Accessibility / Report Error

Does ADHD worsen inhibitory control in preschool children born very premature and/or with very low birth weight?

Abstract

Introduction

Deficits in executive functioning, especially in inhibitory control, are present in children born very premature and/or with very low birth weight (VP/VLBW) and in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).

Objective

To evaluate whether ADHD imposes additional inhibitory control (IC) deficits in preschoolers born VP/VLBW.

Methods

79 VP/VLBW (4 to 7 years) children were assessed for ADHD using the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School Aged Children – Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL). IC was measured with Conners’ Kiddie Continuous Performance Test (K-CPT 2) and the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function – Preschool Version (BRIEF-P).Results: No significant differences were found between ADHD (n = 24) and non-ADHD children (n = 55) for any of the measures (p = 0.062 to p = 0.903). Both groups had deficits in most K-CPT 2 scores compared to normative samples, indicating poor IC and inconsistent reaction times.

Conclusions

ADHD does not aggravate IC deficits in VP/VLBW children. Either neuropsychological tasks and parent reports of executive functions (EFs) may not be sensitive enough to differentiate VP/VLBW preschoolers with and without ADHD, or these children’s EFs are already so impaired that there is not much room for additional impairments imposed by ADHD.

Inhibitory control; prematurity; preschool; ADHD

Introduction

Preterm birth is defined as birth occurring before 37 full weeks of gestation. Around the world, its prevalence ranges from 5% to 18%.11. World Health Organization [Internet]. Preterm birth. 2017. https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/preterm-birth.
https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheet...
There are sub-categories of preterm birth, based on gestational age: extremely premature (EP): < 28 weeks; very premature (VP): 28 to < 32 weeks, and moderate to late preterm infants (MLP): 32 to < 37 weeks of gestation. Prematurity can also be classified according to birth weight: extremely low birth weight (ELBW): < 1,000 grams and very low birth weight (VLBW): 1,000 to < 1,500 grams.11. World Health Organization [Internet]. Preterm birth. 2017. https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/preterm-birth.
https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheet...
, 22. Glass H, Costarino TA, Stayer AS, Brett C, Cladis F, Davis JP. Outcomes for extremely premature infants. Anesth Analg. 2015;120:1337-51. Our study will focus on very preterm and/or very low birth weight (VP/VLBW) children.

Although the survival rates for VP and VLBW babies have increased over recent years due to advances in perinatal and neonatal care,33. Aarnoudse-Moens H, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van Goudoever JB, Oosterlaan J. Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics. 2009;124:717-28. these groups are still at high risk of death and disability.22. Glass H, Costarino TA, Stayer AS, Brett C, Cladis F, Davis JP. Outcomes for extremely premature infants. Anesth Analg. 2015;120:1337-51. For instance, several structures of the central nervous system may be compromised in VP/VLBW children.44. Zomignani AP, Zambelli HJL, Antonio MA. Desenvolvimento cerebral em recém-nascidos prematuros. Rev Paul Pediatr. 2009;27:198-203. Abnormalities of brain structure may, in turn, translate to neuropsychological deficits in executive functions.33. Aarnoudse-Moens H, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van Goudoever JB, Oosterlaan J. Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics. 2009;124:717-28.

Prematurity is frequently associated with neurodevelopmental disorders, such as ADHD.55. O’Shea MT, Downey C, Kuban CK. Extreme prematurity and attention deficit: epidemiology and prevention. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:578. This disorder is characterized by inattention and/or hyperactivity/impulsivity interfering with functioning.66. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington: APA; 1952. The validity of ADHD diagnosis in preschool children has been established.77. Lahey BB, Pelham WE, Loney J, Kipp H, Ehrhardt A, Lee SS, et al. Three-year predictive validity of DSM-IV attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children diagnosed at 4-6 years of age. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:2014-20. Overall, neuropsychological studies of preschoolers with ADHD show similar results to studies of school-aged children,88. Seidman JL. Neuropsychological functioning in people with ADHD across the lifespan. Clin Psychol Rev. 2006;26:466-85. including deficits in response inhibition, delay aversion, working memory, and sustained attention.99. Breaux R, Griffith S, Harvey E. Preschool neuropsychological measures as predictors of later attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2016;44:1455-71. , 1010. Pauli-Pott U, Becker K. Neuropsychological basic deficits in preschoolers at risk for ADHD: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31:626-37. Inhibitory control is an executive control function that enables an ongoing response to be stopped. It enables inhibition and alteration of inappropriate strategies, such as detecting and correcting an error in an academic task.1111. Schachar R, Tannock R, Logan, GD. Inhibitory control, impulsivity and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clin Psychol Rev. 1993;13:721-39. Preschool children with ADHD tend to perform significantly worse in inhibitory control tasks and inventories, when compared to those without the disorder.1212. Jacobson LA, Crocetti D, Dirlikov B, Slifer K, Denckla MB, Mostofsky SH, et al. Anomalous brain development is evident in preschoolers with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2018;24:531-9.

13. Çak TH, Kültür ÇES, Gökler B, Öktem F, Taskıran S. The Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function and Continuous Performance Test in preschoolers with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Investig. 2017;14:260-70.
- 1414. Zhang HF, Shuai L, Zhang JS, Wang YF, Lu TF, Tan X, et al. Neuropsychological profile related with executive function of Chinese preschoolers with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: neuropsychological measures and Behavior Rating Scale of Executive Function-Preschool Version. Chin Med J (Engl). 2018;131:648-56.

Although ADHD is a prevalent disorder in children born prematurely and/or at very low birth weight,33. Aarnoudse-Moens H, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van Goudoever JB, Oosterlaan J. Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics. 2009;124:717-28. , 1515. James SN, Rommel AS, Cheung C, McLoughlin G, Brandeis D, Banaschewski T, et al. Association of preterm birth with ADHD-like cognitive impairments and additional subtle impairments in attention and arousal malleability. Psychol Med. 2018;48:1484-93. few studies have investigated whether ADHD is associated with additional impairment of inhibitory control performances in premature children. To our knowledge, only one study has compared preterm children with and without ADHD using a neuropsychological task. Both neurophysiological and neuropsychological tasks were used, and inhibitory impairments and reaction time variability were only found in preterm and term children with ADHD.1616. Potgieter S, Vervisch J, Lagae, L. Event related potentials during attention tasks in VLBW children with and without attention deficit disorder. Clin Neurophysiol. 2003;114:1841-9. It is crucial to note, however, that this study had a limited sample of 21 preterm participants.

Based on the literature reviewed above, we aimed to assess whether ADHD imposes additional inhibitory control deficits on VP/VLBW preschool children. Since ADHD and prematurity are both associated with inhibitory control deficits, we hypothesized that premature infants with ADHD would present greater impairment in this function when compared to those without the disorder.

Method

Participants

This is a case control study in which preschool children born very prematurely (less than 32 weeks) and/or with very low birth weight (under 1500g) diagnosed with ADHD were compared to those without the disorder. The sample included children between 4 and 7 years of age at the time of assessment who had previously been hospitalized at the neonatology unit of our university hospital in Porto Alegre (Hospital de Clínicas de Porto Alegre [HCPA]), in the state of Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil). The survivors among those born from January 1st, 2010 to July 31st, 2012 were eligible for the study (n = 129). Children diagnosed with either a genetic syndrome, cerebral palsy, or congenital infections (HIV or syphilis) were excluded from the sample (n = 25; see Figure 1 ). Two families of children in this sample (n = 104) refused to participate and 6 could not be located, so data were collected from 96 families. For the purposes of this study, children with an intelligence quotient (IQ) lower than 50 and those diagnosed with bipolar disorder or autism spectrum disorder were excluded. Thus, our final sample comprised 79 children for whom parents provided valid responses to the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function – Preschool Version (BRIEF-P) and 70 preschoolers with valid Conners’ Kiddie Continuous Performance Test (K-CPT 2) scores (see Figure 1 ).

Figure 1
Sample selection flow diagram. BRIEF-P = Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function – Preschool Version; IQ = intelligence quotient; K-CPT 2 = Conners’ Kiddie Continuous Performance Test; VP/VLBW = very preterm and/or very low birth weight.

Data collection and diagnostic procedures

Eligible participants were selected from the neonatology service’s records. They were contacted by phone calls, e-mail, mail, social media, or home visits. Data were collected at the hospital or at the family residence and took an average of two hours with the parents and 30 minutes with the children. The diagnostic process relied on use of a semi-structured interview, the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School Aged Children – Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL),1717. Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, et al. Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime Version (KSADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36:980-8. administered by a trained child psychiatrist and answered by the child’s parents.

Participants provided verbal agreement to participation in the study and parents provided written informed consent. This study was approved by the ethics committee at the HCPA (protocol number 15-0384).

Neuropsychological assessment

Trained psychologists assessed children in a single session. IQ was estimated using a short version1818. Kaufman S. A short form of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence. J Consult Clin Psychol.1972;39:361-9. of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI)1919. Wechsler D. WPPSI-R – Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence – Revised. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1989. composed of four subtests: block design, comprehension, picture completion, and arithmetic. Inhibitory control was measured using a computerized task (K-CPT 2), performed by the children, and a behavioral inventory (BRIEF-P), responded by parents or legal guardians. These instruments are described below:

  1. Conners’ Kiddie Continuous Performance Test (K-CPT 2)2020. Conners CK. Conners’ Kiddie Continuous Performance Test for Windows [computer program]. Toronto: Multi-Health; 2000. is a computerized task designed to assess inhibitory control, sustained attention and visual-motor speed. The participant is asked to respond (by pressing the space bar) to all targets presented on a computer screen, but refrain from responding to non-targets.

  2. The Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function – Preschool Version (BRIEF-P) evaluates daily behaviors associated with specific domains of executive functioning, based on parents’ reports. Three indexes based on theoretical and empirical factor analysis findings are provided2121. Gioia GA, Espy KA, Isquith PK. Behavior rating inventory of executive function-preschool version. Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2003.: an inhibitory self-control index (ISCI), a flexibility index (FI), and an emergent metacognition index (EMI). A translated Brazilian Portuguese version was adapted and provided by the publisher, Psychological Assessment Resources (PAR Inc.).

Statistical analysis

Descriptive analyses were conducted to characterize the sample. Potential confounders investigated were age, IQ, gender, gestational age, socioeconomic status, and comorbidities (anxiety disorders and oppositional defiant disorder [ODD]), using the chi-square test for categorical variables and the t test for continuous variables. Variables with p ≤ 0.10 were included in the model (IQ and ODD). Since neuropsychological test scores are influenced by developmental aspects, age was also included in the models because scores were analyzed uncorrected for age. K-CPT 2 and BRIEF-P scores were analyzed using two independent analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs), comparing ADHD against non-ADHD groups. The analyses were performed using version 18.0 of the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) and p < 0.05 was considered significant. Effect size was calculated using partial eta squared and its magnitude defined according to the following interpretation: eta squared < 0.06 = small; 0.06-0.14 = medium, > 0.14 = large effect size.2222. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988. The frequency of cases with worse performance (> 1 standard deviation from normative data) was also calculated, and between-group (ADHD vs. non-ADHD) differences were investigated using chi-square tests. Normative scores for both tests were obtained respectively from a Brazilian sample2323. Miranda M, Sinnes E, Pompeia S, Bueno O. O K-CPT em uma amostra brasileira: descrição do desempenho e comparação com as normas norte-americanas. Rev Psiquiatr Rio Gd Sul. 2009;31:60-6. and the American sample used for the original BRIEF-P manual.2121. Gioia GA, Espy KA, Isquith PK. Behavior rating inventory of executive function-preschool version. Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2003.

Results

Data from 79 participants were analyzed for BRIEF-P scores. Nine participants refused to finish the K-CPT 2, or their assessments were considered invalid, resulting in a total sample of 70 children with K-CPT 2 data. Demographic and clinical data can be found in Table 1 . ADHD and non-ADHD groups did not differ in terms of age, gender, IQ, gestational weeks, or socioeconomic status. The frequency of anxiety disorders was also similar between groups, but the ADHD group had significantly higher rates of ODD, which is in line with previous literature.2424. Posner K, Melvin GA, Murray DW, Gugga SS, Fisher P, Skrobala A, et al. Clinical presentation of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in preschool children: the preschoolers with attention-deficit/hyperactivity treatment study (PATS). J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2007;17:547-62.

Table 1
Sample description

Findings emerging from ANCOVAs did not reveal significant differences between ADHD and non-ADHD groups, either for any of the BRIEF-P scores or for K-CPT 2 variables (p = 0.062 to p = 0.903). All effect sizes were small ( Table 2 ). Correlations between K-CPT 2 and BRIEF-P scores were all non-significant and ranged from -0.197 to 0.121.

Table 2
ADHD vs. non-ADHD performance in measures of executive function

The frequency of cases with worse performance (> 1 standard deviation [SD] from the normative mean) on both instruments was also analyzed. Regarding BRIEF-P scores, 12 children (15.2%) satisfied this condition for the ISCI. For the FI and the EMI, 6 (7.6%) and 17 (21.5%) children respectively were included in the groups with the worst performance. No association was found between ADHD and worse performance in BRIEF-P indexes (see Table 3 ).

Table 3
Executive function deficits in ADHD vs. non-ADHD children

Regarding worse K-CPT 2 performance (defined as > 1 SD from the normative mean), 69 children (98.6%) fulfilled this condition for the hit reaction time standard deviation (HRT SD) index, 60 (85.7%) for variability, 42 (60%) for hit reaction time block change (HRT block change), and 55 (78.6%) for hit reaction time interstimulus-interval (HRT ISI). The numbers of children classified with worse performance according to the results for the indexes were as follows: omissions 38 (54.3%), commissions 31 (44.3%), perseverations 15 (21.4%), and HRT 20 (28.6%). We also did not find an association between ADHD and worse performance in the K-CPT 2 scores (see Table 3 ).

Discussion

Consistent findings of behavioral and/or cognitive profiles in preterm born children have provided evidence of a “preterm phenotype”. Psychiatric disorders, especially ADHD, anxiety, and autism spectrum disorder, are the most prevalent among subjects born preterm. This preterm behavioral phenotype is usually found in association with cognitive impairment.2525. Johnson S, Marlow, N. Preterm birth and childhood psychiatric. Pediatric Res. 2011;69:5-2. Investigations with adults born prematurely at VLBW support these findings and show that preterm birth has long-term consequences,2626. Pyhälä R, Wolford E, Kautiainen H, Andersson S, Bartman, Baumann N, et al. Self-reported mental health problems among premature adults: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2017;139:20162690. including neurocognitive deficits.2727. Pyhälä R, Lahti J, Heinonen K, Pesonen AK, Strang-Karlsson S, Hovi P, et al. Neurocognitive abilities in young adults with very low birth weight. Neurology. 2011;77:2052-60.

Specifically with relation to inhibitory control, current evidence shows that a deficit is present both in ADHD,88. Seidman JL. Neuropsychological functioning in people with ADHD across the lifespan. Clin Psychol Rev. 2006;26:466-85. , 1010. Pauli-Pott U, Becker K. Neuropsychological basic deficits in preschoolers at risk for ADHD: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31:626-37. , 1313. Çak TH, Kültür ÇES, Gökler B, Öktem F, Taskıran S. The Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function and Continuous Performance Test in preschoolers with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Investig. 2017;14:260-70. , 1414. Zhang HF, Shuai L, Zhang JS, Wang YF, Lu TF, Tan X, et al. Neuropsychological profile related with executive function of Chinese preschoolers with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: neuropsychological measures and Behavior Rating Scale of Executive Function-Preschool Version. Chin Med J (Engl). 2018;131:648-56. and in children born very prematurely.33. Aarnoudse-Moens H, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van Goudoever JB, Oosterlaan J. Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics. 2009;124:717-28. , 2828. Jaekel J, Eryigit-Madzwamuse S, Wolke D. Preterm toddlers’ inhibitory control abilities predict attention regulation and academic achievement at age 8 years. J Pediatr. 2016;169:87-92. However, to our knowledge, only one study has compared inhibitory control in VP/VLBW children with and without ADHD. Contrary to our original hypothesis, our results revealed no significant differences in inhibitory control between VP/VLBW children with and without ADHD.

In general, weak correlations are found between tests of executive function performance and behavior scales in the literature.2929. Barkley RA, Murphy KR. Impairment in occupational functioning and adult ADHD: the predictive utility of executive function (EF) ratings versus EF tests. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2010;25:157-73. The type of measurement selected (behavioral reports or laboratory measures) can also influence the results, either by their intercorrelations or by their associations with ADHD.3030. Sjöwall D, Thorell LB. A critical appraisal of the role of neuropsychological deficits in preschool ADHD. Child Neuropsychology. 2018;25:60-80. This is why we used both a performance measure (K-CPT 2) and a parent-rated behavior scale (BRIEF-P). We found consistent results across instruments, indicating no differences between ADHD and non-ADHD groups. Also, as described in the literature, correlations between the two types of measures were low.

Given this finding, we also analyzed whether the groups’ results were 1 SD or more from the normative mean, indicating a deficit in comparison to the normative samples for K-CPT2 and BRIEF-P. We chose a lenient threshold, of one standard deviation or 15.9% from the normal distribution mean, to identify cases that showed even a mild difficulty. The BRIEF-P results indicated that the ADHD group had a slightly higher frequency than that predicted by a normal distribution in the inhibitory self-control and emergent metacognition scales. This was only observed for emergent metacognition in the non-ADHD group. Nevertheless, the differences were not significant, and at least 70.83% of the children had performance considered to be in the normal range in both groups. Although previous studies show that EF deficits are present in ADHD and VP/VLBW patients, rating scales focus on a global and less specific observation of executive functioning in the everyday context. They also depend on parent ratings, which might be biased by different development expectations.3131. Isquith PK, Crawford JS, Espy KA, Gioia GA. Assessment of executive function in preschool-aged children. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2005;11:209-15. This could have influenced our results since a global developmental delay characterizes our sample.

Overall, K-CPT 2 scores indicated a higher number of participants in the clinical-suggestive range, with no significant differences between groups. Omissions, commissions, HRT, and HRT block change results indicated that 26 to 67% of the children had performance more than one standard deviation from the normative mean, independent of group status. These measures are related to inattention, impulsivity, speed of processing, and vigilance, respectively. With regard specifically to commissions, which is a measure related to impulsivity and inhibitory control, 38% of the ADHD group and 46% of the non-ADHD group fell within the clinical range, as suggested by the literature.1010. Pauli-Pott U, Becker K. Neuropsychological basic deficits in preschoolers at risk for ADHD: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31:626-37. , 3232. Willcutt GE, Sonuga-Barke JSE, Nigg TJ, Sergeant AJ. Recent developments in neuropsychological models of childhood psychiatric disorders. Adv Biol Psychiatry. 2008;24:195-226.

Regarding reaction time variability (RTV), 76.2% to 100% of the sample had impaired performance and once again no differences were found between groups. This variability is associated with most childhood psychiatric disorders, traumatic brain injury, dementia, and aging populations.3232. Willcutt GE, Sonuga-Barke JSE, Nigg TJ, Sergeant AJ. Recent developments in neuropsychological models of childhood psychiatric disorders. Adv Biol Psychiatry. 2008;24:195-226. , 3333. Tamm L, Narad ME, Antonini TN, O’Brien KM, Hawk LW, Epstein JN. Reaction time variability in ADHD: a review. Neurotherapeutics. 2012;9:500-8. Few studies have investigated this issue in premature children. Adolescents born prematurely show an impairment in reaction time variability when compared to controls. Regardless, this impairment is milder than the impairment observed in term-born ADHD adolescents.1515. James SN, Rommel AS, Cheung C, McLoughlin G, Brandeis D, Banaschewski T, et al. Association of preterm birth with ADHD-like cognitive impairments and additional subtle impairments in attention and arousal malleability. Psychol Med. 2018;48:1484-93. However, this result was not observed in the same sample when a different cognitive test was analyzed.3434. Rommel AS, James SN, McLoughlin G, Brandeis D, Banaschewski T, Asherson P, et al. Association of preterm birth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-like and wider-ranging neurophysiological impairments of attention and inhibition. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;56:40-50. Although this construct still remains understudied in prematurity, it is well established that a higher than usual RTV is present in ADHD and is also a marker of general psychopathology.3333. Tamm L, Narad ME, Antonini TN, O’Brien KM, Hawk LW, Epstein JN. Reaction time variability in ADHD: a review. Neurotherapeutics. 2012;9:500-8.

34. Rommel AS, James SN, McLoughlin G, Brandeis D, Banaschewski T, Asherson P, et al. Association of preterm birth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-like and wider-ranging neurophysiological impairments of attention and inhibition. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;56:40-50.
- 3535. Kofler MJ, Rapport MD, Sarver DE, Raike JS, Orban SA, Friedman LM, et al. Reaction time variability in ADHD: a meta-analytic review of 319 studies. Clin Psychol Rev. 2013;33:795-811. The pathophysiology of RTV is usually associated with abnormal frontal lobe volume and/or activation.3333. Tamm L, Narad ME, Antonini TN, O’Brien KM, Hawk LW, Epstein JN. Reaction time variability in ADHD: a review. Neurotherapeutics. 2012;9:500-8. Cortical maturation of the frontal lobe is delayed in ADHD children,3636. Shaw P, Eckstrand K, Sharp W, Blumenthal J, Lerch JP, Greenstein D, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:19649-54. and a meta-analysis showed an overall reduction in brain volume, including white and gray matter in VP/VLBW school-aged children.3737. Kieviet JF, Zoetebier L, van Elburg RM, Vermeulen RJ, Oosterlaan, J. Brain development of very preterm and very low-birthweight children in childhood and adolescence: a meta-analysis. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2012;54:313-23. Specifically in preschool children, a delay in normal cortical and surface development is associated with prematurity.3838. Phillips J, Montague E, Aragon M, Lowe J, Schrader R, Ohls R, et al. Prematurity affects cortical maturation in early childhood. Pediatr Neurol. 2011;45:213-9.

Another critical aspect to discuss is that our sample was characterized by a below-average mean IQ, irrespective of group. However, this is not an unexpected finding since low birth weight is also associated with below-average IQ. The high degree of nervous system immaturity and the greater susceptibility to neonatal complications may lead to cognitive impairment. The cerebral networks of children with low birth weight are less connected brain networks, with lower brain volumes and lower cortical surface area, which might result in impaired cognitive functions.22. Glass H, Costarino TA, Stayer AS, Brett C, Cladis F, Davis JP. Outcomes for extremely premature infants. Anesth Analg. 2015;120:1337-51. , 3939. Gu H, Wang L, Liu L, Luo X, Wang J, Li, J et al. A gradient relationship between low birth weight and IQ: a meta-analysis. Sci Rep. 2017;7:18035. Moreover, results of a meta-analysis also showed that ADHD is associated with lower overall cognitive ability when compared to healthy controls.4040. Frazier TW, Demaree HA, Youngstrom EA. Meta-analysis of intellectual and neuropsychological test performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology. 2004;18:543-55.

The nature of the relationship between intelligence and executive functions is still controversial.4141. García-Molina A, Tirapu-Ustárroz J, Luna-Lario P, Ibáñez J, Duque P. Are intelligence and executive functions the same thing? Rev Neurol. 2010;50:738-46. Although many variables are included in the broader concept of executive functions, they are separable to a certain degree and differently correlated to each other as well as to the many areas of the brain.4242. Royall DR, Lauterbach EC, Cummings JL, Reeve A, Rummans TA, Kaufer DI, et al. Executive control function: a review of its promise and challenges for clinical research. A report from the Committee on Research of the American Neuropsychiatric Association. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2002;14:377-405. Among all other executive functions, inhibition has been found to have a low correlation to IQ.4343. Friedman PN, Miyake A, Corley PR, Young ES, DeFries CJ, Hewitt KJ. Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychol Sci. 2006;17:172-9. The same is valid for RTV: although higher inconsistency is related to lower IQ, the variance explained by IQ is usually small.4444. Jensen RA. The importance of intraindividual variation in reaction time. Pers Individ Differ. 1992;13:869-81. A recent meta-analysis showed evidence for a low to moderate correlation with matrix intelligence tests - which are less affected by cultural aspects.4545. Doebler P, Sheffler B. The relationship of choice reaction time variability and intelligence: a meta-analysis. Learn Individ Differ. 2016;52:157-66. Considering the above, the participants’ low IQ may only partially explain the deficits in inhibitory control and RTVs found in our sample.

Our findings should be understood in the context of some limitations. Although the low average IQ observed in our sample is a common finding in VP/VLBW children and it may not fully explain our results, we cannot discard it as a limitation. Further studies investigating the performance of average-IQ preterm/low-birth preschoolers might improve our understanding of the impact of ADHD on executive functions in this population. Additionally, our moderate sample size in both groups might have decreased the power for detection of between-group differences. However, it is essential to note that all between-group ES were small for both BRIEF-P and K-CPT 2 scores.

Furthermore, the absence of a full-term control sample imposes limitations on our analyses, making us rely on normative data for comparisons. Still, regarding K-CPT 2 and BRIEF-P, validity studies for the Brazilian population were not available. Finally, another aspect we did not adequately control was use of medications. Although none of the participants were using ADHD medication, we do not have information accurately recorded in our dataset about other clinical medications that might interfere with inhibitory control.

Conclusions

This study provides evidence of performance deficits on inhibitory control tests in preschool children born VP and/or with VLBW. Presence of ADHD did not impose any additional burden on the children’s test performance. Parents’ reports seem to be less sensitive for capturing executive deficits in this population. Impairment in RTV, a more basic cognitive process, was found in most participants, independent of group status. Our findings suggest either that assessment of EFs using neuropsychological tasks and parent reports may not be sufficiently sensitive to differentiate between ADHD and non-ADHD in VP/VLBW preschool children, or that these children already have a level of impairment in executive functions that does not leave much room for additional impairment. More large-scale studies investigating the nature and long-term effects of inhibitory control deficits in these vulnerable populations are needed.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by Fundo para Pesquisa e Eventos de Incentivo (Fund for Incentive Research and Events) and Programa de Transtornos de Déficit de Atenção/Hiperatividade (ProDAH), Hospital de Clínicas de Porto Alegre (HCPA).

Bárbara Lacerda is a research scholarship recipient from Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq).

References

  • 1
    World Health Organization [Internet]. Preterm birth. 2017. https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/preterm-birth
    » https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/preterm-birth
  • 2
    Glass H, Costarino TA, Stayer AS, Brett C, Cladis F, Davis JP. Outcomes for extremely premature infants. Anesth Analg. 2015;120:1337-51.
  • 3
    Aarnoudse-Moens H, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van Goudoever JB, Oosterlaan J. Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics. 2009;124:717-28.
  • 4
    Zomignani AP, Zambelli HJL, Antonio MA. Desenvolvimento cerebral em recém-nascidos prematuros. Rev Paul Pediatr. 2009;27:198-203.
  • 5
    O’Shea MT, Downey C, Kuban CK. Extreme prematurity and attention deficit: epidemiology and prevention. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:578.
  • 6
    American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington: APA; 1952.
  • 7
    Lahey BB, Pelham WE, Loney J, Kipp H, Ehrhardt A, Lee SS, et al. Three-year predictive validity of DSM-IV attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children diagnosed at 4-6 years of age. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:2014-20.
  • 8
    Seidman JL. Neuropsychological functioning in people with ADHD across the lifespan. Clin Psychol Rev. 2006;26:466-85.
  • 9
    Breaux R, Griffith S, Harvey E. Preschool neuropsychological measures as predictors of later attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2016;44:1455-71.
  • 10
    Pauli-Pott U, Becker K. Neuropsychological basic deficits in preschoolers at risk for ADHD: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31:626-37.
  • 11
    Schachar R, Tannock R, Logan, GD. Inhibitory control, impulsivity and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clin Psychol Rev. 1993;13:721-39.
  • 12
    Jacobson LA, Crocetti D, Dirlikov B, Slifer K, Denckla MB, Mostofsky SH, et al. Anomalous brain development is evident in preschoolers with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2018;24:531-9.
  • 13
    Çak TH, Kültür ÇES, Gökler B, Öktem F, Taskıran S. The Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function and Continuous Performance Test in preschoolers with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Investig. 2017;14:260-70.
  • 14
    Zhang HF, Shuai L, Zhang JS, Wang YF, Lu TF, Tan X, et al. Neuropsychological profile related with executive function of Chinese preschoolers with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: neuropsychological measures and Behavior Rating Scale of Executive Function-Preschool Version. Chin Med J (Engl). 2018;131:648-56.
  • 15
    James SN, Rommel AS, Cheung C, McLoughlin G, Brandeis D, Banaschewski T, et al. Association of preterm birth with ADHD-like cognitive impairments and additional subtle impairments in attention and arousal malleability. Psychol Med. 2018;48:1484-93.
  • 16
    Potgieter S, Vervisch J, Lagae, L. Event related potentials during attention tasks in VLBW children with and without attention deficit disorder. Clin Neurophysiol. 2003;114:1841-9.
  • 17
    Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, et al. Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime Version (KSADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36:980-8.
  • 18
    Kaufman S. A short form of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence. J Consult Clin Psychol.1972;39:361-9.
  • 19
    Wechsler D. WPPSI-R – Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence – Revised. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1989.
  • 20
    Conners CK. Conners’ Kiddie Continuous Performance Test for Windows [computer program]. Toronto: Multi-Health; 2000.
  • 21
    Gioia GA, Espy KA, Isquith PK. Behavior rating inventory of executive function-preschool version. Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2003.
  • 22
    Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988.
  • 23
    Miranda M, Sinnes E, Pompeia S, Bueno O. O K-CPT em uma amostra brasileira: descrição do desempenho e comparação com as normas norte-americanas. Rev Psiquiatr Rio Gd Sul. 2009;31:60-6.
  • 24
    Posner K, Melvin GA, Murray DW, Gugga SS, Fisher P, Skrobala A, et al. Clinical presentation of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in preschool children: the preschoolers with attention-deficit/hyperactivity treatment study (PATS). J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2007;17:547-62.
  • 25
    Johnson S, Marlow, N. Preterm birth and childhood psychiatric. Pediatric Res. 2011;69:5-2.
  • 26
    Pyhälä R, Wolford E, Kautiainen H, Andersson S, Bartman, Baumann N, et al. Self-reported mental health problems among premature adults: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2017;139:20162690.
  • 27
    Pyhälä R, Lahti J, Heinonen K, Pesonen AK, Strang-Karlsson S, Hovi P, et al. Neurocognitive abilities in young adults with very low birth weight. Neurology. 2011;77:2052-60.
  • 28
    Jaekel J, Eryigit-Madzwamuse S, Wolke D. Preterm toddlers’ inhibitory control abilities predict attention regulation and academic achievement at age 8 years. J Pediatr. 2016;169:87-92.
  • 29
    Barkley RA, Murphy KR. Impairment in occupational functioning and adult ADHD: the predictive utility of executive function (EF) ratings versus EF tests. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2010;25:157-73.
  • 30
    Sjöwall D, Thorell LB. A critical appraisal of the role of neuropsychological deficits in preschool ADHD. Child Neuropsychology. 2018;25:60-80.
  • 31
    Isquith PK, Crawford JS, Espy KA, Gioia GA. Assessment of executive function in preschool-aged children. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2005;11:209-15.
  • 32
    Willcutt GE, Sonuga-Barke JSE, Nigg TJ, Sergeant AJ. Recent developments in neuropsychological models of childhood psychiatric disorders. Adv Biol Psychiatry. 2008;24:195-226.
  • 33
    Tamm L, Narad ME, Antonini TN, O’Brien KM, Hawk LW, Epstein JN. Reaction time variability in ADHD: a review. Neurotherapeutics. 2012;9:500-8.
  • 34
    Rommel AS, James SN, McLoughlin G, Brandeis D, Banaschewski T, Asherson P, et al. Association of preterm birth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-like and wider-ranging neurophysiological impairments of attention and inhibition. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;56:40-50.
  • 35
    Kofler MJ, Rapport MD, Sarver DE, Raike JS, Orban SA, Friedman LM, et al. Reaction time variability in ADHD: a meta-analytic review of 319 studies. Clin Psychol Rev. 2013;33:795-811.
  • 36
    Shaw P, Eckstrand K, Sharp W, Blumenthal J, Lerch JP, Greenstein D, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:19649-54.
  • 37
    Kieviet JF, Zoetebier L, van Elburg RM, Vermeulen RJ, Oosterlaan, J. Brain development of very preterm and very low-birthweight children in childhood and adolescence: a meta-analysis. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2012;54:313-23.
  • 38
    Phillips J, Montague E, Aragon M, Lowe J, Schrader R, Ohls R, et al. Prematurity affects cortical maturation in early childhood. Pediatr Neurol. 2011;45:213-9.
  • 39
    Gu H, Wang L, Liu L, Luo X, Wang J, Li, J et al. A gradient relationship between low birth weight and IQ: a meta-analysis. Sci Rep. 2017;7:18035.
  • 40
    Frazier TW, Demaree HA, Youngstrom EA. Meta-analysis of intellectual and neuropsychological test performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology. 2004;18:543-55.
  • 41
    García-Molina A, Tirapu-Ustárroz J, Luna-Lario P, Ibáñez J, Duque P. Are intelligence and executive functions the same thing? Rev Neurol. 2010;50:738-46.
  • 42
    Royall DR, Lauterbach EC, Cummings JL, Reeve A, Rummans TA, Kaufer DI, et al. Executive control function: a review of its promise and challenges for clinical research. A report from the Committee on Research of the American Neuropsychiatric Association. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2002;14:377-405.
  • 43
    Friedman PN, Miyake A, Corley PR, Young ES, DeFries CJ, Hewitt KJ. Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychol Sci. 2006;17:172-9.
  • 44
    Jensen RA. The importance of intraindividual variation in reaction time. Pers Individ Differ. 1992;13:869-81.
  • 45
    Doebler P, Sheffler B. The relationship of choice reaction time variability and intelligence: a meta-analysis. Learn Individ Differ. 2016;52:157-66.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    25 Nov 2020
  • Date of issue
    Oct-Dec 2020

History

  • Received
    09 Sept 2019
  • Accepted
    30 Mar 2020
Associação de Psiquiatria do Rio Grande do Sul Av. Ipiranga, 5311/202, 90610-001 Porto Alegre RS/ Brasil, Tel./Fax: (55 51) 3024 4846 - Porto Alegre - RS - Brazil
E-mail: trends@aprs.org.br