Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Discourse in aging: Narrative and Persuasive

DISCURSO NO ENVELHECIMENTO: NARRATIVA E PERSUASIVA

ABSTRACT

The growth in the elderly population has posed a social, economic and health challenge for the twenty-first century.

Objective:

Aging is often characterized by changes in cognitive functions which affect the receptive and expressive capabilities of language. Since language plays a significant role in human life, we evaluate the existence of age-related differences in narrative and persuasive discourses.

Methods:

The narrative discourse of 91 adults and persuasive discourse of 92 adults,aged from 19 to 75 years and stratified into four age groups,were examined.

Results:

There was a statistically significant difference between coherence in the elderly group and each of the other three age groups for both types of discourse. There was also a significant difference for the cohesion variable between the elderly and the first age group for narrative discourse only.

Conclusion:

The results of this study demonstrate that discourse is influenced by aging and type (genre) of discourse task. Therefore, it is essential for clinicians to take into account the linguistic needs of elderly and incorporate these into their clinical programs. Also, this finding can help clinicians to distinguish between discourses of normal aging and other neurologic disorders (for example dementia, right hemisphere damage, aphasia).

Key words:
aging; discourse; narration; persuasion

RESUMO

O aumento da população idosa colocou um desafio social, econômico e de saúde para o século XXI.

Objetivo:

O envelhecimento é frequentemente caracterizado pela presença de alterações nas funções cognitivas que afetam as capacidades receptivas e expressivas da linguagem. Como a linguagem desempenha um papel significativo na vida humana, avaliamos a existência de diferenças relacionadas à idade nos discursos narrativos e persuasivos.

Métodos:

O discurso narrativo de 91 adultos e o discurso persuasivo de 92 adultos com idade entre 19 e 75 anos, divididos em quatro faixas etárias, foram examinados.

Resultados:

Existe uma diferença estatisticamente significativa entre a coerência no grupo de envelhecimento e cada um dos outros três grupos nos dois tipos de discursos. Existe também uma diferença significativa para a variável coesão entre idosos e a primeira faixa etária apenas no discurso narrativo.

Conclusão:

Os resultados deste estudo demonstram que o discurso é influenciado pelo envelhecimento e tipo da tarefa discursiva. Portanto, é essencial que os médicos levem em consideração as necessidades linguísticas dos idosos e as incorporem em seus programas clínicos. Além disso, esses resultados ajudam os médicos a distinguir entre discursos de envelhecimento normal e outros distúrbios neurológicos (por exemplo, demência, lesões do hemisfério direito, afasia).

Palavras-chave:
envelhecimento; discurso; narração; persuasão

The growth in the elderly population has posed a social, economic and health challenge for the twenty-first century. According to the World Health Organization (WHO), the number of over-60s as a proportion of the global population is dramatically increasing.11 Beard JR, Officer AM, Cassels AK. The world report on ageing and health. Oxford University Press US; 2016. Older age is usually accompanied by a decline in physiological and physical performance.22 Nejaddadgar N, Fathi S, Amani F, Sadeghi R. Awareness about Alzheimer's disease and the prevalence of its preventive behaviors among elderly referred to health centers in Tehran, 2012. J Health Dev. 2014;3(3):212-21.

Aging is often characterized by changes in cognitive function which affect receptive and expressive language abilities.33 Marini A, Boewe A, Caltagirone C, Carlomagno S. Age-related differences in the production of textual descriptions. J Psycholing Res. 2005;34(5):439-63.,44 Riegel KF, Riegel RM. Changes in associative behavior during later years of life: A cross-sectional analysis. Hum Dev. 1964;7(1):1-32. Paying attention to language dysfunction is of immense importance as it dramatically affects patient communication with others. Language sample analysis is considered one of the best approaches for language skills assessment. In this method, different aspects of the language are analyzed after collecting linguistic samples.55 Justice LM, Bowles R, Pence K, Gosse C. A scalable tool for assessing children's language abilities within a narrative context: The NAP (Narrative Assessment Protocol). Early Child Res Q. 2010;25(2):218-34. When collecting a linguistic sample, various types of discourse, including descriptive, narrative, procedural, persuasive, interpretive, and conversational discourse can be used.66 Ball MJ, Perkins MR, Müller N, Howard S. The handbook of clinical linguistics: Wiley-Blackwell; 2009. These discourse types differ according to the linguistic and cognitive demands placed on the individual.77 Bliss LS, McCabe A. Comparison of discourse genres: Clinical implications. NHM. 2006;26:37.

Narrative discourse is a type of discourse involving the expression of events and activities in a temporal sequence66 Ball MJ, Perkins MR, Müller N, Howard S. The handbook of clinical linguistics: Wiley-Blackwell; 2009. and mostly elicited by visual stimuli, wherein the essential feature includes temporal and causal relations between events in these images.88 Mar RA. The neuropsychology of narrative: story comprehension, story production and their interrelation. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42(10): 1414-34. Picture description is one of the main methods for eliciting narrative discourse,99 Cannizzaro MS, Coelho CA. Analysis of narrative discourse structure as an ecologically relevant measure of executive function in adults. J Psycholing Res. 2013;42(6):527-49. leading to a better understanding of language skills.1010 Capilouto GJ, Wright HH, Maddy KM. Microlinguistic processes that contribute to the ability to relay main events: influence of age. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2016;23(4):445-63. Persuasion is a complex and essential skill, which continues to develop into early adulthood.1111 Nippold MA, Ward-Lonergan JM, Fanning JL. Persuasive Writing in Children, Adolescents, and AdultsA Study of Syntactic, Semantic, and Pragmatic Development. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2005;36(2):125-38. In general, compared to narrative discourse, persuasive discourse is considered more cognitively demanding.1212 Felton M, Kuhn D. The development of argumentive discourse skill. Discourse processes. 2001;32(2-3):135-53. Persuasive discourse attempts to express an opinion and give reasons to support that opinion66 Ball MJ, Perkins MR, Müller N, Howard S. The handbook of clinical linguistics: Wiley-Blackwell; 2009. This type of discourse may occur in both formal situations (e.g. school debates, school essays) and informal situations (e.g. convincing a friend to see a movie or parents to purchase the latest electronic gadget). The ability to persuade and use arguments effectively is considered a fundamental social interaction skill.1313 Bartsch K, London K, Campbell MD. Children's attention to beliefs in interactive persuasion tasks. Dev Psychol. 2007;43(1):111.

In fact, discourse is a complex and dynamic cognitive system in terms of some important processing dimensions: micro-linguistic, macro-structure and macro-linguistic. The micro-linguistic dimension is responsible for inter-sentential functions, the macro-structure is for across-sentence analyses, while the macro-linguistic dimension is responsible for intra-sentential functioning.1414 Coelho CA. Management of discourse deficits following traumatic brain injury: Progress, caveats, and needs. Semin Speech Lang. 2007; 28(2):122.,1515 Marini A, Andreetta S, del Tin S, Carlomagno S. A multi-level approach to the analysis of narrative language in aphasia. Aphasiology. 2011;25(11): 1372-92.

The micro-linguistic dimension organizes phonological (or graphemic) sequence chains and words (lexical processing), and determines the syntax needed for each word to make attractive sentences (syntactic processing). The macro-structural level of discourse or cohesion is defined as the set of the possibilities that exists in a sentence for connecting a given statement with previous statements. On the other hand, sentences are conjoined by various kinds of meaning relations described as cohesive ties.1414 Coelho CA. Management of discourse deficits following traumatic brain injury: Progress, caveats, and needs. Semin Speech Lang. 2007; 28(2):122. The macro-linguistic dimension determines the meaning of a word or sentence that has a proper context and connects sentences and statements together by means of cohesion and coherence, so as to formulate the main subject of discourse and integrate cognitive and linguistic characteristics.1515 Marini A, Andreetta S, del Tin S, Carlomagno S. A multi-level approach to the analysis of narrative language in aphasia. Aphasiology. 2011;25(11): 1372-92. In other words, coherence refers to overall stability in discourse. Hence, overall coherence indicates how each sentence in a discourse sample is related to the overall subject of the text.1616 Enos T. Encyclopedia of rhetoric and composition: Communication from ancient times to the information age: Routledge; 2011.

Studies on age-related differences in adult discourse abilities have yielded different results. Although some studies have been conducted regarding the linguistic changes in old age, they have produced conflicting results. Some scholars believe that elderly discourse varies in terms of microstructure.1717 Au R, Joung P, Nicholas M, Obler LK, Kass R, Albert ML. Naming ability across the adult life span. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 1995;2(4):300-11. However, findings of other researchers show that this aspect of language does not change in old age.33 Marini A, Boewe A, Caltagirone C, Carlomagno S. Age-related differences in the production of textual descriptions. J Psycholing Res. 2005;34(5):439-63.,1818 Evans DA, Beckett LA, Albert MS, Hebert LE, Scherr PA, Funkenstein HH, et al. Level of education and change in cognitive function in a community population of older persons. Ann Epidemiol. 1993;3(1):71-7. Although the findings of other studies reveal that older adults differ from young adults in terms of some microstructural aspects, including semantic paraphasia, no difference in sentence complexity was found.33 Marini A, Boewe A, Caltagirone C, Carlomagno S. Age-related differences in the production of textual descriptions. J Psycholing Res. 2005;34(5):439-63. This inconsistency in results is less present in the macro-structural dimension. Furthermore, according to one study, the number of correct information units (CIU)1919 Capilouto G, Wright HH, Wagovich SA. CIU and main event analyses of the structured discourse of older and younger adults. J Commun Disord. 2005;38(6):431-44. and main events1919 Capilouto G, Wright HH, Wagovich SA. CIU and main event analyses of the structured discourse of older and younger adults. J Commun Disord. 2005;38(6):431-44.,2020 Wright HH, Capilouto G, Wagovich S, Cranfill T, Davis J. Development and reliability of a quantitative measure of adults' narratives. Aphasiology. 2005;19(3-5):263-73. is significantly higher in young people’s discourse than in that of the elderly. Marini et al. also reported that older people produce discourses with significantly less thematic informativeness than young people.33 Marini A, Boewe A, Caltagirone C, Carlomagno S. Age-related differences in the production of textual descriptions. J Psycholing Res. 2005;34(5):439-63.

Difficulty in spoken language may erode the ability and desire of adults to communicate, which can affect evaluation of their language capability by themselves and others. Negative self-assessment may promote avoidance of social interaction, whereas being negatively evaluated by others may lead to speech oversimplification. This process can lead to a downward spiral,underscoring the importance of identifying the change in pattern of language during adulthood and old age.2121 Craik FI, Salthouse TA. The handbook of aging and cognition, Third Edition, New York, NY: Psychology Press, 2011.

Also, since disorders such as dementia, right hemisphere damage, aphasia, Alzheimer’s disease and other neurologic disorders compromise discourse,2222 Kemper S, Thompson M, Marquis J. Longitudinal change in language production: Effects of aging and dementia on grammatical complexity and semantic content. Psychol Aging. 2001;16(4):600-14.

23 Marini A, Carlomagno S, Caltagirone C, Nocentini U. The role played by the right hemisphere in the organization of complex textual structures. Brain Lang. 2005;93(1):46-54.

24 Glosser G, Deser T. Patterns of discourse production among neurological patients with fluent language disorders. Brain Lang. 1991;40(1):67-88.
-2525 Farivar M, Anaraki ZG, Derakhshandeh F, Baharloei N, Poorjavad M. Narrative discourse in Persian-speaking patients with mild Alzheimer's disease. Dement Neuropsychol. 2019;13(2):225-31. discourse changes during normal aging need to be reviewed. This could help clinicians screen for these disorders in older adults.

Features and rules that influence communication in different languages vary and the results of discourse studies might be affected by these disparities among languages assessed. As an example, in contrast to English, Persian is a pro-drop language with stronger conjugation, where verbs change according to sentence subject.2626 Mohammad R. A Contrastive and Comparative Study of Ellipsis in Persian and English. Mediterranean Journal of Humanities II/2, 2012: 213-8.,2727 Ghayoumi Anaraki Z, Marini A, Yadegari F, Mahmoodi Bakhtiari B, Fakharian E, Rahgozar M, et al. Narrative Discourse Impairments in Persian-Speaking Persons with Traumatic Brain Injury: A Pilot Study. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 2014;66(6):273-9. Therefore, studying discourse features in the Persian language which might be affected by aging, can be of great help to speech and language therapists to better manage elderly people to achieve more effective communication.

Taking into consideration this assumption, the aim of this study is to evaluate the existence of age-related differences in macro-structural and macro-linguistic levels of narrative and persuasive discourse - the two most useful types of discourse- in Persian individuals aged 19 to 75 years of different age groups.

METHODS

Participants

Of 100 subjects invited to participate in the research, 92 subjects took part in the persuasive discourse test and 91 subjects in the narrative discourse test. Participants were stratified into 4 age groups: 19-24, 25-39, 40-60, and 75-60 years. The World Health Organization uses age 60 and older when referring to elderly populations.2121 Craik FI, Salthouse TA. The handbook of aging and cognition, Third Edition, New York, NY: Psychology Press, 2011.,2828 WHO. Health topics-Ageing 2016. Available from: http://www.who.int/topics/ageing/en/.
http://www.who.int/topics/ageing/en/...

The inclusion criteria for the study were: (a) to be monolingual (Persian); (b) having reading and writing literacy skills; (c) no history of stroke or neurological dysfunction; (d) a healthy auditory system; e) a healthy visual system; (f) no history of psychiatric illness, progressive neurological disease, non-verbal cognitive disease or dementia and depression; (g), no cognitive impairment; (i) being in the 19-74 age group. Participants’ demographic information is presented in Table 1.

Table 1
Demographic characteristics of subjects in different age groups.

Procedures

This is a cross-sectional study and the research protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation sciences. All participants met personally with the examiner to carry out the narrative tasks. The examiner did not impose any time limit, and recorded all sessions for later analysis. Initially, each subject filled out a written consent form regarding participation in the research and personal information.

The overall cognitive functioning of all participants was evaluated by administering the MMSE to ensure that the presence of residual deficits in cognition did not prevent meaningful participation, including the ability to execute the task and comprehend and follow instructions. The MMSE is a brief 30-item questionnaire used for screening cognitive impairments. The maximum score is 30. Cognitive impairment is diagnosed for scores <23.2929 Ansari NN, Naghdi S, Hasson S, Valizadeh L, Jalaie S. Validation of a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) for the Persian population: a pilot study. Appl Neuropsychol. 2010;17(3):190-5. All participants scored ≥23 in the adopted version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) in Persian, thus ensuring they were oriented to self, environment, and place and could participate in the study. A hearing screener confirmed that the included participants had hearing acuity within normal limits. A review of medical records and rehabilitation reports established that visual acuity and perception were sufficient to allow discrimination of pictures. None of the participants had a previous history of psychiatric or neurological illnesses.

Finally, in order to assess the abilities of narrative discourse, the Persian Narrative Discourse Test was used.2727 Ghayoumi Anaraki Z, Marini A, Yadegari F, Mahmoodi Bakhtiari B, Fakharian E, Rahgozar M, et al. Narrative Discourse Impairments in Persian-Speaking Persons with Traumatic Brain Injury: A Pilot Study. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 2014;66(6):273-9. The assessment of narrative skills is performed using story retelling, single picture description or serial picture descriptions. This test uses six serial picture descriptions-all on the same page- to evaluation narrative discourse. The topic of the narrative is a family party. The discourse parameters encoded in narratives will be described later.

Each participant was asked to narrate a story using six serial pictures. In order to avoid poor performance as a result of short-term memory constraints, the pictures were available to participants for viewing until the end of the narration. In narrative assessments using serial or single pictures, these pictures usually remain in front of narrator.2020 Wright HH, Capilouto G, Wagovich S, Cranfill T, Davis J. Development and reliability of a quantitative measure of adults' narratives. Aphasiology. 2005;19(3-5):263-73. The participants were asked to narrate what they saw in the pictures as a story. A narrative was considered complete when a participant indicated that he/she had nothing more to add.

The assessment of persuasive abilities was performed using a question. Participants were asked to give an opinion about whether it is better to use public transportation or a private vehicle and why participants would agree or disagree to the claims and reasons presented.2929 Ansari NN, Naghdi S, Hasson S, Valizadeh L, Jalaie S. Validation of a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) for the Persian population: a pilot study. Appl Neuropsychol. 2010;17(3):190-5. The topic was chosen because it seems to have a widespread appeal and increases the likelihood that participants have personal experiences and ideas. For feedback, the examiner responded only with encouragement to further the discourse (like “Anything else?”) and natural conversational acknowledgements. A discourse was complete when the subject expressed that he/she had nothing more to add.

While expressing the narrative and persuasive discourses, the speech of participants was recorded and then statements were transcribed. These transcriptions included phonemes, pauses, false primers,outsiders and phrases.

Finally, transcribed discourses were analyzed for the cohesion and general coherence indicators.

Linguistic measures

Connection between utterances in discourse is made by grammatical, conceptual or lexical ties called cohesion. These cohesive ties, as stated by Halliday and Hasan, are categorized into five classes: substitutions, ellipses, references, conjunctions, and lexical markers.3030 Halliday MAK, Hasan R. Cohesion in English: Routledge; 1976. Cohesion is also considered the macro-structural level of discourse.1414 Coelho CA. Management of discourse deficits following traumatic brain injury: Progress, caveats, and needs. Semin Speech Lang. 2007; 28(2):122. Analysis of cohesion in this study involves a measure of cohesion per C-unit, in which the cohesion is the total number of all cohesive ties counted in each discourse sample. A C-unit is ‘an independent clause with its modifiers’. It includes one main clause with all subordinate clauses attached to it.3131 Loban W. Language Development: Kindergarten through Grade Twelve. NCTE Committee on Research Report No. 18. 1976.

Another measure is global coherence or macro-linguistic level of discourse, which refers to the correlation of the meaning or content of an utterance to the general topic of the story.3131 Loban W. Language Development: Kindergarten through Grade Twelve. NCTE Committee on Research Report No. 18. 1976.,3232 Wright HH, Capilouto GJ, Koutsoftas A. Evaluating measures of global coherence ability in stories in adults. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2013;48(3):249-56. In other words, discourse coherence reflects the listener’s ability to interpret the overall meaning conveyed by the discourse. In this study, the four-point scale devised by Wright et al.3232 Wright HH, Capilouto GJ, Koutsoftas A. Evaluating measures of global coherence ability in stories in adults. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2013;48(3):249-56. (Table 2) is used to score the ability of participants to establish global coherence. An individual score is given to each C-unit, and then the average global coherence score is calculated.

Table 2
Scoring criteria for 4-point global coherence rating scale.32

Reliability

Inter-rater reliability for encodings was determined for the 10%of randomly selected recorded samples. A trained independent speech therapist transcribed and rescored 10% of there corded discourses for assessing inter-rater reliability.

Statistical analysis

The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was used to check the normality of the data. After confirmation of the normality of the data by the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test and the homogeneity of the variance of discourse cohesion, ANOVA and the Scheffe post-hoc test were used. Also, Kruskal-Wallis and Mann-Whitney tests were used for coherence, which had a non-normal distribution. Inter-rater reliability was analyzed by Cohen’s Kappa test. Data analysis was performed using SPSS software version 16 and the significance level was set at 0.05.

RESULTS

In this study, a narrative discourse of 91 adults and persuasive discourse of 92 adults aged between 19 and 75 year were examined. The mean and standard deviation of each of the variables have been presented in Table 3. The results of ANOVA test show that cohesion in narrative discourse differed statistically among groups (P<0.01) (Table 4).Also, the results of the Scheffe post-hoc test indicated that this variable differed between the elderly group and first age group only.

Table 3
Mean and standard deviation of values measured for subjects in different age groups.
Table 4
Significance levels for macrostructure level (cohesion) in different age groups.

Regarding coherence, the results of the Kruskal-Wallis test and its post-hoc test showed that coherence differed significantly between the elderly group and all other age groups on both narrative and persuasive discourses (P<0.01) (Table 5), but there were no significant differences between the other 3 groups.

Table 5
Significance levels for macrolinguistic level (coherence) in different age groups.

Reliability

The correlation coefficient for inter-rater reliability was >80% for all variables measured (p<0.05).

DISCUSSION

The purpose of the current study was to compare the narrative and persuasive discourses of four groups of healthy adults in order to test age-related changes in macro-structural (cohesion) and macro-linguistic level (coherence) measures using a narrative discourse and persuasive discourse task. The analysis of data indicated that aging had an impact in reducing the macro-linguistic level for both types of discourse examined, but there was a selective effect on macro-structural level according to type of discourse.

The measure of cohesion reflects how a participant connects his/her utterances and ideas, by means of different kinds of cohesive ties in the surface structure of discourse for a listener to follow. In this study, older participants had narrative discourses with significantly fewer cohesive ties per C-unit compared to the other age groups. This result may suggest that the older group had difficulties (conceptually and linguistically)linking their utterances in narrative discourse, but not in persuasive discourse. The type of discourse task has been cited among the factors that influence an individual’s performance.77 Bliss LS, McCabe A. Comparison of discourse genres: Clinical implications. NHM. 2006;26:37. This result is likely due to the fact that, unlike persuasive discourse tasks, narrative discourse uses picture sequences to facilitate the organization of the story structure by visually providing the temporal and logical sequence of events, making the establishment of cohesion easier in stories from picture sequences.33 Marini A, Boewe A, Caltagirone C, Carlomagno S. Age-related differences in the production of textual descriptions. J Psycholing Res. 2005;34(5):439-63.,99 Cannizzaro MS, Coelho CA. Analysis of narrative discourse structure as an ecologically relevant measure of executive function in adults. J Psycholing Res. 2013;42(6):527-49. However, older adults cannot exploit these structural aids for the formation of discourse as much as other participant groups.

In the study by Glosser and Desser, no difference was found in the use of cohesive ties in the narrative discourses.3333 Glosser G, Deser T. A comparison of changes in macrolinguistic and microlinguistic aspects of discourse production in normal aging. J Gerontol. 1992;47(4):P266-P72. It is noteworthy that, in their study, the narrations were elicited via an informal interview, but in the current study,sequential pictures elicited the narrative discourse.

Coherence is defined as relationships between the meanings underlying the surface structure of a discourse. It refers to a cognitive representation that reflects the interaction between linguistic/discourse characteristics and world knowledge.3333 Glosser G, Deser T. A comparison of changes in macrolinguistic and microlinguistic aspects of discourse production in normal aging. J Gerontol. 1992;47(4):P266-P72.,3434 Van Dijk TA. Discourse studies: A multidisciplinary introduction: SAGE Publications Ltd; 2011. On the other hand, impairments in a number of cognitive tasks (working and long-term memory) are seen as part of the normal ageing process in humans.3434 Van Dijk TA. Discourse studies: A multidisciplinary introduction: SAGE Publications Ltd; 2011.,3535 Connor L. Memory in old age: patterns of decline and preservation. Semin Speech Lang. 2001;22(2):117-25. It has been proposed that executive functions may be an important contributor to discourse generation.3535 Connor L. Memory in old age: patterns of decline and preservation. Semin Speech Lang. 2001;22(2):117-25.,3636 Douglas JM. Relation of executive functioning to pragmatic outcome following severe traumatic brain injury. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2010;53(2):365-82. Accordingly, this supra-sentential level of discourse is expected to be affected by aging.

The findings of the present study of decreased discourse ability for average cohesion and coherence during aging are in line with previous studies33 Marini A, Boewe A, Caltagirone C, Carlomagno S. Age-related differences in the production of textual descriptions. J Psycholing Res. 2005;34(5):439-63.,1919 Capilouto G, Wright HH, Wagovich SA. CIU and main event analyses of the structured discourse of older and younger adults. J Commun Disord. 2005;38(6):431-44.,3333 Glosser G, Deser T. A comparison of changes in macrolinguistic and microlinguistic aspects of discourse production in normal aging. J Gerontol. 1992;47(4):P266-P72.,3737 North AJ, Ulatowska HK, Macaluso-Haynes S, Bell H. Discourse performance in older adults. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 1986;23(4):267-83.

38 Schneider BA, Daneman M, Murphy DR, See SK. Listening to discourse in distracting settings: the effects of aging. Psychol Aging. 2000;15(1): 110.
-3939 Juncos-Rabadán O, Pereiro AX, Rodríguez MS. Narrative speech in aging: Quantity, information content, and cohesion. Brain Lang. 2005;95(3):423-34. indicating decline in discourse performance with age.

Although Mackenzie showed that educational level does not affect the narrative discourse of individuals with primary and higher education.4040 Mackenzie C. Adult spoken discourse: The influences of age and education. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2000;35(2):269-85. Because of the heterogeneity in educational level of the different age groups in the study, the results should be interpreted with caution. Additionally, as the sample size of the elderly group was small, the generalization of findings should be made with more caution.

To conclude, in general, the results of this study demonstrated that discourse is influenced by aging and type (genre) of discourse task. The elderly group had more difficulty on the narrative discourse. Therefore, it is essential for speech-language pathologists to take the linguistic and cognitive needs of elderly individuals into account and incorporate these into their clinical programs.

  • This study was conducted at the Department of Speech Therapy, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, Iran.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Beard JR, Officer AM, Cassels AK. The world report on ageing and health. Oxford University Press US; 2016.
  • 2
    Nejaddadgar N, Fathi S, Amani F, Sadeghi R. Awareness about Alzheimer's disease and the prevalence of its preventive behaviors among elderly referred to health centers in Tehran, 2012. J Health Dev. 2014;3(3):212-21.
  • 3
    Marini A, Boewe A, Caltagirone C, Carlomagno S. Age-related differences in the production of textual descriptions. J Psycholing Res. 2005;34(5):439-63.
  • 4
    Riegel KF, Riegel RM. Changes in associative behavior during later years of life: A cross-sectional analysis. Hum Dev. 1964;7(1):1-32.
  • 5
    Justice LM, Bowles R, Pence K, Gosse C. A scalable tool for assessing children's language abilities within a narrative context: The NAP (Narrative Assessment Protocol). Early Child Res Q. 2010;25(2):218-34.
  • 6
    Ball MJ, Perkins MR, Müller N, Howard S. The handbook of clinical linguistics: Wiley-Blackwell; 2009.
  • 7
    Bliss LS, McCabe A. Comparison of discourse genres: Clinical implications. NHM. 2006;26:37.
  • 8
    Mar RA. The neuropsychology of narrative: story comprehension, story production and their interrelation. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42(10): 1414-34.
  • 9
    Cannizzaro MS, Coelho CA. Analysis of narrative discourse structure as an ecologically relevant measure of executive function in adults. J Psycholing Res. 2013;42(6):527-49.
  • 10
    Capilouto GJ, Wright HH, Maddy KM. Microlinguistic processes that contribute to the ability to relay main events: influence of age. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2016;23(4):445-63.
  • 11
    Nippold MA, Ward-Lonergan JM, Fanning JL. Persuasive Writing in Children, Adolescents, and AdultsA Study of Syntactic, Semantic, and Pragmatic Development. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2005;36(2):125-38.
  • 12
    Felton M, Kuhn D. The development of argumentive discourse skill. Discourse processes. 2001;32(2-3):135-53.
  • 13
    Bartsch K, London K, Campbell MD. Children's attention to beliefs in interactive persuasion tasks. Dev Psychol. 2007;43(1):111.
  • 14
    Coelho CA. Management of discourse deficits following traumatic brain injury: Progress, caveats, and needs. Semin Speech Lang. 2007; 28(2):122.
  • 15
    Marini A, Andreetta S, del Tin S, Carlomagno S. A multi-level approach to the analysis of narrative language in aphasia. Aphasiology. 2011;25(11): 1372-92.
  • 16
    Enos T. Encyclopedia of rhetoric and composition: Communication from ancient times to the information age: Routledge; 2011.
  • 17
    Au R, Joung P, Nicholas M, Obler LK, Kass R, Albert ML. Naming ability across the adult life span. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 1995;2(4):300-11.
  • 18
    Evans DA, Beckett LA, Albert MS, Hebert LE, Scherr PA, Funkenstein HH, et al. Level of education and change in cognitive function in a community population of older persons. Ann Epidemiol. 1993;3(1):71-7.
  • 19
    Capilouto G, Wright HH, Wagovich SA. CIU and main event analyses of the structured discourse of older and younger adults. J Commun Disord. 2005;38(6):431-44.
  • 20
    Wright HH, Capilouto G, Wagovich S, Cranfill T, Davis J. Development and reliability of a quantitative measure of adults' narratives. Aphasiology. 2005;19(3-5):263-73.
  • 21
    Craik FI, Salthouse TA. The handbook of aging and cognition, Third Edition, New York, NY: Psychology Press, 2011.
  • 22
    Kemper S, Thompson M, Marquis J. Longitudinal change in language production: Effects of aging and dementia on grammatical complexity and semantic content. Psychol Aging. 2001;16(4):600-14.
  • 23
    Marini A, Carlomagno S, Caltagirone C, Nocentini U. The role played by the right hemisphere in the organization of complex textual structures. Brain Lang. 2005;93(1):46-54.
  • 24
    Glosser G, Deser T. Patterns of discourse production among neurological patients with fluent language disorders. Brain Lang. 1991;40(1):67-88.
  • 25
    Farivar M, Anaraki ZG, Derakhshandeh F, Baharloei N, Poorjavad M. Narrative discourse in Persian-speaking patients with mild Alzheimer's disease. Dement Neuropsychol. 2019;13(2):225-31.
  • 26
    Mohammad R. A Contrastive and Comparative Study of Ellipsis in Persian and English. Mediterranean Journal of Humanities II/2, 2012: 213-8.
  • 27
    Ghayoumi Anaraki Z, Marini A, Yadegari F, Mahmoodi Bakhtiari B, Fakharian E, Rahgozar M, et al. Narrative Discourse Impairments in Persian-Speaking Persons with Traumatic Brain Injury: A Pilot Study. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 2014;66(6):273-9.
  • 28
    WHO. Health topics-Ageing 2016. Available from: http://www.who.int/topics/ageing/en/
    » http://www.who.int/topics/ageing/en/
  • 29
    Ansari NN, Naghdi S, Hasson S, Valizadeh L, Jalaie S. Validation of a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) for the Persian population: a pilot study. Appl Neuropsychol. 2010;17(3):190-5.
  • 30
    Halliday MAK, Hasan R. Cohesion in English: Routledge; 1976.
  • 31
    Loban W. Language Development: Kindergarten through Grade Twelve. NCTE Committee on Research Report No. 18. 1976.
  • 32
    Wright HH, Capilouto GJ, Koutsoftas A. Evaluating measures of global coherence ability in stories in adults. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2013;48(3):249-56.
  • 33
    Glosser G, Deser T. A comparison of changes in macrolinguistic and microlinguistic aspects of discourse production in normal aging. J Gerontol. 1992;47(4):P266-P72.
  • 34
    Van Dijk TA. Discourse studies: A multidisciplinary introduction: SAGE Publications Ltd; 2011.
  • 35
    Connor L. Memory in old age: patterns of decline and preservation. Semin Speech Lang. 2001;22(2):117-25.
  • 36
    Douglas JM. Relation of executive functioning to pragmatic outcome following severe traumatic brain injury. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2010;53(2):365-82.
  • 37
    North AJ, Ulatowska HK, Macaluso-Haynes S, Bell H. Discourse performance in older adults. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 1986;23(4):267-83.
  • 38
    Schneider BA, Daneman M, Murphy DR, See SK. Listening to discourse in distracting settings: the effects of aging. Psychol Aging. 2000;15(1): 110.
  • 39
    Juncos-Rabadán O, Pereiro AX, Rodríguez MS. Narrative speech in aging: Quantity, information content, and cohesion. Brain Lang. 2005;95(3):423-34.
  • 40
    Mackenzie C. Adult spoken discourse: The influences of age and education. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2000;35(2):269-85.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    09 Dec 2019
  • Date of issue
    Oct-Dec 2019

History

  • Received
    09 Sept 2019
  • Accepted
    12 Apr 2019
Academia Brasileira de Neurologia, Departamento de Neurologia Cognitiva e Envelhecimento R. Vergueiro, 1353 sl.1404 - Ed. Top Towers Offices, Torre Norte, São Paulo, SP, Brazil, CEP 04101-000, Tel.: +55 11 5084-9463 | +55 11 5083-3876 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistadementia@abneuro.org.br | demneuropsy@uol.com.br