Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

The relationship between morphological awareness and word reading in Brazilian Portuguese: a longitudinal study

Abstract

Studies have provided evidence that morphological awareness contributes to word reading in opaque languages such as English. However, this relationship is not yet established for Brazilian Portuguese, a much less opaque language. The present study performed a longitudinal investigation of the relationship between morphological awareness and word reading in 162 children from 2nd to 5th grades of elementary school. The children were evaluated in the final quarter of the respective school year (time 1) and 1 year later (time 2). Hierarchical regression analyses controlling for intelligence, phonological awareness, and phonological working memory were conducted. The hierarchical regression analyses were followed up by cross-lagged panel correlations, and both results converged to word reading measured in the 2nd and 3rd grades contributing to morphological awareness 1 year later. In addition, morphological awareness measured in the 4th grade contributed to word reading 1 year later. The greater transparency of Brazilian Portuguese may make morphological awareness less important for word reading in the early grades; however, in older children, morphological awareness is important for word reading performance.

Keywords
Morphological awareness; Reading; Phonological working memory; Phonological awareness

Alphabetical linguistic systems exhibit a strong relationship between letters and speech sounds. Thus, several studies have shown that phonological awareness, the ability to manipulate and reflect on speech sounds, has a positive and significant relationship with the ability to read and write (see Melby-Lervåg et al., 2012Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S. A. H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 322-352. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026744.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026744...
for a review). However, reading is more than converting letters into sounds, and for comprehension to occur, phonological forms need to be mapped onto semantic information (Kuo & Anderson, 2006Kuo, L., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41, 161-180. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_3.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_...
). Morphemes are defined as the smallest units of meaning, and a great deal of words can be analyzed into these smaller meaningful parts such as prefixes, roots, and suffixes (Nagy et al., 2014Nagy, W. W., Carlisle, J. F., & Goodwin, A. P. (2014). Morphological knowledge and literacy acquisition. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(1), 3-12. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967...
). Thus, morphological awareness, the ability to reflect on and to manipulate the morphemic structure of words (Carlisle, 1995Carlisle, J. F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In L. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 189-209). Erlbaum.), could play a role in learning to read and write. Several studies in different languages that control for phonological awareness and intelligence show a significant relationship between scores on tasks that measure morphological awareness and scores on tasks that measure reading comprehension (e.g., Carlisle, 1995Carlisle, J. F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In L. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 189-209). Erlbaum.; Deacon et al., 2018Deacon, S. H., Holliman, A. J., Dobson, G. J., & Harrison, E. C. J. (2018). Assessing direct contributions of morphological awareness and prosodic sensitivity to children’s word reading and reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22, 527-534. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2018.1483376.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2018.14...
; Kirby et al., 2012Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. L., Bowers, P. N., Izenberg, L., Wade-Wooley, L., & Parrila, R. (2012). Children’s morphological awareness and reading ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 389-410. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-5.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-...
; Kruk & Bergman, 2013Kruk, R. S., & Bergman, K. (2013). The reciprocal relations between morphological processes and reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 10-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.014.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.0...
; Levesque et al., 2018Levesque, K. C., Kieffer, M. J., & Deacon, S. H. (2018). Inferring meaning from meaningful parts: The contributions of morphological skills to the development of children’s reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 54, 63-80. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.219.
https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.219...
; Lyster et al., 2021Lyster, S. -A. H., Snowling, M. J., Hulme, C., and Lervåg, A. O. (2021) Preschool phonological, morphological and semantic skills explain it all: following reading development through a 9-year period. Journal of Research in Reading, 44, 175-188. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12312.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12312...
; Nagy et al., 2006Nagy, W., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2006). Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98 (1), 134-147. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.134.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.1...
; Oliveira et al., 2020Oliveira, M., Levesque, K. C., Deacon, S. H., & Mota, M. M. P. E. (2020). Evaluating models of how morphological awareness connects to reading comprehension: A study in Portuguese. Journal of Research in Reading, 43(2), 161-179. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12296.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12296...
; Pittas & Nunes, 2014Pittas, E., & Nunes, T. (2014). The relation between morphological awareness and reading and spelling in Greek: A longitudinal study. Reading and Writring: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 27, 1507-1527. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-014-9503-6.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-014-9503-...
; Ruan et al., 2018Ruan, Y., Georgiou, G. K., Song, S., Li, Y., & Shu, H. (2018). Does writing system influence the associations between phonological awareness, morphological awareness, and reading? A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110(2), 180-202. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000216.
https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000216...
; To et al., 2016To, N. L., Tighe, E. L., & Binder, K. S. (2016). Investigating morphological awareness and the processing of transparent and opaque words in adults with low literacy skills and in skilled readers. Journal of Research in Reading, 39(2), 171-188. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12036.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12036...
).

There are also studies about the relationship between morphological awareness and single word reading; however, the results of these studies are mixed, with morphological awareness contributing to word reading in some studies (e.g., Deacon et al., 2013Deacon, S. H., Benere, J., & Pasquerella, A. (2013). Reciprocal relationship: Children’s morphological awareness and their reading accuracy across grade 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 49 (6), 1113-1126. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474...
; Diamanti et al., 2017Diamanti, V., Mouzaki, A., Ralli, A., Antoniou, F., Papaioannou, S., & Protopapas, A. (2017). Preschool phonological and morphological awareness as longitudinal predictors of early reading and spelling development in Greek. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2039. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02039.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02039...
; Kruk & Bergman, 2013Kruk, R. S., & Bergman, K. (2013). The reciprocal relations between morphological processes and reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 10-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.014.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.0...
; Muroya et al., 2017Muroya, N., Inoue, T., Hosokawa, M., Georgiou, G., Maekawa, H., & Parrila, R. (2017) The Role of Morphological Awareness in Word Reading Skills in Japanese: A Within-Language Cross-Orthographic Perspective, Scientific Studies of Reading, 21:6, 449-462, https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.1323906.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.13...
) but not in others (e.g., Colé et al., 2018Colé, P., Cavalli, E., Duncan, L. G., Theurel, A., Gentaz, E., Sprenger-Charolles, L., & El-Ahmadi, A. (2018). What is the influence of morphological knowledge in the early stages of reading acquisition among low SES children? A graphical modeling approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 547. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00547.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00547...
; Deacon & Kirby, 2004Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25 (2), 223-238. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716404001110.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271640400111...
; Law & Ghesquière, 2017Law, J. M., & Ghesquière, P. (2017). Early development and predictors of morphological awareness: Disentangling the impact of decoding skills and phonologial awareness. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 67, 47-59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2017.05.003.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2017.05.0...
; Manolitsis et al., 2017Manolitsis, G., Grigorakis, I., Georgiou, G. (2017). The longitudinal contribution of early morphological awareness skills to reading fluency and comprehension in Greek. Frontiers in Psychology, 8 (1793). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01793.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01793...
; Simpson et al., 2020Simpson, I. C., Moreno-Pérez, F. J., Rodríguez-Ortiz, I. d. l. R., Valdés-Coronel, M., & Saldaña, D. (2020). The effects of morphological and syntactic knowledge on reading comprehension in Spanish speaking children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 33(2), 329-348. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09964-5.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09964...
). For example, in English, Deacon and Kirby (2004)Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25 (2), 223-238. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716404001110.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271640400111...
conducted a 4-year (grades 2 to 5) longitudinal study. In this study, after controlling for intelligence and phonological awareness, morphological awareness did not contributed for single word reading (although it contributed for pseudoword reading in the 4th and 5th grades). On the other hand, Deacon et al. (2013)Deacon, S. H., Benere, J., & Pasquerella, A. (2013). Reciprocal relationship: Children’s morphological awareness and their reading accuracy across grade 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 49 (6), 1113-1126. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474...
observed that morphological awareness assessed in the 2nd grade contributed to a composite measure of reading (words and pseudowords) in the 3rd grade. The same kind of mixed results appears to happen in other languages like Greek. For example, after controlling for vocabulary and phonological awareness, Diamanti et al. (2017)Diamanti, V., Mouzaki, A., Ralli, A., Antoniou, F., Papaioannou, S., & Protopapas, A. (2017). Preschool phonological and morphological awareness as longitudinal predictors of early reading and spelling development in Greek. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2039. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02039.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02039...
have shown that morphological awareness assessed in the kindergarten predicted word reading accuracy in grade 1. On the other hand, in a study that controlled for vocabulary, nonverbal IQ, phonological awareness, and rapid automatized naming, Manolitsis et al. (2017)Manolitsis, G., Grigorakis, I., Georgiou, G. (2017). The longitudinal contribution of early morphological awareness skills to reading fluency and comprehension in Greek. Frontiers in Psychology, 8 (1793). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01793.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01793...
have not detected a contribution of morphological awareness assessed in grade 1 to word reading fluency assessed both in the end of grade 1 and in the second grade.

Complicating matters even further, Desrochers et al. (2018)Desrochers, A., Manolitsis, G., Gaudreau, P., & Georgiou, G. (2018). Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 31, 1695-1719. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9772-y.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9772-...
have demonstrated that language can be a modulating factor on the relationship between morphological awareness and reading. In their study, Desrochers et al. (2018)Desrochers, A., Manolitsis, G., Gaudreau, P., & Georgiou, G. (2018). Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 31, 1695-1719. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9772-y.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9772-...
compared children of different countries learning to read in English, French, or Greek. The three writing systems differ greatly in their degree of consistency in grapheme-phoneme mapping with English being the most inconsistent, followed by French and Greek (representing a system with a very consistent grapheme-phoneme mapping). Desrochers et al. (2018)Desrochers, A., Manolitsis, G., Gaudreau, P., & Georgiou, G. (2018). Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 31, 1695-1719. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9772-y.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9772-...
observed that morphological awareness predicted single word reading in 2nd graders learning to read in English (reading accuracy and fluency) and French (reading fluency), but not in 2nd graders learning to read in Greek. Thus, it seems that in inconsistent writing systems such as English and French, morphological awareness predicts word reading very early. However, it is important to notice that morphological awareness predicted reading comprehension in the three writing systems.

In another recent study, Mousikou et al. (2020)Mousikou, P, Beyersmann, E, Ktori, M, Javourey-Drevet, L., Crepaldi, D., Ziegler, J., Grainger, J., & Schroeder, S. (2020). Orthographic consistency influences morphological processing in reading aloud: Evidence from a crosslinguis- tic study. Developmental Science. 23, e12952. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12952.
https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12952...
suggest that language could modulate morphological processing during reading aloud. In their study, Mousikou et al. compared children learning to read in English, French, German, and Italian. Children from the 3rd grade performed a reading aloud task with words and pseudowords. The critical manipulation was in the pseudowords which belonged to four conditions: stem + suffix, stem + nonsuffix, non-stem + suffix, and non-stem + non-suffix. They observed a main effect of stem and suffix, that is, pseudowords made of real stem and real suffix were named faster. However, they observed interactions between stem and language and between suffix and language. These interactions occurred mainly because the stem and suffix effects were much larger in English than in the other languages, suggesting that developing readers of English use morphology in reading aloud to a greater extent than readers of French, German, and Italian. A similar pattern of results was observed in adult proficient readers in these four languages. Thus, morphological processing appears to be more robust in English than in more consistent orthographies. It is important to notice that the kind of tacit morphological processing measured by Mousikou et al. is different from the strategic and conscious application of morphological knowledge as measured by Desrochers et al. (2018)Desrochers, A., Manolitsis, G., Gaudreau, P., & Georgiou, G. (2018). Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 31, 1695-1719. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9772-y.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9772-...
morphological awareness’ tasks. However, as Nagy et al. (2014)Nagy, W. W., Carlisle, J. F., & Goodwin, A. P. (2014). Morphological knowledge and literacy acquisition. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(1), 3-12. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967...
point out, this limit is not easy to set, and measures of morphological awareness necessarily draw on both, reflecting differences in the awareness of morphological relationships between words and in the tacit morphological processing of words.

The studies by Desrochers et al. (2018)Desrochers, A., Manolitsis, G., Gaudreau, P., & Georgiou, G. (2018). Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 31, 1695-1719. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9772-y.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9772-...
and Mous- ikou et al. (2020) suggest that the results observed in the English language do not necessarily apply in the same way to other languages. This state of affairs highlights the importance of conducting reading research in other languages besides English (Share, 2008Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134(4), 584-615. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.584.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.4....
). Unfortunately, when we consider longitudinal studies designed to investigate the direction of the relationship between morphological awareness and single word reading, the number of studies in other languages besides Chinese and English is limited. In an attempt to reduce this gap in the literature, the present study reports a longitudinal investigation of the relationship between morphological awareness and single word reading in Brazilian Portuguese. A second important question investigated in the present study is whether the contribution of morphological awareness to reading ability changes with development (e.g., Kirby et al., 2012Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. L., Bowers, P. N., Izenberg, L., Wade-Wooley, L., & Parrila, R. (2012). Children’s morphological awareness and reading ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 389-410. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-5.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-...
; Kuo & Anderson, 2006Kuo, L., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41, 161-180. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_3.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_...
). For example, in Kirby et al. (2012)Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. L., Bowers, P. N., Izenberg, L., Wade-Wooley, L., & Parrila, R. (2012). Children’s morphological awareness and reading ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 389-410. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-5.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-...
study, after controlling for IQ and phonological awareness, morphological awareness measured in the 1st grade did not contribute to word reading in the 3rd grade (neither accuracy nor speed). Although when morphological awareness was measured in the 2nd grade, it contributed to word reading accuracy, and when measured in the 3rd grade, it contributed to both word reading accuracy and speed. Thus, it is possible to argue that the relationship between morphological awareness and reading gets stronger as children advance through grades, possibly because the texts they are expected to read contain more morphologically complex words (Kuo & Anderson, 2006Kuo, L., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41, 161-180. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_3.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_...
). To address this question, we followed children from the 2nd, 3rd, 4th, and 5th grades for 1 year.

Since nonverbal intelligence, phonological awareness, and phonological working memory are well-established predictors of reading that share variance with morphological awareness (e.g., Deacon & Kirby, 2004Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25 (2), 223-238. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716404001110.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271640400111...
; Justi & Roazzi, 2012Justi, C. N. G., & Roazzi, A. (2012). A contribuição de variáveis cognitivas para a leitura e a escrita no Português Brasileiro. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 25 (3), 605-614. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722012000300021.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7972201200...
), we included controls for these variables in the present study. In the following, we characterize Brazilian Portuguese and review the literature concerning morphological awareness and reading in this language. After this, we discuss some theoretical accounts on how morphological awareness relates to single word reading and present the rationale for the present study hypotheses.

Brazilian Portuguese

Portuguese is an inflectional Romance language spoken in countries of four continents: Africa, Asia, Europe, and South America (Estivalet & Meunier, 2015Estivalet, G. L., & Meunier, F. (2015). The Brazilian Portuguese Lexicon: an instrument for psycholinguistic research. PLoS ONE, 10 (12), e0144016. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144016.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.014...
). In comparison with the complex syllabic structure of Germanic languages, Portuguese has a simple syllabic structure with a reasonable number of open CV (consonant–vowel) syllables (Seymour, 2005Seymour, P. H. K. (2005) Early reading development in European orthographies. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme. The science of reading: a handbook. Oxford: Blackwell.). On the other hand, considering its grapheme-phoneme mapping, some researches characterize Portuguese as being in an intermediate position between deep orthographies, such as English (which has the less consistent grapheme-phoneme mapping), and shallow orthographies, such as Finnish and Italian, which have very consistent grapheme-phoneme mappings (Lima e Castro, 2010; Ziegler et al., 2010Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D., Csépe, V., Reis, A., Faísca, L., Saine, N., Lyytinen, H., Vaessen, A., & Blomert, L. (2010). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: A cross-language investigation. Psychological Science, 21, 551-559. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610363406.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610363406...
; Seymour et al., 2003Seymour, P., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. (2003). Foundations literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143-174. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712603321661859.
https://doi.org/10.1348/0007126033216618...
). However, since different criteria can be employed, it is not an easy task to classify an orthography as shallow or deep (Schmalz et al., 2015Schmalz, X., Marinus, E., Coltheart, M., & Castles, A. (2015). Getting to the bottom of orthographic depth. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(6), 1614-1629. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0835-2.
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0835-...
). According to Schmalz et al. (2015)Schmalz, X., Marinus, E., Coltheart, M., & Castles, A. (2015). Getting to the bottom of orthographic depth. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(6), 1614-1629. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0835-2.
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0835-...
, one can consider the complexity of the letter-to-sound mapping which can be simple one letter to one phoneme, or complex, involving multiple letters to one phoneme. Considering this dimension, Brazilian Portuguese is relatively simple with a good deal of one letter to one phoneme mappings and a set of simple contextual rules governing the pronunciation of some letters. For example, the letter < s > between vowels is mapped to the phoneme /z/.

Another dimension discussed by Schmalz et al. (2015)Schmalz, X., Marinus, E., Coltheart, M., & Castles, A. (2015). Getting to the bottom of orthographic depth. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(6), 1614-1629. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0835-2.
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0835-...
is the consistency of the grapheme-phoneme mapping, that is, to what extent this mapping is always the same. In Brazilian Portuguese, most words either are completely consistent in their grapheme-phoneme mapping, or they can be read aloud correctly with a few contextual rules (Pinheiro, Lucio & Silva, 2008). According to Pinheiro et al. (2008)Pinheiro, A. M. V., Lúcio, P. S., & Silva, D. M. R. (2008). Avaliação cognitiva de leitura: o efeito de regularidade grafema-fonema e fonema-grafema na leitura em voz alta de palavras isoladas no português do Brasil. Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 10 (2),16-30. Retrieved from http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1516-36872008000200002&lng=pt&tlng=pt.
http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?scr...
, there are three possibilities for inconsistent grapheme-phoneme mapping in Brazilian Portuguese: (1) the grapheme < x > which can represent the phonemes /s/ (e.g., “texto” [TEXT]), /z/ (e.g., “exato” [EXACT]), /ks/ (e.g., “boxe” [BOXING]), and /s/ (e.g., “lixo” [TRASH]); (2) the grapheme < e > which can represent the phonemes /e/ (e.g., “pelo” [FUR]) and /ε;/ (e.g., “meta” [GOAL]); and (3) the grapheme < o > which can represent the phonemes /o/ (e.g., “soco” [PUNCH]) and /s/ (e.g., “foca” [SEAL]). In the case of the graphemes < e > and < o >, the inconsistency occurs only when they are in the stressed syllable of trochaic/paroxytone words, i.e., they are the vowel of stressed syllable followed by an unstressed last syllable.

Another way to index grapheme-phoneme ambiguity is in terms of entropy (Borgwaldt et al., 2005Borgwaldt, S. R., Hellwig, F. M., & De Groot, A. M. B. (2005). Onset entropy matters - Letter-to-phoneme mappings in seven languages. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18 (3), 211-229. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3001-9.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3001-...
). In this approach, if a given grapheme always corresponds to one specific phoneme, the mapping is completely predictable, and the corresponding entropy value is zero; as far as this mapping becomes less predictable, the higher its entropy value will be (Borleffs et al., 2017Borleffs, E., Maassen, B. A. M., Lyytinen, H., & Zwarts, F. (2017). Measuring orthographic transparency and morphological-syllabic complexity in alphabetic orthographies: a narrative review. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 30, 1617-1638. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9741-5.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9741-...
). Borgwaldt et al. (2005)Borgwaldt, S. R., Hellwig, F. M., & De Groot, A. M. B. (2005). Onset entropy matters - Letter-to-phoneme mappings in seven languages. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18 (3), 211-229. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3001-9.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3001-...
calculate word-initial letter-phoneme entropies for seven languages: Dutch, English, French, German, Hungarian, Italian, and Brazilian Portuguese. In their analysis, Brazilian Portuguese was ranked in the middle of the orthographic deep spectrum close to French and German, while English represented the less predictable orthography and Hungarian and Italian the most predictable. Overall, these results are in accordance with other studies (Lima e Castro, 2010Lima, C. F., & Castro, S. L. (2010). Reading strategies in orthographies of intermediate depth are flexible: Modulation of length effects in Portuguese. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22(2), 190-215. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440902750145.
https://doi.org/10.1080/0954144090275014...
; Ziegler et al., 2010Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D., Csépe, V., Reis, A., Faísca, L., Saine, N., Lyytinen, H., Vaessen, A., & Blomert, L. (2010). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: A cross-language investigation. Psychological Science, 21, 551-559. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610363406.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610363406...
; Seymour et al., 2003Seymour, P., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. (2003). Foundations literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143-174. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712603321661859.
https://doi.org/10.1348/0007126033216618...
) classifying Brazilian Portuguese in the middle of the orthographic deep spectrum.

Morphological awareness and reading in Brazilian Portuguese

In Brazil, reading instruction begins in the first grade of elementary school (although in an unsystematic way) with children around 6 to 7 years old, and reading is supposed to be fully automatized only at the end of the 3rd grade. According to Sargiani et al. (2018)Sargiani, R., Ehri, L., & Maluf, M. (2018). Orthographic mapping instruction to facilitate reading and spelling in Brazilian emergent readers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39, 1405-1437. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716418000371.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271641800037...
, literacy instruction in Brazil is mainly based on a meaning-centered approach in which grapheme-phoneme correspondences are only incidentally taught. Considering the relationship between morphological awareness and spelling in Brazilian Portuguese, there is consistent evidence for an independent contribution of morphological awareness to spelling (e.g., Mota, Anibal & Lima, 2008; Mota & Silva, 2007Mota, M., & Silva, K. (2007). Consciência morfológica e desenvolvimento ortográfico: Um estudo exploratório. Psicologia em Pesquisa, 1 (2), 86-92. https://doi.org/10.24879/200700100200432.
https://doi.org/10.24879/200700100200432...
; Justi & Roazzi, 2012Justi, C. N. G., & Roazzi, A. (2012). A contribuição de variáveis cognitivas para a leitura e a escrita no Português Brasileiro. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 25 (3), 605-614. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722012000300021.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7972201200...
). In addition, there is solid evidence for an independent contribution of morphological awareness to reading comprehension, even after controlling for phonological awareness, IQ, or vocabulary (e.g., De Freitas et al., 2018De Freitas, P. V., Mota, M. M. P. E., & Deacon, S. H. (2018). Morphological awareness, word reading, and reading comprehension in Portuguese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39, 507-525. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000479.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271641700047...
; Mota et al., 2012Mota, M., Vieira, M., Bastos, R., Dias, J., Paiva, N., Mansur-Lisboa, S., & Andrade- Silva, D. (2012). Leitura contextual e processamento metalingüístico no português do Brasil: um estudo longitudinal. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 25(1), 114-120. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722012000100014.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7972201200...
). For example, Mota et al. (2012)Mota, M., Vieira, M., Bastos, R., Dias, J., Paiva, N., Mansur-Lisboa, S., & Andrade- Silva, D. (2012). Leitura contextual e processamento metalingüístico no português do Brasil: um estudo longitudinal. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 25(1), 114-120. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722012000100014.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7972201200...
investigated longitudinally the relationship between reading comprehension and morphological awareness in second- and third-grade Brazilian children. In this study, even after controlling for school grade, vocabulary, and phonological awareness, morphological awareness predicted reading comprehension 1 year later.

On the other hand, similarly to studies in other languages (e.g., Colé et al., 2018Colé, P., Cavalli, E., Duncan, L. G., Theurel, A., Gentaz, E., Sprenger-Charolles, L., & El-Ahmadi, A. (2018). What is the influence of morphological knowledge in the early stages of reading acquisition among low SES children? A graphical modeling approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 547. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00547.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00547...
; Deacon & Kirby, 2004Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25 (2), 223-238. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716404001110.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271640400111...
; Deacon et al., 2013Deacon, S. H., Benere, J., & Pasquerella, A. (2013). Reciprocal relationship: Children’s morphological awareness and their reading accuracy across grade 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 49 (6), 1113-1126. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474...
; Diamanti et al., 2017Diamanti, V., Mouzaki, A., Ralli, A., Antoniou, F., Papaioannou, S., & Protopapas, A. (2017). Preschool phonological and morphological awareness as longitudinal predictors of early reading and spelling development in Greek. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2039. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02039.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02039...
; Kruk & Bergman, 2013Kruk, R. S., & Bergman, K. (2013). The reciprocal relations between morphological processes and reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 10-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.014.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.0...
; Law & Ghesquière, 2017Law, J. M., & Ghesquière, P. (2017). Early development and predictors of morphological awareness: Disentangling the impact of decoding skills and phonologial awareness. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 67, 47-59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2017.05.003.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2017.05.0...
; Manolitsis et al., 2017Manolitsis, G., Grigorakis, I., Georgiou, G. (2017). The longitudinal contribution of early morphological awareness skills to reading fluency and comprehension in Greek. Frontiers in Psychology, 8 (1793). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01793.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01793...
; Muroya et al., 2017Muroya, N., Inoue, T., Hosokawa, M., Georgiou, G., Maekawa, H., & Parrila, R. (2017) The Role of Morphological Awareness in Word Reading Skills in Japanese: A Within-Language Cross-Orthographic Perspective, Scientific Studies of Reading, 21:6, 449-462, https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.1323906.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.13...
; Simpson et al., 2020Simpson, I. C., Moreno-Pérez, F. J., Rodríguez-Ortiz, I. d. l. R., Valdés-Coronel, M., & Saldaña, D. (2020). The effects of morphological and syntactic knowledge on reading comprehension in Spanish speaking children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 33(2), 329-348. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09964-5.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09964...
), the relationship between morphological awareness and single word reading in Brazilian Portuguese is not clear. An important issue in this situation is if morphological awareness can contribute to word reading in Brazilian Portuguese after the statistical control of phonological awareness. However, the results of the studies which applied this kind of statistical control are mixed; some studies have found an independent contribution of morphological awareness to word reading (e.g., De Freitas et al., 2018De Freitas, P. V., Mota, M. M. P. E., & Deacon, S. H. (2018). Morphological awareness, word reading, and reading comprehension in Portuguese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39, 507-525. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000479.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271641700047...
; Oliveira & Justi, 2017aOliveira, B. S. F. d., & Justi, F. R. d. R. (2017a). A contribuição da consciência morfológica para a leitura no português brasileiro [The contribution of morphological awareness to reading in Brazilian Portuguese]. Revista Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 19(3), 270-286. https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicologia.v19n3p270-286.
https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicol...
), whereas others have not (e.g., Justi & Roazzi, 2012Justi, C. N. G., & Roazzi, A. (2012). A contribuição de variáveis cognitivas para a leitura e a escrita no Português Brasileiro. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 25 (3), 605-614. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722012000300021.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7972201200...
; Miranda & Mota, 2013Miranda, L., & Mota, M. M. P. (2013). Há uma relação específica entre consciência morfológica e reconhecimento de Palavras? Psico-USF, Bragança Paulista, 18(2), 241-247. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-82712013000200008.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-8271201300...
). In de Freitas et al. (2018De Freitas, P. V., Mota, M. M. P. E., & Deacon, S. H. (2018). Morphological awareness, word reading, and reading comprehension in Portuguese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39, 507-525. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000479.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271641700047...
) study, morphological awareness predicted word reading (accuracy and fluency) and reading comprehension in 4th graders, even after controlling for nonverbal intelligence and phonological awareness. On the other hand, in a cross-sectional study, Oliveira and Justi (2017a)Oliveira, B. S. F. d., & Justi, F. R. d. R. (2017a). A contribuição da consciência morfológica para a leitura no português brasileiro [The contribution of morphological awareness to reading in Brazilian Portuguese]. Revista Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 19(3), 270-286. https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicologia.v19n3p270-286.
https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicol...
did not find a statistically significant effect of morphological awareness for word reading accuracy in 2nd and 3rd graders after controlling for nonverbal intelligence, phonological awareness, and phonological working memory. However, in 4th and 5th graders, morphological awareness had a statistically significant effect on word reading accuracy, even after controlling for nonverbal intelligence, phonological awareness, and phonological working memory.

Together, the results by de Freitas et al. (2018De Freitas, P. V., Mota, M. M. P. E., & Deacon, S. H. (2018). Morphological awareness, word reading, and reading comprehension in Portuguese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39, 507-525. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000479.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271641700047...
) and Oliveira and Justi (2017a)Oliveira, B. S. F. d., & Justi, F. R. d. R. (2017a). A contribuição da consciência morfológica para a leitura no português brasileiro [The contribution of morphological awareness to reading in Brazilian Portuguese]. Revista Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 19(3), 270-286. https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicologia.v19n3p270-286.
https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicol...
suggest that morphological awareness predicts single word reading in Brazilian Portuguese at least in the 4th and 5th grades of elementary school (but see Justi & Roazzi, 2012Justi, C. N. G., & Roazzi, A. (2012). A contribuição de variáveis cognitivas para a leitura e a escrita no Português Brasileiro. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 25 (3), 605-614. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722012000300021.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7972201200...
, for an exception). Since some studies have not found evidence for a relationship between morphological awareness and word reading in 2nd and 3rd grades (e.g., Guimarães & Mota, 2016Guimarães, S. B., & Mota, M. M. P. E. (2016). Qual a contribuição da consciência morfológica das crianças na precisão de leitura de palavras e compreensão de texto no português? Estudos De Psicologia (natal), 21 (3), 239-248. https://doi.org/10.5935/1678-4669.20160023.
https://doi.org/10.5935/1678-4669.201600...
; Miranda & Mota, 2013Miranda, L., & Mota, M. M. P. (2013). Há uma relação específica entre consciência morfológica e reconhecimento de Palavras? Psico-USF, Bragança Paulista, 18(2), 241-247. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-82712013000200008.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-8271201300...
; Oliveira & Justi, 2017aOliveira, B. S. F. d., & Justi, F. R. d. R. (2017a). A contribuição da consciência morfológica para a leitura no português brasileiro [The contribution of morphological awareness to reading in Brazilian Portuguese]. Revista Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 19(3), 270-286. https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicologia.v19n3p270-286.
https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicol...
), it seems that morphological awareness does not predict word reading in Brazilian Portuguese in the early years of elementary school. On the other hand, in a recent study, Oliveira et al. (2020)Oliveira, M., Levesque, K. C., Deacon, S. H., & Mota, M. M. P. E. (2020). Evaluating models of how morphological awareness connects to reading comprehension: A study in Portuguese. Journal of Research in Reading, 43(2), 161-179. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12296.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12296...
employed a path analysis and observed that word reading and listening comprehension fully mediated the relationship between morphological awareness and reading comprehension in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking 2nd graders. More importantly, morphological awareness contributed to word reading even when controlling for phonological awareness and nonverbal intelligence. However, one limitation of Oliveira et al. (2020)Oliveira, M., Levesque, K. C., Deacon, S. H., & Mota, M. M. P. E. (2020). Evaluating models of how morphological awareness connects to reading comprehension: A study in Portuguese. Journal of Research in Reading, 43(2), 161-179. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12296.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12296...
study is its cross-sectional nature which prevents the authors drawing strong conclusions concerning the direction of the relationship between morphological awareness and single word reading.

The direction of the relationship between morphological awareness and word reading is an important theoretical question (e.g., Deacon et al., 2013Deacon, S. H., Benere, J., & Pasquerella, A. (2013). Reciprocal relationship: Children’s morphological awareness and their reading accuracy across grade 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 49 (6), 1113-1126. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474...
; Kruk & Bergman, 2013Kruk, R. S., & Bergman, K. (2013). The reciprocal relations between morphological processes and reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 10-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.014.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.0...
). Thus, a study that follows children over time and allows for verifying how morphological awareness can predict word reading while controlling for other cognitive abilities, mainly phonological awareness, is necessary. To our knowledge, in the case of Brazilian Portuguese, only one longitudinal study has been reported that evaluated morphological awareness and reading comprehension in 2nd and 3rd graders (Mota et al., 2012Mota, M., Vieira, M., Bastos, R., Dias, J., Paiva, N., Mansur-Lisboa, S., & Andrade- Silva, D. (2012). Leitura contextual e processamento metalingüístico no português do Brasil: um estudo longitudinal. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 25(1), 114-120. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722012000100014.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7972201200...
). In other words, no longitudinal study has evaluated the relationship between morphological awareness and single word reading in Brazilian Portuguese.

Another issue needing investigation is whether the relationship between morphological awareness and word reading changes with development (e.g., Kirby et al., 2012Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. L., Bowers, P. N., Izenberg, L., Wade-Wooley, L., & Parrila, R. (2012). Children’s morphological awareness and reading ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 389-410. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-5.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-...
; Kuo & Anderson, 2006Kuo, L., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41, 161-180. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_3.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_...
). This is a relevant theoretical question because there are suggestions that the relationship between morphological awareness and reading gets stronger after the initial years of reading instruction (e.g., Kirby et al., 2012Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. L., Bowers, P. N., Izenberg, L., Wade-Wooley, L., & Parrila, R. (2012). Children’s morphological awareness and reading ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 389-410. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-5.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-...
; Kuo & Anderson, 2006Kuo, L., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41, 161-180. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_3.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_...
). It is important to notice that few studies in Brazilian Portuguese (Guimarães & Mota, 2016Guimarães, S. B., & Mota, M. M. P. E. (2016). Qual a contribuição da consciência morfológica das crianças na precisão de leitura de palavras e compreensão de texto no português? Estudos De Psicologia (natal), 21 (3), 239-248. https://doi.org/10.5935/1678-4669.20160023.
https://doi.org/10.5935/1678-4669.201600...
; Oliveira & Justi, 2017aOliveira, B. S. F. d., & Justi, F. R. d. R. (2017a). A contribuição da consciência morfológica para a leitura no português brasileiro [The contribution of morphological awareness to reading in Brazilian Portuguese]. Revista Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 19(3), 270-286. https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicologia.v19n3p270-286.
https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicol...
) considered a broad age range when investigating morphological awareness and word reading. To address this question, we followed children from the 2nd, 3rd, 4th, and 5th grades for 1 year.

The present study

The present study aims to evaluate longitudinally the relationship between morphological awareness and word reading accuracy in Brazilian children. Thus, children from 2nd to 5th grades were evaluated in the end of the school year and 1 year later. The present study has two main goals: (1) to investigate when morphological awareness contributes to word reading accuracy in Brazilian Portuguese and (2) to investigate longitudinally the direction of this relationship between morphological awareness and word reading in Brazilian Portuguese. Since nonverbal intelligence, phonological awareness, and phonological working memory are well-established predictors of reading that share variance with morphological awareness (e.g., Deacon & Kirby, 2004Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25 (2), 223-238. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716404001110.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271640400111...
; Justi & Roazzi, 2012Justi, C. N. G., & Roazzi, A. (2012). A contribuição de variáveis cognitivas para a leitura e a escrita no Português Brasileiro. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 25 (3), 605-614. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722012000300021.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7972201200...
), we included controls for these variables in the present study.

Theoretical rationale for the present study’s hypotheses

There are different ways by which morphological awareness can contribute to word reading (e.g., Nagy et al, 2014Nagy, W. W., Carlisle, J. F., & Goodwin, A. P. (2014). Morphological knowledge and literacy acquisition. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(1), 3-12. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967...
; Levesque et al., 2021Levesque, K. C., Breadmore, H. L., & Deacon, S. H. (2021) How morphology impacts reading and spelling: advancing the role of morphology in models of literacy development. Journal of Research in Reading, 44, 10-26. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313...
). One possibility is morphological decoding (Levesque et al., 2021Levesque, K. C., Breadmore, H. L., & Deacon, S. H. (2021) How morphology impacts reading and spelling: advancing the role of morphology in models of literacy development. Journal of Research in Reading, 44, 10-26. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313...
), that is, using morphological knowledge to decompose a word form in its constituent morphemes to recognize or read aloud a morphological complex word (Levesque et al., 2021Levesque, K. C., Breadmore, H. L., & Deacon, S. H. (2021) How morphology impacts reading and spelling: advancing the role of morphology in models of literacy development. Journal of Research in Reading, 44, 10-26. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313...
). For example, a person can use his or her morphological knowledge to break a word like “react” in its constituent morphemes and use those units to pronounce the word (Deacon et al., 2018Deacon, S. H., Holliman, A. J., Dobson, G. J., & Harrison, E. C. J. (2018). Assessing direct contributions of morphological awareness and prosodic sensitivity to children’s word reading and reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22, 527-534. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2018.1483376.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2018.14...
). As Deacon et al. (2018)Deacon, S. H., Holliman, A. J., Dobson, G. J., & Harrison, E. C. J. (2018). Assessing direct contributions of morphological awareness and prosodic sensitivity to children’s word reading and reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22, 527-534. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2018.1483376.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2018.14...
point out, the morphemic boundaries in “reading” and “react” determine the different pronunciations of “ea” in these words.

Considering single word reading in Brazilian Portuguese, we do not expect readers to rely on morphological decoding (Levesque et al., 2021Levesque, K. C., Breadmore, H. L., & Deacon, S. H. (2021) How morphology impacts reading and spelling: advancing the role of morphology in models of literacy development. Journal of Research in Reading, 44, 10-26. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313...
) to sound out words because the grapheme to phoneme mapping in this language is relatively consistent. Thus, we do not see too many opportunities for this kind of strategic morphological decoding (Levesque et al., 2021Levesque, K. C., Breadmore, H. L., & Deacon, S. H. (2021) How morphology impacts reading and spelling: advancing the role of morphology in models of literacy development. Journal of Research in Reading, 44, 10-26. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313...
) to help in the disambiguation of the words pronunciation. For example, think about the Brazilian Portuguese words “refazer” /ʁ.fa. zeʁ/ [TO REMAKE] and “refeitório” /ʁe.fei. to.ɔɾio/ [REFECTORY]; the “re” in “refazer” is a morpheme; however, the pronounce of the letters “re” / ʁe/ in “refazer” and “refeitório” is the same.

Another possibility for morphological awareness to contribute for word reading is through chunking. A rich structure of morphological knowledge would contribute to tightening the links between the representations of sounds, spellings, and meanings of words and morphemes, supporting the reading and spelling of words in morphemic chunks (Nagy et al., 2014Nagy, W. W., Carlisle, J. F., & Goodwin, A. P. (2014). Morphological knowledge and literacy acquisition. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(1), 3-12. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967...
). These chunks would also result in higher quality lexical representations (Perfetti & Hart, 2002Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. T. Verhoeven, C. Elbro, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 189-213). John Benjamins Publishing.) boosting meaning retrieval and word recognition. Thus, as de Freitas et al. (2018De Freitas, P. V., Mota, M. M. P. E., & Deacon, S. H. (2018). Morphological awareness, word reading, and reading comprehension in Portuguese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39, 507-525. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000479.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271641700047...
) argue, Brazilian Portuguese morphologically complex words such as ‘infelizmente’ [UNHAPPINESS] may be read more quickly if broken into their morphemes “in”- “feliz”- “mente.” In this situation, a person using phonological decoding would achieve the same pronounce; however, the co-activation of the morphemic chunks would result in an easier and more automatic reading.

According to some reading acquisition theories (e.g., Ehri, 2005Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9 (2), 167-188. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0902_4.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0902...
; Frith, 1985Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In k. E. Patterson, J. C. Marshall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface dyslexia: Neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 301-330). London: Erlbaum.) and visual word recognition models (e.g., Grainger & Beyersman, 2017Grainger, J., & Beyersman, E. (2017). Edge-aligned embedded word activation initiates morpho-orthographic segmentation. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 285-317). Elsevier Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2017.03.009.
https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2017.03.0...
), the use of morphemic chunks during reading is a later achievement. For example, in Ehri’s (2005g) phase theory of reading, it is only in the last phase (the consolidated phase) that grapheme-phoneme connections in the words become consolidated into larger units. According to Ehri (2005)Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9 (2), 167-188. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0902_4.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0902...
, in the consolidated phase, readers retain increasingly more sight words in memory, and, as they become familiar with letter patterns that recur in different words, these letter chunks which include spellings of rimes, syllables, morphemes, and whole words become unitized.

In Grainger and Beyersman’s visual word recognition model (2017), children first establish relationships between whole word orthographic representations and morpho-semantic representations. In this model, it is only with repeated exposure to form meaning relationships that children begin to discover relationships between semantically related morphological units. In addition, affix representations are a later achievement, and they are abstracted away as orthographic units as a consequence of exposure to written words containing the affixes.

As seen above, both in Ehri’s (2005) theory and Grainger and Beyersman’s (2017) model, the use of morphemic chunks during reading would depend on reading experience to abstract away those morphemic chunks in the first place. As a consequence, we do not expect morphological awareness to contribute to word reading in Brazilian Portuguese children in the early years of reading instruction. However, it is important to notice that Ehri’s and Grainger and Beyersman’s theories do not specify age or grade levels for the use of morphemic chunks in their models. Thus, it is not an easy task to establish when it is early or late based only on these models because one phase could start early or late and then extend itself for a longer period depending on reading experience. Considering that in Brazil, the teaching of reading is very unsystematic (Sargiani et al., 2018Sargiani, R., Ehri, L., & Maluf, M. (2018). Orthographic mapping instruction to facilitate reading and spelling in Brazilian emergent readers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39, 1405-1437. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716418000371.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271641800037...
), and that children are expected to be still automatizing reading in the 3rd grade; it seems reasonable to assume that in 2nd and 3rd graders, reading is not entirely automatized. In addition, in a previous cross-sectional study with children from the 2nd to 5th grades, Oliveira and Justi (2017b)Oliveira, B. S. F., & Justi, F. R. R. (2017b). Morphological priming development in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking children. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 30(4). https://doi.org/10.1186/s41155-017-0058-8.
https://doi.org/10.1186/s41155-017-0058-...
demonstrated that, compared to an orthographic priming condition, Brazilian children exhibited morphological priming effects from the 3rd grade onwards. However, these priming effects occurred in more automatic situations (with a 60 ms stimulus onset ssynchrony) only in the 5th graders. Since in Grainger and Beyersman’s model, morphological priming effects are explained by the co-activation of orthographic representations of shared morphemic chunks, and then based on Oliveira and Justi’s results, it seems fair to assume that it is only in the 5th and maybe 4th grade that Brazilian children can use morphemic chunks during reading.

Once children become more familiar with orthographic patterns corresponding to morphemes, a rich structure of morphological knowledge would contribute to tightening the links between the representations of sounds, spellings, and meanings of words and morphemes (Nagy et al., 2014Nagy, W. W., Carlisle, J. F., & Goodwin, A. P. (2014). Morphological knowledge and literacy acquisition. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(1), 3-12. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967...
). Since morphological awareness is also an index of this kind of morphological knowledge (Levesque et al., 2021Levesque, K. C., Breadmore, H. L., & Deacon, S. H. (2021) How morphology impacts reading and spelling: advancing the role of morphology in models of literacy development. Journal of Research in Reading, 44, 10-26. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313...
), we expect a contribution of morphological awareness for word reading after the early years of reading instruction when children are able to use morphemic chunks during reading (from the 4th grade onwards). Furthermore, considering the possibility of a bidirectional relationship between morphological awareness and reading (Deacon et al., 2013Deacon, S. H., Benere, J., & Pasquerella, A. (2013). Reciprocal relationship: Children’s morphological awareness and their reading accuracy across grade 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 49 (6), 1113-1126. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474...
), we expect that in the early grades (2nd and 3rd grades), it is word reading that contributes to morphological awareness. This could happen because the exposition to words and word-affix combinations in written language could increase children’s sensitivity to the morphological markings of the words, facilitating the development of morphological abilities (Deacon et al., 2013Deacon, S. H., Benere, J., & Pasquerella, A. (2013). Reciprocal relationship: Children’s morphological awareness and their reading accuracy across grade 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 49 (6), 1113-1126. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474...
; Kruk & Bergman, 2013Kruk, R. S., & Bergman, K. (2013). The reciprocal relations between morphological processes and reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 10-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.014.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.0...
; Kuo & Anderson, 2006Kuo, L., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41, 161-180. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_3.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_...
).

Method

The present study performed a longitudinal investigation of the relationship between morphological awareness and reading in 162 children from the 2nd to 5th grades of elementary school. The children were evaluated in the final quarter of the respective school year (time 1) and approximately 1 year later (time 2). The first wave of this study (old cohort) was previously published as cross-sectional data (Oliveira & Justi, 2017aOliveira, B. S. F. d., & Justi, F. R. d. R. (2017a). A contribuição da consciência morfológica para a leitura no português brasileiro [The contribution of morphological awareness to reading in Brazilian Portuguese]. Revista Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 19(3), 270-286. https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicologia.v19n3p270-286.
https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicol...
, 2017bOliveira, B. S. F., & Justi, F. R. R. (2017b). Morphological priming development in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking children. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 30(4). https://doi.org/10.1186/s41155-017-0058-8.
https://doi.org/10.1186/s41155-017-0058-...
) and included additional tasks not used in the present study. The present study added a new sample (new cohort) of children from 2nd to 5th grades to increase sample size and included the retest of all participants approximately 1 year later (second wave for the old cohort and first and second waves for the new cohort). Thus, the present paper presents new and original data relating time 1 to time 2 to evaluate the longitudinal relationship between morphological awareness and reading in Brazilian Portuguese.

Participants

The full study sample (counting the old and new cohorts) was of 219 children from the 2nd to 5th grades of elementary school. The present study sample consisted only of the 162 children (80 males, and 82 females) who performed all the tasks in time 1 and time 2. The number of children per grade is as follows: forty-two 2nd graders (mean age = 8 years; SD = 0.23); forty-three 3rd graders (mean age = 9.02 years; SD = 0.40); thirty-eight 4th graders (mean age = 9.93 years; SD = 0.35); and thirty- nine 5th graders (mean age = 10.98 years; SD = 0.38). The main reasons for the loss of participants from time 1 to time 2 were as follows: children who changed schools, children who missed one of the testing sessions, and children who did not want to participate in time 2. All children had Brazilian Portuguese as their native language and were from two private schools in a city of approximately 500,000 inhabitants in southeastern Brazil.

Measures

Morphological awareness

Word analogy task (derivational) (Nunes et al., 1997Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Bindman, M. (1997). Morphological spelling strategies: Developmental stages and processes. Developmental Psychology, 33(4), 637-649. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.4.637.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.4.6...
, adapted by Justi & Roazzi, 2012Justi, C. N. G., & Roazzi, A. (2012). A contribuição de variáveis cognitivas para a leitura e a escrita no Português Brasileiro. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 25 (3), 605-614. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722012000300021.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7972201200...
)

This task is an analogy task with the a:b::c:d format. In this task, first, the experimenter presents a pair of words (a:b) with a morphological relationship (e.g., “ajuda” [HELP] — “ajudante” [HELPER]), and then, a new word (c) is presented to the child (e.g., “jogo” [PLAY]). Using analogical reasoning, the child must respond with a word (d) that completes the second pair (e.g., “jogador” [PLAYER]) presenting the same type of morphological relationship of the first pair. The score on this task equals the number of items answered correctly. It is important to notice that, for a response to be considered correct, it needs to have the same type of morphological relationship of the first pair of words. For example, if the first pair (a:b) was “ajuda”- “ajudante” [HELP-HELPER], and the target word (c) was “jogo” [PLAY], the correct answer (d) is “jogador” [PLAYER]. Thus, if the child merely responds with a semantic-related word which does not present the same type of morphological relationship of the first pair, the answer is considered incorrect (e.g., for the pair “ajuda”- “ajudante” [HELP-HELPER], responding “partida” [MATCH] for the target “jogo” [PLAY] is an incorrect answer). In the present task, we used pairs in which the children could not answer correctly by merely repeating the end sound of the words (e.g., in “ajuda”- “ajudante” and “jogo”- “jogador,” the end sounds are different); thus, to correctly respond in this task, children cannot rely on phonology alone; they need to process the morphological structure of the words. The present task focuses on derivational morphology because only the derivational morphology task presented evidence of validity for discrimination between the 2nd-, 3rd-, and 4th-grade children in the study by Mota et al. (2014)Mota, M., Santos, A., & Guimarães, S. (2014). Evidências de validade e consistência interna de tarefas de analogia gramatical. Estudos De Psicologia (natal), 19 (4), 250-257. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-294X2014000400002.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-294X201400...
with Brazilian children. The present task has four training items, followed by 12 testing items, and the Cronbach’s alpha of this task is 0.71 (calculated based on the present study’s sample).

Word reading

Reading subtest of the Teste de Desempenho Escolar [school performance test] (Stein, 1994Stein, L. (1994). TDE: Teste de Desempenho Escolar: manual para aplicação e interpretação. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo.)

The subtest was applied and scored according to the suggestions of Lucio and Pinheiro (2014), which counts as errors mispronounced words, syllabifications, and selfcorrections. Thus, although it is mainly a reading accuracy test, it takes into account bad fluency by penalizing syllabifications and self-corrections. In this test, children need to read aloud a list of 25 words organized in order of difficulty. The majority of the words in this list are morphologically complex (68%). The score on this task equals the number of correctly read aloud words, and according to Lucio and Pineiro (2014), the Cronbach’s alpha for this task is 0.90.

Phonological awareness

Phoneme elision task Based on the study of Justi and Roazzi (2012)Justi, C. N. G., & Roazzi, A. (2012). A contribuição de variáveis cognitivas para a leitura e a escrita no Português Brasileiro. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 25 (3), 605-614. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722012000300021.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7972201200...
, this task involves the oral presentation of a word (e.g., “saco”) and requires the participant to mentally subtract a particular sound (e.g., /s/) and then say the sound that remains (e.g., /aku/). According to Justi and Roazzi (2012)Justi, C. N. G., & Roazzi, A. (2012). A contribuição de variáveis cognitivas para a leitura e a escrita no Português Brasileiro. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 25 (3), 605-614. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722012000300021.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7972201200...
, the Cronbach’s alpha of this task is 0.80.

Phonological working memory

Digits subtest of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children ([WISC-III], Wechsler, 2002Wechsler, D. (2002). WISC-III: Escala de inteligência Wechsler para crianças. Manual (3. ed., V. L. M. de Figueiredo, adaptação e padronização brasileira). São Paulo, SP: Casa do Psicólogo.)

This subtest was used to evaluate phonological working memory. It consists of two sets of tasks: direct order and reverse order. In the direct order task, the children must repeat a sequence of digits in the same order as that performed by the examiner. In the reverse order task, the children must repeat a sequence of digits in the reverse order as the one performed by the examiner. The subtest was applied and corrected according to the specifications of the manual (Guttman’s split-half reliability of 0.74).

Nonverbal intelligence

Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices test This test consists of figures with missing parts that require completion by selecting a correct choice among five choices. The goal is to determine whether children can indicate the part that properly completes the figure. The test was applied and scored according to the Brazilian author’s manual (Angelini, Alves, Custódio, Duarte & Duarte, 1999) and has a Spearman-Brown split-half reliability of 0.92.

Procedure

Procedures in this study adhere to ethical research policies and were approved by the research ethics committee of the authors’ institution and by the board of the children’s schools. The inclusion criterion was the signing of the informed consent form by the children’s guardians. The exclusion criterion was the presence of uncorrected sensory deficiencies in children, such as blindness or deafness. Oral assent was obtained from each child at every testing session.

The tasks were implemented on days and times agreed upon with the school board and teachers. In time 1 (T1), each child participated individually in two testing sessions of approximately 30 min each. In one session, the Raven’s test was administered, and in the other session, all the other tasks were administered. In time 2 (T2), the children participated only in a session of approximately 30 min, during which the tasks of word reading, morphological awareness, and phonological awareness were administered. All tasks were conducted in a location determined by the respective school board where the research was conducted.

Results

As there was a loss of participants from time 1 to time 2, one can ask whether the participants who left the study (N = 57) performed worse on the word reading test than those who remained in the study (N = 162). Thus, we compared the word reading performance of the participants who left the study (M = 14.12, SD = 6.5), with the word reading performance of the participants who remained in the study (M = 13.06, SD = 6.6). Since there was no difference between the two groups, t (217) = 1.06 and p = 0.29, the loss of participants does not seem to have led to a sampling bias. In all the following analyses, we considered data only from the 162 participants who remained in the study (that is, who performed all the tasks in time 1 and time 2).

Regression analyses

Considering previous research in Brazilian Portuguese (e.g., Coelho & Correa, 2017Coelho, C. L. G., & Correa, J. (2017). Compreensão de leitura: Habilidades cognitivas e tipos de texto. Psico, 48, 40-49. https://doi.org/10.15448/1980-8623.2017.1.23417.
https://doi.org/10.15448/1980-8623.2017....
; de Freitas et al., 2018De Freitas, P. V., Mota, M. M. P. E., & Deacon, S. H. (2018). Morphological awareness, word reading, and reading comprehension in Portuguese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39, 507-525. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000479.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271641700047...
; Guimarães & Mota, 2016Guimarães, S. B., & Mota, M. M. P. E. (2016). Qual a contribuição da consciência morfológica das crianças na precisão de leitura de palavras e compreensão de texto no português? Estudos De Psicologia (natal), 21 (3), 239-248. https://doi.org/10.5935/1678-4669.20160023.
https://doi.org/10.5935/1678-4669.201600...
; Miranda & Mota, 2013Miranda, L., & Mota, M. M. P. (2013). Há uma relação específica entre consciência morfológica e reconhecimento de Palavras? Psico-USF, Bragança Paulista, 18(2), 241-247. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-82712013000200008.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-8271201300...
; Oliveira & Justi, 2017aOliveira, B. S. F. d., & Justi, F. R. d. R. (2017a). A contribuição da consciência morfológica para a leitura no português brasileiro [The contribution of morphological awareness to reading in Brazilian Portuguese]. Revista Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 19(3), 270-286. https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicologia.v19n3p270-286.
https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicol...
, 2017b) and the present study’s hypothesis, in the following analyses, the data were divided into two groups: the first group included data concerning T1 2nd and 3rd graders, and the second group included data concerning T1 4th and 5th graders. We adopted this division because, as previously said, in Brazil, reading is expected to be fully automatized at the end of the 3rd grade, and the automatic use of morphemic chunks during reading by Brazilian children appears to be a latter achievement (Oliveira & Justi, 2017bOliveira, B. S. F., & Justi, F. R. R. (2017b). Morphological priming development in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking children. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 30(4). https://doi.org/10.1186/s41155-017-0058-8.
https://doi.org/10.1186/s41155-017-0058-...
). Once we hypothesized that morphological awareness would not contribute to word reading in the early years of reading instruction, it is reasonable to assume the 2nd and 3rd grades as early years (children are still learning to read) and the 4th and the 5th grades as late years of reading instruction (children are mainly reading to learn). Table 1 presents descriptive statistics for these two groups. Since the skew and kurtosis values for all the variables fell within the acceptable range (Kline, 2005Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). Guilford Press.), normality of distribution was assumed for all variables.

Table 1
Descriptive statistics of reading, morphological awareness, phonological awareness, phonological working memory, and intelligence tasks

To explore the direction of the relationship between morphological awareness and word reading, multiple linear regression analyses were performed as follows.

Word reading and morphological awareness in T2 were alternately used as criteria variables. In all analyses, the Raven, phonological working memory, and phonological awareness tasks were used as controls in step 1. In step 2, the respective autoregressive variable of the criterion variable was considered, and in step 3, the morphological awareness in T1 or word reading in T1 was considered. As pointed out previously, due to the reasonable consistency in the Brazilian Portuguese grapheme-phoneme mapping, we do not expect morphological awareness to predict word reading in the T1 sample of 2nd and 3rd graders (Desrochers et al., 2018Desrochers, A., Manolitsis, G., Gaudreau, P., & Georgiou, G. (2018). Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 31, 1695-1719. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9772-y.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9772-...
; Miranda & Mota, 2013Miranda, L., & Mota, M. M. P. (2013). Há uma relação específica entre consciência morfológica e reconhecimento de Palavras? Psico-USF, Bragança Paulista, 18(2), 241-247. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-82712013000200008.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-8271201300...
; Oliveira & Justi, 2017aOliveira, B. S. F. d., & Justi, F. R. d. R. (2017a). A contribuição da consciência morfológica para a leitura no português brasileiro [The contribution of morphological awareness to reading in Brazilian Portuguese]. Revista Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 19(3), 270-286. https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicologia.v19n3p270-286.
https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicol...
); however, we do expect morphological awareness to predict word reading from the T1 sample of 4th and 5th graders (De Freitas et al., 2018De Freitas, P. V., Mota, M. M. P. E., & Deacon, S. H. (2018). Morphological awareness, word reading, and reading comprehension in Portuguese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39, 507-525. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000479.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271641700047...
; Oliveira & Justi, 2017aOliveira, B. S. F. d., & Justi, F. R. d. R. (2017a). A contribuição da consciência morfológica para a leitura no português brasileiro [The contribution of morphological awareness to reading in Brazilian Portuguese]. Revista Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 19(3), 270-286. https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicologia.v19n3p270-286.
https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicol...
). In addition, considering the possibility of a bidirectional relationship between morphological awareness and reading (Deacon et al., 2013Deacon, S. H., Benere, J., & Pasquerella, A. (2013). Reciprocal relationship: Children’s morphological awareness and their reading accuracy across grade 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 49 (6), 1113-1126. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474...
), we hypothesize that word reading will predict morphological awareness in the T1 sample of 2nd and 3rd graders. Table 2 presents the results of the hierarchical regression analyses.

Table 2
Hierarchical regression analysis

Predicting word reading in time 2

As shown in Table 2, the results indicate that in the T1 sample of 2nd and 3rd graders, morphological awareness in time 1 (T1) does not contribute independently to word reading in time 2 (T2) when nonverbal intelligence, phonological working memory, phonological awareness, and word reading in T1 were controlled (p = 0.49). Considering all predictors in the model (3rd step), only word reading in T1 was important for predicting word reading in T2 (standardized β = 0.71, p < 0.01). In the T1 sample of 4th and 5th graders, after controlling for these same variables, T1 morphological awareness contributes to explain an additional 4% of the variation in word reading in T2 (p = 0.01). Considering all predictors in the model (3rd step), only word reading in T1 (standardized β = 0.53, p < 0.01) and morphological awareness in T1 (standardized β = 0.28, p = 0.01) were important for predicting word reading in T2. The full-standardized β for each variable per step can be observed in Table S2 in the supplementary material. In addition, one of the reviewers asked for an analysis of the whole sample with a morphological awareness × grade interaction term. This analysis is presented in Table S1 in the supplemented material. As expected, the interaction term contributed significantly to predicting word reading in T2, which corroborates our division of the sample in early (2nd and 3rd grades) and later (4th and 5th grades) years of reading instruction.

Predicting morphological awareness in time 2

The analyses of the T1 sample of 2nd and 3rd graders considering morphological awareness in T2 as a criterion variable showed that T1 word reading contributes to explaining an additional 8% of the morphological awareness variation in T2 (p < 0.01) after controlling for nonverbal intelligence, phonological working memory, phonological awareness, and morphological awareness in T1. Considering all predictors in the model (3rd step), nonverbal IQ (standardized β = 0.26, p = 0.01), word reading in T1 (standardized β = 0.32, p < 0.01), and morphological awareness in T1 (standardized β = 0.24, p = 0.01) were important for predicting morphological awareness in T2. In the T1 sample of 4th and 5th graders, after controlling for these same variables, T1 word reading does not contribute to morphological awareness in T2 (p = 0.91). Considering all predictors in the model (3rd step), only morphological awareness in T1 was important for predicting morphological awareness in T2 (standardized β = 0.74, p < 0.01). The full-standardized β for each variable per step can be observed in Table S2 in the supplementary material. In addition, one of the reviewers asked for an analysis of the whole sample with a reading × grade interaction term. This analysis is presented in Table S1 in the supplemented material. As expected, the interaction term contributed significantly to predicting morphological awareness in T2, which corroborates our division of the sample in early (2nd and 3rd grades) and later (4th and 5th grades) years of reading instruction.

Cross-lagged panel correlation analyses

Our hypotheses concerning the relationship between morphological awareness and word reading in early and late years were corroborated. However, to present a more fine-grained analysis of the relationship between morphological awareness and word reading, we decided to follow up the regression analyses with year-by-year cross-lagged panel correlations. Based on the present study hypotheses and previous results, we have reasons to expect that in the T1 sample of 2nd and 3rd graders, it is word reading that influences morphological awareness, and we also have reasons to expect that in the T1 sample of 4th and 5th graders, it is morphological awareness that influences word reading; thus, all the following cross-lagged analyses are one-tailed to increase power. In addition, for every statistically significant difference between the cross-lagged correlations, we also tested for differences between the synchronous correlations, since stationarity is an assumption of the cross-lagged panel correlation (Kenny, 1975Kenny, D. (1975). Cross-lagged panel correlation: A test for spuriousness. Psychological Bulletin, 82(6), 887-903. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.82.6.887.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.82.6.8...
). Table 3 presents the Pearson product-moment correlation between morphological awareness and word reading in T1 and T2 for each school grade.

Table 3
Person correlations between morphological awareness and word reading across grades

As expected for the early grades, in the T1 sample of 2nd graders, the cross-lags show a statistically significant difference (Z = - 1.72; p < 0.05, one-tailed; the synchronous correlations were not statistically different, p > 0.05) which indicates that word reading influences morphological awareness. The direction of the relationship is the same in the T1 sample of 3rd graders, and the cross-lags show a statistically significant difference (Z = - 1.67; p < 0.05, one-tailed); however, the synchronous correlations were statistically different (Z = - 2.13, p < 0.05). The difference in the synchronous correlations can probably be attributed to an increase in the strength of the relationship between word reading and morphological awareness in time 2 (see Table 3).

Considering the T1 sample of 4th graders, as expected, the relationship changes its direction; the cross-lags show a statistically significant difference (Z = 1.98; p < 0.05, one-tailed; and the synchronous correlations were not statistically different, p > 0.05), which indicates that morphological awareness influences word reading. In the T1 sample of 5th graders, the cross-lags did not show a statistically significant difference (Z = - 0.01; p > 0.05).

Discussion

The present study aimed to evaluate the independent contribution of morphological awareness to word reading in Brazilian Portuguese and to investigate the existence of a developmental pattern in this relationship for children from the 2nd to the 5th grades of elementary school. The results of the regression analysis indicate that in the T1 sample of 2nd and 3rd graders, T1 morphological awareness does not contribute independently to word reading 1 year later; however, T1 word reading contributes independently to morphological awareness 1 year later in these same children. On the other hand, considering older children in the present study, we have the opposite pattern, that is, T1 morphological awareness contributes independently to word reading 1 year later, and T1 word reading does not contribute independently to morphological awareness 1 year later in these same children. It is important to notice that the regression analyses were followed up by year-by-year cross-lagged panel correlations. The results of these analyses were virtually the same as those of the regression analysis: word reading influencing morphological awareness in the T1 sample of 2nd and 3rd graders and morphological awareness influencing word reading in the T1 sample of 4th graders. The main difference was in the T1 sample of 5th graders, with a null effect. This lack of statistically significant difference in the 5th grade is probably due to a ceiling effect on morphological awareness measures (mean of 9.62 out of 12 in time 1 and mean of 10.08 in time 2) because the cross-lagged analysis is not appropriate for examining the causal effect of variables that do not change over time (Kenny, 1975Kenny, D. (1975). Cross-lagged panel correlation: A test for spuriousness. Psychological Bulletin, 82(6), 887-903. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.82.6.887.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.82.6.8...
). This ceiling effect did not affect the regression analysis results because the data of T1 4th and 5th graders were aggregated in these analyses.

Most Brazilian Portuguese studies that have investigated the relationship between morphological awareness and word reading in children from the 2nd and 3rd grades suggest that the direction of this relationship is from morphological awareness to word reading (e.g., Guimarães, 2005Guimarães, R. K. G. (2005). Influência da variação linguística e da consciência morfossintática no desempenho em leitura e escrita. Interação Em Psicologia, 9(2), 261-271. https://doi.org/10.5380/psi.v9i2.4795.
https://doi.org/10.5380/psi.v9i2.4795...
; Guimarães & Mota, 2016Guimarães, S. B., & Mota, M. M. P. E. (2016). Qual a contribuição da consciência morfológica das crianças na precisão de leitura de palavras e compreensão de texto no português? Estudos De Psicologia (natal), 21 (3), 239-248. https://doi.org/10.5935/1678-4669.20160023.
https://doi.org/10.5935/1678-4669.201600...
; Mota, et al., 2008aMota, M., Annibal, L., & Lima, S. (2008a). A Morfologia derivacional contribui para a leitura e escrita no português? Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 21 (2), 311-318. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722008a000200017.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722008a0...
, 2008bMota, M., Gontijo, R., Mansur-Lisboa, S., Olive, R., Silva, D., Dias, J., Delgado, N., & Kamisaki, R.. (2008b). Avaliação da consciência da morfologia deriva- cional: fidedignidade e validade. Avaliação Psicológica, 7(2), 151-157. Retrieved from http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1677-04712008b000200006&lng=pt&tlng=pt.
http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?scr...
; Oliveira et al., 2020Oliveira, M., Levesque, K. C., Deacon, S. H., & Mota, M. M. P. E. (2020). Evaluating models of how morphological awareness connects to reading comprehension: A study in Portuguese. Journal of Research in Reading, 43(2), 161-179. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12296.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12296...
). However, these studies employed cross-sectional designs, preventing them from drawing reliable inferences concerning the direction of the relationship between the variables. In addition, as pointed out by Oliveira and Justi (2016)Oliveira, B. S. F. & Justi, F. R. R. (2016). Uma meta-análise da relação entre a consciência morfológica e a leitura e escrita no português brasileiro. In: Pereira-Silva, N. L., Barbosa, A. J. G., Rodrigues, M. C. (Orgs.). Pesquisa em desenvolvimento humano e educação. Curitiba: CRV., few studies that have investigated the relationship between morphological awareness and reading in Brazilian Portuguese have controlled for phonological awareness. Thus, the present study is in better position in this regard because in the regression analysis carried out, even when controlling for nonverbal intelligence, phonological working memory, phonological awareness, and particularly morphological awareness in T1, T1 word reading continued to explain 8% of the morphological awareness variation 1 year later (T2).

One possible explanation for this effect of word reading on morphological awareness in the T1 sample of 2nd and 3rd graders is that reading experience helps children to realize that the spelling of morphologically related words tends to be preserved in Brazilian Portuguese (e.g., “laranja” [ORANGE] and “laranjeira” [ORANGE TREE]). Thus, as they are exposed to a greater number of written words, this enables them to perceive the words’ morphological markings and helps them develop morphological awareness (Deacon et al., 2013Deacon, S. H., Benere, J., & Pasquerella, A. (2013). Reciprocal relationship: Children’s morphological awareness and their reading accuracy across grade 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 49 (6), 1113-1126. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474...
).

In relation to the T1 sample of 4th and 5th graders, in the regression analyses, the contribution of T1 morphological awareness to T2 word reading was independent of phonological awareness, phonological working memory, nonverbal intelligence, and initial word reading ability. The cross-lagged panel correlation analyses corroborated these findings except for T1 5th graders (probably due to a ceiling effect on morphological awareness). This result is in line with cross-sectional studies carried out on 4th- and 5th-grade students in Brazil (e.g., de Freitas et al., 2018De Freitas, P. V., Mota, M. M. P. E., & Deacon, S. H. (2018). Morphological awareness, word reading, and reading comprehension in Portuguese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39, 507-525. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000479.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271641700047...
; Oliveira & Justi, 2017aOliveira, B. S. F. d., & Justi, F. R. d. R. (2017a). A contribuição da consciência morfológica para a leitura no português brasileiro [The contribution of morphological awareness to reading in Brazilian Portuguese]. Revista Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 19(3), 270-286. https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicologia.v19n3p270-286.
https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicol...
) and complements these studies, providing longitudinal data indicating that morphological awareness contributes to word reading in these late grades.

As hypothesized, we expected a contribution of morphological awareness for word reading in later grades because morphological awareness can contribute to tightening the links between the representations of sounds, spellings, and meanings of words and morphemes (Nagy et al., 2014Nagy, W. W., Carlisle, J. F., & Goodwin, A. P. (2014). Morphological knowledge and literacy acquisition. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(1), 3-12. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967...
; Levesque et al., 2021Levesque, K. C., Breadmore, H. L., & Deacon, S. H. (2021) How morphology impacts reading and spelling: advancing the role of morphology in models of literacy development. Journal of Research in Reading, 44, 10-26. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313...
), resulting in a boost to word recognition due to high-quality lexical representations (Perfetti & Hart, 2002Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. T. Verhoeven, C. Elbro, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 189-213). John Benjamins Publishing.). These high-quality lexical representations would result in an easier and more automatic reading with the co-acti- vation of morphemic chunks. Since these “morpho- orthographic chunks” would depend on letter patterns abstracted away with reading experience (e.g., Ehri, 2005Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9 (2), 167-188. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0902_4.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0902...
; Grainger & Beyersmann, 2017Grainger, J., & Beyersman, E. (2017). Edge-aligned embedded word activation initiates morpho-orthographic segmentation. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 285-317). Elsevier Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2017.03.009.
https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2017.03.0...
), it makes sense to think about a later contribution of morphological awareness. After all, the idea of tightening the links between the representations of sounds, spellings, and meanings of words and morphemes (Nagy et al., 2014Nagy, W. W., Carlisle, J. F., & Goodwin, A. P. (2014). Morphological knowledge and literacy acquisition. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(1), 3-12. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967...
; Levesque et al., 2021Levesque, K. C., Breadmore, H. L., & Deacon, S. H. (2021) How morphology impacts reading and spelling: advancing the role of morphology in models of literacy development. Journal of Research in Reading, 44, 10-26. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313...
) presupposes that there are orthographic representations of morphemes which could be mapped to sound and meaning.

One question that can be raised is as follows: if morphological awareness depends on some degree of reading experience (abstraction of morpho-othographic chunks) to start contributing to single word reading, how could this happen earlier in opaque languages like English? First, it is important to point out that there are a reasonable number of studies which have not found a morphological awareness contribution for single word reading in English at least until 4th grade (e.g., Apel et al., 2013Apel, K., Diehm, E., & Apel, L. (2013). Using multiple measures of morphological awareness to assess its relation to reading. Topics in Language Disorders, 33, 42-56. https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0b013e318280f57b.
https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0b013e318280...
; Deacon et al., 2004Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25 (2), 223-238. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716404001110.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271640400111...
; Nagy et al., 2003Nagy, W., Berninger, V., Abbott, R., Vaughan, K., & Vermeulen, K. (2003). Relationship of morphology and other language skills to literacy skills in at risk second grade readers and at-risk fourth grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 730-742. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.730.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.7...
). Thus, it is not completely clear how early morphological awareness contributes for single word reading in English. However, assuming that morphological awareness contributes early for word reading in opaque languages, one possible explanation is that in opaque languages like English, children strategically pay attention to letter clusters that represent morphemes and try to sound them out as units, as a way to solve ambiguities which a letter to sound conversion procedure would produce. This idea is in accordance with psycholinguistic grain size theory which assumes that readers of opaque orthographies are likely to rely on larger grain sizes like morphemes, syllables, and rimes when reading aloud (Goswami & Ziegler, 2006Goswami, U., & Ziegler, J. C. (2006). Fluency, phonology and morphology: a response to the commentaries on becoming literate in different languages. Developmental Science, 9, 451-453. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00511.x.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006...
; Mousikou et al. 2020Mousikou, P, Beyersmann, E, Ktori, M, Javourey-Drevet, L., Crepaldi, D., Ziegler, J., Grainger, J., & Schroeder, S. (2020). Orthographic consistency influences morphological processing in reading aloud: Evidence from a crosslinguis- tic study. Developmental Science. 23, e12952. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12952.
https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12952...
; Ziegler & Goswami, 2005Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulettin, 131, 3-29. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1....
). Thus, it is possible that children reading in opaque orthographies become more sensible to the words’ morphology earlier, resulting in a stronger relationship between morphological awareness and single word reading.

On the other hand, in the case of Brazilian Portuguese, and maybe other more transparent orthographies, a simple grapheme to phoneme strategy would be enough for sounding out the words in the early years. Thus, children would have no need to strategically pay attention to larger letter clusters as a way to disambiguate the words pronounce. As a consequence, morphological awareness would not be so important for single word reading in Brazilian Portuguese in these early years. However, morphological awareness would still be important for the development of high-quality lexical representations (Perfetti & Hart, 2002Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. T. Verhoeven, C. Elbro, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 189-213). John Benjamins Publishing.) after some degree of reading experience.

In short, the present study adds novel and important longitudinal data concerning the relationship between morphological awareness and word reading in Brazilian Portuguese, considering a large span of school grades (2nd to 5th grades). It shows that in the early years of reading instruction (T1 sample of 2nd and 3rd graders), it is word reading that contributes to morphological awareness, and that in the more advanced years (T1 sample of 4th and 5th graders), it is morphological awareness which contributes to single word reading. However, it is crucial to notice that these results do not imply a unidirectional relationship between word reading and morphological awareness. They are entirely compatible with a bidirectional interpretation of that relationship (e.g., Deacon et al., 2013Deacon, S. H., Benere, J., & Pasquerella, A. (2013). Reciprocal relationship: Children’s morphological awareness and their reading accuracy across grade 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 49 (6), 1113-1126. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474...
; Kruk & Bergman, 2013Kruk, R. S., & Bergman, K. (2013). The reciprocal relations between morphological processes and reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 10-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.014.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.0...
; Kuo & Anderson, 2006Kuo, L., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41, 161-180. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_3.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_...
). In this light, it is possible to think that, in the beginning, word reading has a more substantial influence on morphological awareness than morphological awareness has on word reading because experience with written language helps children to realize that the spellings of morphologically related words tend to be preserved, facilitating the perception of the morphological markings of the words and the development of morphological awareness (Deacon et al., 2013Deacon, S. H., Benere, J., & Pasquerella, A. (2013). Reciprocal relationship: Children’s morphological awareness and their reading accuracy across grade 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 49 (6), 1113-1126. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474...
). On the other hand, as reading instruction advances and children abstract away more and more “morpho-orthographic chunks” (Ehri, 2005Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9 (2), 167-188. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0902_4.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0902...
; Grainger & Beyersmann, 2017Grainger, J., & Beyersman, E. (2017). Edge-aligned embedded word activation initiates morpho-orthographic segmentation. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 285-317). Elsevier Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2017.03.009.
https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2017.03.0...
), the influence of morphological awareness on word reading becomes stronger because morphological awareness helps tighten the links between the representations of sounds, spellings, and meanings of words and morphemes (Nagy et al., 2014Nagy, W. W., Carlisle, J. F., & Goodwin, A. P. (2014). Morphological knowledge and literacy acquisition. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(1), 3-12. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967...
; Levesque et al., 2021Levesque, K. C., Breadmore, H. L., & Deacon, S. H. (2021) How morphology impacts reading and spelling: advancing the role of morphology in models of literacy development. Journal of Research in Reading, 44, 10-26. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313...
), resulting in a boost to word recognition due to high-quality lexical representations (Perfetti & Hart, 2002Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. T. Verhoeven, C. Elbro, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 189-213). John Benjamins Publishing.).

Present study’s limitations

One of the limitations of the present study is the use of only one measure to evaluate morphological awareness. It is certainly better to have more than one measure of a construct; however, the kind of word analogy task used in the present study is the most frequently used task to evaluate morphological awareness in Brazil (Oliveira & Justi, 2016Oliveira, B. S. F. & Justi, F. R. R. (2016). Uma meta-análise da relação entre a consciência morfológica e a leitura e escrita no português brasileiro. In: Pereira-Silva, N. L., Barbosa, A. J. G., Rodrigues, M. C. (Orgs.). Pesquisa em desenvolvimento humano e educação. Curitiba: CRV.). In addition, as shown in Mota et al., (2008aMota, M., Annibal, L., & Lima, S. (2008a). A Morfologia derivacional contribui para a leitura e escrita no português? Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 21 (2), 311-318. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722008a000200017.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722008a0...
, 2008bMota, M., Gontijo, R., Mansur-Lisboa, S., Olive, R., Silva, D., Dias, J., Delgado, N., & Kamisaki, R.. (2008b). Avaliação da consciência da morfologia deriva- cional: fidedignidade e validade. Avaliação Psicológica, 7(2), 151-157. Retrieved from http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1677-04712008b000200006&lng=pt&tlng=pt.
http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?scr...
, 2014Mota, M., Santos, A., & Guimarães, S. (2014). Evidências de validade e consistência interna de tarefas de analogia gramatical. Estudos De Psicologia (natal), 19 (4), 250-257. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-294X2014000400002.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-294X201400...
) studies, the word analogy task (derivational morphology version) is the best morphological awareness task in Brazilian Portuguese considering children from the 2nd to the 4th grades. Thus, we have used probably the best possible task, considering that other morphological awareness tasks easily present ceiling effects in Brazilian Portuguese (see, for example, the flexional morphology task from Mota et al., 2014Mota, M., Santos, A., & Guimarães, S. (2014). Evidências de validade e consistência interna de tarefas de analogia gramatical. Estudos De Psicologia (natal), 19 (4), 250-257. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-294X2014000400002.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-294X201400...
or the morphose- mantic decision task from Mota et al., (2008aMota, M., Annibal, L., & Lima, S. (2008a). A Morfologia derivacional contribui para a leitura e escrita no português? Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 21 (2), 311-318. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722008a000200017.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722008a0...
, 2008bMota, M., Gontijo, R., Mansur-Lisboa, S., Olive, R., Silva, D., Dias, J., Delgado, N., & Kamisaki, R.. (2008b). Avaliação da consciência da morfologia deriva- cional: fidedignidade e validade. Avaliação Psicológica, 7(2), 151-157. Retrieved from http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1677-04712008b000200006&lng=pt&tlng=pt.
http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?scr...
) and Oliveira and Justi (2017a)Oliveira, B. S. F. d., & Justi, F. R. d. R. (2017a). A contribuição da consciência morfológica para a leitura no português brasileiro [The contribution of morphological awareness to reading in Brazilian Portuguese]. Revista Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 19(3), 270-286. https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicologia.v19n3p270-286.
https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicol...
.

Another limitation of the present study has to do with the study sample of children from private schools. Considering the city region of the private schools, the present study sample is of children from middle-class families. Thus, one may wonder if these results could generalize to children from lower-income families. In this regard, it is important to notice that the present study replicated the results of the study by de Freitas et al. (2018De Freitas, P. V., Mota, M. M. P. E., & Deacon, S. H. (2018). Morphological awareness, word reading, and reading comprehension in Portuguese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39, 507-525. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000479.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271641700047...
) conducted with 4th-grade children from public schools (attended mainly by children from poor families). Thus, in both studies, independently of children being from private or public schools, morphological awareness contributed to word reading in 4th graders. In addition, as argued by de Freitas et al. (2018De Freitas, P. V., Mota, M. M. P. E., & Deacon, S. H. (2018). Morphological awareness, word reading, and reading comprehension in Portuguese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39, 507-525. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000479.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271641700047...
), although socioeconomic status certainly explain variation in metalinguistic and reading skills, there is no clear reason to suspect that the underlying process of learning to read or the relations between metalinguistic skills and reading would differ greatly as a result of socioeconomic status.

One last limitation that we want to discuss is the lack of control of vocabulary. There are reasons to suspect that morphological awareness and vocabulary could influence each other (Kuo & Anderson, 2006Kuo, L., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41, 161-180. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_3.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_...
; Nagy et al., 2014Nagy, W. W., Carlisle, J. F., & Goodwin, A. P. (2014). Morphological knowledge and literacy acquisition. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(1), 3-12. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967...
). However, it is important to notice that a reasonable number of studies had shown that morphological awareness contributes to word reading over and above vocabulary (e.g., Kirby et al., 2012Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. L., Bowers, P. N., Izenberg, L., Wade-Wooley, L., & Parrila, R. (2012). Children’s morphological awareness and reading ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 389-410. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-5.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-...
; Deacon et al., 2013Deacon, S. H., Benere, J., & Pasquerella, A. (2013). Reciprocal relationship: Children’s morphological awareness and their reading accuracy across grade 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 49 (6), 1113-1126. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474...
; Diamanti et al., 2017Diamanti, V., Mouzaki, A., Ralli, A., Antoniou, F., Papaioannou, S., & Protopapas, A. (2017). Preschool phonological and morphological awareness as longitudinal predictors of early reading and spelling development in Greek. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2039. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02039.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02039...
; Kruk et al., 2013Kruk, R. S., & Bergman, K. (2013). The reciprocal relations between morphological processes and reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 10-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.014.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.0...
; Muroya et al., 2017Muroya, N., Inoue, T., Hosokawa, M., Georgiou, G., Maekawa, H., & Parrila, R. (2017) The Role of Morphological Awareness in Word Reading Skills in Japanese: A Within-Language Cross-Orthographic Perspective, Scientific Studies of Reading, 21:6, 449-462, https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.1323906.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.13...
). Thus, instead of controlling for vocabulary, we decided to control for phonological working memory in the present study because phonological working memory could be relevant for performance in the morphological awareness tasks employed in the present study (e.g., Deacon & Kirby, 2004Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25 (2), 223-238. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716404001110.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271640400111...
; Justi & Roazzi, 2012Justi, C. N. G., & Roazzi, A. (2012). A contribuição de variáveis cognitivas para a leitura e a escrita no Português Brasileiro. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 25 (3), 605-614. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722012000300021.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7972201200...
). In addition, in comparison with vocabulary, there are not too many studies which have controlled for phonological working memory while investigating the relationship between morphological awareness and single word reading. Just to give an example, none of the above cited studies which controlled for vocabulary controlled for phonological working memory (Deacon et al., 2013Deacon, S. H., Benere, J., & Pasquerella, A. (2013). Reciprocal relationship: Children’s morphological awareness and their reading accuracy across grade 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 49 (6), 1113-1126. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474...
; Diamanti et al., 2017Diamanti, V., Mouzaki, A., Ralli, A., Antoniou, F., Papaioannou, S., & Protopapas, A. (2017). Preschool phonological and morphological awareness as longitudinal predictors of early reading and spelling development in Greek. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2039. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02039.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02039...
; Kirby et al., 2012Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. L., Bowers, P. N., Izenberg, L., Wade-Wooley, L., & Parrila, R. (2012). Children’s morphological awareness and reading ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 389-410. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-5.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-...
; Kruk et al., 2013Kruk, R. S., & Bergman, K. (2013). The reciprocal relations between morphological processes and reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 10-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.014.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.0...
; Muroya et al., 2017Muroya, N., Inoue, T., Hosokawa, M., Georgiou, G., Maekawa, H., & Parrila, R. (2017) The Role of Morphological Awareness in Word Reading Skills in Japanese: A Within-Language Cross-Orthographic Perspective, Scientific Studies of Reading, 21:6, 449-462, https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.1323906.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.13...
). Since practical issues prevent us from controlling for every important variable, we opted to control for phonological working memory because it is already shown that morphological awareness contributes for word reading over and above vocabulary (e.g., Deacon et al., 2013Deacon, S. H., Benere, J., & Pasquerella, A. (2013). Reciprocal relationship: Children’s morphological awareness and their reading accuracy across grade 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 49 (6), 1113-1126. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474...
; Diamanti et al., 2017Diamanti, V., Mouzaki, A., Ralli, A., Antoniou, F., Papaioannou, S., & Protopapas, A. (2017). Preschool phonological and morphological awareness as longitudinal predictors of early reading and spelling development in Greek. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2039. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02039.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02039...
; Kirby et al., 2012Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. L., Bowers, P. N., Izenberg, L., Wade-Wooley, L., & Parrila, R. (2012). Children’s morphological awareness and reading ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 389-410. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-5.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-...
; Kruk et al., 2013Kruk, R. S., & Bergman, K. (2013). The reciprocal relations between morphological processes and reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 10-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.014.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.0...
; Muroya et al., 2017Muroya, N., Inoue, T., Hosokawa, M., Georgiou, G., Maekawa, H., & Parrila, R. (2017) The Role of Morphological Awareness in Word Reading Skills in Japanese: A Within-Language Cross-Orthographic Perspective, Scientific Studies of Reading, 21:6, 449-462, https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.1323906.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.13...
). Thus, depending on how you look at it, this can be viewed as an advantage (controlling for phonological working memory) or as a limitation (lack of control for vocabulary) of the present study.

Conclusions

The present study adds critical longitudinal data concerning the relationship between morphological awareness and word reading in Brazilian Portuguese, considering a large span of school grades. It shows that in the early years of reading instruction, it is word reading that contributes to morphological awareness, and that in the more advanced years, it is morphological awareness which contributes to word reading. In addition, all these relationships hold even when controlling for intelligence, phonological awareness, and phonological working memory. The present results highlight the importance of conducting reading research in other languages besides English since, in that language, an early contribution of morphological awareness is generally observed.

  • Funding During the conduction of this study, the second author received a scholarship from Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior, Brasil (CAPES) — Finance Code 001.
  • Availability of data and materials The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
  • Declarations Ethics approval and consent to participate Procedures in this study adhere to ethical research policies and were approved by the research ethics committee of the authors’ institution and by the board of the children’s schools. The children’s guardians signed an informed consent form consenting the children participation. In addition, oral assent was obtained from each child at every testing session.
  • Consent for publication Not applicable.
  • Competing interests The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

References

  • Angelini, A., Alves, I., Custódio, E., Duarte, W., & Duarte, J. (1999). Manual matrizes progressivas coloridas de Raven: escala especial. São Paulo: Centro Editor de Testes e Pesquisas em Psicologia
  • Apel, K., Diehm, E., & Apel, L. (2013). Using multiple measures of morphological awareness to assess its relation to reading. Topics in Language Disorders, 33, 42-56. https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0b013e318280f57b
    » https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0b013e318280f57b
  • Borleffs, E., Maassen, B. A. M., Lyytinen, H., & Zwarts, F. (2017). Measuring orthographic transparency and morphological-syllabic complexity in alphabetic orthographies: a narrative review. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 30, 1617-1638. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9741-5
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9741-5
  • Borgwaldt, S. R., Hellwig, F. M., & De Groot, A. M. B. (2005). Onset entropy matters - Letter-to-phoneme mappings in seven languages. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18 (3), 211-229. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3001-9
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3001-9
  • Carlisle, J. F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In L. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 189-209). Erlbaum.
  • Coelho, C. L. G., & Correa, J. (2017). Compreensão de leitura: Habilidades cognitivas e tipos de texto. Psico, 48, 40-49. https://doi.org/10.15448/1980-8623.2017.1.23417
    » https://doi.org/10.15448/1980-8623.2017.1.23417
  • Colé, P., Cavalli, E., Duncan, L. G., Theurel, A., Gentaz, E., Sprenger-Charolles, L., & El-Ahmadi, A. (2018). What is the influence of morphological knowledge in the early stages of reading acquisition among low SES children? A graphical modeling approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 547. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00547
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00547
  • De Freitas, P. V., Mota, M. M. P. E., & Deacon, S. H. (2018). Morphological awareness, word reading, and reading comprehension in Portuguese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39, 507-525. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000479
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716417000479
  • Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25 (2), 223-238. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716404001110
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716404001110
  • Deacon, S. H., Benere, J., & Pasquerella, A. (2013). Reciprocal relationship: Children’s morphological awareness and their reading accuracy across grade 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 49 (6), 1113-1126. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029474
  • Deacon, S. H., Holliman, A. J., Dobson, G. J., & Harrison, E. C. J. (2018). Assessing direct contributions of morphological awareness and prosodic sensitivity to children’s word reading and reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22, 527-534. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2018.1483376
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2018.1483376
  • Desrochers, A., Manolitsis, G., Gaudreau, P., & Georgiou, G. (2018). Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 31, 1695-1719. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9772-y
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9772-y
  • Diamanti, V., Mouzaki, A., Ralli, A., Antoniou, F., Papaioannou, S., & Protopapas, A. (2017). Preschool phonological and morphological awareness as longitudinal predictors of early reading and spelling development in Greek. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2039. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02039
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02039
  • Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9 (2), 167-188. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0902_4
    » https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0902_4
  • Estivalet, G. L., & Meunier, F. (2015). The Brazilian Portuguese Lexicon: an instrument for psycholinguistic research. PLoS ONE, 10 (12), e0144016. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144016
    » https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144016
  • Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In k. E. Patterson, J. C. Marshall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface dyslexia: Neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 301-330). London: Erlbaum.
  • Goswami, U., & Ziegler, J. C. (2006). Fluency, phonology and morphology: a response to the commentaries on becoming literate in different languages. Developmental Science, 9, 451-453. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00511.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00511.x
  • Grainger, J., & Beyersman, E. (2017). Edge-aligned embedded word activation initiates morpho-orthographic segmentation. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 285-317). Elsevier Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2017.03.009
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2017.03.009
  • Guimarães, R. K. G. (2005). Influência da variação linguística e da consciência morfossintática no desempenho em leitura e escrita. Interação Em Psicologia, 9(2), 261-271. https://doi.org/10.5380/psi.v9i2.4795
    » https://doi.org/10.5380/psi.v9i2.4795
  • Guimarães, S. B., & Mota, M. M. P. E. (2016). Qual a contribuição da consciência morfológica das crianças na precisão de leitura de palavras e compreensão de texto no português? Estudos De Psicologia (natal), 21 (3), 239-248. https://doi.org/10.5935/1678-4669.20160023
    » https://doi.org/10.5935/1678-4669.20160023
  • Justi, C. N. G., & Roazzi, A. (2012). A contribuição de variáveis cognitivas para a leitura e a escrita no Português Brasileiro. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 25 (3), 605-614. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722012000300021
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722012000300021
  • Kenny, D. (1975). Cross-lagged panel correlation: A test for spuriousness. Psychological Bulletin, 82(6), 887-903. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.82.6.887
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.82.6.887
  • Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. L., Bowers, P. N., Izenberg, L., Wade-Wooley, L., & Parrila, R. (2012). Children’s morphological awareness and reading ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 389-410. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-5
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9276-5
  • Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). Guilford Press.
  • Kruk, R. S., & Bergman, K. (2013). The reciprocal relations between morphological processes and reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 10-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.014
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.014
  • Kuo, L., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41, 161-180. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_3
    » https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4103_3
  • Law, J. M., & Ghesquière, P. (2017). Early development and predictors of morphological awareness: Disentangling the impact of decoding skills and phonologial awareness. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 67, 47-59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2017.05.003
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2017.05.003
  • Levesque, K. C., Breadmore, H. L., & Deacon, S. H. (2021) How morphology impacts reading and spelling: advancing the role of morphology in models of literacy development. Journal of Research in Reading, 44, 10-26. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12313
  • Levesque, K. C., Kieffer, M. J., & Deacon, S. H. (2018). Inferring meaning from meaningful parts: The contributions of morphological skills to the development of children’s reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 54, 63-80. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.219
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.219
  • Lima, C. F., & Castro, S. L. (2010). Reading strategies in orthographies of intermediate depth are flexible: Modulation of length effects in Portuguese. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22(2), 190-215. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440902750145
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440902750145
  • Lúcio, P. S., & Pinheiro, A. M. V. (2014). Novos estudos psicométricos para o subteste de leiturado teste de desempenho escolar. Temas Em Psicologia (ribeirão Preto), 22, 109-119. https://doi.org/10.9788/TP2014.1-09
    » https://doi.org/10.9788/TP2014.1-09
  • Lyster, S. -A. H., Snowling, M. J., Hulme, C., and Lervåg, A. O. (2021) Preschool phonological, morphological and semantic skills explain it all: following reading development through a 9-year period. Journal of Research in Reading, 44, 175-188. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12312
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12312
  • Manolitsis, G., Grigorakis, I., Georgiou, G. (2017). The longitudinal contribution of early morphological awareness skills to reading fluency and comprehension in Greek. Frontiers in Psychology, 8 (1793). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01793
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01793
  • Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S. A. H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 322-352. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026744
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026744
  • Miranda, L., & Mota, M. M. P. (2013). Há uma relação específica entre consciência morfológica e reconhecimento de Palavras? Psico-USF, Bragança Paulista, 18(2), 241-247. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-82712013000200008
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-82712013000200008
  • Mota, M., Santos, A., & Guimarães, S. (2014). Evidências de validade e consistência interna de tarefas de analogia gramatical. Estudos De Psicologia (natal), 19 (4), 250-257. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-294X2014000400002
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-294X2014000400002
  • Mota, M., & Silva, K. (2007). Consciência morfológica e desenvolvimento ortográfico: Um estudo exploratório. Psicologia em Pesquisa, 1 (2), 86-92. https://doi.org/10.24879/200700100200432
    » https://doi.org/10.24879/200700100200432
  • Mota, M., Annibal, L., & Lima, S. (2008a). A Morfologia derivacional contribui para a leitura e escrita no português? Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 21 (2), 311-318. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722008a000200017
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722008a000200017
  • Mota, M., Gontijo, R., Mansur-Lisboa, S., Olive, R., Silva, D., Dias, J., Delgado, N., & Kamisaki, R.. (2008b). Avaliação da consciência da morfologia deriva- cional: fidedignidade e validade. Avaliação Psicológica, 7(2), 151-157. Retrieved from http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1677-04712008b000200006&lng=pt&tlng=pt.
    » http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1677-04712008b000200006&lng=pt&tlng=pt
  • Mota, M., Vieira, M., Bastos, R., Dias, J., Paiva, N., Mansur-Lisboa, S., & Andrade- Silva, D. (2012). Leitura contextual e processamento metalingüístico no português do Brasil: um estudo longitudinal. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 25(1), 114-120. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722012000100014
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722012000100014
  • Muroya, N., Inoue, T., Hosokawa, M., Georgiou, G., Maekawa, H., & Parrila, R. (2017) The Role of Morphological Awareness in Word Reading Skills in Japanese: A Within-Language Cross-Orthographic Perspective, Scientific Studies of Reading, 21:6, 449-462, https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.1323906
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.1323906
  • Mousikou, P, Beyersmann, E, Ktori, M, Javourey-Drevet, L., Crepaldi, D., Ziegler, J., Grainger, J., & Schroeder, S. (2020). Orthographic consistency influences morphological processing in reading aloud: Evidence from a crosslinguis- tic study. Developmental Science. 23, e12952. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12952
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12952
  • Nagy, W., Berninger, V., Abbott, R., Vaughan, K., & Vermeulen, K. (2003). Relationship of morphology and other language skills to literacy skills in at risk second grade readers and at-risk fourth grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 730-742. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.730
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.730
  • Nagy, W., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2006). Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98 (1), 134-147. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.134
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.134
  • Nagy, W. W., Carlisle, J. F., & Goodwin, A. P. (2014). Morphological knowledge and literacy acquisition. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(1), 3-12. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413509967
  • Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Bindman, M. (1997). Morphological spelling strategies: Developmental stages and processes. Developmental Psychology, 33(4), 637-649. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.4.637
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.4.637
  • Oliveira, B. S. F. d., & Justi, F. R. d. R. (2017a). A contribuição da consciência morfológica para a leitura no português brasileiro [The contribution of morphological awareness to reading in Brazilian Portuguese]. Revista Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 19(3), 270-286. https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicologia.v19n3p270-286
    » https://doi.org/10.5935/1980-6906/psicologia.v19n3p270-286
  • Oliveira, B. S. F., & Justi, F. R. R. (2017b). Morphological priming development in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking children. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 30(4). https://doi.org/10.1186/s41155-017-0058-8
    » https://doi.org/10.1186/s41155-017-0058-8
  • Oliveira, M., Levesque, K. C., Deacon, S. H., & Mota, M. M. P. E. (2020). Evaluating models of how morphological awareness connects to reading comprehension: A study in Portuguese. Journal of Research in Reading, 43(2), 161-179. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12296
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12296
  • Oliveira, B. S. F. & Justi, F. R. R. (2016). Uma meta-análise da relação entre a consciência morfológica e a leitura e escrita no português brasileiro. In: Pereira-Silva, N. L., Barbosa, A. J. G., Rodrigues, M. C. (Orgs.). Pesquisa em desenvolvimento humano e educação. Curitiba: CRV.
  • Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. T. Verhoeven, C. Elbro, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 189-213). John Benjamins Publishing.
  • Pinheiro, A. M. V., Lúcio, P. S., & Silva, D. M. R. (2008). Avaliação cognitiva de leitura: o efeito de regularidade grafema-fonema e fonema-grafema na leitura em voz alta de palavras isoladas no português do Brasil. Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 10 (2),16-30. Retrieved from http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1516-36872008000200002&lng=pt&tlng=pt.
    » http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1516-36872008000200002&lng=pt&tlng=pt
  • Pittas, E., & Nunes, T. (2014). The relation between morphological awareness and reading and spelling in Greek: A longitudinal study. Reading and Writring: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 27, 1507-1527. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-014-9503-6
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-014-9503-6
  • Ruan, Y., Georgiou, G. K., Song, S., Li, Y., & Shu, H. (2018). Does writing system influence the associations between phonological awareness, morphological awareness, and reading? A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110(2), 180-202. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000216
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000216
  • Sargiani, R., Ehri, L., & Maluf, M. (2018). Orthographic mapping instruction to facilitate reading and spelling in Brazilian emergent readers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39, 1405-1437. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716418000371
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716418000371
  • Schmalz, X., Marinus, E., Coltheart, M., & Castles, A. (2015). Getting to the bottom of orthographic depth. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(6), 1614-1629. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0835-2
    » https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0835-2
  • Seymour, P., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. (2003). Foundations literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143-174. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712603321661859
    » https://doi.org/10.1348/000712603321661859
  • Seymour, P. H. K. (2005) Early reading development in European orthographies. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme. The science of reading: a handbook. Oxford: Blackwell.
  • Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134(4), 584-615. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.584
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.584
  • Simpson, I. C., Moreno-Pérez, F. J., Rodríguez-Ortiz, I. d. l. R., Valdés-Coronel, M., & Saldaña, D. (2020). The effects of morphological and syntactic knowledge on reading comprehension in Spanish speaking children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 33(2), 329-348. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09964-5
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09964-5
  • Stein, L. (1994). TDE: Teste de Desempenho Escolar: manual para aplicação e interpretação. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo.
  • To, N. L., Tighe, E. L., & Binder, K. S. (2016). Investigating morphological awareness and the processing of transparent and opaque words in adults with low literacy skills and in skilled readers. Journal of Research in Reading, 39(2), 171-188. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12036
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12036
  • Wechsler, D. (2002). WISC-III: Escala de inteligência Wechsler para crianças. Manual (3. ed., V. L. M. de Figueiredo, adaptação e padronização brasileira). São Paulo, SP: Casa do Psicólogo.
  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulettin, 131, 3-29. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3
  • Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D., Csépe, V., Reis, A., Faísca, L., Saine, N., Lyytinen, H., Vaessen, A., & Blomert, L. (2010). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: A cross-language investigation. Psychological Science, 21, 551-559. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610363406
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610363406

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://doi.org/10.1186/s41155-022-00245-9.

Additional file 1:

Table S1. Hierarchical Regression Analyses of the Full Sample with Interaction Terms. Table S2. Hierarchical Regression Analysis.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    24 Mar 2023
  • Date of issue
    2023

History

  • Received
    28 Mar 2022
  • Accepted
    29 Dec 2022
  • Published
    03 Feb 2023
Curso de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia da Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul Rua Ramiro Barcelos, 2600 - sala 110, 90035-003 Porto Alegre RS - Brazil, Tel.: +55 51 3308-5691 - Porto Alegre - RS - Brazil
E-mail: prc@springeropen.com