Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Lesion localization and performance on Theory of Mind tests in stroke survivors: a systematic review

Abstract

Background:

Theory of Mind (ToM) is the ability to attribute mental states to oneself and others. Individuals with a brain lesion following a stroke exhibit a compromised ability to perform ToM tasks.

Objective:

To analyze studies that evaluated ToM in stroke survivors considering the lesion localization and performance on ToM tests.

Methods:

The searches were carried out until November 28, 2018, using the following search terms: “social cognition” or “Theory of Mind” and “stroke”. Searches were conducted in the PubMed, PsycInfo, Web of Science and Scopus data bases. The initial search led to the retrieval of 425 articles. After the exclusion of duplicates and the analysis of the titles, abstracts and full texts, 20 articles were selected for the present review.

Results:

The studies showed that patients with lesion in the right hemisphere present lower performance on ToM tasks compared to those with lesion in the left hemisphere. In addition, patients with lesion in the right hemisphere presented significant impairment in the performance on ToM tasks compared to healthy individuals. Furthermore, the studies that evaluated lesions in specific regions such as temporal lobe, prefrontal cortex, posterior parietal cortex, and temporo-parietal junction, indicated a significant deficit in ToM performance of these patients compared to healthy individuals.

Discussion:

This review showed that stroke survivors have a poor performance on ToM tasks. The right hemisphere and prefrontal cortex seem to be associated with the deficit of this ability.

Keywords:
Social cognition; Theory of Mind; stroke

Introduction

The term “Theory of Mind” (ToM) emerged at the end of the 1970s with experimental studies on animal cognition. Premack and Woodruff11. Premack D, Woodruff G. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci. 1978;1(4):515-26. investigated the ability of chimpanzees to infer the intentions of humans in a problem situation shown on video. The results suggested that primates were able to understand and identify options compatible with these intentions.

Abilities related to ToM emerge throughout the development process22. Saxe R, Powell LJ. It's the thought that counts: specific brain regions for one component of theory of mind. Psychol Sci. 2006;17(8):692-9.. ToM is the capacity to make inferences regarding the thoughts, intentions, beliefs and emotions of others to predict and explain their behavior11. Premack D, Woodruff G. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci. 1978;1(4):515-26.. The construct of ToM is comprised of affective and cognitive components33. Hamilton J, Radlak B, Morris PG, Phillips LH. Theory of Mind and Executive Functioning Following Stroke. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2017;32(5):507-18.. The cognitive component regards the ability to distinguish the thoughts, beliefs and intentions of another person, whereas understanding the feelings or others is attributed to the affective component44. Pluta A, Gawron N, Sobańska M, Wójcik AD, Łojek E. The nature of the relationship between neurocognition and theory of mind impairments in stroke patients. Neuropsychology. 2017;31(6):666-81..

Neuroimaging studies report a network of active brain regions involved in the processing of ToM, including the anterior cingulate cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, medial prefrontal cortex, precuneus, inferior frontal gyrus, superior temporal sulcus and temporoparietal junction22. Saxe R, Powell LJ. It's the thought that counts: specific brain regions for one component of theory of mind. Psychol Sci. 2006;17(8):692-9.,55. Schurz M, Radua J, Aichhorn M, Richlan F, Perner J. Fractionating theory of mind: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;42:9-34.,66. Mahy CEV, Moses LJ, Pfeifer JH. How and where: Theory-of-mind in the brain. Develop Cogn Neurosci. 2014;9:68-81.. Therefore, brain lesions that affect these regions may result in an impaired ability of ToM.

Studies on stroke survivors commonly investigate the influence of the cerebral lateralization in the ability of ToM. Evidence indicates that individuals with the right hemisphere affected have a deficit regarding the ability of ToM compared to patients with lesion in the left hemisphere33. Hamilton J, Radlak B, Morris PG, Phillips LH. Theory of Mind and Executive Functioning Following Stroke. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2017;32(5):507-18.. According to Tompkins et al.77. Tompkins CA, Scharp VL, Fassbinder W, Meigh KM, Armstrong EM. A different story on “theory of mind” deficit in adults with right hemisphere brain damage. Aphasiology. 2008;22(1):42-61. right hemisphere brain lesions can result in impaired communication and social interactions. However, divergent results are found on the lateralization of the function of ToM, indicating that stroke patients, regardless of the location of the lesion, have significant impairment in the ability of ToM compared to the control group88. Yeh ZT, Tsai CF. Impairment on theory of mind and empathy in patients with stroke. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2014;68(8):612-20..

The aim of the present study was to perform a systematic review of all studies published that have evaluated ToM in stroke survivors considering the lesion localization and performance on ToM tests.

Methods

A systematic review was performed of studies conducted to evaluate ToM in adult stroke survivors. The searches were carried out until November 28, 2018 in the databases: Pubmed, PsycInfo, Web of Science and Scopus, using the following search terms: (“social cognition” or “Theory of Mind”) and stroke. The inclusion criteria were studies published in English that evaluated post-stroke ToM in individuals aged 18 years or older. No restriction was imposed on the year of publication. Studies that evaluated ToM in specific clinical samples (individuals with dementia, schizophrenia, autism, Williams syndrome, Parkinson's disease, epilepsy, etc.) in the absence of stroke and post-stroke neuroimaging studies that did not evaluate ToM were excluded from the review. Books, book chapters, opinions, case studies, bibliographic/systematic reviews and meta-analyses were also excluded.

Two independent researchers performed the data extraction and documented the authors’ names, year of publication, sample size, sex, age and schooling of the participants, time elapsed since the stroke event, site of the brain lesion and instruments used to assess ToM. Divergences of opinion between the reviewers were discussed until a consensus was reached. The present systematic review was conducted in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA)99. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DGA. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: The PRISMA statement. Ann Inter Med. 2009;151(4):264-9..

The initial search led to the retrieval of 425 articles. After the removal of duplicates, the titles and abstracts of 268 articles were analyzed for eligibility, 172 of which were preselected. Following the full-text analysis, 152 articles were excluded and 20 were selected for the present review. Figure 1 displays the flowchart of the selection process.

Figure 1

In addition, the studies selected in the present review were evaluated for methodological quality based on the guidelines of strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology (STROBE). This instrument consists of 22 check items and aims to provide greater transparency and improve the quality of the description and presentation of observational study findings1010. Vandenbroucke JP, Von Elm E, Altman DG, Gøtzsche PC, Mulrow CD, Pocock SJ, et al. Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE): explanation and elaboration. Ann Intern Med. 2007;147(8):163.. We also used as a basis the Dictionary of STROBE, indicated by the authors as an important theoretical framework for the critical analysis of scientific articles1111. Von Elm E, Altman DG, Egger M, Pocock SJ, Gøtzsche PC, Vandenbroucke JP. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. Ann Intern Med. 2007;147(8):573-7..

Regarding the score, studies with a score of 80 to 100% are considered studies that have a strong methodological quality. Studies with a score of 60%-79% are classified as moderate methodological quality, and studies with lower than 60% scores are classified in the STROBE checklist1212. de Paula Eduardo JAF, de Rezende MG, Menezes PR, Del-Ben CM. Preterm birth as a risk factor for postpartum depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2019;259:392-403..

Results

Table 1 displays the data extracted from the 20 articles selected for the present review. The articles were published between 19961313. Siegal M, Carrington J, Radel M. Theory of mind and pragmatic understanding following right hemisphere damage. Brain Lang. 1996;53(1): 40-50. and 201744. Pluta A, Gawron N, Sobańska M, Wójcik AD, Łojek E. The nature of the relationship between neurocognition and theory of mind impairments in stroke patients. Neuropsychology. 2017;31(6):666-81.. The sample size ranged from 111414. Roca M, Gleichgerrcht E, Abáñez A, Torralva T, Manes F. Cerebellar stroke impairs executive functions but not theory of mind. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2013;25(1):E48-9. to 8044. Pluta A, Gawron N, Sobańska M, Wójcik AD, Łojek E. The nature of the relationship between neurocognition and theory of mind impairments in stroke patients. Neuropsychology. 2017;31(6):666-81. participants, the majority of whom was male (55.9%), and mean age ranged from 34.121515. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Tomer R, Berger BD, Goldsher DMD, Aharon-Peretz JMD. Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005;18(1):55-67. to 73 years1616. Besharati S, Forkel SJ, Kopelman M, Solms M, Jenkinson PM, Fotopoulou A, et al. Mentalizing the body: spatial and social cognition in anosognosia for hemiplegia. Brain. 2016;139(3):971-85.,1717. Happé F, Brownwll H, Winner E. Acquired theory of mind impairments following stroke. Cognition. 1999;70(3):211-40.. Educational level of the participants ranged from 7.31818. Surian L, Siegal M. Sources of performance on theory of mind tasks in right hemisphere-damaged patients. Brain Lang. 2001;78(2):224-32. to 15 years of study1919. Griffin R, Friedman O, Ween J, Winner E, Happé F, Brownell H. Theory of mind and the right cerebral hemisphere: Refining the scope of impairment. Laterality. 2006;11(3):195-225.; five articles did not specify the schooling of the participants2020. Apperly IA, Samson D, Chiavarino C, Humphreys GW. Frontal and temporo-parietal lobe contributions to theory of mind: neuropsychological evidence from a false-belief task with reduced language and executive demands. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16(10):1773-84.

21. Martin I, Mcdonald S. That can't be right! What causes pragmatic language impairment following right hemisphere damage? Brain Impairment. 2006;7(3):202-11.

22. Weed E, McGregor W, Nielsen JF, Roepstorff A, Frith U. Theory of Mind in adults with right hemisphere damage: What's the story? Brain Lang. 2010;113(2):65-72.

23. Humphreys GW, Bedford J. The relations between joint action and theory of mind: a neuropsychological analysis. Exp Brain Res. 2011;211(34):357-69.
-2424. Wilkos E, Brown TJ, Slawinska K, Kucharska KA. Social cognitive and neurocognitive deficits in inpatients with unilateral thalamic lesions – Pilot study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2015;11:1031-8..

Table 1
Characteristics of the selected studies according to inclusion criteria

Regarding the affected region of the brain, six studies evaluated patients with damage to both hemispheres33. Hamilton J, Radlak B, Morris PG, Phillips LH. Theory of Mind and Executive Functioning Following Stroke. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2017;32(5):507-18.,44. Pluta A, Gawron N, Sobańska M, Wójcik AD, Łojek E. The nature of the relationship between neurocognition and theory of mind impairments in stroke patients. Neuropsychology. 2017;31(6):666-81.,88. Yeh ZT, Tsai CF. Impairment on theory of mind and empathy in patients with stroke. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2014;68(8):612-20.,1313. Siegal M, Carrington J, Radel M. Theory of mind and pragmatic understanding following right hemisphere damage. Brain Lang. 1996;53(1): 40-50.,1717. Happé F, Brownwll H, Winner E. Acquired theory of mind impairments following stroke. Cognition. 1999;70(3):211-40.,1818. Surian L, Siegal M. Sources of performance on theory of mind tasks in right hemisphere-damaged patients. Brain Lang. 2001;78(2):224-32.. Three studies showed that patients with lesion in right hemisphere present significant impairment in the performance on ToM tasks compared to patients with lesion in the left hemisphere33. Hamilton J, Radlak B, Morris PG, Phillips LH. Theory of Mind and Executive Functioning Following Stroke. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2017;32(5):507-18.,1313. Siegal M, Carrington J, Radel M. Theory of mind and pragmatic understanding following right hemisphere damage. Brain Lang. 1996;53(1): 40-50.,1717. Happé F, Brownwll H, Winner E. Acquired theory of mind impairments following stroke. Cognition. 1999;70(3):211-40. and control group1717. Happé F, Brownwll H, Winner E. Acquired theory of mind impairments following stroke. Cognition. 1999;70(3):211-40.. In contrast, Yeh and Tsai88. Yeh ZT, Tsai CF. Impairment on theory of mind and empathy in patients with stroke. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2014;68(8):612-20. and Pluta et al.44. Pluta A, Gawron N, Sobańska M, Wójcik AD, Łojek E. The nature of the relationship between neurocognition and theory of mind impairments in stroke patients. Neuropsychology. 2017;31(6):666-81. demonstrated that patients with stroke showed impairment in the ToM abilities compared to control group, regardless of the hemisphere of the lesion. The study of Surian and Siegal1818. Surian L, Siegal M. Sources of performance on theory of mind tasks in right hemisphere-damaged patients. Brain Lang. 2001;78(2):224-32. found no association between the performance on ToM tasks and post-stoke lesion.

In addition, seven studies evaluated patients with damage only in the right hemisphere77. Tompkins CA, Scharp VL, Fassbinder W, Meigh KM, Armstrong EM. A different story on “theory of mind” deficit in adults with right hemisphere brain damage. Aphasiology. 2008;22(1):42-61.,1616. Besharati S, Forkel SJ, Kopelman M, Solms M, Jenkinson PM, Fotopoulou A, et al. Mentalizing the body: spatial and social cognition in anosognosia for hemiplegia. Brain. 2016;139(3):971-85.,1919. Griffin R, Friedman O, Ween J, Winner E, Happé F, Brownell H. Theory of mind and the right cerebral hemisphere: Refining the scope of impairment. Laterality. 2006;11(3):195-225.,2121. Martin I, Mcdonald S. That can't be right! What causes pragmatic language impairment following right hemisphere damage? Brain Impairment. 2006;7(3):202-11.,2222. Weed E, McGregor W, Nielsen JF, Roepstorff A, Frith U. Theory of Mind in adults with right hemisphere damage: What's the story? Brain Lang. 2010;113(2):65-72.,2525. Winner E, Brownell H, Happé F, Blum A, Pincus D. Distinguishing lies from jokes: Theory of mind deficits and discourse interpretation in right hemisphere brain-damaged patients. Brain Lang. 1998;62(1):89-106.,2626. Champagne-Lavau M, Joanette Y. Pragmatics, theory of mind and executive functions after a right-hemisphere lesion: Different patterns of deficits. J Neurolinguistics. 2009;22(5):413-26. and only one study did not show impairment in the ToM abilities in patients with lesion in the right hemisphere compared to healthy patients77. Tompkins CA, Scharp VL, Fassbinder W, Meigh KM, Armstrong EM. A different story on “theory of mind” deficit in adults with right hemisphere brain damage. Aphasiology. 2008;22(1):42-61..

Seven studies evaluated specific regions of the brain, such as the temporal lobe, prefrontal cortex, posterior parietal cortex and temporoparietal junction1515. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Tomer R, Berger BD, Goldsher DMD, Aharon-Peretz JMD. Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005;18(1):55-67.,2020. Apperly IA, Samson D, Chiavarino C, Humphreys GW. Frontal and temporo-parietal lobe contributions to theory of mind: neuropsychological evidence from a false-belief task with reduced language and executive demands. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16(10):1773-84.,2323. Humphreys GW, Bedford J. The relations between joint action and theory of mind: a neuropsychological analysis. Exp Brain Res. 2011;211(34):357-69.,2424. Wilkos E, Brown TJ, Slawinska K, Kucharska KA. Social cognitive and neurocognitive deficits in inpatients with unilateral thalamic lesions – Pilot study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2015;11:1031-8.,2727. Mah L, Arnold MC, Grafman J. Impairment of social perception associated with lesions of the prefrontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(7):1247-55.,2828. Xi C, Zhu Y, Zhu C, Song D, Wang Y, Wang K. Deficit of theory of mind after temporal lobe cerebral infarction. Behav Brain Funct. 2013;9:15., and these patients showed significant impairment on the performance on ToM tasks compared to healthy individuals. Roca et al.1414. Roca M, Gleichgerrcht E, Abáñez A, Torralva T, Manes F. Cerebellar stroke impairs executive functions but not theory of mind. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2013;25(1):E48-9. did not specify the site of the lesion. The time elapsed since the stroke event ranged from three weeks2424. Wilkos E, Brown TJ, Slawinska K, Kucharska KA. Social cognitive and neurocognitive deficits in inpatients with unilateral thalamic lesions – Pilot study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2015;11:1031-8. to 23 years1717. Happé F, Brownwll H, Winner E. Acquired theory of mind impairments following stroke. Cognition. 1999;70(3):211-40.. Four studies did not specify the time elapsed since the stroke event66. Mahy CEV, Moses LJ, Pfeifer JH. How and where: Theory-of-mind in the brain. Develop Cogn Neurosci. 2014;9:68-81.,1414. Roca M, Gleichgerrcht E, Abáñez A, Torralva T, Manes F. Cerebellar stroke impairs executive functions but not theory of mind. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2013;25(1):E48-9.,1919. Griffin R, Friedman O, Ween J, Winner E, Happé F, Brownell H. Theory of mind and the right cerebral hemisphere: Refining the scope of impairment. Laterality. 2006;11(3):195-225.,2323. Humphreys GW, Bedford J. The relations between joint action and theory of mind: a neuropsychological analysis. Exp Brain Res. 2011;211(34):357-69..

For the evaluation of ToM, the most frequently used tasks were: The False Belief and True Belief task44. Pluta A, Gawron N, Sobańska M, Wójcik AD, Łojek E. The nature of the relationship between neurocognition and theory of mind impairments in stroke patients. Neuropsychology. 2017;31(6):666-81.,77. Tompkins CA, Scharp VL, Fassbinder W, Meigh KM, Armstrong EM. A different story on “theory of mind” deficit in adults with right hemisphere brain damage. Aphasiology. 2008;22(1):42-61.,1313. Siegal M, Carrington J, Radel M. Theory of mind and pragmatic understanding following right hemisphere damage. Brain Lang. 1996;53(1): 40-50.,1515. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Tomer R, Berger BD, Goldsher DMD, Aharon-Peretz JMD. Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005;18(1):55-67.

16. Besharati S, Forkel SJ, Kopelman M, Solms M, Jenkinson PM, Fotopoulou A, et al. Mentalizing the body: spatial and social cognition in anosognosia for hemiplegia. Brain. 2016;139(3):971-85.

17. Happé F, Brownwll H, Winner E. Acquired theory of mind impairments following stroke. Cognition. 1999;70(3):211-40.

18. Surian L, Siegal M. Sources of performance on theory of mind tasks in right hemisphere-damaged patients. Brain Lang. 2001;78(2):224-32.

19. Griffin R, Friedman O, Ween J, Winner E, Happé F, Brownell H. Theory of mind and the right cerebral hemisphere: Refining the scope of impairment. Laterality. 2006;11(3):195-225.
-2020. Apperly IA, Samson D, Chiavarino C, Humphreys GW. Frontal and temporo-parietal lobe contributions to theory of mind: neuropsychological evidence from a false-belief task with reduced language and executive demands. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16(10):1773-84.,2323. Humphreys GW, Bedford J. The relations between joint action and theory of mind: a neuropsychological analysis. Exp Brain Res. 2011;211(34):357-69.,2525. Winner E, Brownell H, Happé F, Blum A, Pincus D. Distinguishing lies from jokes: Theory of mind deficits and discourse interpretation in right hemisphere brain-damaged patients. Brain Lang. 1998;62(1):89-106.

26. Champagne-Lavau M, Joanette Y. Pragmatics, theory of mind and executive functions after a right-hemisphere lesion: Different patterns of deficits. J Neurolinguistics. 2009;22(5):413-26.
-2727. Mah L, Arnold MC, Grafman J. Impairment of social perception associated with lesions of the prefrontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(7):1247-55., which is composed of stories that require the attribution of false and true beliefs, with the aim of verifying the participant's ability to make inferences about mental states, and these can be divided into false first-order beliefs, which concerns understanding the mental state of the other and second order, which is ability to understand what someone thinks about what someone else thinks; The FauxPas Detection test88. Yeh ZT, Tsai CF. Impairment on theory of mind and empathy in patients with stroke. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2014;68(8):612-20.,1414. Roca M, Gleichgerrcht E, Abáñez A, Torralva T, Manes F. Cerebellar stroke impairs executive functions but not theory of mind. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2013;25(1):E48-9.,1515. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Tomer R, Berger BD, Goldsher DMD, Aharon-Peretz JMD. Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005;18(1):55-67.,2828. Xi C, Zhu Y, Zhu C, Song D, Wang Y, Wang K. Deficit of theory of mind after temporal lobe cerebral infarction. Behav Brain Funct. 2013;9:15. examine the individual's ability to understand an embarrassing social situation, such as when one individual says something to another without considering that he would not like to hear1515. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Tomer R, Berger BD, Goldsher DMD, Aharon-Peretz JMD. Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005;18(1):55-67.; The Reading the Mind in the Eyes test33. Hamilton J, Radlak B, Morris PG, Phillips LH. Theory of Mind and Executive Functioning Following Stroke. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2017;32(5):507-18.,2424. Wilkos E, Brown TJ, Slawinska K, Kucharska KA. Social cognitive and neurocognitive deficits in inpatients with unilateral thalamic lesions – Pilot study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2015;11:1031-8.,2828. Xi C, Zhu Y, Zhu C, Song D, Wang Y, Wang K. Deficit of theory of mind after temporal lobe cerebral infarction. Behav Brain Funct. 2013;9:15., which consists of presenting black and white photographs of the eye region, in which participants should choose a word that best describes what the person in the image is thinking or feeling, with the intention of assess the ability to interpret the mental states of the other.

In relation to the methodological quality of the studies, of the 20 studies evaluated according to STROBE, six studies1414. Roca M, Gleichgerrcht E, Abáñez A, Torralva T, Manes F. Cerebellar stroke impairs executive functions but not theory of mind. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2013;25(1):E48-9.,1919. Griffin R, Friedman O, Ween J, Winner E, Happé F, Brownell H. Theory of mind and the right cerebral hemisphere: Refining the scope of impairment. Laterality. 2006;11(3):195-225.,2020. Apperly IA, Samson D, Chiavarino C, Humphreys GW. Frontal and temporo-parietal lobe contributions to theory of mind: neuropsychological evidence from a false-belief task with reduced language and executive demands. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16(10):1773-84.,2323. Humphreys GW, Bedford J. The relations between joint action and theory of mind: a neuropsychological analysis. Exp Brain Res. 2011;211(34):357-69.,2727. Mah L, Arnold MC, Grafman J. Impairment of social perception associated with lesions of the prefrontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(7):1247-55.,2828. Xi C, Zhu Y, Zhu C, Song D, Wang Y, Wang K. Deficit of theory of mind after temporal lobe cerebral infarction. Behav Brain Funct. 2013;9:15. obtained scores lower than 60% and were considered with poor methodological quality and fourteen33. Hamilton J, Radlak B, Morris PG, Phillips LH. Theory of Mind and Executive Functioning Following Stroke. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2017;32(5):507-18.,44. Pluta A, Gawron N, Sobańska M, Wójcik AD, Łojek E. The nature of the relationship between neurocognition and theory of mind impairments in stroke patients. Neuropsychology. 2017;31(6):666-81.,77. Tompkins CA, Scharp VL, Fassbinder W, Meigh KM, Armstrong EM. A different story on “theory of mind” deficit in adults with right hemisphere brain damage. Aphasiology. 2008;22(1):42-61.,88. Yeh ZT, Tsai CF. Impairment on theory of mind and empathy in patients with stroke. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2014;68(8):612-20.,1313. Siegal M, Carrington J, Radel M. Theory of mind and pragmatic understanding following right hemisphere damage. Brain Lang. 1996;53(1): 40-50.,1515. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Tomer R, Berger BD, Goldsher DMD, Aharon-Peretz JMD. Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005;18(1):55-67.

16. Besharati S, Forkel SJ, Kopelman M, Solms M, Jenkinson PM, Fotopoulou A, et al. Mentalizing the body: spatial and social cognition in anosognosia for hemiplegia. Brain. 2016;139(3):971-85.

17. Happé F, Brownwll H, Winner E. Acquired theory of mind impairments following stroke. Cognition. 1999;70(3):211-40.
-1818. Surian L, Siegal M. Sources of performance on theory of mind tasks in right hemisphere-damaged patients. Brain Lang. 2001;78(2):224-32.,2121. Martin I, Mcdonald S. That can't be right! What causes pragmatic language impairment following right hemisphere damage? Brain Impairment. 2006;7(3):202-11.,2222. Weed E, McGregor W, Nielsen JF, Roepstorff A, Frith U. Theory of Mind in adults with right hemisphere damage: What's the story? Brain Lang. 2010;113(2):65-72.,2424. Wilkos E, Brown TJ, Slawinska K, Kucharska KA. Social cognitive and neurocognitive deficits in inpatients with unilateral thalamic lesions – Pilot study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2015;11:1031-8.

25. Winner E, Brownell H, Happé F, Blum A, Pincus D. Distinguishing lies from jokes: Theory of mind deficits and discourse interpretation in right hemisphere brain-damaged patients. Brain Lang. 1998;62(1):89-106.
-2626. Champagne-Lavau M, Joanette Y. Pragmatics, theory of mind and executive functions after a right-hemisphere lesion: Different patterns of deficits. J Neurolinguistics. 2009;22(5):413-26. studies obtained a score of 60% to 79% and were classified with moderate quality. None of the studies evaluated reached a score above 80% and therefore no study was considered with strong methodological quality. The main weaknesses identified according to STROBE were: non-identification of the study's design in the title or abstract (n = 20), absence of flow diagram (n = 19), absence of explanation of how the study size was arrived at (n = 7), absence of explain how missing data were addressed (n = 19) and the description of sensitivity analyses (n = 17).

Discussion

The studies included in the present review generally indicate that stroke survivors have a poorer performance on ToM tasks. Moreover, the association between a lesion in the right hemisphere and the performance on these tasks was a predominant characteristic of this review.

In general, patients with lesion in the right hemisphere showed lower performance on ToM tasks compared to those with lesion in the left hemisphere. Furthermore, when compared to healthy individuals, patients with lesion on the right hemisphere present impairment on the performance on ToM tasks. Likewise, patients with lesions in specific regions, such as temporal lobe, prefrontal cortex, posterior parietal cortex and temporoparietal junction, presented deficit in the performance on ToM tasks in relation to the healthy individuals.

Studies that evaluated the cerebral location of the stroke event found heterogeneous results regarding the lateralization of the function of ToM. Stroke survivors with the right hemisphere affected exhibited greater impairment on tasks that evaluate ToM than those with lesions in the left hemisphere33. Hamilton J, Radlak B, Morris PG, Phillips LH. Theory of Mind and Executive Functioning Following Stroke. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2017;32(5):507-18.,1313. Siegal M, Carrington J, Radel M. Theory of mind and pragmatic understanding following right hemisphere damage. Brain Lang. 1996;53(1): 40-50.,1717. Happé F, Brownwll H, Winner E. Acquired theory of mind impairments following stroke. Cognition. 1999;70(3):211-40. or healthy individuals1919. Griffin R, Friedman O, Ween J, Winner E, Happé F, Brownell H. Theory of mind and the right cerebral hemisphere: Refining the scope of impairment. Laterality. 2006;11(3):195-225.,2121. Martin I, Mcdonald S. That can't be right! What causes pragmatic language impairment following right hemisphere damage? Brain Impairment. 2006;7(3):202-11.,2222. Weed E, McGregor W, Nielsen JF, Roepstorff A, Frith U. Theory of Mind in adults with right hemisphere damage: What's the story? Brain Lang. 2010;113(2):65-72.,2525. Winner E, Brownell H, Happé F, Blum A, Pincus D. Distinguishing lies from jokes: Theory of mind deficits and discourse interpretation in right hemisphere brain-damaged patients. Brain Lang. 1998;62(1):89-106.. In contrast, Pluta et al.44. Pluta A, Gawron N, Sobańska M, Wójcik AD, Łojek E. The nature of the relationship between neurocognition and theory of mind impairments in stroke patients. Neuropsychology. 2017;31(6):666-81. compared the performance on ToM tasks among individuals with right hemisphere lesions, left hemisphere lesions and bilateral lesions and found that stroke survivors exhibit impairment on these tasks, but found no difference with regard to the site of the lesion. Likewise, Surian and Siegal1414. Roca M, Gleichgerrcht E, Abáñez A, Torralva T, Manes F. Cerebellar stroke impairs executive functions but not theory of mind. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2013;25(1):E48-9. found no significant difference in the performance on the False Belief and True Belief test between two groups separated into right hemisphere and left hemisphere lesions.

Tompkins et al.77. Tompkins CA, Scharp VL, Fassbinder W, Meigh KM, Armstrong EM. A different story on “theory of mind” deficit in adults with right hemisphere brain damage. Aphasiology. 2008;22(1):42-61. also found no difference on ToM tasks between patients with a right hemisphere lesion and a control group. Happé et al.1717. Happé F, Brownwll H, Winner E. Acquired theory of mind impairments following stroke. Cognition. 1999;70(3):211-40. found that eight out of 14 patients in the group with right hemisphere lesions had a poorer performance on ToM tasks, whereas only two out of 21 patients with left hemisphere lesions exhibited a compromised ability on these tasks in the study by Tompkins et al.77. Tompkins CA, Scharp VL, Fassbinder W, Meigh KM, Armstrong EM. A different story on “theory of mind” deficit in adults with right hemisphere brain damage. Aphasiology. 2008;22(1):42-61., indicating greater impairment on ToM tasks following a stroke in the right hemisphere. However, it is important to stress the methodological difference between the studies, as different tests were used to assess ToM77. Tompkins CA, Scharp VL, Fassbinder W, Meigh KM, Armstrong EM. A different story on “theory of mind” deficit in adults with right hemisphere brain damage. Aphasiology. 2008;22(1):42-61.. According to Happé et al.1717. Happé F, Brownwll H, Winner E. Acquired theory of mind impairments following stroke. Cognition. 1999;70(3):211-40. the association between right hemisphere lesions and performance on ToM tasks may stem from the characteristics of the lesion; stroke survivors with the right hemisphere affected may have more or more severe lesions in comparison to those with the left hemisphere affected.

In the study by Roca et al.1414. Roca M, Gleichgerrcht E, Abáñez A, Torralva T, Manes F. Cerebellar stroke impairs executive functions but not theory of mind. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2013;25(1):E48-9. patients with cerebellar strokes demonstrated no impairment on the Faux Pas Detection task. According to the authors, these tasks require other functions that may be related to the cerebellum, such as language, which may have exerted an influence on the results.

In a study comparing abilities on pragmatic ToM tasks and executive functions in stroke survivors with right hemisphere lesions and healthy individuals, Champagne-Lavau et al.2626. Champagne-Lavau M, Joanette Y. Pragmatics, theory of mind and executive functions after a right-hemisphere lesion: Different patterns of deficits. J Neurolinguistics. 2009;22(5):413-26. found that the ability to understand pragmatic aspects of language is closely associated with the ability to make inferences regarding the intentions of others. The researchers also found an association between an impaired ToM and executive dysfunction in subgroups of individuals with damage in the right hemisphere.

Shamay-Tsoory et al.1515. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Tomer R, Berger BD, Goldsher DMD, Aharon-Peretz JMD. Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005;18(1):55-67. evaluated patients with lesions in the prefrontal cortex, posterior lesions and participants without lesions and found that those with prefrontal lesions, specifically ventromedial prefrontal lesions, exhibited impairment on ToM tasks. Likewise, Mah et al.2727. Mah L, Arnold MC, Grafman J. Impairment of social perception associated with lesions of the prefrontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(7):1247-55. compared patients with lesions in the prefrontal cortex to healthy volunteers and found that the patients with lesions, especially in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, demonstrated impaired abilities regarding ToM. These findings support that notion that the prefrontal cortex is associated with ToM skills and that the ventromedial prefrontal cortex is essential to the regulation of emotions2929. Leopold A, Krueger F, dal Monte O, Pardini M, Pulaski SJ, Solomon J, et al. Damage to the left ventromedial prefrontal cortex impacts affective theory of mind. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2011;7(8):871-80..

According to Shamay-Tsoory et al.1515. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Tomer R, Berger BD, Goldsher DMD, Aharon-Peretz JMD. Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005;18(1):55-67., the difference between sites and the asymmetry of the lesions may influence the results of ToM tasks due to the involvement of different cognitive processes. Stroke is associated with greater impairment in social cognition assessments that require other cognitive processes, such as working memory, language, executive function and attention, and which may also be affected post-stroke33. Hamilton J, Radlak B, Morris PG, Phillips LH. Theory of Mind and Executive Functioning Following Stroke. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2017;32(5):507-18.. In addition, it is expected that the impairment after stroke is greater for more complex second-order tasks compared to the first order tasks of the ToM and for the classification of emotions2626. Champagne-Lavau M, Joanette Y. Pragmatics, theory of mind and executive functions after a right-hemisphere lesion: Different patterns of deficits. J Neurolinguistics. 2009;22(5):413-26.. Therefore, the choice of the task and lesion site may influence the results of the studies.

It is known that patients with lesion in the right hemisphere seem to perform worse in the ability to attribute intentional second-order states compared to first-order mental states1919. Griffin R, Friedman O, Ween J, Winner E, Happé F, Brownell H. Theory of mind and the right cerebral hemisphere: Refining the scope of impairment. Laterality. 2006;11(3):195-225.,2525. Winner E, Brownell H, Happé F, Blum A, Pincus D. Distinguishing lies from jokes: Theory of mind deficits and discourse interpretation in right hemisphere brain-damaged patients. Brain Lang. 1998;62(1):89-106.. There is also evidence that these tasks may not allow the distinction of effects in patients with stroke and healthy controls77. Tompkins CA, Scharp VL, Fassbinder W, Meigh KM, Armstrong EM. A different story on “theory of mind” deficit in adults with right hemisphere brain damage. Aphasiology. 2008;22(1):42-61.. For example, in the study by Griffin et al.1919. Griffin R, Friedman O, Ween J, Winner E, Happé F, Brownell H. Theory of mind and the right cerebral hemisphere: Refining the scope of impairment. Laterality. 2006;11(3):195-225., RHL patients differed from non-brain-damaged controls in the ability to attribute second order intentional states, however, these authors were not found differences between groups with regard to attribute first order intentional states.

Another question considered as an influencing factor that can influence in the performance in the of ToM tasks is the tool used. Evidence indicates that this task RMET can have biased responses and, consequently, limit its psychometric validity3030. Johnston L, Miles L, McKinlay A. A critical review of the eyes test as a measure of social‐cognitive impairment. Australian J Psychol. 2008;60(3):135-41.. The study by Hamilton et al.33. Hamilton J, Radlak B, Morris PG, Phillips LH. Theory of Mind and Executive Functioning Following Stroke. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2017;32(5):507-18. used the RMET task to assess the difference between the performance of in ToM in of patients with RHL, LHL and healthy controls, and the results showed that stroke participants with RHL were significantly more impaired on the RMET than those with LHL, who performed similarly to healthy controls. The authors present as a limitation the use of RMET only for the assessment of ToM, since the test involves the recognition of complex emotions and, therefore, evaluates this ability restrictively.

Concerning to evaluation, the articles included in this review used a wide variety of tools to evaluate ToM. In overall, the studies found indicate that ToM evaluation has as purposes: (i) detect mistakes and contextual information using, for instance, false beliefs tasks44. Pluta A, Gawron N, Sobańska M, Wójcik AD, Łojek E. The nature of the relationship between neurocognition and theory of mind impairments in stroke patients. Neuropsychology. 2017;31(6):666-81.,77. Tompkins CA, Scharp VL, Fassbinder W, Meigh KM, Armstrong EM. A different story on “theory of mind” deficit in adults with right hemisphere brain damage. Aphasiology. 2008;22(1):42-61.,1313. Siegal M, Carrington J, Radel M. Theory of mind and pragmatic understanding following right hemisphere damage. Brain Lang. 1996;53(1): 40-50.,1515. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Tomer R, Berger BD, Goldsher DMD, Aharon-Peretz JMD. Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005;18(1):55-67.

16. Besharati S, Forkel SJ, Kopelman M, Solms M, Jenkinson PM, Fotopoulou A, et al. Mentalizing the body: spatial and social cognition in anosognosia for hemiplegia. Brain. 2016;139(3):971-85.

17. Happé F, Brownwll H, Winner E. Acquired theory of mind impairments following stroke. Cognition. 1999;70(3):211-40.

18. Surian L, Siegal M. Sources of performance on theory of mind tasks in right hemisphere-damaged patients. Brain Lang. 2001;78(2):224-32.

19. Griffin R, Friedman O, Ween J, Winner E, Happé F, Brownell H. Theory of mind and the right cerebral hemisphere: Refining the scope of impairment. Laterality. 2006;11(3):195-225.
-2020. Apperly IA, Samson D, Chiavarino C, Humphreys GW. Frontal and temporo-parietal lobe contributions to theory of mind: neuropsychological evidence from a false-belief task with reduced language and executive demands. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16(10):1773-84.,2323. Humphreys GW, Bedford J. The relations between joint action and theory of mind: a neuropsychological analysis. Exp Brain Res. 2011;211(34):357-69.,2525. Winner E, Brownell H, Happé F, Blum A, Pincus D. Distinguishing lies from jokes: Theory of mind deficits and discourse interpretation in right hemisphere brain-damaged patients. Brain Lang. 1998;62(1):89-106.

26. Champagne-Lavau M, Joanette Y. Pragmatics, theory of mind and executive functions after a right-hemisphere lesion: Different patterns of deficits. J Neurolinguistics. 2009;22(5):413-26.
-2727. Mah L, Arnold MC, Grafman J. Impairment of social perception associated with lesions of the prefrontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(7):1247-55. e The Faux Pas Detection test88. Yeh ZT, Tsai CF. Impairment on theory of mind and empathy in patients with stroke. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2014;68(8):612-20.,1414. Roca M, Gleichgerrcht E, Abáñez A, Torralva T, Manes F. Cerebellar stroke impairs executive functions but not theory of mind. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2013;25(1):E48-9.,1515. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Tomer R, Berger BD, Goldsher DMD, Aharon-Peretz JMD. Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005;18(1):55-67.,2828. Xi C, Zhu Y, Zhu C, Song D, Wang Y, Wang K. Deficit of theory of mind after temporal lobe cerebral infarction. Behav Brain Funct. 2013;9:15.; (ii) analyze the pragmatic understanding discourse2121. Martin I, Mcdonald S. That can't be right! What causes pragmatic language impairment following right hemisphere damage? Brain Impairment. 2006;7(3):202-11.; (iii) verify the non-verbal contents abstraction through The Reading the Mind in the Eyes test33. Hamilton J, Radlak B, Morris PG, Phillips LH. Theory of Mind and Executive Functioning Following Stroke. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2017;32(5):507-18.,2424. Wilkos E, Brown TJ, Slawinska K, Kucharska KA. Social cognitive and neurocognitive deficits in inpatients with unilateral thalamic lesions – Pilot study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2015;11:1031-8.,2828. Xi C, Zhu Y, Zhu C, Song D, Wang Y, Wang K. Deficit of theory of mind after temporal lobe cerebral infarction. Behav Brain Funct. 2013;9:15. and tasks composed of geometric elements2222. Weed E, McGregor W, Nielsen JF, Roepstorff A, Frith U. Theory of Mind in adults with right hemisphere damage: What's the story? Brain Lang. 2010;113(2):65-72.; in addition to studies that integrate these different evaluation methods1515. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Tomer R, Berger BD, Goldsher DMD, Aharon-Peretz JMD. Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005;18(1):55-67.,2828. Xi C, Zhu Y, Zhu C, Song D, Wang Y, Wang K. Deficit of theory of mind after temporal lobe cerebral infarction. Behav Brain Funct. 2013;9:15.. The variability of measures of ToM assessment used by the selected studies may be considered as limitation, as it made difficult the systematization and the comparison between the results, invalidating the meta-analysis accomplishment.

With regard to the time post stroke event, none of the articles selected for the present review discussed the influence of this variable on the results. Therefore, further studies are needed to investigate this aspect. Another topic that could be explored by new studies is the frequency of people post stroke with significant damage on ToM ability.

The limitation of the present review resides in the inconsistency of some of the data extracted for the construction of the table in the results section, as some information considered pertinent for a systematic evaluation of this topic was missing. Furthermore, the absence of realization of cross-reference search, based on the descriptors used, may have made it difficult to refinement the articles, limiting the findings found. Finally, the general low quality of the articles analyzed can also be considered as a limitation.

This review has important clinical implications, since an impaired ability of ToM can affect the process of capturing and transmitting information through social interaction. Therefore, accurate identification of impaired ToM ability based on clinical evaluation is essential to indicate the most appropriate treatment.

References

  • 1
    Premack D, Woodruff G. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci. 1978;1(4):515-26.
  • 2
    Saxe R, Powell LJ. It's the thought that counts: specific brain regions for one component of theory of mind. Psychol Sci. 2006;17(8):692-9.
  • 3
    Hamilton J, Radlak B, Morris PG, Phillips LH. Theory of Mind and Executive Functioning Following Stroke. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2017;32(5):507-18.
  • 4
    Pluta A, Gawron N, Sobańska M, Wójcik AD, Łojek E. The nature of the relationship between neurocognition and theory of mind impairments in stroke patients. Neuropsychology. 2017;31(6):666-81.
  • 5
    Schurz M, Radua J, Aichhorn M, Richlan F, Perner J. Fractionating theory of mind: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;42:9-34.
  • 6
    Mahy CEV, Moses LJ, Pfeifer JH. How and where: Theory-of-mind in the brain. Develop Cogn Neurosci. 2014;9:68-81.
  • 7
    Tompkins CA, Scharp VL, Fassbinder W, Meigh KM, Armstrong EM. A different story on “theory of mind” deficit in adults with right hemisphere brain damage. Aphasiology. 2008;22(1):42-61.
  • 8
    Yeh ZT, Tsai CF. Impairment on theory of mind and empathy in patients with stroke. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2014;68(8):612-20.
  • 9
    Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DGA. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: The PRISMA statement. Ann Inter Med. 2009;151(4):264-9.
  • 10
    Vandenbroucke JP, Von Elm E, Altman DG, Gøtzsche PC, Mulrow CD, Pocock SJ, et al. Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE): explanation and elaboration. Ann Intern Med. 2007;147(8):163.
  • 11
    Von Elm E, Altman DG, Egger M, Pocock SJ, Gøtzsche PC, Vandenbroucke JP. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. Ann Intern Med. 2007;147(8):573-7.
  • 12
    de Paula Eduardo JAF, de Rezende MG, Menezes PR, Del-Ben CM. Preterm birth as a risk factor for postpartum depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2019;259:392-403.
  • 13
    Siegal M, Carrington J, Radel M. Theory of mind and pragmatic understanding following right hemisphere damage. Brain Lang. 1996;53(1): 40-50.
  • 14
    Roca M, Gleichgerrcht E, Abáñez A, Torralva T, Manes F. Cerebellar stroke impairs executive functions but not theory of mind. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2013;25(1):E48-9.
  • 15
    Shamay-Tsoory SG, Tomer R, Berger BD, Goldsher DMD, Aharon-Peretz JMD. Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005;18(1):55-67.
  • 16
    Besharati S, Forkel SJ, Kopelman M, Solms M, Jenkinson PM, Fotopoulou A, et al. Mentalizing the body: spatial and social cognition in anosognosia for hemiplegia. Brain. 2016;139(3):971-85.
  • 17
    Happé F, Brownwll H, Winner E. Acquired theory of mind impairments following stroke. Cognition. 1999;70(3):211-40.
  • 18
    Surian L, Siegal M. Sources of performance on theory of mind tasks in right hemisphere-damaged patients. Brain Lang. 2001;78(2):224-32.
  • 19
    Griffin R, Friedman O, Ween J, Winner E, Happé F, Brownell H. Theory of mind and the right cerebral hemisphere: Refining the scope of impairment. Laterality. 2006;11(3):195-225.
  • 20
    Apperly IA, Samson D, Chiavarino C, Humphreys GW. Frontal and temporo-parietal lobe contributions to theory of mind: neuropsychological evidence from a false-belief task with reduced language and executive demands. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16(10):1773-84.
  • 21
    Martin I, Mcdonald S. That can't be right! What causes pragmatic language impairment following right hemisphere damage? Brain Impairment. 2006;7(3):202-11.
  • 22
    Weed E, McGregor W, Nielsen JF, Roepstorff A, Frith U. Theory of Mind in adults with right hemisphere damage: What's the story? Brain Lang. 2010;113(2):65-72.
  • 23
    Humphreys GW, Bedford J. The relations between joint action and theory of mind: a neuropsychological analysis. Exp Brain Res. 2011;211(34):357-69.
  • 24
    Wilkos E, Brown TJ, Slawinska K, Kucharska KA. Social cognitive and neurocognitive deficits in inpatients with unilateral thalamic lesions – Pilot study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2015;11:1031-8.
  • 25
    Winner E, Brownell H, Happé F, Blum A, Pincus D. Distinguishing lies from jokes: Theory of mind deficits and discourse interpretation in right hemisphere brain-damaged patients. Brain Lang. 1998;62(1):89-106.
  • 26
    Champagne-Lavau M, Joanette Y. Pragmatics, theory of mind and executive functions after a right-hemisphere lesion: Different patterns of deficits. J Neurolinguistics. 2009;22(5):413-26.
  • 27
    Mah L, Arnold MC, Grafman J. Impairment of social perception associated with lesions of the prefrontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(7):1247-55.
  • 28
    Xi C, Zhu Y, Zhu C, Song D, Wang Y, Wang K. Deficit of theory of mind after temporal lobe cerebral infarction. Behav Brain Funct. 2013;9:15.
  • 29
    Leopold A, Krueger F, dal Monte O, Pardini M, Pulaski SJ, Solomon J, et al. Damage to the left ventromedial prefrontal cortex impacts affective theory of mind. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2011;7(8):871-80.
  • 30
    Johnston L, Miles L, McKinlay A. A critical review of the eyes test as a measure of social‐cognitive impairment. Australian J Psychol. 2008;60(3):135-41.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    12 Mar 2021
  • Date of issue
    Sep-Oct 2020

History

  • Received
    30 May 2019
  • Accepted
    29 June 2020
Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de São Paulo Rua Ovídio Pires de Campos, 785 , 05403-010 São Paulo SP Brasil, Tel./Fax: +55 11 2661-8011 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: archives@usp.br