Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Time orientation and executive functions in the prediction of mortality in the elderly: Epidoso study

Abstracts

OBJECTIVE: To analyze the predictive ability of a functional cognitive index of mortality in the elderly. METHODS: Cohort study performed with 1,667 elderly individuals aged more than 65 years and living in the city of São Paulo, Southeastern Brazil, between 1991 and 2001. Functional cognitive index was constructed from time orientation and executive functions (going shopping and taking medication), controlled by sociodemographic variables, life habits, morbidity, self-perception of health, hospitalization, edentulism and social support. Deaths occurred during this period were analyzed with family members in home interviews, notary public offices and records from the Fundação Seade (State System of Data Analysis Foundation), until 2003. Crude and adjusted relative risks were calculated with their respective 95% confidence intervals, using bivariate and multiple analysis with Poisson regression and p<0.05. RESULTS: In the final multivariate model, the following independent risk factors were identified by the index: partial loss of time orientation or executive functions (RR=1.37; 95% CI: 1.03;1.83); total loss of orientation and partial loss of functions (RR=1.71; 95% CI: 1.24;2.37); partial loss of orientation and total loss of functions (RR=1.76; 95% CI: 1.35;2.28); and total loss of orientation and functions (RR=1.64; 95% CI: 1.30;2.06). As regards health conditions, the following were observed: hospitalization (RR=1.45; 95% CI: 1.22;1.73); diabetes (RR=1.20; 95% CI: 1.00;1.44); and total edentulism (RR=1.34; 95% CI: 1.09;1.66). Monthly contact with relatives was identified as a protective factor (RR=0.83; 95% CI: 0.69;1.00). CONCLUSIONS: The Functional Cognitive Index can help clinicians and health planners to make decisions on strategies for follow-up and prevention of treatable causes of cognitive deficit and functional loss to reduce mortality in the elderly.

Aged; Cognition; Time Perception; Activities of Daily Living; Cohort Studies


OBJETIVO: Analisar a capacidade preditiva de índice cognitivo funcional para mortalidade entre idosos. MÉTODOS: Estudo de coorte realizado com 1.667 idosos acima de 65 anos residentes no município de São Paulo, SP, no período 1991-2001. O índice cognitivo funcional foi construído a partir da orientação temporal e funções executivas (fazer compras e tomar medicação), controlado por variáveis sociodemográficas, hábitos de vida, morbidade, autopercepção de saúde, internação, edentulismo e suporte social. Os óbitos ocorridos no período foram investigados com familiares em entrevistas domiciliares, em cartórios e registros da Fundação Seade (até 2003). Foram calculados riscos relativos brutos e ajustados com respectivos intervalos com 95% de confiança por meio de análise bivariada e múltipla com regressão de Poisson, adotando-se p<0,05. RESULTADOS: No modelo multivariado final os fatores de risco independentes identificados pelo índice foram: perda parcial da orientação temporal ou funções executivas (RR=1,37; IC 95%: 1,03;1,83); perda total da orientação e parcial das funções (RR=1,71; IC 95%: 1,24;2,37); perda parcial da orientação e total das funções (RR=1,76; IC 95%: 1,35;2,28); perda total da orientação e das funções (RR=1,64; IC 95%: 1,30;2,06), Quanto às condições de saúde: internação (RR=1,45; IC 95%: 1,22;1,73); diabetes (RR=1,20; IC 95%: 1,00;1,44); edentulismo total (RR=1,34; IC 95%: 1,09;1,66). Relacionamento mensal com parentes foi identificado como fator protetor (RR=0,83; IC 95%: 0,69;1,00). CONCLUSÕES: O Índice Cognitivo Funcional pode auxiliar clínicos e planejadores em decisões sobre estratégias de seguimento e prevenção de causas tratáveis de déficit cognitivo e perda funcional para diminuir a mortalidade entre os idosos.

Idoso; Cognição; Percepção do Tempo; Atividades Cotidianas; Estudos de Coortes


OBJETIVO: Analizar la capacidad predictiva del índice cognitivo funcional para mortalidad entre ancianos. MÉTODOS: Estudio de cohorte realizado con 1.667 ancianos encima de 65 años residentes en el municipio de São Paulo, Sureste de Brasil, en el período 1991-2001. El índice cognitivo funcional fue construido a partir de la orientación temporal y funciones ejecutivas (hacer compras y tomar medicamento), controlado por variables sociodemográficas, hábitos de vida, morbilidad, autopercepción de salud, internación, edentulismo y soporte social. Los óbitos ocurridos en el período fueron investigados con familiares en entrevistas domiciliares, en oficinas del notario y registros de la Fundación Seade (hasta 2003). Fueron calculados riesgos relativos brutos y ajustados con respectivos intervalos con 95% de confianza por medio de análisis bivariado y múltiple con regresión de Poisson, adoptándose p<0,05. RESULTADOS: En el modelo multivariado final los factores de riesgo independientes identificados por el índice fueron: pérdida parcial de la orientación temporal o funciones ejecutivas (RR=1,37; IC 95%: 1,03;1,83); pérdida total de la orientación y parcial de las funciones (RR=1,71; IC 95%: 1,24;2,37); pérdida parcial de la orientación y total de las funciones (RR=1,76; IC 95%: 1,35;2,28); pérdida total de la orientación y de las funciones (RR=1,64; IC 95%: 1,30;2,06). Con relación a las condiciones de salud: internación (RR=1,45; IC 95%:1,22;1,73); diabetes (RR=1,20; IC 95%: 1,00;1,44); edentulismo total (RR=1,34; IC 95%: 1,09;1,66). Relacionamiento mensual con parientes fue identificado como factor protector (RR=0,83; IC 95%: 0,69;1,00). CONCLUSIONES: El Índice Cognitivo Funcional puede auxiliar clínicos y planificadores en decisiones sobre estrategias de seguimiento y prevención de causas tratables de déficit cognitivo y pérdida funcional para disminuir la mortalidad entre los ancianos.


ORIGINAL ARTICLES

IPrograma de pós-graduação em Informática em Saúde. Departamento de Informática em Saúde. Universidade Federal de São Paulo. São Paulo, SP, Brasil

IIPrograma de Pós-Graduação em Saúde Coletiva. Centro de Ciências da Saúde. Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina. Florianópolis, SC, Brasil

IIIPrograma de Pós-Graduação em Saúde Coletiva. Departamento de Medicina Preventiva. Universidade Federal de São Paulo. São Paulo, SP, Brasil

Correspondence

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To analyze the predictive ability of a functional cognitive index of mortality in the elderly.

METHODS: Cohort study performed with 1,667 elderly individuals aged more than 65 years and living in the city of São Paulo, Southeastern Brazil, between 1991 and 2001. Functional cognitive index was constructed from time orientation and executive functions (going shopping and taking medication), controlled by sociodemographic variables, life habits, morbidity, self-perception of health, hospitalization, edentulism and social support. Deaths occurred during this period were analyzed with family members in home interviews, notary public offices and records from the Fundação Seade (State System of Data Analysis Foundation), until 2003. Crude and adjusted relative risks were calculated with their respective 95% confidence intervals, using bivariate and multiple analysis with Poisson regression and p<0.05.

RESULTS: In the final multivariate model, the following independent risk factors were identified by the index: partial loss of time orientation or executive functions (RR=1.37; 95% CI: 1.03;1.83); total loss of orientation and partial loss of functions (RR=1.71; 95% CI: 1.24;2.37); partial loss of orientation and total loss of functions (RR=1.76; 95% CI: 1.35;2.28); and total loss of orientation and functions (RR=1.64; 95% CI: 1.30;2.06). As regards health conditions, the following were observed: hospitalization (RR=1.45; 95% CI: 1.22;1.73); diabetes (RR=1.20; 95% CI: 1.00;1.44); and total edentulism (RR=1.34; 95% CI: 1.09;1.66). Monthly contact with relatives was identified as a protective factor (RR=0.83; 95% CI: 0.69;1.00).

CONCLUSIONS: The Functional Cognitive Index can help clinicians and health planners to make decisions on strategies for follow-up and prevention of treatable causes of cognitive deficit and functional loss to reduce mortality in the elderly.

Descriptors: Aged. Cognition. Time Perception. Activities of Daily Living. Cohort Studies.

INTRODUCTION

The association among cognitive state, functional capacity and mortality in the elderly is well known in the literature.1,2 Good cognitive and functional conditions reduce the risk of death, despite the presence of comorbities.3,18-20 The Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and the Questionário Brasileiro de Avaliação Funcional Multidimensional (BOMFAQ/OARS - Brazilian OARS Multidimensional Functional Assessment Questionnaire), adapted from the Older Americans Resources and Services questionnaire, both validated in Brazil, are indices used to measure cognitive state and functional capacity in the elderly, respectively. Several studies on elderly health2,4,15,20,21 have used these parameters to distinguish determining factors for healthy aging, once they are indirect, although sensitive, measures of risk of death.

Cognitive impairments are very prevalent in the elderly, although usually having a treatable cause. Likewise, transitions from poorer to better states of functional capacity are frequent in this population.7 This emphasizes the choice for these changeable factors when assessing risk of death in the elderly.

Executive functions are the most complex components of functional capacity, being responsible for organizing information, setting objectives, maintaining control of changeable environmental circumstances, foreseeing possibilities and changing objectives and plans in a coherent way. Brain regions that enable executive functions are the last to mature, usually not before early adulthood. During their development, executive functions become progressively more connected to domains of knowledge about facts, numbers, words and images and are applied to behavior linked to goals, management, learning and critical awareness.11 Both age and the presence of chronic diseases are associated with possible losses of executive functions.22,23 On the other hand, these functions enable learning and organization throughout life. Assessment of executive functions can be done independently of the educational level.

Orientation to time is also an early marker to access cognitive impairments with high specificity and lower dependence on the level of education than other tasks, such as attention and calculation, spatial orientation, language and drawing; thus providing stable scoring24,25 that can be compared to executive functions.

The objective of present study was to analyze the ability of a functional cognitive index to predict mortality in the elderly.

METHODS

Data from the study entitled "Epidemiologia do Idoso" (Epidoso - Elderly Epidemiology),ª performed in a residential area of the city of São Paulo, Southeastern Brazil, were analyzed in this article. Epidoso was the first longitudinal study in Latin America that aimed to identify factors associated with healthy aging and risk factors of mortality and functional incapacity in community-dwelling elderly.18-20 Participants were followed by qualified interviewers for ten years, in four waves of household surveys. The first survey was conducted in 1991/1992, the second in 1994/1995, the third in 1998/1999, and the fourth in 2000/2001.

The geographical area selected was the district known as "Saúde", one of the 55 districts of the city of São Paulo. Participants were selected using the census conducted in 52 census tracts of this district, which identified all residents aged more than 65 years as eligible for this study, of which 1,667 were interviewed in 1991-1992. Home interviews followed a structured questionnaire, adapted from the BOMFAQ/OARS, previously used in cross-sectional studies with elderly residents of São Paulo.18-20 The instrument collected information about socioeconomic and demographic characteristics, informal support (not provided by the government or specialized institutions), level of independence in daily life activities (functional capacity), chronic diseases, mental health, cognitive state (MMSE) and self-perception of health. The period analyzed was between the first survey (1992) and the end of the collection for identification of deaths. Surveys were conducted in notary public offices and in the online archives of the Fundação Seade (São Paulo State Data Analysis System Foundation) between 2000 and 2003.

In the present analysis, data on mortality were obtained by interviewers in the other three successive waves of household surveys, in addition to a telephone contact and review of death certificates from notary public offices until 2000. Moreover, from this year until 2003, EPIDOSO mortality data were obtained from the Fundação Seade archives. A total of 544 deaths were identified (32.6% from the 1991/1992 initial cohort) until 2003. It was not possible to obtain information about the vital status of 134 elderly individuals (8.0%), who were thus excluded from the analysis.

Variables potentially associated with mortality were classified as: sociodemographic, self-perception of health, morbidity, life styles or habits, social support and Functional Cognitive Index (FCI) of orientation to time and executive functions.

The FCI was conceived to integrate the concepts of cognition and functional capacity. It is of simple execution and capable to organize healthcare into a hierarchy, based on the risk of death of each group identified. The FCI was obtained from five questions about orientation to time from the MMSE and two questions from the BOMFAQ/OARS that fit into the concept of executive functions and which were representative of autonomy and independence (going shopping and taking medication at the right time and without help).

As regards the "functional capacity" variable, which comprised the FCI, questions about more complex executive-function-related instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) were selected.

This selection was also based on a model of loss of functional capacity, proposed by Ramos,18 and on a new proposal of a hierarchy of cognitive state and functional capacity, based on growing levels of complexity proposed by Xavier,26 in addition to geriatric criteria of autonomy and independence.

Selection of the "orientation to time" variable (capacity to be oriented in time as measured by calendars)24,25 to be included in the FCI was made from the analysis of multiple discriminating capacity of MMSE sub-items, using a response variable with Bernoulli group distribution (1/10).8 To achieve this, data from the first (1991-1992) and second surveys (1993-1994) of the Estudo Epidoso were used. The following MMSE items were tested: orientation to time, spatial orientation, recall, calculation, language and complex commands.

Orientation to time, spatial orientation and serial sevens (calculation) were categorized as: 4/5 correct responses, 2/3 correct responses, 0/1 correct responses. Registration, language and complex commands were categorized as: was able to answer - yes/no. Recall memory was categorized as: was able to recall three objects, two objects, one object or no objects.

The following statistical parameters were used: correctly predicted percentage, sensitivity and specificity of model. First, a model with all MMSE items was designed and its discriminating power verified. Next, a model of each item was designed and individual parameters of each item were compared to the parameters of the complete model with all items, seeking to identify the item(s) that best discriminated the total MMSE score.

MMSE score equal to or below 24 points was considered as cognitive loss.20 The discriminating capacity was tested in terms of cognitive loss in two moments: during the same survey (1991-1992) and two years later (1993-1994).

As regards the 15 daily life activities assessed by the BOMFAQ/OARS, the following basic non-instrumental activities, not associated with executive functions, were excluded: lying in/getting up from bed, eating, combing hair, walking on a level surface, taking a shower, getting dressed, going to the bathroom in time, going up (one flight) of stairs, and clipping nails. After the analysis of all variables associated with executive functions, those less related to gender were selected: preparing meals is an instrumental daily life activity more associated with women, especially elderly ones, once activities such as house cleaning and taking public transportation can be strongly influenced by physical and sensory limitations, in addition to economic ones. Finally, two executive functions were selected, representative of an independent and autonomous life in the community and also able to be simulated or compensated in digital environments with the purpose of rehabilitation: taking medication at the right time and going shopping.

The discriminating capacity of the "going shopping" and "taking medication at the right time" variables (executive functions) was tested by BOMFAQ/OARS18 for functional loss of up to two instrumental daily life activities in the same survey (1991-1992) and two years later (1993-1994).

From this conceptual basis, a preliminary FCI with nine possibilities was constructed from the finest (00) to the worst (22) functional and cognitive state (Table 1). Some preliminary FCI categories did not show significant association with risk of death (categories "20", "10" and "02"). These categories were recombined with category "01", which shared the fact that at least one of their components (orientation to time or the ability to go shopping and taking medication alone) was preserved (represented by "0"). Thus, the FCI was created with six categories, which, when tested, were significantly associated with risk of death in all categories.

After the discriminating analysis and use of geriatric criteria to construct the FCI, bivariate and multivariate analyses were performed. Associations between the proportions of elderly individuals who had died were tested, according to categories of variables of interest and using Pearson chi-square test or linear trend chi-square, when applicable, to a 95% significance level (p<0.05).

Crude and adjusted relative risks and their respective 95% confidence intervals were calculated using Poisson regression, aiming to identify the best set of predictive variables of mortality and to test the FCI predictive ability. In the multiple model, all variables with p<0.05 in the bivariate analysis were tested. Variables were included one by one to construct the model, with p<0.05 as criterion of inclusion and maintaining a criterion of variables of p<0.10 in the following model. Analysis was performed using the Stata 9.0 software.

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Universidade Federal de São Paulo, under record CEP 1663.07. During the entire research development, the ethical principles described by Resolution 196/96 from the Conselho Nacional de Saúde (Brazilian Health Council) were followed.

RESULTS

In the discriminating analysis and in relation to the first survey, orientation to time predicted 87.2% of cognitive loss correctly, represented by MMSE equal to or below 24 points, with a sensitivity of 92.0% and specificity of 76.3%. As regards the second survey, conducted two years later, orientation to time predicted 81.1% of cognitive loss, represented by MMSE equal to or below 24 points, with a sensitivity of 86.6% and specificity of 59.2%. Only orientation to time had the same predictive ability than the complete model with the 30 MMSE questions.

In terms of the first survey, the executive function "taking medication correctly and without difficulties" predicted 57.5% of those with functional loss (more than two ADLs), with a sensitivity of 100.0% and specificity of 35.9%. The executive function "going shopping without difficulties" predicted 60.9% of those with functional loss, with a sensitivity of 100.0% and specificity of 41.1%. As regards the second survey, "taking medication correctly without difficulties" predicted 61.9% of those with functional loss, with a sensitivity of 90.1% and specificity of 16.8%. "Going shopping without difficulties" predicted 72.8% of those with functional loss, with a sensitivity of 90.8% and specificity of 44.1%.

In the bivariate analysis, among sociodemographic variables (Table 2), a significantly higher percentage of deaths in elderly males was found, when compared to elderly females.

The percentage of deaths increased with age: 56.9% of elderly individuals aged more than 80 years died, whereas only 22.7% died in the youngest age group (65 to 69 years) (Table 2).

A significantly higher percentage of deaths in elderly individuals with lower level of education (illiterates or those who only knew how to read and write) and per capita income of up to US$99 was observed (Table 2).

Among the variables related to morbidity in the elderly, the following were associated with higher mortality: diagnosis of mental disorder, diabetes, stroke and urinary incontinence. Obesity had a protective effect on mortality (Table 3).

In addition, partial or total absence of teeth and hospitalization in the last six months were also significantly associated with mortality, as well as good, poor and very poor self-perception of health, when compared to excellent self-perception (Table 4).

The FCI was significantly associated with mortality (Table 4). Among elderly individuals with the worst index value, i.e. those who answered no questions or only one question about orientation to time correctly and who neither went shopping nor took medication correctly alone (category "22"), 78.6% died, whereas among those with preserved orientation to time and the ability to go shopping and taking medication alone (category "00"), only 29.1% died. Probability of death was 8.92 times higher (95% CI: 4.64; 17.14) in group "22" than in group "00" (Table 6). This probability decreased as FCI categories progressed towards better preserved orientation to time and executive functions. Bivariate analysis showed a 48% higher probability of death in elderly individuals in the "01, 02, 10 and 20" categories (those with preserved orientation to time or ability to go shopping and take medication). Next, category "11" remained (those who correctly answered two or three questions about orientation to time and managed to go shopping or taking medication alone), with a probability of death that was 2.35 times higher. Subsequently, categories "21" and "12" showed a probability of death 3.76 and 7.70 times higher, respectively, while the categories in which one of the FCI components (orientation to time or the ability to go shopping or taking medication alone) had already been completely lost. Probability of death was higher for category "12" (correctly answered 2-3 questions about orientation to time, but could not go shopping or take medication alone) than for "21" (correctly answered 0-1 question about orientation to time and could go shopping or take medication alone), showing greater influence of loss of executive functions than that of orientation to time on the probability of death.

Physical activity showed lower association with mortality (Table 5). All variables related to social support were associated with mortality, especially among those who do not have a relationship with neighbors, relatives or friends.

In the multiple regression analysis, the following risk factors of mortality remained in the final model: male sex, age group (75 to 79 years; 80 years and more), self-perception of health (good, fair and very poor, compared to excellent), diabetes, hospitalization in the last six months, partial or total absence of teeth and FCI. A monthly relationship with relatives was found to be a protective factor (Table 6).

Comparison of crude and adjusted relative risks in the final model (Table 6) revealed that, for the majority of covariables studied, the adjusted relative risk showed a value close to or below that of the crude relative risk, indicating that part of the individual effect of each variable was due to the effect of other covariables. The FCI showed the greatest magnitude of association, observed both in the crude and adjusted relative risks, with a consistent increasing trend as index categories became worse.

DISCUSSION

The previous analysis of mortality between the first and the second Epidoso surveys (with an interval of two years between them) identified the following as independent factors of risk of death: male sex, more advanced age, history of hospitalization in the last six months, reduction in functional capacity (measured by the level of independence to perform daily life activities) and cognitive deficit.18

In the present study, the FCI was the main factor of risk of mortality among the elderly individuals studied. As regards the analysis previously performed,18 which used the MMSE (cut-off point below 18) and ADL (dependence on seven or more ADLs) separately, the FCI predictive ability was greater, with adjusted relative risks reaching higher values.

In addition, the FCI overcame a great methodological difficulty, namely, to associate ADL functional capacity and the cognitive state, represented by the MMSE, in which cut-off points to indicate cognitive impairment vary according to level of education. Another advantage of the FCI is the possibility of separating sub-groups with higher risk of death, which show greater cognitive and functional impairment.

The combination between questions about orientation to time and executive functions has the advantage of showing a reduced number of questions (only seven). The results found reveal that orientation to time and executive functions act together to provide a protective effect on risk of death, one compensating the other. This phenomenon was shown by the combination of categories, where one of these categories (functional capacity or cognitive state) was entirely preserved (10, 20, 01, 02). Findings from the present study are similar in several aspects to those found by other authors who investigated predictive factors of mortality in the elderly.

Ginsberg et al6 found an independent effect of difficulty on more than one instrumental activity of daily living ADL, cognitive deficit and the fact of having been in bed in the previous two weeks, after control for sex, comorbidities, income, social support and smoking. All study participants (mean age of 70 years) with cognitive deficit and incapacity in more than two IADLs died, suggesting that the use of measures combining cognitive state and functional capacity can be an effective and low-cost method to identify elderly individuals at high risk of death.

Bassuk et al2 found a greater effect of cognitive loss on risk of death among younger elderly individuals than among those aged more than 80 years. This study emphasizes the selection of the "going shopping" and "taking medication correctly" executive functions, once they are daily life activities of younger elderly individuals.

Blazer et al,3 while assessing the effect of depression on risk of death, found that depression lost its effect when cognitive loss, functional incapacity, comorbidities and social support were included in the model, suggesting that the association between depression and mortality occurs through several mechanisms, among which is loss of functional and cognitive capacity.

Ansley et al,1 Landi et al9 and Nguyen et al15 found an independent effect of cognitive loss and functional capacity on risk of death, even in the presence of co-morbidities. Ganguli et al5 observed a more important effect of functional capacity than that of the cognitive state to predict risk of death.

Nybo e al,17 by analyzing mortality above 90 years of age, identified that sociodemographic factors, smoking and obesity lost their effect, whereas functional incapacity, cognitive loss and low self-perception of health remained as independent factors of risk of death.

According to Lieto & Schmidt,10 not to use medication correctly increases the risk of hospitalization of elderly individuals and is associated with cognitive loss. This IADL can be useful to detect cognitive loss in the elderly.

McGuire et al12 concluded that cognitive deficits lead to higher probability of death or show dependence. In addition, they found statistically significant association between anatomical-pathological injuries typical of Alzheimer's disease and time and spatial orientation impairment.

In the literature review, only the study by Njegovan et al16 was found. This study classified daily life activities in hierarchical levels and showed that certain hierarchically higher functions among IADLs (going shopping, taking care of finances, cooking) are lost before (on higher cognitive levels) basic ADL functions (eating, getting dressed, walking). Cognitive loss was associated with a specific pattern of loss of functional capacity, enabling the pattern of functional reduction and, as a result, their health care needs to be predicted.16

The FCI compares information obtained from interviews (BOMFAQ/OARS) with that obtained from testing (MMSE), thus increasing reliability of results.

In the present study, elderly individuals with partial or total loss of teeth had significant higher risk, independent from death, than those with little or no dental loss. This finding is in agreement with studies that associate poor dental health and higher morbimortality by all causes in the elderly.13,14 In the present analysis, as well as in the study by Ramos,18 level of education lost statistical significance and was excluded in the multiple model, thus not being a risk factor of mortality that was independent from the cognitive state and functional capacity in this population. One possible explanation for this fact would be the low variability of participants' level of education, once, in the area studied, middle-class residents predominated. Income also lost its predictive ability of mortality, when controlled by the remaining variables.

In conclusion, the FCI of orientation to time and executive functions was the most important changeable predictor of risk of death. Moreover, this index can help clinicians and health planners to promote health in the elderly, aiming to reduce mortality by coherently allocating the necessary resources to maintain and rehabilitate cognitive state and functional (executive) capacity. The FCI is simple and it can be used by higher and secondary education health professionals, in addition to tele-health services.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Anstey KJ, Luszcz MA, Giles LC, Andrews GR. Demographic, health, cognitive, and sensory variables as predictors of mortality in very old adults. Psychol Aging. 2001;16(1):3-11. DOI:10.1037/0882-7974.16.1.3
  • 2
    Bassuk SS, Wypij D, Berkman LF. Cognitive impairment and mortality in the community-dwelling elderly. Am J Epidemiol. 2000;151(7):676-88.
  • 3
    Blazer DG, Hybels CF, Pieper CF. The association of depression and mortality in elderly persons: a case for multiple, independent pathways. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2001;56(8):M505-9.
  • 4
    Folstein, MF, Folstein, SE, McHugh, PR. "Mini-mental state": a pratical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinicians. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189-98. DOI:10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
  • 5
    Ganguli M, Dodge HH, Mulsant BH. Rates and predictors of mortality in an aging, rural, community-based cohort: the role of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59(11):1046-52. DOI:10.1001/archpsyc.59.11.1046
  • 6
    Ginsberg GM, Hammerman-Rozenberg R, Cohen A, Stessman J. Independence in instrumental activities of daily living and its effect on mortality. Aging (Milano). 1999;11(3):161-8.
  • 7
    Hardy SE, Dubin JA, Holford TR, Gill TM. Transitions between states of disability and independence among older persons. Am J Epidemiol. 2005;161(6):575-84. DOI:10.1093/aje/kwi083.
  • 8
    Huberty CJ. Applied discriminant analysis. New York: John Wiley; 1994.
  • 9
    Landi F, Onder G, Cattel C, Gambassi G, Lattanzio F, Cesari M, et al. Functional status and clinical correlates in cognitively impaired community-living older people. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2001;14(1):21-7. DOI:10.1177/089198870101400106
  • 10
    Lieto JM, Schmidt KS. Reduced ability to self-administer medication is associated with assisted living placement in a continuing care retirement community. J Am Med Dir Assoc 2005;6(4):246-9. DOI:10.1016/j.jamda.2005.04.002.
  • 11
    Magila MC, Caramelli P. Funções Executivas no Idoso. Forlenza OV, Caramelli P, organizadores. Neuropsiquiatria Geriátrica. São Paulo; Atheneu; 2000. p 517-25.
  • 12
    McGuire LC, Ford ES, Ajani UA. Cognitive Functioning as a Predictor of Functional Disability in Later Life. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14(1):36-42. DOI:10.1097/01.JGP.0000192502.10692.d6
  • 13
    Meurman JH, Hamalainen P. Oral health and morbidity-implications of oral infections on the elderly. Gerodontology 2006;23(1):3-16. DOI:10.1111/j.1741-2358.2006.00102.x
  • 14
    Morita I, Nakagaki H, Kato K, Murakami T, Tsuboi S, Hayashizaki J, et al. Relationship between survival rates and number of natural teeth in an elderly Japanese population. Gerontodology 2006;23(4):214-8. DOI:10.1111/j.1741-2358.2006.00134.x
  • 15
    Nguyen HT, Black SA, Ray LA, Espino DV, Markides KS. Cognitive impairment and mortality in older Mexican Americans. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51(2):178-83. DOI:10.1046/j.1532-5415.2003.51055.x
  • 16
    Njegovan V, Hing MM, Mitchell SL, Molnar FJ. The hierarchy of functional loss associated with cognitive decline in older persons. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2001;56(10):M638-43.
  • 17
    Nybo H, Petersen HC, Gaist D, Jeune B, Andersen K, McGue M, et al. Predictors of mortality in 2,249 nonagenarians-the Danish 1905-Cohort Survey. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51(10):1365-73. DOI:10.1046/j.1532-5415.2003.51453.x
  • 18
    Ramos LR. Fatores determinantes do envelhecimento saudável em idosos residentes em centro urbano: Projeto Epidoso, São Paulo. Cad Saude Publica. 2003;19(3):793-7. DOI:10.1590/S0102-311X2003000300011
  • 19
    Ramos LR, Simoes EJ, Albert MS. Dependence in activities of daily living and cognitive impairment strongly predicted mortality in older urban residents in Brazil: a 2-year follow-up. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2001;49(9):1168-75. DOI:10.1046/j.1532-5415.2001.49233.x
  • 20
    Ramos LR, Toniolo NJ, Cendoroglo MS, Garcia JT, Nayas MS, Perracini M. Estudo de seguimento por dois anos de idosos residentes em São Paulo, Brasil: metodologia e resultados preliminares. Rev Saude Publica. 1998;32(5):397-407. DOI:10.1590/S0034-89101998000500001
  • 21
    Rosa TEC, Benício MHA, Latorre MRDO, Ramos LR. Fatores determinantes da capacidade funcional entre idosos. Rev Saude Publica. 2003;37(1):40-8. DOI:10.1590/S0034-89102003000100008
  • 22
    Thornton W, Deria S, Gelb S, Shapiro R, Hill A. Neuropsychological mediators of the links among age, chronic illness, and everyday problem solving. Psychol Aging. 2007;22(3):470-81. DOI:10.1037/0882-7974.22.3.470
  • 23
    Treitz F, Heyder K, Daum I. Differential course of executive control changes during normal aging. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2007;14(4):370-93. DOI:10.1080/13825580600678442
  • 24
    Tractenberg RE, Weiner MF, Aisen PS, Kaye JA, Fuh JL. A simple method to rule out dementia with temporal orientation. Alzheimers Dement. 2007;3(1):28-32. DOI:10.1016/j.jalz.2006.10.005
  • 25
    Tractenberg RE, Weinstein M, Weiner MF, Aisen PS, Fuh JL, Goldman N, et al. Benchmarking a test of temporal orientation with data from American and Taiwanese persons with Alzheimer's disease and American normal elderly. Neuroepidemiology 2005;24(1-2):110-6. DOI:10.1159/000081610
  • 26
    Xavier A, Sales M, Ramos L, Anção M, Sigulem D. Cognition, interaction and ageing: an Internet workshops exploratory study. Stud Health Technol Inform. 2004;103:289-95.
  • Time orientation and executive functions in the prediction of mortality in the elderly: Epidoso study

    André Junqueira XavierI; Eleonora d'OrsiII; Daniel SigulemI; Luiz Roberto RamosIII
  • Publication Dates

    • Publication in this collection
      21 June 2010
    • Date of issue
      Feb 2010

    History

    • Accepted
      02 Oct 2009
    • Reviewed
      22 Aug 2009
    • Received
      04 Aug 2008
    Faculdade de Saúde Pública da Universidade de São Paulo Avenida Dr. Arnaldo, 715, 01246-904 São Paulo SP Brazil, Tel./Fax: +55 11 3061-7985 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
    E-mail: revsp@usp.br