Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Effect of histamine H1 and H2 receptor antagonists, microinjected into cerebellar vermis, on emotional memory consolidation in mice

Abstract

This study investigated the effects of histamine H1 or H2 receptor antagonists on emotional memory consolidation in mice submitted to the elevated plus maze (EPM). The cerebellar vermis of male mice (Swiss albino) was implanted using a cannula guide. Three days after recovery, behavioral tests were performed in the EPM on 2 consecutive days (T1 and T2). Immediately after exposure to the EPM (T1), animals received a microinjection of saline (SAL) or the H1 antagonist chlorpheniramine (CPA; 0.016, 0.052, or 0.16 nmol/0.1 µL) in Experiment 1, and SAL or the H2 antagonist ranitidine (RA; 0.57, 2.85, or 5.7 nmol/0.1 µL) in Experiment 2. Twenty-four hours later, mice were reexposed to the EPM (T2) under the same experimental conditions but they did not receive any injection. Data were analyzed using one-way ANOVA and the Duncan test. In Experiment 1, mice microinjected with SAL and with CPA entered the open arms less often (%OAE) and spent less time in the open arms (%OAT) in T2, and there was no difference among groups. The results of Experiment 2 demonstrated that the values of %OAE and %OAT in T2 were lower compared to T1 for the groups that were microinjected with SAL and 2.85 nmol/0.1 µL RA. However, when animals were microinjected with 5.7 nmol/0.1 µL RA, they did not show a reduction in %OAE and %OAT. These results demonstrate that CPA did not affect behavior at the doses used in this study, while 5.7 nmol/0.1 µL RA induced impairment of memory consolidation in the EPM.

Chlorpheniramine; Ranitidine; Cerebellar vermis; Emotional memory consolidation


Introduction

The central histaminergic nervous system originates from the tuberomammillary nucleus (TMN) of the hypothalamus, and, in many species, it widely innervates almost the whole brain including the cerebellum and other subcortical motor structures (11. Haas H, Panula P. The role of histamine and the tuberomamillary nucleus in the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci 2003; 4: 121-130, doi: 10.1038/nrn1034.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1034...
). The four histamine receptors identified as H1, H2, H3, and H4 subtypes are expressed in the brain (11. Haas H, Panula P. The role of histamine and the tuberomamillary nucleus in the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci 2003; 4: 121-130, doi: 10.1038/nrn1034.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1034...
,22. Strakhova MI, Nikkel AL, Manelli AM, Hsieh GC, Esbenshade TA, Brioni JD, et al. Localization of histamine H4 receptors in the central nervous system of human and rat. Brain Res 2009; 1250: 41-48, doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.11.018.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2008....
). H1 and H2 receptors potentiate excitatory inputs while H3 receptors down regulate histamine synthesis and release as well as the release of other neurotransmitters (33. Leurs R, Smit MJ, Timmerman H. Molecular pharmacological aspects of histamine receptors. Pharmacol Ther 1995; 66: 413-463, doi: 10.1016/0163-7258(95)00006-3.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-7258(95)000...
). Because the H4 receptor has been recently discovered in the mammalian central nervous system, its role in the brain remains unclear.

The neural histaminergic system is involved in several behavioral and neurobiological functions, such as arousal, food intake, motor activity, nociception, learning, and memory (11. Haas H, Panula P. The role of histamine and the tuberomamillary nucleus in the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci 2003; 4: 121-130, doi: 10.1038/nrn1034.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1034...
,44. Brown RE, Stevens DR, Haas HL. The physiology of brain histamine. Prog Neurobiol 2001; 63: 637-672, doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(00)00039-3.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0301-0082(00)00...
). However, the part that histaminergic circuits play in mnemonic effects is complex. Histamine seems to have different effects in distinct brain regions and may have modulatory effects that differ according to memory type. The exact role of this neurotransmitter in learning processes and memory consolidation and the action of the receptor subtypes and how they affect key circuits related to a specific memory system are not well understood (55. Kohler CA, da Silva WC, Benetti F, Bonini JS. Histaminergic mechanisms for modulation of memory systems. Neural Plast 2011; 2011: 328602, doi: 10.1155/2011/328602.
https://doi.org/10.1155/2011/328602...
).

Previous studies have shown that the histamine-containing fibers project from the tuberomammillary nucleus to the cerebellar cortex and the deep cerebellar nucleus, with a high density of histaminergic terminations in the vermis and flocculus (66. Panula P, Pirvola U, Auvinen S, Airaksinen MS. Histamine-immunoreactive nerve fibers in the rat brain. Neuroscience 1989; 28: 585-610, doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(89)90007-9.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4522(89)900...
,77. Wada H, Inagaki N, Yamatodani A, Watanabe T. Is the histaminergic neuron system a regulatory center for whole-brain activity? Trends Neurosci 1991; 14: 415-418, doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(91)90034-R.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(91)900...
). A moderately dense network of histamine fibers has been seen in the molecular and granular layers of the cerebellum in several species including humans (11. Haas H, Panula P. The role of histamine and the tuberomamillary nucleus in the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci 2003; 4: 121-130, doi: 10.1038/nrn1034.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1034...
). These fibers run parallel to the Purkinje cell layer after traversing it perpendicularly.

Autoradiographic mapping and in situ hybridization experiments demonstrated the presence of H1 and H2 receptors in the rat cerebellar cortex and deep in the cerebellar nuclei (88. Arrang JM, Drutel G, Garbarg M, Ruat M, Traiffort E, Schwartz JC. Molecular and functional diversity of histamine receptor subtypes. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1995; 757: 314-323, doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb17489.x.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995...
). These studies suggest that histamine may play an important role in modulating the excitability of cerebellar neurons. The Purkinje cells of the cerebellar cortex and the neurons in the nucleus interpositus all exhibit H2-receptor-mediated excitatory responses when exposed to a histamine bath perfusion (99. Shen B, Li HZ, Wang JJ. Excitatory effects of histamine on cerebellar interpositus nuclear cells of rats through H(2) receptors in vitro. Brain Res 2002; 948: 64-71, doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(02)02950-5.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-8993(02)02...
). Granule cells are excited through the activation of H1 and H2 receptors (1010. Li WC, Tang XH, Li HZ, Wang JJ. Histamine excites rat cerebellar granule cells in vitro through H1 and H2 receptors. J Physiol Paris 1999; 93: 239-244, doi: 10.1016/S0928-4257(99)80157-0.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0928-4257(99)80...
,1111. Tian L, Wen YQ, Li HZ, Zuo CC, Wang JJ. Histamine excites rat cerebellar Purkinje cells via H2 receptors in vitro. Neurosci Res 2000; 36: 61-66, doi: 10.1016/S0168-0102(99)00109-1.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-0102(99)00...
).

The cerebellum has traditionally been considered an important motor structure, but several lines of evidence support the role of the cerebellum as more complex than previously thought and include more than just the regulation of motor responses (1212. Strata P, Scelfo B, Sacchetti B. Involvement of cerebellum in emotional behavior. Physiol Res 2011; 60 (Suppl 1): S39-S48.). An increasing number of studies have demonstrated its involvement in cognitive and emotional functions. Functional neuroimaging studies and studies of patients with cerebellar lesions have been conducted to elucidate the role of the cerebellum in the processing of emotion (1313. Schmahmann JD, Weilburg JB, Sherman JC. The neuropsychiatry of the cerebellum - insights from the clinic. Cerebellum 2007; 6: 254-267, doi: 10.1080/14734220701490995.
https://doi.org/10.1080/1473422070149099...

14. Tavano A, Borgatti R. Evidence for a link among cognition, language and emotion in cerebellar malformations. Cortex 2010; 46: 907-918, doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.07.017.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.07...
-1515. Turner BM, Paradiso S, Marvel CL, Pierson R, Boles Ponto LL, Hichwa RD, et al. The cerebellum and emotional experience. Neuropsychologia 2007; 45: 1331-1341, doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.023.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsycholog...
). Moreover, Ruediger et al. (1616. Ruediger S, Vittori C, Bednarek E, Genoud C, Strata P, Sacchetti B, et al. Learning-related feedforward inhibitory connectivity growth required for memory precision. Nature 2011; 473: 514-518, doi: 10.1038/nature09946.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09946...
) demonstrated that fear conditioning learning is specifically correlated with the growth of feedforward inhibition connectivity in hippocampal and cerebellar circuits.

Experimental evidence indicates that the cerebellum plays a role in emotional learning. The capacity to learn and retain fear-conditioned responses was investigated in hotfoot mutant mice. These animals are characterized by a primary deficiency in the synapses made by parallel fibers onto the Purkinje cells. In these mutant mice, the cerebellar dysfunction impairs learning, which suggests that these synapses are involved in fear memory consolidation (1717. Sacchetti B, Scelfo B, Strata P. The cerebellum: synaptic changes and fear conditioning. Neuroscientist 2005; 11: 217-227, doi: 10.1177/1073858405276428.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858405276428...
). Studies have related the cerebellar vermis to emotional memory consolidation, since vermis inactivation caused amnesic effects after a fear conditioning task (1818. Sacchetti B, Sacco T, Strata P. Reversible inactivation of amygdala and cerebellum but not perirhinal cortex impairs reactivated fear memories. Eur J Neurosci 2007; 25: 2875-2884, doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05508.x.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007...
). Thus, the participation of the vermis in emotional memory is independent of its role in sensory or motor processes, and the vermis may represent an interface between sensory stimuli, emotional state, and motor responses (1212. Strata P, Scelfo B, Sacchetti B. Involvement of cerebellum in emotional behavior. Physiol Res 2011; 60 (Suppl 1): S39-S48.,1818. Sacchetti B, Sacco T, Strata P. Reversible inactivation of amygdala and cerebellum but not perirhinal cortex impairs reactivated fear memories. Eur J Neurosci 2007; 25: 2875-2884, doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05508.x.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007...
).

Histaminergic modulation of learning and memory was studied using lesions and pharmacological interventions in the tuberomammilary nucleus and other decisive brain regions. In our first study (1919. Gianlorenco AC, Canto-de-Souza A, Mattioli R. Microinjection of histamine into the cerebellar vermis impairs emotional memory consolidation in mice. Brain Res Bull 2011; 86: 134-138, doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.05.014.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2...
), microinjection of histamine into the cerebellar vermis demonstrated that the cerebellar histaminergic system is involved in the process of consolidation of emotional memory. These results indicated that there was a dose-dependent inhibition of memory consolidation when histamine was injected into the cerebellar vermis in mice reexposed to the elevated plus maze (EPM). Therefore, in the present study, we investigated the effects of H1 and/or H2 receptor antagonists on emotional memory consolidation.

Material and Methods

Animals

Male Swiss mice (Universidade Federal de São Carlos, Brazil) weighing 25-35 g at the beginning of the experiments were housed in polypropylene cages (31×20×13 cm) in groups of five and were maintained under a 12:12-h light-dark cycle (lights on at 7:00 am) in a controlled environment at a temperature of 23±1°C and a humidity level of 50±5%. Food and drinking water were provided ad libitum, except during the brief test periods. All mice were experimentally naive, and the experimental sessions were conducted during the light period of the cycle (9:00 am to 1:00 pm).

Drugs

The H1 receptor antagonist chlorpheniramine (CPA) maleate salt and the H2 receptor antagonist ranitidine hydrochloride (RA; Sigma Chemical Co., USA) were prepared using saline as vehicle. Saline (SAL) was used as an experimental control. The doses were based on previous research (2020. Serafim KR, Gianlorenco AC, Daher FP, Mattioli R. H1-histamine receptors in the amygdala are involved in emotional memory but do not mediate anxiety-related behaviors in mice submitted to EPM testing. Brain Res Bull 2012; 89: 1-7, doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2012.06.009.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2...
) and on pilot work in our laboratory. The substances were coded, and the experimenter was blinded to the codes when the tests and behavioral analysis were performed.

EPM apparatus

The EPM used was similar to the one originally described by Lister (2121. Lister RG. The use of a plus-maze to measure anxiety in the mouse. Psychopharmacology 1987; 92: 180-185.). The EPM consisted of two open arms (30×5×0.25 cm) and two enclosed arms (30×5×15 cm) that were connected to a common central platform (5×5 cm). The apparatus was made of crystal acrylic and was raised 38.5 cm above floor level.

Stereotaxic surgery and drug infusion

Mice were intraperitoneally anesthetized using 100 mg/kg ketamine hydrochloride and 10 mg/kg xylazine solution in association with local anesthesia (3% lidocaine with norepinephrine 1:50,000) and were placed in a Stoelting stereotaxic instrument. A single, 7-mm stainless steel guide cannula (25 gauge) was implanted in the cerebellar vermis according to the following coordinates from the mouse brain atlas (2222. Franklin KBJ, Paxinos G. The mouse brain: in the stereotaxic coordinates. 2nd edn. San Diego: Academic Press; 2001.): 6.5 mm posterior to the bregma, 0 mm lateral to the midline, and 2.0 mm ventral to the skull surface. The guide cannula was fixed to the skull using dental acrylic and jeweler's screws. A dummy cannula (33-gauge stainless steel wire) was inserted into the guide cannula at the time of surgery to reduce the incidence of occlusion. Postoperative analgesia was provided for 3 days by adding acetaminophen (200 mg/mL) to the drinking water in a ratio of 0.2 mL acetaminophen to 250 mL water (i.e., the final concentration was 0.16 mg/mL).

Saline and drug solutions were infused into the cerebellar vermis using a microinjection unit (33-gauge cannula; Insight Equipamentos Científicos Ltda., Brazil), which extended 2.0 mm beyond the tip of the guide cannula. The microinjection unit was attached to a 5-µL Hamilton microsyringe via polyethylene tubing (PE-10), and the administration was controlled by an infusion pump (Insight Equipamentos Científicos Ltda.) that was programmed to deliver a volume of 0.1 µL over a period of 60 s. The microinjection procedure consisted of gently restraining the animal, inserting the injection unit, infusing the solution, and keeping the injection needle in situ for a further 60 s to avoid reflux. Confirmation of successful infusion was obtained by monitoring the movement of a small air bubble inside the PE-10 tubing.

General conditions and data collection

Three days after surgery, the animals were transported to the behavioral space and left undisturbed for at least 1 h before testing, to facilitate adaptation. The test was performed on 2 consecutive days, and the trials in the EPM were denoted Trial 1 and Trial 2. Mice were individually placed on the central platform of the maze facing the open arm and were able to explore the maze for 5 min.

In Trial 1, immediately after exposure to the EPM, the animals received a microinjection of the drugs as follows: in Experiment 1, SAL or 0.016, 0.052, or 0.16 nmol/0.1 µL CPA, and in Experiment 2, SAL or 0.57, 2.85, or 5.7 nmol/0.1 µL RA. Twenty-four hours later (Trial 2), the mice were reexposed to the EPM under the same experimental conditions as in Trial 1, with the exception that they did not receive an injection. Between subjects, the maze was thoroughly cleaned with 5% ethanol and a dry cloth. All tests were conducted under moderate illumination (140 lux) as measured on the central platform of the EPM and in an environment isolated from the rest of the room by a black protective curtain.

All sessions were video recorded with a digital camera that was linked to a computer in an adjacent room. Images were analyzed by a highly trained observer using X-PLO-RAT, which is an ethological analysis software package developed at the Laboratory of Exploratory Behavior, USP, Ribeirão Preto (2323. Garcia AM, Cardenas FP, Morato S. The effects of pentylenetetrazol, chlordiazepoxide and caffeine in rats tested in the elevated plus-maze depend on the experimental illumination. Behav Brain Res 2011; 217: 171-177, doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.09.032.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.09.03...
). Behavioral parameters were defined in a way that was consistent with previous studies (2121. Lister RG. The use of a plus-maze to measure anxiety in the mouse. Psychopharmacology 1987; 92: 180-185.,2424. Rodgers RJ, Cao BJ, Dalvi A, Holmes A. Animal models of anxiety: an ethological perspective. Braz J Med Biol Res 1997; 30: 289-304, doi: 10.1590/S0100-879X1997000300002.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X199700...
) and included the following observations: the frequency of open- and enclosed-arm entries (OAE and EAE; where an entry was defined as the entry of all four of an animal's paws into an arm) and the total amount of time spent in the open arms (OAT), enclosed arms (EAT), and central area (CT). These data were used to calculate the percentage of OAE [%OAE=(open entries/open+enclosed entries)×100], the percentage of OAT [%OAT=(open time/300)×100], and the percentage of EAT [%EAT=(enclosed time/300)×100]. The number of stretched-attend postures (SAP; an exploratory posture in which the body stretches forward and then retracts to its original position without any forward locomotion), immobility time (stillness but some movement of the chest), and the frequency of head dipping (HD; exploratory movement of head or shoulders over the sides of the maze) were also scored. Total SAP was considered a primary index of risk assessment, and HD was considered an index of exploratory behavior.

Histology

At the end of testing, all animals received a 0.1-µL infusion of 1% methylene blue according to the microinjection procedure described earlier. The animals received an anesthetic overdose, their brains were removed, and the injection sites were verified histologically according to the atlas of Franklin and Paxinos (2222. Franklin KBJ, Paxinos G. The mouse brain: in the stereotaxic coordinates. 2nd edn. San Diego: Academic Press; 2001.). Data from animals with injection sites outside the cerebellar vermis were excluded from the study. The final sample size of each cohort ranged between 9 and 12. Histology confirmed that a total of 78 mice had accurate cannula placement in the cerebellar dorsal vermis (Figure 1).

Figure 1
A, Schematic representation (adapted from Ref. 22) of sites of microinfusion (filled circles) into the cerebellum of mice. B, Photomicrograph showing a typical injection site (indicated by an arrow) in the cerebellar vermis.

Statistical analysis

All results were initially analyzed using the Levene test for homogeneity of variance. Data were analyzed using one-way ANOVA. When differences were indicated by significant F values, they were further analyzed using the Duncan multiple range test. A P value of less than 0.05 was considered to be significant.

Ethics

The experiments performed as part of this study were approved by the Animal Ethics Commission of the Universidade Federal de São Carlos (CEEA #049/09) and were in compliance with the norms of the Brazilian Neuroscience and Behavior Society, which are based on the US National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.

Results

Experiment 1: effects of the H1 receptor antagonist CPA on memory consolidation

One-way ANOVA showed no significant difference between the groups in Trial 1 for all the measures analyzed (Table 1). Therefore, the data were pooled because the animals had received no pharmacological treatment at that point. Data are summarized in Figure 2A and B, and Table 2. ANOVA showed differences in %OAE between sessions (F4,65=6.12, P=0.0003). The post hoc Duncan test indicated that all groups entered the open arms less often in Trial 2 in comparison with Trial 1.

Figure 2
Effects of H1 antagonist chlorpheniramine (CPA; 0.016, 0.052, or 0.16 nmol/0.1 µL) microinjected into the cerebellar vermis on A, the percentage of open-arm entries (%OAE) and B, the percentage of time spent in the open arms (%OAT) in Trials 1 and 2 in the elevated plus maze. Pool: animals exposed to the elevated plus maze with no pharmacological treatment. Data are reported as means±SE (n=9). *P<0.05 Trial 2 vs Trial 1 (Duncan test).

Figure 2B shows %OAT for the first and second sessions. ANOVA detected differences in %OAT between sessions (F4,65=6.94, P=0.0001), and post hoc analysis determined that animals explored the open arms for a shorter time in the second trial when they had been microinjected with SAL or CPA (0.016, 0.052, and 0.16 nmol/0.1 µL). These results demonstrate that CPA did not alter behavioral parameters at the doses used.

Table 2 shows the results for all other behaviors. ANOVA did not indicate differences in EAE (F4,65=1.08, P=0.37), which is an EPM index of general exploratory activity. ANOVA revealed significant differences between trials in OAT (F4,65=6.94, P=0.0001) and OAE (F4,65=3.96, P=0.006). Furthermore, there were differences between sessions for EAT (F4,65=10.20, P<0.0001), %EAT (F4,65=10.20, P<0.0001), and frequency of HD (F4,65=6.33, P=0.0002). ANOVA did not detect any significant differences in immobility time (F4,65=0.45, P=0.77), CT (F4,65=0.33, P=0.85), and total SAP (F4,65=1.13, P=0.34) between trials.

Experiment 2: effects of the H2 receptor antagonist RA on memory consolidation

One-way ANOVA did not determine that there were significant differences between the groups in Trial 1 for any of the measures analyzed (Table 1). ANOVA indicated that there were differences in %OAE (F4,87=3.76, P=0.007) between trials. The post hoc analysis indicated that differences in %OAE existed in groups that were microinjected with SAL (P=0.03), and 0.57 and 2.85 nmol/0.1 µL RA (P<0.05). The animals that received treatment with 5.7 nmol/0.1 µL RA did not have a reduced %OAE (Figure 3A).

Figure 3
Effects of H2 antagonist ranitidine (RA; 0.57, 2.85, or 5.7 nmol/0.1 µL) microinjected into the cerebellar vermis on A, the percentage of open-arm entries (%OAE) and B, the percentage of time spent in the open arms (%OAT) in Trials 1 and 2 in the elevated plus maze. Pool: animals exposed to the elevated plus maze with no pharmacological treatment. Data are reported as means± SE (n=9-12). *P<0.05 Trial 2 vs Trial 1 (Duncan test).

Figure 3B shows %OAT for the first and second sessions. ANOVA determined that there were differences in %OAT between sessions (F4,87=3.38, P=0.013). The Duncan test indicated that the animals that were microinjected with SAL (P=0.03) and 2.85 nmol/0.1 µL RA (P=0.009) exhibited a decreased %OAT in Trial 2 relative to Trial 1, while the groups microinjected with 0.57 and 5.7 nmol/0.1 µL RA did not reduce %OAT. These results demonstrated that the H2 antagonist RA, at the dose of 5.7 nmol/0.1 µL, impairs memory consolidation in mice reexposed to the EPM.

Table 3 shows the results for all other behaviors. ANOVA revealed significant differences in the OAT between trials (F4,87=3.38, P=0.01). Post hoc comparisons indicated that differences existed for the groups microinjected with SAL (P=0.03) and 2.85 nmol/0.1 µL RA (P=0.03). Additionally, differences between sessions for OAE (F4,87=2.99, P=0.02), EAT (F4,87=7.59, P<0.001), %EAT (F4,87=7.59, P<0.001), CT (F4,87=4.60, P=0.002), and EAE (F4,87=3.22, P=0.01) and the frequency of HD (F4,87=5.14, P=0.0009) were detected. ANOVA did not detect any significant differences between trials in immobility time (F4,87=1.30, P=0.28) or total SAP (F4,87=1.41, P=0.23).

Discussion

The primary findings of the present study are that infusion into the cerebellar vermis with the H1 antagonist CPA (0.016, 0.052, and 0.16 nmol/0.1 µL) did not show any behavioral effects, whereas microinjection with the H2 antagonist 5.7 nmol/0.1 µL RA impaired emotional memory consolidation in mice reexposed to the EPM.

In the EPM, the behavior expressed during the test are due to a conflict between motivation to explore the maze and the natural tendency to avoid open spaces (2424. Rodgers RJ, Cao BJ, Dalvi A, Holmes A. Animal models of anxiety: an ethological perspective. Braz J Med Biol Res 1997; 30: 289-304, doi: 10.1590/S0100-879X1997000300002.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X199700...
,2525. Bertoglio LJ, Carobrez AP. Previous maze experience required to increase open arms avoidance in rats submitted to the elevated plus-maze model of anxiety. Behav Brain Res 2000; 108: 197-203, doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00148-5.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00...
). According to File (2626. File SE. The interplay of learning and anxiety in the elevated plus-maze. Behav Brain Res 1993; 58: 199-202, doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(93)90103-W.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(93)901...
), after the initial exploration of the apparatus, rodents acquire, consolidate, and retrieve some memory related to exploration of potentially dangerous areas of the maze. Several studies show that EPM-experienced animals exhibit a significant decrease in %OAE and %OAT during retesting (1919. Gianlorenco AC, Canto-de-Souza A, Mattioli R. Microinjection of histamine into the cerebellar vermis impairs emotional memory consolidation in mice. Brain Res Bull 2011; 86: 134-138, doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.05.014.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2...
,2020. Serafim KR, Gianlorenco AC, Daher FP, Mattioli R. H1-histamine receptors in the amygdala are involved in emotional memory but do not mediate anxiety-related behaviors in mice submitted to EPM testing. Brain Res Bull 2012; 89: 1-7, doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2012.06.009.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2...
,2525. Bertoglio LJ, Carobrez AP. Previous maze experience required to increase open arms avoidance in rats submitted to the elevated plus-maze model of anxiety. Behav Brain Res 2000; 108: 197-203, doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00148-5.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00...
). Therefore, the test/retest protocol in the EPM has demonstrated usefulness to investigate both short- and long-term memory components (2727. Gazarini L, Stern CA, Bertoglio LJ. Protein synthesis in dorsal hippocampus supports the drug tolerance induced by prior elevated plus-maze experience. Neuroscience 2011; 179: 179-187, doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.01.057.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2...
).

Histaminergic projections to the cerebellar vermis, amygdala, and hippocampus can be involved in the modulation of emotional memory consolidation. It has been previously proposed that the amygdala and cerebellum are functionally interconnected during aversive learning (1212. Strata P, Scelfo B, Sacchetti B. Involvement of cerebellum in emotional behavior. Physiol Res 2011; 60 (Suppl 1): S39-S48.,1515. Turner BM, Paradiso S, Marvel CL, Pierson R, Boles Ponto LL, Hichwa RD, et al. The cerebellum and emotional experience. Neuropsychologia 2007; 45: 1331-1341, doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.023.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsycholog...
). According to Sacchetti et al. (2828. Sacchetti B, Scelfo B, Strata P. Cerebellum and emotional behavior. Neuroscience 2009; 162: 756-762, doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.01.064.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2...
), the vermis and amygdala may interact, and the vermal electrical stimulation modulates amygdala activity. These effects are mediated by both direct and indirect anatomical connections between the cerebellum and the limbic areas.

Evidence demonstrates the existence of the histaminergic receptors H1, H2, and H3 in the cerebellum of rodents (88. Arrang JM, Drutel G, Garbarg M, Ruat M, Traiffort E, Schwartz JC. Molecular and functional diversity of histamine receptor subtypes. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1995; 757: 314-323, doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb17489.x.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995...
). Generally, H1 and H2 receptors excite or potentiate excitatory impulses, while H3 activation mediates autoinhibition of TMN neurons (66. Panula P, Pirvola U, Auvinen S, Airaksinen MS. Histamine-immunoreactive nerve fibers in the rat brain. Neuroscience 1989; 28: 585-610, doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(89)90007-9.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4522(89)900...
,88. Arrang JM, Drutel G, Garbarg M, Ruat M, Traiffort E, Schwartz JC. Molecular and functional diversity of histamine receptor subtypes. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1995; 757: 314-323, doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb17489.x.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995...
).

Evidence regarding the role of the histaminergic system in the learning and memory process is controversial, and the function of the histaminergic receptors and how they affect the memory system is still unclear (55. Kohler CA, da Silva WC, Benetti F, Bonini JS. Histaminergic mechanisms for modulation of memory systems. Neural Plast 2011; 2011: 328602, doi: 10.1155/2011/328602.
https://doi.org/10.1155/2011/328602...
). H1 receptors mediate actions on brain activity, and classic antihistamines such as CPA act as H1 antagonists (11. Haas H, Panula P. The role of histamine and the tuberomamillary nucleus in the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci 2003; 4: 121-130, doi: 10.1038/nrn1034.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1034...
). Several studies have demonstrated the anxiolytic effects of CPA in behavioral tests (11. Haas H, Panula P. The role of histamine and the tuberomamillary nucleus in the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci 2003; 4: 121-130, doi: 10.1038/nrn1034.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1034...
,2929. Privou C, Knoche A, Hasenohrl RU, Huston JP. The H1- and H2-histamine blockers chlorpheniramine and ranitidine applied to the nucleus basalis magnocellularis region modulate anxiety and reinforcement related processes. Neuropharmacology 1998; 37: 1019-1032, doi: 10.1016/S0028-3908(98)00087-2.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3908(98)00...
). Furthermore, CPA involvement has been proposed in spatial learning and emotional memory processes; however, results in this area are indefinite. For instance, the pharmacological blockade of the H1 receptor with CPA improved spatial learning in the Morris water maze (3030. Hasenohrl RU, Weth K, Huston JP. Intraventricular infusion of the histamine H(1) receptor antagonist chlorpheniramine improves maze performance and has anxiolytic-like effects in aged hybrid Fischer 344xBrown Norway rats. Exp Brain Res 1999; 128: 435-440, doi: 10.1007/s002210050866.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s002210050866...
), but it conversely impaired spatial learning in the eight-arm radial maze (3131. Masuoka T, Mikami A, Yasuda M, Shinomiya K, Kamei C. Effects of histamine H(1) receptor antagonists on hippocampal theta rhythm during spatial memory performance in rats. Eur J Pharmacol 2007; 576: 77-82, doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2007.08.020.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejphar.2007.08...
). In a recent study, Serafim et al. (2020. Serafim KR, Gianlorenco AC, Daher FP, Mattioli R. H1-histamine receptors in the amygdala are involved in emotional memory but do not mediate anxiety-related behaviors in mice submitted to EPM testing. Brain Res Bull 2012; 89: 1-7, doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2012.06.009.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2...
) showed that microinjection of CPA in the amygdala impaired emotional memory performances at a dose of 0.16 nmol/0.1 µL, and suggested that the H1 receptors in the amygdala are not implicated in anxiety-like behaviors but are involved in emotional states induced by the T1/T2 EPM protocol in mice. However, in the present study, CPA microinjected into the cerebellar vermis did not affect behavioral measures in mice reexposed to the EPM.

Some studies have examined the role of H2 receptors on cognitive performance. In the study by Benetti et al. (3232. Benetti F, Baldi E, Bucherelli C, Blandina P, Passani MB. Histaminergic ligands injected into the nucleus basalis magnocellularis differentially affect fear conditioning consolidation. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2013; 16: 575-582, doi: 10.1017/S1461145712000181.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S146114571200018...
), the H2 receptor agonist ampthamine improved fear memory expression, and the authors suggested that activation of postsynaptic H2 receptors within the nucleus basalis magnocellularis by endogenous histamine is involved in the expression of fear responses. Another H2 receptor agonist, dimaprit, improved extinction of aversive memory, while the H2 receptor antagonist, RA, impaired this memory (3333. Bonini JS, da Silva WC, Da Silveira CK, Kohler CA, Izquierdo I, Cammarota M. Histamine facilitates consolidation of fear extinction. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2011; 14: 1209-1217, doi: 10.1017/S1461145710001501.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S146114571000150...
,3434. Fiorenza NG, Rosa J, Izquierdo I, Myskiw JC. Modulation of the extinction of two different fear-motivated tasks in three distinct brain areas. Behav Brain Res 2012; 232: 210-216, doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.04.015.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.04.01...
). Da Silva et al. (3535. da Silva WC, Bonini JS, Bevilaqua LR, Izquierdo I, Cammarota M. Histamine enhances inhibitory avoidance memory consolidation through a H2 receptor-dependent mechanism. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2006; 86: 100-106, doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.01.001.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2006.01.00...
) showed that knockout mice lacking the H1 and H2 receptor subtypes enhanced learning and memory in auditory and contextual fear conditioning tests, but impaired learning of the Barnes maze and short-term memory of an object recognition test. In addition, electrophysiological examination indicated that both knockout mice H1 and H2 showed impaired long-term potentiation in CA1 areas of the hippocampus (3535. da Silva WC, Bonini JS, Bevilaqua LR, Izquierdo I, Cammarota M. Histamine enhances inhibitory avoidance memory consolidation through a H2 receptor-dependent mechanism. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2006; 86: 100-106, doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.01.001.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2006.01.00...
). Recently, Da Silveira et al. (3636. Da Silveira CK, Furini CR, Benetti F, Monteiro SC, Izquierdo I. The role of histamine receptors in the consolidation of object recognition memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2013; 103: 64-71, doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.04.001.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2013.04.00...
) showed that the H1 receptor antagonist, pyrilamine, the H2 receptor antagonist, RA, and the H3 receptor agonist, imetit, injected in the CA1 region immediately, 30, 120, or 360 min posttraining, blocked long-term memory retention in a time-dependent manner (30-120 min) without affecting general exploratory behavior, anxiety state, or hippocampal function.

In the present study, animals that received 5.7 nmol/0.1 µL RA did not reduce open-arm exploration during retesting on the apparatus, which demonstrates that RA microinjected into the cerebellar vermis impairs emotional memory consolidation in mice reexposed to the EPM. Using a similar protocol, we recently demonstrated that animals microinjected with histamine into the cerebellar vermis did not avoid the open arms less on retesting, and pretreatment with CPA abolished the inhibitory effect of histamine on memory consolidation, while pretreatment with RA did not show any memory effect, which indicated that the H1 receptor is probably involved in histamine-induced emotional memory impairment (3737. Gianlorenco AC, Serafim KR, Canto-de-Souza A, Mattioli R. Emotional memory consolidation impairment induced by histamine is mediated by H1 but not H2 receptors. Brain Res Bull 2012; 89: 197-202, doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2012.09.003.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2...
). The present results did not reveal memory effects for the two lower doses of RA and further indicate that H1 mediation seems to be the main mechanism involved in EPM memory impairment. Therefore, the results of the higher dose of RA may be due to a massive blockade of H2 receptors by RA, which induced endogenous histamine to bind only to the H1 receptors reproducing the same kind of effect found with microinfusion of exogenous histamine.

Because emotional memory has an important role in controlling behavior and it is critical for survival, a memory deficit can expose the animal to a needlessly dangerous situation while persistence of an aversive memory is a considerable factor associated with the development of anxiety and fear disorders, including phobias and posttraumatic stress disorder. Therefore, in view of our results, these histamine-modulated effects leading to memory erasure are potentially important and may give clues to new studies regarding the histaminergic system and trauma-related disorders (3737. Gianlorenco AC, Serafim KR, Canto-de-Souza A, Mattioli R. Emotional memory consolidation impairment induced by histamine is mediated by H1 but not H2 receptors. Brain Res Bull 2012; 89: 197-202, doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2012.09.003.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2...
).

In conclusion, the results of the present study demonstrated that H2 receptor antagonist ranitidine in the cerebellar vermis impaired emotional memory consolidation in mice reexposed to the EPM.

Research supported by FAPESP (#2009/17496-3 and #2009/15836-1) and by CNPq (#300543/2010-7).

References

  • 1
    Haas H, Panula P. The role of histamine and the tuberomamillary nucleus in the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci 2003; 4: 121-130, doi: 10.1038/nrn1034.
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1034
  • 2
    Strakhova MI, Nikkel AL, Manelli AM, Hsieh GC, Esbenshade TA, Brioni JD, et al. Localization of histamine H4 receptors in the central nervous system of human and rat. Brain Res 2009; 1250: 41-48, doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.11.018.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2008.11.018
  • 3
    Leurs R, Smit MJ, Timmerman H. Molecular pharmacological aspects of histamine receptors. Pharmacol Ther 1995; 66: 413-463, doi: 10.1016/0163-7258(95)00006-3.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-7258(95)00006-3
  • 4
    Brown RE, Stevens DR, Haas HL. The physiology of brain histamine. Prog Neurobiol 2001; 63: 637-672, doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(00)00039-3.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S0301-0082(00)00039-3
  • 5
    Kohler CA, da Silva WC, Benetti F, Bonini JS. Histaminergic mechanisms for modulation of memory systems. Neural Plast 2011; 2011: 328602, doi: 10.1155/2011/328602.
    » https://doi.org/10.1155/2011/328602
  • 6
    Panula P, Pirvola U, Auvinen S, Airaksinen MS. Histamine-immunoreactive nerve fibers in the rat brain. Neuroscience 1989; 28: 585-610, doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(89)90007-9.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4522(89)90007-9
  • 7
    Wada H, Inagaki N, Yamatodani A, Watanabe T. Is the histaminergic neuron system a regulatory center for whole-brain activity? Trends Neurosci 1991; 14: 415-418, doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(91)90034-R.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(91)90034-R
  • 8
    Arrang JM, Drutel G, Garbarg M, Ruat M, Traiffort E, Schwartz JC. Molecular and functional diversity of histamine receptor subtypes. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1995; 757: 314-323, doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb17489.x.
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb17489.x
  • 9
    Shen B, Li HZ, Wang JJ. Excitatory effects of histamine on cerebellar interpositus nuclear cells of rats through H(2) receptors in vitro Brain Res 2002; 948: 64-71, doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(02)02950-5.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-8993(02)02950-5
  • 10
    Li WC, Tang XH, Li HZ, Wang JJ. Histamine excites rat cerebellar granule cells in vitro through H1 and H2 receptors. J Physiol Paris 1999; 93: 239-244, doi: 10.1016/S0928-4257(99)80157-0.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S0928-4257(99)80157-0
  • 11
    Tian L, Wen YQ, Li HZ, Zuo CC, Wang JJ. Histamine excites rat cerebellar Purkinje cells via H2 receptors in vitro Neurosci Res 2000; 36: 61-66, doi: 10.1016/S0168-0102(99)00109-1.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-0102(99)00109-1
  • 12
    Strata P, Scelfo B, Sacchetti B. Involvement of cerebellum in emotional behavior. Physiol Res 2011; 60 (Suppl 1): S39-S48.
  • 13
    Schmahmann JD, Weilburg JB, Sherman JC. The neuropsychiatry of the cerebellum - insights from the clinic. Cerebellum 2007; 6: 254-267, doi: 10.1080/14734220701490995.
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/14734220701490995
  • 14
    Tavano A, Borgatti R. Evidence for a link among cognition, language and emotion in cerebellar malformations. Cortex 2010; 46: 907-918, doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.07.017.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.07.017
  • 15
    Turner BM, Paradiso S, Marvel CL, Pierson R, Boles Ponto LL, Hichwa RD, et al. The cerebellum and emotional experience. Neuropsychologia 2007; 45: 1331-1341, doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.023.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.023
  • 16
    Ruediger S, Vittori C, Bednarek E, Genoud C, Strata P, Sacchetti B, et al. Learning-related feedforward inhibitory connectivity growth required for memory precision. Nature 2011; 473: 514-518, doi: 10.1038/nature09946.
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09946
  • 17
    Sacchetti B, Scelfo B, Strata P. The cerebellum: synaptic changes and fear conditioning. Neuroscientist 2005; 11: 217-227, doi: 10.1177/1073858405276428.
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858405276428
  • 18
    Sacchetti B, Sacco T, Strata P. Reversible inactivation of amygdala and cerebellum but not perirhinal cortex impairs reactivated fear memories. Eur J Neurosci 2007; 25: 2875-2884, doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05508.x.
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05508.x
  • 19
    Gianlorenco AC, Canto-de-Souza A, Mattioli R. Microinjection of histamine into the cerebellar vermis impairs emotional memory consolidation in mice. Brain Res Bull 2011; 86: 134-138, doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.05.014.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.05.014
  • 20
    Serafim KR, Gianlorenco AC, Daher FP, Mattioli R. H1-histamine receptors in the amygdala are involved in emotional memory but do not mediate anxiety-related behaviors in mice submitted to EPM testing. Brain Res Bull 2012; 89: 1-7, doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2012.06.009.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2012.06.009
  • 21
    Lister RG. The use of a plus-maze to measure anxiety in the mouse. Psychopharmacology 1987; 92: 180-185.
  • 22
    Franklin KBJ, Paxinos G. The mouse brain: in the stereotaxic coordinates. 2nd edn. San Diego: Academic Press; 2001.
  • 23
    Garcia AM, Cardenas FP, Morato S. The effects of pentylenetetrazol, chlordiazepoxide and caffeine in rats tested in the elevated plus-maze depend on the experimental illumination. Behav Brain Res 2011; 217: 171-177, doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.09.032.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.09.032
  • 24
    Rodgers RJ, Cao BJ, Dalvi A, Holmes A. Animal models of anxiety: an ethological perspective. Braz J Med Biol Res 1997; 30: 289-304, doi: 10.1590/S0100-879X1997000300002.
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X1997000300002
  • 25
    Bertoglio LJ, Carobrez AP. Previous maze experience required to increase open arms avoidance in rats submitted to the elevated plus-maze model of anxiety. Behav Brain Res 2000; 108: 197-203, doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00148-5.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00148-5
  • 26
    File SE. The interplay of learning and anxiety in the elevated plus-maze. Behav Brain Res 1993; 58: 199-202, doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(93)90103-W.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(93)90103-W
  • 27
    Gazarini L, Stern CA, Bertoglio LJ. Protein synthesis in dorsal hippocampus supports the drug tolerance induced by prior elevated plus-maze experience. Neuroscience 2011; 179: 179-187, doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.01.057.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.01.057
  • 28
    Sacchetti B, Scelfo B, Strata P. Cerebellum and emotional behavior. Neuroscience 2009; 162: 756-762, doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.01.064.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.01.064
  • 29
    Privou C, Knoche A, Hasenohrl RU, Huston JP. The H1- and H2-histamine blockers chlorpheniramine and ranitidine applied to the nucleus basalis magnocellularis region modulate anxiety and reinforcement related processes. Neuropharmacology 1998; 37: 1019-1032, doi: 10.1016/S0028-3908(98)00087-2.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3908(98)00087-2
  • 30
    Hasenohrl RU, Weth K, Huston JP. Intraventricular infusion of the histamine H(1) receptor antagonist chlorpheniramine improves maze performance and has anxiolytic-like effects in aged hybrid Fischer 344xBrown Norway rats. Exp Brain Res 1999; 128: 435-440, doi: 10.1007/s002210050866.
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s002210050866
  • 31
    Masuoka T, Mikami A, Yasuda M, Shinomiya K, Kamei C. Effects of histamine H(1) receptor antagonists on hippocampal theta rhythm during spatial memory performance in rats. Eur J Pharmacol 2007; 576: 77-82, doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2007.08.020.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejphar.2007.08.020
  • 32
    Benetti F, Baldi E, Bucherelli C, Blandina P, Passani MB. Histaminergic ligands injected into the nucleus basalis magnocellularis differentially affect fear conditioning consolidation. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2013; 16: 575-582, doi: 10.1017/S1461145712000181.
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/S1461145712000181
  • 33
    Bonini JS, da Silva WC, Da Silveira CK, Kohler CA, Izquierdo I, Cammarota M. Histamine facilitates consolidation of fear extinction. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2011; 14: 1209-1217, doi: 10.1017/S1461145710001501.
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/S1461145710001501
  • 34
    Fiorenza NG, Rosa J, Izquierdo I, Myskiw JC. Modulation of the extinction of two different fear-motivated tasks in three distinct brain areas. Behav Brain Res 2012; 232: 210-216, doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.04.015.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.04.015
  • 35
    da Silva WC, Bonini JS, Bevilaqua LR, Izquierdo I, Cammarota M. Histamine enhances inhibitory avoidance memory consolidation through a H2 receptor-dependent mechanism. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2006; 86: 100-106, doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.01.001.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2006.01.001
  • 36
    Da Silveira CK, Furini CR, Benetti F, Monteiro SC, Izquierdo I. The role of histamine receptors in the consolidation of object recognition memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2013; 103: 64-71, doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.04.001.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2013.04.001
  • 37
    Gianlorenco AC, Serafim KR, Canto-de-Souza A, Mattioli R. Emotional memory consolidation impairment induced by histamine is mediated by H1 but not H2 receptors. Brain Res Bull 2012; 89: 197-202, doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2012.09.003.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2012.09.003
  • First published online January 17, 2014.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Feb 2014

History

  • Received
    20 Aug 2013
  • Accepted
    10 Oct 2013
Associação Brasileira de Divulgação Científica Av. Bandeirantes, 3900, 14049-900 Ribeirão Preto SP Brazil, Tel. / Fax: +55 16 3315-9120 - Ribeirão Preto - SP - Brazil
E-mail: bjournal@terra.com.br