinheiro MD (evaluated by neurologists, psychiatrists, psychologists and nutritional experts). We excluded participants who had scores suggestive of cognitive decline in Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) according to the cutoff points for education, scores ≥ 7 at the Geriatric Depression Scale of 15 points (GDS-15), and self-reported diseases neurological or psychiatric disorders. In the sample for the FAS verbal fluency, illiterate participants were not included. The final sample consisted of 218 participants in phonemic verbal fluency task and 265 participants for semantic verbal fluency task. Other sociodemographic informations of the participants are shown in Table 1.

Table 1.
Description of Sociodemographic and Clinical Data of the Participants

Procedures and Instruments

After signing the informed consent form (research protocol approved - 10/04967), participants were evaluated in a session of approximately 90 minutes duration. The older adults completed the sociodemographic and health questionnaire, MMSE, GDS-15, and two tasks of verbal fluency: phonemic modality and semantic modality.

In phonemic modality used the FAS verbal fluency task (Benton & Hamsher, 1976Benton, A. L., & Hamsher, K. S. (1976). Multilingual Aphasia Examination. Iowa City, IA: University of Iowa.) which consists in evoking words that begin with the letters "F", "A" and "S", for a total of three minutes, one minute for each letter. The final score refers to the sum of correct words spoken in each letter (Strauss, Sherman, & Spreen, 2006Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, norms and commentary. New York: Oxford University Press.). In semantic modality used the Animals verbal fluency task, which refers to evoke the greatest possible number of animals within 60 seconds. The total score refers to the number of animals cited correctly (Strauss et al., 2006).

The evaluations were conducted by students and professionals in the health field such as medicine, psychology and speech therapy, trained for administration tasks. Corrections of tests were performed by a professional who did not participate in the data collection stage.

Data Analysis

Data were analyzed descriptively and inferentially. We used a multiple linear regression analysis with stepwise variable selection to assess the participation of the variables age, education and gender on performance in both verbal fluency tasks. The dependent variables presented a normal distribution in the Kolmogorov-Smirnov Test (p ≥ .05). To compare the performance between age groups and gender on verbal fluency tasks, FAS and Animals, we used the Student t Test for independent samples. To compare the schooling groups in semantic verbal fluency task we use the One Way ANOVA analysis with Scheffe post hoc test. This same procedure was used to analyze the performance of different age groups distributed across schooling groups. Results were considered significant when p ≤ .05. The statistical package used was the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) version 17.

Results

Table 1 presents the mean age, education, MMSE and GDS-15 scores of participant samples of verbal fluency tasks. Regarding the distribution of gender, 139 (64%) were women and 79 (36%) were men among the participants who answered the FAS task. In the Animals task distribution was 164 (62%) and 101 (38%), respectively.

Table 2 shows stepwise multiple regression analysis. According to the model investigated, the variables age and education demonstrate a significant contribution to performance in both verbal fluency tasks, and the number of years studied got greater influence than the age variable. However, the gender variable showed no significant influence.

Table 2.
Multiple Linear Regression Analysis for the Variables Education, Age and Gender on Verbal Fluency Tasks

Comparisons between age groups, education and gender are presented in Table 3. According to analyzes for the FAS task, the elderly group aged 60-69 had significantly higher performance at age ≥ 70. The group with more years of schooling achieved better results than those participants who studied only between 1 and 4 years of formal education. No significant statistical differences were observed in relation to gender.

Table 3.
Comparison of Performance on Verbal Fluency Tasks according to Age, Education and Gender Groups

In the Animals task, there were no significant differences between age groups and gender. However, the illiterate participants demonstrated significantly lower scores than participants who had between 1-4 (p ≤ .001) and 5-8 (p ≤ .001) years of formal education.

Finally, in Table 4 sets out the normative data according to age groups and distributed as years of formal education. According to the ANOVA analysis significant differences are observed when comparing the groups on both tasks verbal fluency.

Table 4.
Normative Data according to Age Group and Distributed by Schooling

Discussion

The objective of this study was to investigate the effect of varying age, years of education and gender on performance on two tasks of verbal fluency (FAS and Animals) in Brazilian elderly population neurologically preserved. From these data, we intended to present normative data adjusted for variables that were influential in the scores of these tasks.

In this study, the performance in both tests was associated with age and education, but not with sex. Still, the education variable was shown to have a greater impact on scores in phonemic and semantic tests than age, suggesting that the number of years of education predicts better results, like the lower the age. These results are similar to other studies that did not find association between gender and the number of words generated, but the influence of age and education (Amaral-Carvalho & Caramelli, 2012Amaral-Carvalho, V., & Caramelli, P. (2012). Normative data for healthy middle-aged and elderly performance on the Addenbrooke Cognitive Examination-Revised. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 25(2), 72-76. doi:10.1097/WNN.0b013e318259594b
https://doi.org/10.1097/WNN.0b013e318259...
; Munro et al., 2012Munro, C. A., Winicki, J. M., Schretlen, D. J., Gower, E. W., Turano, K. A., Muñoz, B., ...West, S. K. (2012). Sex differences in cognition in healthy elderly individuals. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 19(6), 759-68. doi:10.1080/13825585.2012.690366
https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2012.69...
; Ryu et al., 2012Ryu, S. H., Kim, K. W., Kim, S., Jeong, H. G., Kim, J. L., Moon, S. W., ...Cho, M. J. (2012). Normative study of the category fluency test (CFT) from nationwide data on community-dwelling elderly in Korea. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 54(2), 305-309. doi:10.1016/j.archger.2011.05.010
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2011.0...
; Snitz et al., 2009Snitz, B. E., Unverzagt, F. W., Chang, C. C., Bilt, J. V., Gao, S., Saxton, J., ...Ganguli, M. (2009). Effects of age, gender, education and race on two tests of language ability in community-based older adults. International Psychogeriatrics, 21(6), 1051-1062. doi:10.1017/S1041610209990214
https://doi.org/10.1017/S104161020999021...
; Venegas & Mansur, 2011Venegas, M. J., & Mansur, L. L. (2011). Verbal fluency: Effect of time on item generation. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 5(2), 104-107.).

The influence of education in the maintenance of cognitive abilities has received increasing attention (Meijer, van Boxtel, Van Gerven, van Hooren, & Jolles, 2009Meijer, W. A., van Boxtel, M. P., Van Gerven, P. W., van Hooren, S. A., & Jolles, J. (2009). Interaction effects of education and health status on cognitive change: A 6-year follow-up of the Maastricht Aging Study. Aging & Mental Health, 13(4), 521-529. doi:10.1080/13607860902860821
https://doi.org/10.1080/1360786090286082...
; Santos et al., 2012Santos, N. C., Costa, P. S., Cunha, P., Cotter, J., Sampaio, A., Zihl, J., ...Sousa, N. (2012). Mood is a key determinant of cognitive performance in community-dwelling older adults: A cross-sectional analysis. Age (Dordrecht). Advance online publication. doi:10.1007/s11357-012-9482-y
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11357-012-9482-...
; Strout & Howard, 2012Strout, K. A., & Howard, E. P. (2012). The six dimensions of wellness and cognition in aging adults. Journal of Holist Nursing, 30(3), 195-204.). This is due to the fact that the number of years of study could be a factor of cognitive reserve (Stern, 2009Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia, 47(10), 2015-2028. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsycholog...
, 2012). According to the hypothesis of cognitive reserve activities such as reading, writing, learning languages, physical activity and recreation, for example, help to decrease the decline of cognitive functions. Thus, this could explain how some patients with sequel brain would show good results (Beausoleil, Fortin, Le Blanc, & Joanette, 2003).

However, Zahodne et al. (2011)Zahodne, L. B., Glymour, M. M., Sparks, C., Bontempo, D., Dixon, R. A., MacDonald, S. W., & Manly, J. J. (2011). Education does not slow cognitive decline with aging: 12-year evidence from the victoria longitudinal study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(6), 1039-1046. doi:10.1017/S1355617711001044
https://doi.org/10.1017/S135561771100104...
, through a longitudinal study, found that number of years of education influence performance on cognitive tasks, especially verbal fluency, but not related to the changes over the years in any cognitive domain (verbal processing speed, working memory, verbal fluency and verbal episodic memory). That is, the years of study are related to performance, but not with cognitive decline. Thus, individuals with higher education levels continue to show better cognitive performance than people with the same age and with less education, but decline at similar rates.

Regarding age, there is controversy between the results that combine the best results in cognitive abilities. Studies show that age is not a predictor of performance on tests of verbal fluency (Brucki & Rocha, 2004; Venegas & Mansur, 2011). In contrast, Amaral-Carvalho and Caramelli (2012) found an association between age and performance of verbal fluency tasks, as well as the results of other studies (Ryu et al., 2012; Silva et al., 2011; Snitz et al., 2009) corroborate the idea that the greater the age of the person, the smaller the words production of animals.

The difference in the effect of age on verbal fluency tasks may have occurred because the categorical and semantics modalities activate different brain regions (Ryu et al., 2012). Robinson, Shallice, Bozzali and Cipolotti (2012)Robinson, G., Shallice, T., Bozzali, M., & Cipolotti, L. (2012). The differing roles of the frontal cortex in fluency tests. Brain, 135(Pt. 7), 2202-2214. doi:10.1093/brain/aws142
https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/aws142...
investigated how the frontal cortex behaves in different fluency tasks (verbal, drawings, gestures, for example) with neurological patients (tumor and stroke). In verbal fluency tasks observed that both types of verbal fluency activates the frontal lobe, although the phonemic task was more specific in differentiating patients with frontal lesions of posterior lesions. This may have occurred because of the phonological modality requiring the creation of search strategies of words from a letter, making this task unusual and less automated search, resulting in increased activation of the frontal lobe (Rodrigues et al., 2008).

The results of this study demonstrate its importance in that it presents normative data on two tasks widely used in clinical practice and research to the cognitive assessment of older adults. Moreover, the performance standards that have been presented are appropriate for comparing clinical populations, since they were not included elderly with cognitive decline and depressive symptoms in the sample. A limitation of this study can highlight the lack of seniors with more years of schooling and the division of the sample into smaller age ranges.

References

  • Amaral-Carvalho, V., & Caramelli, P. (2012). Normative data for healthy middle-aged and elderly performance on the Addenbrooke Cognitive Examination-Revised. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 25(2), 72-76. doi:10.1097/WNN.0b013e318259594b
    » https://doi.org/10.1097/WNN.0b013e318259594b
  • Beausoleil, N., Fortin, R., Le Blanc, B., & Joanette, Y. (2003). Unconstrained oral naming performance in right and left hemisphere damaged individuals: When education overrides the lesion. Aphasiology, 17(2), 143-158.
  • Beausoleil, N., Monetta, L., Leblanc, B., & Joanette, Y. (2001). Unconstrained oral naming abilities of right-hemisphere damaged individuals: A qualitative and time-course analysis. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 15(1-2), 73-77. doi:10.3109/02699200109167634
    » https://doi.org/10.3109/02699200109167634
  • Benton, A. L., & Hamsher, K. S. (1976). Multilingual Aphasia Examination. Iowa City, IA: University of Iowa.
  • Bolla, K. I., Gray, S., Resnick, S. M., Galante, R., & Kawas, C. (1998). Category and letter fluency in highly educated older adults. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 2(3), 330-338.
  • Brucki, S. M., & Rocha, M. S. G. (2004). Category fluency test: Effects of age, gender and education on total scores, clustering and switching in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking subjects. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 37(12), 1771-1777. doi:10.1590/S0100-879X2004001200002
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001200002
  • Carnero, C., Lendínez, A., Maestre, J., & Zunzunegui, M. V. (1999). Fluencia verbal semántica en pacientes neurológicos sin demencia y bajo nivel educativo. Revista de Neurología, 28(9), 858-862.
  • Chaves, M. L. F., Godinho, C. C., Porto, C. S., Mansur, L., Carthery-Goulart, M. T., Yassuda, M. S., & Beato, R. (2011). Cognitive, functional and behavioral assessment Alzheimer's disease. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 5(3), 153-166.
  • Di Biasio, F., Vanacore, N., Fasano, A., Modugno, N., Gandolfi, B., Lena, F., ...Ruggieri, S. (2012). Neuropsychology, neuroimaging or motor phenotype in diagnosis of Parkinson's disease-dementia: Which matters most? Journal of Neural Transmission, 119(5), 597-604. doi:10.1007/s00702-011-0733-3
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-011-0733-3
  • Dubois, B., Slachevsky, A., Litvan, I., & Pillon, B. (2000). The FAB: A Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside. Neurology, 55(11), 1621-1626. doi:10.1212/WNL.55.11.1621
    » https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.55.11.1621
  • Fillenbaum, G. G., van Belle, G., Morris, J. C., Mohs, R. C., Mirra, S. S., Davis, P. C., ...Heyman, A. (2008). Consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer's disease (cerad): The first twenty years. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 4(2), 96-109. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2007.08.005
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2007.08.005
  • Gomes, I., Nogueira, E. L., Engroff, P., Ely, L. S., Schwanke, C. H. A., De Carli, G. A., & Resende T. L. (2013). The multidimensional study of the elderly in the family health strategy in Porto Alegre, Brazil (EMI-SUS). Pan American Journal of Aging Research, 1(1), 20-24.
  • Gourovitch, M. L., Kirkby, B. S., Goldberg, T. E., Weinberger, D. R., Gold, J. M., Esposito, G., ...Berman, K. F. (2000). A comparison of rCBF patterns during letter and semantic fluency. Neuropsychology, 14(3), 353-360. doi:10.1037//0894-4105.14.3.353
    » https://doi.org/10.1037//0894-4105.14.3.353
  • Kempler, D., Teng, E. L., Dick, M., Taussig, I. M., & Davis, D. S. (1998). The effects of age, education, and ethnicity on verbal fluency. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 4(6), 531-538. doi:10.1017/S1355617798466013
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617798466013
  • Magila, C., & Caramelli, P. (2001). Funções executivas no idoso. In O. V. Forlenza & P. Caramelli (Eds.), Neuropsiquiatria Geriátrica (pp. 517-525). São Paulo, SP: Atheneu.
  • Martyr, A., Clare, L., Nelis, S. M., Marková, I. S., Roth, I., Woods, R. T., ...Morris, R. G. (2012). Verbal fluency and awareness of functional deficits in early-stage dementia. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 26(3), 501-519. doi:10.1080/13854046.2012.665482
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2012.665482
  • Meijer, W. A., van Boxtel, M. P., Van Gerven, P. W., van Hooren, S. A., & Jolles, J. (2009). Interaction effects of education and health status on cognitive change: A 6-year follow-up of the Maastricht Aging Study. Aging & Mental Health, 13(4), 521-529. doi:10.1080/13607860902860821
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/13607860902860821
  • Munro, C. A., Winicki, J. M., Schretlen, D. J., Gower, E. W., Turano, K. A., Muñoz, B., ...West, S. K. (2012). Sex differences in cognition in healthy elderly individuals. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 19(6), 759-68. doi:10.1080/13825585.2012.690366
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2012.690366
  • Parente, M. A. M. P., Saboskinsk, A. P., Ferreira, E., & Nespoulous, J.-L. (1999). Memória e compreensão da linguagem no envelhecimento. Estudos Interdisciplinares sobre o Envelhecimento, 1, 57-76.
  • Peña-Casanova, J., Quiñones-Ubeda, S., Gramunt-Fombuena, N., Quintana-Aparicio, M., Aguilar, M., Badenes, D. , ...NEURONORMA Study Team. (2009). Spanish Multicenter Normative Studies (NEURONORMA Project): Norms for verbal fluency tests. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24(4), 395-411. doi:10.1093/arclin/acp042
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acp042
  • Robinson, G., Shallice, T., Bozzali, M., & Cipolotti, L. (2012). The differing roles of the frontal cortex in fluency tests. Brain, 135(Pt. 7), 2202-2214. doi:10.1093/brain/aws142
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/aws142
  • Rodrigues, A. B., Yamashita, E. T., & Chiappetta, A. L. M. L. (2008). Teste de fluência verbal no adulto e no idoso: Verificação da aprendizagem verbal. Revista CEFAC, 10(4), 443-451. doi:10.1590/S1516-18462008000400004
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462008000400004
  • Ryu, S. H., Kim, K. W., Kim, S., Jeong, H. G., Kim, J. L., Moon, S. W., ...Cho, M. J. (2012). Normative study of the category fluency test (CFT) from nationwide data on community-dwelling elderly in Korea. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 54(2), 305-309. doi:10.1016/j.archger.2011.05.010
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2011.05.010
  • Santos, N. C., Costa, P. S., Cunha, P., Cotter, J., Sampaio, A., Zihl, J., ...Sousa, N. (2012). Mood is a key determinant of cognitive performance in community-dwelling older adults: A cross-sectional analysis. Age (Dordrecht). Advance online publication. doi:10.1007/s11357-012-9482-y
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s11357-012-9482-y
  • Schlindwein-Zanini, R. (2010). Demência no idoso: Aspectos neuropsicológicos. Revista de Neurociências, 18, 220-226.
  • Silva, T. B. L., Yassuda, M. S., Guimarães, V. V., & Florindo, A. A. (2011). Fluência verbal e variáveis sociodemográficas no processo de envelhecimento: Um estudo epidemiológico. Psicologia: Reflexão & Crítica, 24(4), 739-746. doi:10.1590/S0102-79722011000400014
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722011000400014
  • Snitz, B. E., Unverzagt, F. W., Chang, C. C., Bilt, J. V., Gao, S., Saxton, J., ...Ganguli, M. (2009). Effects of age, gender, education and race on two tests of language ability in community-based older adults. International Psychogeriatrics, 21(6), 1051-1062. doi:10.1017/S1041610209990214
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610209990214
  • Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia, 47(10), 2015-2028. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia
  • Stern, Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurology, 11(11), 1006-1012. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6
  • Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, norms and commentary. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Strout, K. A., & Howard, E. P. (2012). The six dimensions of wellness and cognition in aging adults. Journal of Holist Nursing, 30(3), 195-204.
  • Venegas, M. J., & Mansur, L. L. (2011). Verbal fluency: Effect of time on item generation. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 5(2), 104-107.
  • Zahodne, L. B., Glymour, M. M., Sparks, C., Bontempo, D., Dixon, R. A., MacDonald, S. W., & Manly, J. J. (2011). Education does not slow cognitive decline with aging: 12-year evidence from the victoria longitudinal study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(6), 1039-1046. doi:10.1017/S1355617711001044
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711001044
  • The authors acknowledge the support of National Counsel of Technological and Scientific Development (CNPq) and Coordination for the Improvement of Higher Education Personnel (CAPES).

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Apr-Jun 2015

History

  • Received
    17 Apr 2013
  • Reviewed
    11 Mar 2014
  • Accepted
    21 Mar 2014
Curso de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia da Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul Rua Ramiro Barcelos, 2600 - sala 110, 90035-003 Porto Alegre RS - Brazil, Tel.: +55 51 3308-5691 - Porto Alegre - RS - Brazil
E-mail: prc@springeropen.com