Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Neuropsychological and behavioral assessment of impulsivity in adolescents: a systematic review

Avaliação neuropsicológica e comportamental da impulsividade em adolescentes: uma revisão sistemática

Abstract

Introduction:

The aim of neuropsychological assessment is to understand the multifaceted relationship between behavior, cognition and brain organization. The main objective of this study was to conduct a systematic review of the literature on impulsivity and inhibitory control in healthy adolescents and preadolescents.

Method:

Three researchers conducted a systematic review on November 25, 2014. The first step was to conduct searches on the Web of Knowledge and PubMed databases for articles in English using the following keywords: child; adolescents; impulsivity; inhibition; inhibitory control; and neuropsychology. The second step was to analyze all the references cited in the articles selected from the search results in order to identify further literature.

Results:

Just 13 articles met all the inclusion criteria. There was no consistency in the aims of these studies, or in the instruments or procedures employed.

Conclusion:

There was no uniformity among the articles that met all the criteria in terms of any of the items analyzed. There was considerable heterogeneity in these studies of impulsivity and inhibitory control, and few of them evaluated healthy samples.

Keywords:
Adolescence; neuropsychology; impulsivity; inhibitory control; systematic review

Resumo

Introdução:

O objetivo da avaliação neuropsicológica é compreender a relação multifacetada entre comportamento, cognição e organização cerebral. O principal objetivo deste estudo foi conduzir uma revisão sistemática da literatura sobre impulsividade e controle inibitório em pré-adolescentes e adolescentes saudáveis.

Métodos:

Três pesquisadores conduziram uma revisão sistemática em 25 de novembro de 2014. O primeiro passo foi fazer pesquisas nas bases de dados Web of Knowledge e PubMed buscando artigos em inglês usando as seguintes palavras-chave: child; adolescents; impulsivity; inhibition; inhibitory control; e neuropsychology. O segundo passo foi analisar todas as referências citadas nos artigos selecionados a fim de identificar mais literatura.

Resultados:

Apenas 13 artigos preencheram todos os critérios de inclusão. Não houve consistência nos objetivos desses estudos, ou nos instrumentos ou procedimentos empregados.

Conclusões:

Não houve uniformidade entre os artigos que preencheram todos os critérios em termos de qualquer um dos itens analisados. Houve uma heterogeneidade considerável nesses estudos sobre impulsividade e controle inibitório, e poucos deles avaliaram amostras saudáveis.

Descritores:
Adolescência; neuropsicologia; impulsividade; controle inibitório; revisão sistemática

Introduction

Adolescence is a period of life characterized by many different experiences, including increased social interaction, willingness to try new things, seeking approval in one's environment, physical maturation and brain development.11. Blakemore SJ. The social brain in adolescence. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:267-77.,22. Casey BJ, Jones RM, Hare TA. The adolescent brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124:111-26. This phase also features high risk behavior and impulsive acts, such as the use of drugs and alcohol, criminal activities and unprotected sex.33. Casey BJ, Jones RM. Neurobiology of the adolescent brain and behavior: implications for substance use disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:1189-201.

To understand the cognitive and neurobiological changes in adolescence, it must be comprehended as a transitional period.44. Galvan A, Hare TA, Parra CE, Penn J, Voss H, Glover G, et al. Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in adolescents. J Neurosci. 2006;26:6885-92. The competence of cognitive control capacities increases during this stage of life and this is dependent on maturation of the prefrontal cortex, which continues to develop until early adulthood.55. Hare TA, Tottenham N, Galvan A, Voss HU, Glover GH, Casey BJ. Biological substrates of emotional reactivity and regulation in adolescence during an emotional go-nogo task. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:927-34.

Full development of the brain occurs when the young adult reaches around 21 years of age.33. Casey BJ, Jones RM. Neurobiology of the adolescent brain and behavior: implications for substance use disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:1189-201. However, studies have shown that in some adults the brain's development is never complete.66. Casey BJ, Tottenham N, Liston C, Durston S. Imaging the developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9:104-10. In adolescence, cognitive control is not fully developed and the reward system in this period leads to increased gratification seeking, irrespective of the risks involved or of rules.77. Crone EA. Executive functions in adolescence: inferences from brain and behavior. Dev Sci. 2009;12:825-30.,88. Steinberg L. A dual systems model of adolescent risk-taking. Dev Psychobiol. 2010;52:216-24.

During this period, new learning related to new demands and expectations leads to improvement of skills such as selective attention, divided attention, working memory and executive functions, including inhibitory control.99. Blakemore S-J, Choudhury S. Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47:296-312. This can be explained by the fact that synaptic proliferation and development of the brain in adolescence have neuropsychological consequences, especially for the prefrontal cortex functions.1010. Overman WH, Frassrand K, Ansel S, Trawalter S, Bies B, Redmond A. Performance on the IOWA card task by adolescents and adults. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:1838-51.

Inhibitory control is a broad term and refers to the capacity for the flexibility needed to adapt one's behavior when faced with cognitive conflict, interference or competition.1111. Yücel M, Fornito A, Youssef G, Dwyer D, Whittle S, Wood SJ, et al. Inhibitory control in young adolescents: the role of sex, intelligence, and temperament. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:347-56. This component is essential in regulating the wildest forms of behavior, involving mature resources to delay gratification, inhibit impulsive behavior and organize activities.1111. Yücel M, Fornito A, Youssef G, Dwyer D, Whittle S, Wood SJ, et al. Inhibitory control in young adolescents: the role of sex, intelligence, and temperament. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:347-56.,1212. Aron AR, Durston S, Eagle DM, Logan GD, Stinear CM, Stuphorn V. Converging evidence for a fronto-basal-ganglia network for inhibitory control of action and cognition. J Neurosci. 2007;27:11860-4.

There are few studies on impulsiveness, inhibitory control and neuropsychological assessment in healthy adolescents.1111. Yücel M, Fornito A, Youssef G, Dwyer D, Whittle S, Wood SJ, et al. Inhibitory control in young adolescents: the role of sex, intelligence, and temperament. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:347-56.,1313. Kohls G, Peltzer J, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Konrad K. Differential effects of social and non-social reward on response inhibition in children and adolescents. Dev Sci. 2009;12:614-25.,1414. Romer D, Betancourt L, Giannetta JM, Brodsky NL, Farah M, Hurt H. Executive cognitive functions and impulsivity as correlates of risk taking and problem behavior in preadolescents. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2916-26. The aim of neuropsychological assessment is to understand the multifaceted relationship between behavior, cognition and brain organization.1515. Leon-Carrion J, García-Orza J, Pérez-Santamaría FJ. Development of the inhibitory component of the executive functions in children and adolescents. Int J Neurosci. 2004;114:1291-311.,1616. Oliveira CR de, Pedron AC, Gurgel LG, Reppold CT. Executive functions and sustained attention: comparison between age groups of 19-39 and 40-59 years old. Dement Neuropsychol. 2012;6:29-34. It was first described in studies that evaluated cognitive deficits secondary to neurological conditions.1717. Befi-Lopes DM, Rondon S. Características iniciais da comunicação verbal de pré-escolares com alterações específicas do desenvolvimento da linguagem em fala espontânea. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2010;15:415-20.,1818. Schirmer CR, Fontoura DR, Nunes ML. [Language and learning disorders]. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2004;80:S95-103. However, the field of developmental neuropsychology, the study of the maturation, maintenance and decline of cognitive functions at different ages and phases of life, is an area of growth in scientific research circles.1616. Oliveira CR de, Pedron AC, Gurgel LG, Reppold CT. Executive functions and sustained attention: comparison between age groups of 19-39 and 40-59 years old. Dement Neuropsychol. 2012;6:29-34. Studies have been conducted to assess critical stages of life, including language acquisition in children and memory decline in elderly adults.1717. Befi-Lopes DM, Rondon S. Características iniciais da comunicação verbal de pré-escolares com alterações específicas do desenvolvimento da linguagem em fala espontânea. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2010;15:415-20.

18. Schirmer CR, Fontoura DR, Nunes ML. [Language and learning disorders]. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2004;80:S95-103.
-1919. Davis HP, Small SA, Stern Y, Mayeux R, Feldstein SN, Keller FR. Acquisition, recall, and forgetting of verbal information in long-term memory by young, middle-aged, and elderly individuals. Cortex. 2003;39:1063-91.

Sex and age-based evaluations and comparisons of samples of healthy adolescents are important for understanding normal development at this stage of life. One study in the field found that male adolescents were less efficient than female adolescents in exercising strategic control to reduce their responses to conflict, suggesting there are sex differences in inhibitory control in adolescents.1111. Yücel M, Fornito A, Youssef G, Dwyer D, Whittle S, Wood SJ, et al. Inhibitory control in young adolescents: the role of sex, intelligence, and temperament. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:347-56. A study that investigated maturation of inhibitory functions during childhood and adolescence in a sample of 99 Spanish students aged from 6 to 17 years found that inhibitory functions increased with age.1515. Leon-Carrion J, García-Orza J, Pérez-Santamaría FJ. Development of the inhibitory component of the executive functions in children and adolescents. Int J Neurosci. 2004;114:1291-311. This kind of study reports the results of comparisons and illustrates normal development in adolescence.

It is important to investigate impulsivity and control inhibition in healthy adolescents to gain a greater understanding of the relationships between maturation of the prefrontal cortex and these behaviors.33. Casey BJ, Jones RM. Neurobiology of the adolescent brain and behavior: implications for substance use disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:1189-201.,88. Steinberg L. A dual systems model of adolescent risk-taking. Dev Psychobiol. 2010;52:216-24. It is also important to conduct research that compares male and female adolescents in order to understand sex-linked differences in maturation and the prevalence of these behaviors.1111. Yücel M, Fornito A, Youssef G, Dwyer D, Whittle S, Wood SJ, et al. Inhibitory control in young adolescents: the role of sex, intelligence, and temperament. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:347-56.

The main objective of this study was to investigate the literature on impulsivity and inhibitory control in healthy adolescents and preadolescents by means of a systematic review. Secondary aims were to identify instruments used to assess these phenomena and to determine whether there are any similarities between studies in terms of the ages of participants and the instruments administered. Our focus is on the assessment of impulsivity of people without diagnoses, since this area is not explored in psychology. This paper adopts the positive psychology approach, a research model concerned with potentially healthy aspects of human beings, as opposed to traditional psychology and its emphasis on psychopathology.2020. Yunes MAM. Psicologia positiva e resiliência: o foco no indivíduo e na família. Psicol Estud. 2003;8:75-84.

Method

A systematic review was conducted by three researchers on November 25, 2014. The review was conducted by searching the Web of Knowledge and PubMed databases for articles published in English only. The first step of the research was conducted using six combinations of keywords: [child AND impulsivity AND neuropsychology]; [adolescents AND impulsivity AND neuropsychology]; [child AND inhibitory control AND neuropsychology]; [adolescents AND inhibitory control AND neuropsychology]; [child AND inhibition AND neuropsychology]; [adolescents AND inhibition AND neuropsychology]. Since the selection criterion was to include studies whose samples included preadolescents and adolescents, the keywords were designed to select studies that evaluated both age groups. The keyword "child" was included in order to identify articles describing samples that covered the ages of interest (from preadolescence at 10 years old2121. Shirtcliff EA, Dahl RE, Pollak SD. Pubertal development: correspondence between hormonal and physical development. Child Dev. 2009;80:327-37. up to the age of 18). The keyword "neuropsychology" was included to cover all types of neuropsychological evaluation: tests, tasks, assessments, instruments, questionnaires and others. All keywords used in the search were Medical Subject Headings (MeSH).

Each researcher independently evaluated the studies found in the first step against the following criteria: 1) publication dates from 2003 to 2014; 2) empirical studies; 3) human research that includes preadolescents and/or adolescents (10-18 years); 4) research that includes healthy individuals only; 5) articles that report some kind of neuropsychological assessment; 6) studies involve assessment of impulsivity and inhibitory control. Papers that failed to meet one or more of these criteria were excluded, so only those that met all of them were included in the analysis. Repeated articles were excluded.

Initially, the articles were analyzed on the basis of their abstracts. When the abstract was not conclusive, methods and results sections were also evaluated. The full texts of selected articles were analyzed. Classification was performed by three researchers, who acted as judges in the analysis of concordance between study characteristics and inclusion criteria.

The initial searches identified 652 articles using all of our keyword combinations to query both databases: 398 articles were found on PubMed and 254 on the Web of Knowledge. Twenty of these met all the criteria, based on their abstracts, and these were read in full. This resulted in the rejection of a further seven articles which did not meet all of the selection criteria in this more thorough assessment. Figure 1 contains a flowchart illustrating the analysis. After the systematic review, a second step was performed in which all the references in the articles included were analyzed in search of further relevant literature.

Figure 1
Flowchart of systematic review.

Results

In the first stage of the selection process, the abstracts of 20 articles met all the search criteria, but after examination of the full texts, just 13 of them were found to meet all the criteria. The results of the review are shown in Table 1, listing the following information about the 13 studies: their aims, number of participants, age or average age of participants, instruments used in the assessments, and their results.

Table 1
Analysis of selected articles in systematic review

With regard to the main objective of this review, it was observed that there was no consistency across the 13 articles that evaluated inhibitory control and impulsivity in healthy adolescents in terms of type of evaluation, instruments used, study objectives or age of the participants, demonstrating that there is still heterogeneity in this field. For example, some studies used the Stroop Task,2222. Wright I, Waterman M, Prescott H, Murdoch-Eaton D. A new Stroop-like measure of inhibitory function development: typical developmental trends. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44:561-75.,2626. Nichelli F, Scala G, Vago C, Riva D, Bulgheroni S. Age-related trends in Stroop and conflicting motor response task findings. Child Neuropsychol. 2005;11:431-43. while others used a battery of instruments.1111. Yücel M, Fornito A, Youssef G, Dwyer D, Whittle S, Wood SJ, et al. Inhibitory control in young adolescents: the role of sex, intelligence, and temperament. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:347-56.,2525. Aran-Filippetti V, Richaud de Minzi MC. A structural analysis of executive functions and socioeconomic status in school-age children: cognitive factors as effect mediators. J Genet Psychol. 2012;173:393-416. This will be discussed in detail below, but this heterogeneity in research in this field shows that a system has not yet been developed and that it is difficult to choose which protocol to follow when investigating inhibitory control in healthy adolescents.

It was observed that just one of the papers analyzed in this review studied the adaptation of a new task for the measurement of inhibitory functions (designed for children aged from 3 to 16 years),2222. Wright I, Waterman M, Prescott H, Murdoch-Eaton D. A new Stroop-like measure of inhibitory function development: typical developmental trends. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44:561-75. but this measure was not primarily focused on adolescents. The most common measure in the remaining studies was the Stroop test (six studies used this instrument).

Three of the studies compared males' and females' performance.1111. Yücel M, Fornito A, Youssef G, Dwyer D, Whittle S, Wood SJ, et al. Inhibitory control in young adolescents: the role of sex, intelligence, and temperament. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:347-56.,2323. van Deurzen PA, Buitelaar JK, Agnes Brunnekreef J, Ormel J, Minderaa RB, Hartman CA, et al. Response time variability and response inhibition predict affective problems in adolescent girls, not in boys: the TRAILS study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;21:277-87.,2929. Silveri MM, Rohan ML, Pimentel PJ, Gruber SA, Rosso IM, Yurgelun-Todd DA. Sex differences in the relationship between white matter microstructure and impulsivity in adolescents. Magn Reson Imaging. 2006;24:833-41. These studies were dissimilar because they used different instruments. One used the Amsterdam Neuropsychological Tasks Program.2323. van Deurzen PA, Buitelaar JK, Agnes Brunnekreef J, Ormel J, Minderaa RB, Hartman CA, et al. Response time variability and response inhibition predict affective problems in adolescent girls, not in boys: the TRAILS study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;21:277-87. Another used the Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory and the Stroop Color-Word Test.2929. Silveri MM, Rohan ML, Pimentel PJ, Gruber SA, Rosso IM, Yurgelun-Todd DA. Sex differences in the relationship between white matter microstructure and impulsivity in adolescents. Magn Reson Imaging. 2006;24:833-41. The third also used the Stroop test, but as part of a battery of questions and tasks that were not used in any other study selected for this review, comprising the Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire - Revised (EATQ-R); the parent report version of the Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire (PEATQ) (temperament); and the Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas (SEIFA) Index of Relative Socio-Economic Disadvantage (IRSD); and the WISC-IV.1111. Yücel M, Fornito A, Youssef G, Dwyer D, Whittle S, Wood SJ, et al. Inhibitory control in young adolescents: the role of sex, intelligence, and temperament. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:347-56.

The studies also differed somewhat with regard to the ages of their samples, with participants' ages ranging from 3 to 18 years. Eleven studies included children under the age of 10 and thus did not exclusively cover the period of adolescence. Most of the studies were designed to evaluate young people's development from childhood to adolescence, rather than focusing on a specific age. The two studies that did focus on specific age groups were by Yücel et al.,1111. Yücel M, Fornito A, Youssef G, Dwyer D, Whittle S, Wood SJ, et al. Inhibitory control in young adolescents: the role of sex, intelligence, and temperament. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:347-56. who evaluated youths aged 11-13 years with a mean age of 12.6 years, focused on pre-teens, and by Deurzen et al.,2323. van Deurzen PA, Buitelaar JK, Agnes Brunnekreef J, Ormel J, Minderaa RB, Hartman CA, et al. Response time variability and response inhibition predict affective problems in adolescent girls, not in boys: the TRAILS study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;21:277-87. who also focused on preadolescents, aged 10 to 12 years.

One study found that inhibitory control performance improved with age and that when the task required a stronger inhibitory control the response time trends were more varied.2222. Wright I, Waterman M, Prescott H, Murdoch-Eaton D. A new Stroop-like measure of inhibitory function development: typical developmental trends. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44:561-75. Another paper evaluated four age groups (7, 11, 15 and 21 years) and concluded that there was an improvement in inhibition from 11 to 15 years, when participants attained adult levels in the tasks, although the proportion of perfect solutions increased into adulthood.3232. Huizinga M, Dolan CV, van der Molen MW. Age-related change in executive function: developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:2017-36. The results of one study showed an improvement in the ability to inhibit responses between the ages of 5 and 10 years.2727. Polderman TJ, de Geus EJ, Hoekstra RA, Bartels M, van Leeuwen M, Verhulst FC, et al. Attention problems, inhibitory control, and intelligence index overlapping genetic factors: a study in 9-, 12-, and 18-year-old twins. Neuropsychology. 2009;23:381-91. This improvement remained significant after controlling for the influence of working memory and processing speed.

One study of children, preadolescents and young adults (n = 81) tested cognitive control and used a selective inhibition task that required participants to keep the selection rule active in their working memory.3333. van den Wildenberg WP, van der Molen MW. Developmental trends in simple and selective inhibition of compatible and incompatible responses. J Exp Child Psychol. 2004;87:201-20. The results suggest that the selective inhibition task imposed high demands on cognitive control processes.3333. van den Wildenberg WP, van der Molen MW. Developmental trends in simple and selective inhibition of compatible and incompatible responses. J Exp Child Psychol. 2004;87:201-20. This was the only study of cognitive control and selective inhibition.

In another study, the authors did not mention age in their results, however, they found that most children had high scores on scales with rewards and showed an improvement in the inhibition of responses only when a monetary reward was offered.1313. Kohls G, Peltzer J, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Konrad K. Differential effects of social and non-social reward on response inhibition in children and adolescents. Dev Sci. 2009;12:614-25. This study used the go/no-go task, and the Behavioral Inhibition (BIS)/Behavioral Activation Scale (BAS) to evaluate children and adolescents (n = 65), and was the only study to use this assessment.

Finally, another paper observed no relationship between age and emotional influence on the inhibitory response of emotions and valence (positive or negative), although it was concluded that emotional contexts alter the ability to inhibit response in childhood.2424. Urben S, Van der Linden M, Barisnikov K. Emotional modulation of the ability to inhibit a prepotent response during childhood. Dev Neuropsychol. 2012;37:668-81.

Two papers analyzed the construction of verbal intelligence and reading in correlation with inhibitory control.1515. Leon-Carrion J, García-Orza J, Pérez-Santamaría FJ. Development of the inhibitory component of the executive functions in children and adolescents. Int J Neurosci. 2004;114:1291-311.,2727. Polderman TJ, de Geus EJ, Hoekstra RA, Bartels M, van Leeuwen M, Verhulst FC, et al. Attention problems, inhibitory control, and intelligence index overlapping genetic factors: a study in 9-, 12-, and 18-year-old twins. Neuropsychology. 2009;23:381-91. One found that the reading of words plays an important role in the performance of the Stroop test.1515. Leon-Carrion J, García-Orza J, Pérez-Santamaría FJ. Development of the inhibitory component of the executive functions in children and adolescents. Int J Neurosci. 2004;114:1291-311. In the other study, Polderman et al.2727. Polderman TJ, de Geus EJ, Hoekstra RA, Bartels M, van Leeuwen M, Verhulst FC, et al. Attention problems, inhibitory control, and intelligence index overlapping genetic factors: a study in 9-, 12-, and 18-year-old twins. Neuropsychology. 2009;23:381-91. conducted a study of twins and concluded that the association between attentional and inhibitory control problems reported in the literature could be derived mainly from genetic factors that are shared with intelligence quotient (IQ). Another study featured in this review found a significant effect between socioeconomic status and the executive functions tested.2525. Aran-Filippetti V, Richaud de Minzi MC. A structural analysis of executive functions and socioeconomic status in school-age children: cognitive factors as effect mediators. J Genet Psychol. 2012;173:393-416. This was the only study that attempted to relate socioeconomic status to impulsivity.

Analysis also revealed that one of the papers combined assessment by neuropsychological instruments with neuroimaging. This study used the Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory and Stroop Color-Word Test to assess healthy adolescents who also underwent diffusion tensor imaging using a 3.0-T magnetic resonance imaging system.2929. Silveri MM, Rohan ML, Pimentel PJ, Gruber SA, Rosso IM, Yurgelun-Todd DA. Sex differences in the relationship between white matter microstructure and impulsivity in adolescents. Magn Reson Imaging. 2006;24:833-41.

Discussion

All the articles analyzed in this review differ from each other in terms of the nature of the assessments conducted and the age of participants. The objective of this first systematic review was to find out what instruments are being used and whether there is any similarity between the assessments. There were no similarities across the papers. Rather, each research study used different assessments and evaluated different age groups.

Overall, there are few studies of inhibitory control in healthy adolescents using neuropsychological assessments.1111. Yücel M, Fornito A, Youssef G, Dwyer D, Whittle S, Wood SJ, et al. Inhibitory control in young adolescents: the role of sex, intelligence, and temperament. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:347-56.,1313. Kohls G, Peltzer J, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Konrad K. Differential effects of social and non-social reward on response inhibition in children and adolescents. Dev Sci. 2009;12:614-25.,1414. Romer D, Betancourt L, Giannetta JM, Brodsky NL, Farah M, Hurt H. Executive cognitive functions and impulsivity as correlates of risk taking and problem behavior in preadolescents. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2916-26. This is probably the reason why there is such a lack of uniformity among studies of this population. Adolescence itself is a period of many changes11. Blakemore SJ. The social brain in adolescence. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:267-77.,22. Casey BJ, Jones RM, Hare TA. The adolescent brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124:111-26. and this may explain why studies select just one phase of adolescence to evaluate or choose to compare specific ages, as illustrated by the results of this review. Furthermore, in general, the objectives of these studies were not only to evaluate inhibitory control, but also to compare it with other variables, which necessarily generated differences in the assessments chosen. Notwithstanding, it is important to point out that there was also variation in the type of evaluation used specifically to assess inhibitory control.

As noted in the results, three studies evaluated inhibitory control/impulsivity by comparing male and female adolescents.1111. Yücel M, Fornito A, Youssef G, Dwyer D, Whittle S, Wood SJ, et al. Inhibitory control in young adolescents: the role of sex, intelligence, and temperament. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:347-56.,2323. van Deurzen PA, Buitelaar JK, Agnes Brunnekreef J, Ormel J, Minderaa RB, Hartman CA, et al. Response time variability and response inhibition predict affective problems in adolescent girls, not in boys: the TRAILS study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;21:277-87.,2929. Silveri MM, Rohan ML, Pimentel PJ, Gruber SA, Rosso IM, Yurgelun-Todd DA. Sex differences in the relationship between white matter microstructure and impulsivity in adolescents. Magn Reson Imaging. 2006;24:833-41. Comparison between the sexes is important, since adolescents of the same age may exhibit different results for the measures tested. However, there was no uniformity in the results of these three studies, since one study found that emotional problems in girls can result in poor response inhibition2323. van Deurzen PA, Buitelaar JK, Agnes Brunnekreef J, Ormel J, Minderaa RB, Hartman CA, et al. Response time variability and response inhibition predict affective problems in adolescent girls, not in boys: the TRAILS study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;21:277-87. and another showed that girls tend to avoid conflict and resolve their problems without arguing or fighting.1111. Yücel M, Fornito A, Youssef G, Dwyer D, Whittle S, Wood SJ, et al. Inhibitory control in young adolescents: the role of sex, intelligence, and temperament. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:347-56. Since adolescence is marked by risky behavior and multiple changes,33. Casey BJ, Jones RM. Neurobiology of the adolescent brain and behavior: implications for substance use disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:1189-201. it may be difficult to find consensus in the literature on comparisons between the sexes. It is possible that neurobiological understanding is particularly important when comparing the sexes, since there are differences in brain activation between boys and girls, as shown in the study by Silveri et al.2929. Silveri MM, Rohan ML, Pimentel PJ, Gruber SA, Rosso IM, Yurgelun-Todd DA. Sex differences in the relationship between white matter microstructure and impulsivity in adolescents. Magn Reson Imaging. 2006;24:833-41.

Regarding age, there was a consensus among the studies in this review that there is an increase in inhibitory control as adolescence progresses. This trend has also been observed in other literature reviews.11. Blakemore SJ. The social brain in adolescence. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:267-77.,22. Casey BJ, Jones RM, Hare TA. The adolescent brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124:111-26.,77. Crone EA. Executive functions in adolescence: inferences from brain and behavior. Dev Sci. 2009;12:825-30. As described in the results of this study, 11 out of the 13 articles selected for this systematic review also evaluated children as part of their samples and reported in their findings that inhibitory control is lower among children than in adolescents, demonstrating an increase over childhood levels of inhibitory control during adolescence. A review of the literature on brain development suggests that cognitive development during adolescence is associated with a progressive increase in the efficiency of cognitive abilities of control.3434. Yurgelun-Todd D. Emotional and cognitive changes during adolescence. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2007;17:251-7.

The results also showed that two studies evaluated verbal intelligence in correlation with inhibitory control. Studies like this are important because the literature indicates that inhibitory control can assist in intelligence tests.3535. Gomes CMA, Borges O. Qualidades psicométricas do conjunto de testes de inteligência fluida. Aval Psicol. 2009;8:17-32.

36. Primi R, Cruz MBZ, Nascimento MM, Petrini MC. Validade de Construto de um instrumento informatizado de avaliação dinâmica da Inteligência Fluida. Psico. 2006;37:109-22.

37. Batista AX, Adda CC, Miotto EC, Lúcia MCS de, Scaff M. Torre de Londres e Torre de Hanói: contribuições distintas para avaliação do funcionamento executivo. J Bras Psiquiatr. 2007;56:134-9.
-3838. Shayer B, Carvalho C, Mota M, Argollo N, Abreu N, Bueno OFA. Desempenho de escolares em atenção e funções executivas no Nepsy e inteligência. Psicol Teor Prat. 2015;17:120-35. One of the studies2727. Polderman TJ, de Geus EJ, Hoekstra RA, Bartels M, van Leeuwen M, Verhulst FC, et al. Attention problems, inhibitory control, and intelligence index overlapping genetic factors: a study in 9-, 12-, and 18-year-old twins. Neuropsychology. 2009;23:381-91. that assessed intelligence and inhibitory control (and the only study of this kind, using twins) enriches the field, since it showed that IQ is also shared with genetic factors. Also unique among the studies selected for this systematic review was a study comparing impulsiveness with socioeconomic status.2525. Aran-Filippetti V, Richaud de Minzi MC. A structural analysis of executive functions and socioeconomic status in school-age children: cognitive factors as effect mediators. J Genet Psychol. 2012;173:393-416. This research reported important results for analysis of economic class and impulsive behaviors in young people. Furthermore, the association between socioeconomic status and executive functions is particularly explained by cognitive impulsivity.2525. Aran-Filippetti V, Richaud de Minzi MC. A structural analysis of executive functions and socioeconomic status in school-age children: cognitive factors as effect mediators. J Genet Psychol. 2012;173:393-416.

A limitation of this systematic review was the restriction of article selection to those published in English. If the search had been opened up to more languages and run on a larger number of databases the results might have been different and possibly more articles would have been analyzed and included in the results. Another limitation that can be singled out is the criterion of only selecting papers that assess healthy subjects; if studies had been included that evaluate clinical and control groups, it is likely that more articles would have been eligible for the review. We suggest that clinical groups and controls be included in future systematic reviews.

In conclusion, in this review we found no uniformity between articles in any of the items analyzed. Inhibitory control and impulsivity were correlated with many other factors, such as executive functions, emotion, intelligence, reading and socioeconomic status. There is great diversity in studies of impulsivity and inhibitory control, and few of them include healthy participants in their samples, which was a criterion for selection for analysis in this paper. No articles were found that focused exclusively on the 10-18 years age group, the adolescent phase. However, some studies restricted their samples to the 10-12 years age group and others extended this criterion and investigated both childhood and adolescence.

Final considerations

The importance of this study to the field of neuropsychological evaluation of inhibitory control and impulsivity is founded on the thorough search for articles evaluating these variables in healthy adolescents. This study has a significant impact on the area because it found that there is no uniformity across evaluations, showing that there is a need for a widely-accepted protocol to further research in the area. In conclusion, more research into these behaviors in healthy adolescents is needed in order to provide a better understanding of cognitive functioning in preadolescence and adolescence.

References

  • 1
    Blakemore SJ. The social brain in adolescence. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:267-77.
  • 2
    Casey BJ, Jones RM, Hare TA. The adolescent brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124:111-26.
  • 3
    Casey BJ, Jones RM. Neurobiology of the adolescent brain and behavior: implications for substance use disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:1189-201.
  • 4
    Galvan A, Hare TA, Parra CE, Penn J, Voss H, Glover G, et al. Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in adolescents. J Neurosci. 2006;26:6885-92.
  • 5
    Hare TA, Tottenham N, Galvan A, Voss HU, Glover GH, Casey BJ. Biological substrates of emotional reactivity and regulation in adolescence during an emotional go-nogo task. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:927-34.
  • 6
    Casey BJ, Tottenham N, Liston C, Durston S. Imaging the developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9:104-10.
  • 7
    Crone EA. Executive functions in adolescence: inferences from brain and behavior. Dev Sci. 2009;12:825-30.
  • 8
    Steinberg L. A dual systems model of adolescent risk-taking. Dev Psychobiol. 2010;52:216-24.
  • 9
    Blakemore S-J, Choudhury S. Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47:296-312.
  • 10
    Overman WH, Frassrand K, Ansel S, Trawalter S, Bies B, Redmond A. Performance on the IOWA card task by adolescents and adults. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:1838-51.
  • 11
    Yücel M, Fornito A, Youssef G, Dwyer D, Whittle S, Wood SJ, et al. Inhibitory control in young adolescents: the role of sex, intelligence, and temperament. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:347-56.
  • 12
    Aron AR, Durston S, Eagle DM, Logan GD, Stinear CM, Stuphorn V. Converging evidence for a fronto-basal-ganglia network for inhibitory control of action and cognition. J Neurosci. 2007;27:11860-4.
  • 13
    Kohls G, Peltzer J, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Konrad K. Differential effects of social and non-social reward on response inhibition in children and adolescents. Dev Sci. 2009;12:614-25.
  • 14
    Romer D, Betancourt L, Giannetta JM, Brodsky NL, Farah M, Hurt H. Executive cognitive functions and impulsivity as correlates of risk taking and problem behavior in preadolescents. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2916-26.
  • 15
    Leon-Carrion J, García-Orza J, Pérez-Santamaría FJ. Development of the inhibitory component of the executive functions in children and adolescents. Int J Neurosci. 2004;114:1291-311.
  • 16
    Oliveira CR de, Pedron AC, Gurgel LG, Reppold CT. Executive functions and sustained attention: comparison between age groups of 19-39 and 40-59 years old. Dement Neuropsychol. 2012;6:29-34.
  • 17
    Befi-Lopes DM, Rondon S. Características iniciais da comunicação verbal de pré-escolares com alterações específicas do desenvolvimento da linguagem em fala espontânea. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2010;15:415-20.
  • 18
    Schirmer CR, Fontoura DR, Nunes ML. [Language and learning disorders]. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2004;80:S95-103.
  • 19
    Davis HP, Small SA, Stern Y, Mayeux R, Feldstein SN, Keller FR. Acquisition, recall, and forgetting of verbal information in long-term memory by young, middle-aged, and elderly individuals. Cortex. 2003;39:1063-91.
  • 20
    Yunes MAM. Psicologia positiva e resiliência: o foco no indivíduo e na família. Psicol Estud. 2003;8:75-84.
  • 21
    Shirtcliff EA, Dahl RE, Pollak SD. Pubertal development: correspondence between hormonal and physical development. Child Dev. 2009;80:327-37.
  • 22
    Wright I, Waterman M, Prescott H, Murdoch-Eaton D. A new Stroop-like measure of inhibitory function development: typical developmental trends. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44:561-75.
  • 23
    van Deurzen PA, Buitelaar JK, Agnes Brunnekreef J, Ormel J, Minderaa RB, Hartman CA, et al. Response time variability and response inhibition predict affective problems in adolescent girls, not in boys: the TRAILS study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;21:277-87.
  • 24
    Urben S, Van der Linden M, Barisnikov K. Emotional modulation of the ability to inhibit a prepotent response during childhood. Dev Neuropsychol. 2012;37:668-81.
  • 25
    Aran-Filippetti V, Richaud de Minzi MC. A structural analysis of executive functions and socioeconomic status in school-age children: cognitive factors as effect mediators. J Genet Psychol. 2012;173:393-416.
  • 26
    Nichelli F, Scala G, Vago C, Riva D, Bulgheroni S. Age-related trends in Stroop and conflicting motor response task findings. Child Neuropsychol. 2005;11:431-43.
  • 27
    Polderman TJ, de Geus EJ, Hoekstra RA, Bartels M, van Leeuwen M, Verhulst FC, et al. Attention problems, inhibitory control, and intelligence index overlapping genetic factors: a study in 9-, 12-, and 18-year-old twins. Neuropsychology. 2009;23:381-91.
  • 28
    Urben S, Van der Linden M, Barisnikov K. Development of the ability to inhibit a prepotent response: influence of working memory and processing speed. Br J Dev Psychol. 2011;29:981-98.
  • 29
    Silveri MM, Rohan ML, Pimentel PJ, Gruber SA, Rosso IM, Yurgelun-Todd DA. Sex differences in the relationship between white matter microstructure and impulsivity in adolescents. Magn Reson Imaging. 2006;24:833-41.
  • 30
    Wassenberg R, Hendriksen JG, Hurks PP, Feron FJ, Keulers EH, Vles JS, et al. Development of inattention, impulsivity, and processing speed as measured by the d2 Test: results of a large cross-sectional study in children aged 7-13. Child Neuropsychol. 2008;14:195-210.
  • 31
    Pureza JR, Gonçalves HA, Branco L, Grassi-Oliveira R, Fonseca RP. Executive functions in late childhood: age differences among groups. Psychol Neurosci. 2013;6:79-88.
  • 32
    Huizinga M, Dolan CV, van der Molen MW. Age-related change in executive function: developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:2017-36.
  • 33
    van den Wildenberg WP, van der Molen MW. Developmental trends in simple and selective inhibition of compatible and incompatible responses. J Exp Child Psychol. 2004;87:201-20.
  • 34
    Yurgelun-Todd D. Emotional and cognitive changes during adolescence. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2007;17:251-7.
  • 35
    Gomes CMA, Borges O. Qualidades psicométricas do conjunto de testes de inteligência fluida. Aval Psicol. 2009;8:17-32.
  • 36
    Primi R, Cruz MBZ, Nascimento MM, Petrini MC. Validade de Construto de um instrumento informatizado de avaliação dinâmica da Inteligência Fluida. Psico. 2006;37:109-22.
  • 37
    Batista AX, Adda CC, Miotto EC, Lúcia MCS de, Scaff M. Torre de Londres e Torre de Hanói: contribuições distintas para avaliação do funcionamento executivo. J Bras Psiquiatr. 2007;56:134-9.
  • 38
    Shayer B, Carvalho C, Mota M, Argollo N, Abreu N, Bueno OFA. Desempenho de escolares em atenção e funções executivas no Nepsy e inteligência. Psicol Teor Prat. 2015;17:120-35.
  • Financial support:

    none. Rosa Maria Martins de Almeida is research productivity fellow at Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq), Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil.
  • Suggested citation:

    Willhelm AR, Fortes PM, Czermainski FR, Rates AS, de Almeida RM. Neuropsychological and behavioral assessment of impulsivity in adolescents: a systematic review. Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 2016;38(3):128-135.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Jul-Sep 2016

History

  • Received
    17 Apr 2015
  • Accepted
    26 Jan 2016
Associação de Psiquiatria do Rio Grande do Sul Av. Ipiranga, 5311/202, 90610-001 Porto Alegre RS/ Brasil, Tel./Fax: (55 51) 3024 4846 - Porto Alegre - RS - Brazil
E-mail: trends@aprs.org.br