Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

The influence of educational status on motor performance and learning: a literature review

La influencia de la educación en el rendimiento y el aprendizaje de tareas motoras: una revisión de la literatura

Abstracts

Many studies have shown the impact of the educational status on cognitive and motor control. However, few studies in the area of Physical Therapy and Motor Behavior consider the educational status of the subjects. This study aimed to describe evidences about the influence of the educational status on motor behavior (and its repercussions on physiotherapeutic assessment) and on motor learning (and its repercussions on physiotherapeutic treatment). We included in this review national and international studies from 1998 to 2013 from SciELO, MEDLINE and LILACS databases. We used the keywords: educational status, schooling level, motor control, motor behavior, motor performance, and motor learning. Sixty studies were located and 28 were selected, because they followed the inclusion criteria: (1) to investigate the effect of education on motor performance; (2) be available in Portuguese or English; and (3) be available in Brazil. The review showed that the educational status of patients must be considered by the physiotherapists in experimental and clinical practice, because many studies have shown its influence on assessment and treatment of young and older adults.

Psychomotor Performance; Educational Status; Review


Muchos estudios han demostrado el impacto de la educación en las habilidades cognitivas y motoras. Sin embargo, muy pocos estudios en el campo de la Terapia Física y Comportamiento Motor consideran la escolarización en la muestra. Este estudio tuvo como objetivo describir la evidencia sobre la influencia de la educación en el comportamiento motor (y sus efectos sobre la evaluación de terapia física) y aprendizaje motor (y sus repercusiones en la fisioterapia). Un sondeo 1998-2013 buscó revistas nacionales e internacionales en las bases de datos SciELO, MEDLINE y LILACS, utilizando las palabras clave: educación, nivel de educación, control de motores, el comportamiento del motor, el rendimiento del motor y de aprendizaje motor. Se localizaron 60 estudios. De éstos, 28 fueron seleccionados porque cumplen con los criterios de inclusión: (1) investigar el efecto de la educación sobre el rendimiento del motor; (2) estar en Portugués o Inglés; y (3) estará disponible en Brasil. La revisión mostró que la educación de los pacientes debe ser considerada por los fisioterapeutas en situaciones experimentales y clínicos, ya que varios estudios han demostrado su influencia en la evaluación y el tratamiento de jóvenes y ancianos

Desempeño Psicomotor; Escolaridad; Revisión


Muitos estudos têm mostrado o impacto da escolaridade na cognição e motricidade. Porém, ainda poucos estudos na área de Fisioterapia e Comportamento Motor consideram a escolaridade da amostra. Este trabalho visou descrever evidências sobre a influência da escolaridade no comportamento motor (e suas repercussões na avaliação fisioterapêutica) e na aprendizagem motora (e suas repercussões no tratamento fisioterapêutico). Um levantamento de 1998 a 2013 buscou periódicos nacionais e internacionais nas bases de dados SciELO, MEDLINE e LILACS, com os descritores: escolaridade, nível educacional, controle motor, comportamento motor, desempenho motor e aprendizagem motora. Foram localizados 60 estudos. Desses, 28 foram selecionados por atenderem aos critérios de inclusão: (1) investigar o efeito da escolaridade sobre o desempenho motor; (2) estar em português ou inglês; e (3) estar disponível no Brasil. A revisão mostrou que a escolaridade dos pacientes deve ser considerada pelos fisioterapeutas em situações experimentais e clínicas, pois diversos estudos mostraram sua influência na avaliação e no tratamento de jovens e idosos.

Desempenho Psicomotor; Escolaridade; Revisão


INTRODUCTION

A relevant phenomenon in developing countries is low level of school education. In Brazil, the time of school education is six years among adults and three years among elderly11. Alves LC, Leite IC, Machado CJ. Conceituando e mensurando a incapacidade funcional da população idosa: uma revisão de literatura. Ciênc Saúde Coletiva. 2008;13(4):1199-207.. Learning how to read and write influences the functional organization of the human brain. Individuals with low educational status tend to present lower activation of cortical regions, basal ganglias, thalamus and cerebellum, in cognitive and motor tasks22. Castro-Caldas A, Petersson KM, Reis A, Stone-Elander S, Ingvar M. The illiterate brain: learning to read and write during childhood influences the functional organization of the adult brain. Brain. 1998;121(6):1053-63. and in the fusiform gyrus in perceptive tasks33. Dehaene S, Pegado F, Braga LW, Ventura P, Nunes Filho G, Jobert A, et al. How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science. 2010;330(6009):1359-64.. The low education changes the pattern of brain potentials and events44. Angel L, Fay S, Bouazzaoui B, Baudouin A, Isingrini M. Protective role of educational level on episodic memory aging: an event-related potential study. Brain Cogn. 2010;74(3):312-23. and the activation of the right hippocampus, posterior insula, thalamus and operculum55. Stern Y, Habeck C, Moeller J, Scarmeas N, Anderson KE, Hilton HJ, et al. Brain networks associated with cognitive reserve in healthy young and old adults. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15(4):394-402. while performing memory related tasks. It also diminishes the metabolism f glucose in the performing of cognitive tasks in the posterior cingulate gyrus, precuneus66. Eisenberg DP, London ED, Matochik JA, Derbyshire S, Cohen LJ, Steinfeld M, et al. Education-associated cortical glucose metabolism during sustained attention. Neuroreport. 2005;16(13):1473-6., the lateral, middle and superior temporal gyrus and the medial temporal one to the left77. Chiu NT, Lee BF, Hsiao S, Pai MC. Educational level influences regional cerebral blood flow in patients with Alzheimer's disease. J Nucl Med. 2004;45(11):1860-3.. Among individuals with Alzheimer's disease, the low schooling level increases the negative effect (cognitive changes) of beta-amyloid concentrations in brain tissue88. Bennet DA, Schneider JA, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Arnold SE. Education modifies the association of amyloid but not tangles with cognitive function. Neurology. 2005;65(6):953-5..

Individuals with low school education level have more difficulties with tasks of visual perception (they are slower and make more mistakes), for example, when identifying two-dimensional representations of objects99. Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Herrera Júnior E, Porto CS, Charchat-Fichman H, Carthert MT, et al. Performance of illiterate and literate nondemented elderly subjects in two tests of long-term memory. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10(4):634-8. , 1010. Reis A, Petersson KM, Castro-Caldas A, Ingvar M. Formal schooling influences two- but not three-dimensional naming skills. Brain Cogn. 2001;47(3):397-411.. Also, they take longer and make more mistakes in tasks of pictures cancellation1111. Brucki SM, Nitrini, R. Cancellation task in very low educated people. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2008;23(2):139-47.. As for the cognitive skills, studies show worse performance in language, arithmetics and memory. Illiterate individuals showed worse performance in pseudo words repetition tasks than literate ones and presented greater difficulty during tasks of verbal fluency, calculation, monetary representation and word retention99. Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Herrera Júnior E, Porto CS, Charchat-Fichman H, Carthert MT, et al. Performance of illiterate and literate nondemented elderly subjects in two tests of long-term memory. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10(4):634-8. , 1212. Kawano N, Umegaki H, Suzuki Y, Yamamoto S, Mogi N, Iguchi A. Effects of educational background on verbal fluency task performance in older adults with Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Int Psychogeriatr. 2010;22(6):995-1002.

13. Ardila A, Ostrosky-Solis F, Rosseli M, Gómez C. Age-related cognitive decline during normal aging: the complex effect of education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2000;15(6):495-513.
- 1414. Sahadevan S, Tan NJ, Tan T, Tan S. Cognitive testing of elderly Chinese people in Singapore: influence of education and age on normative scores. Age Ageing. 1997;26(6):481-6..

There are many studies on perceptive and cognitive differences caused by differences in schooling level, though few on motor differences. Neuropsychological tests used motor responses (speaking, writing, drawing), but the discussions tend to be centered in cognitive performance. On the other hand, many works which studied motor behavior ignored the educational status of the sample and do not discuss the possible influence of this trait over performance and learning. This work aimed at describing evidences on the influence of school education level on motor behavior (and their repercussions in Physical Therapy evaluation) and on motor learning (and its repercussions in Physical Therapy treatment).

METHODOLOGY

A bibliographic review of the last 15 years (1998 to 2013) was conducted, in both national and international journals, in the databases of SciELO, MEDLINE and LILACS. We used all combination between: educational status, schooling level, motor control, motor behavior, motor performance, motor learning.

All the articles and thesis which fulfilled the inclusion criteria were analyzed: (1) investigation of the effects of schooling on motor development; (2) written in Portuguese or English; and (3) to be available (print or digital format) in Brazil. Initially, 60 studies were located. From these, 28 were selected for having as a primary or secondary goal the investigation of the influence of school education on motor performance. The remaining ones were not included because they aimed at studying the influence of schooling level on perception (14 studies), on overall cognition, without considering the motor performance (11 studies), on the survival rate, income and/or quality of life (7 studies). Up next, the main results of the 28 selected studies are presented.

RESULTS

The studies found evidenced the effects of low schooling level in the decreased visual and motor abilities, lower ability and dexterity on motor tasks involving both upper and lower limbs99. Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Herrera Júnior E, Porto CS, Charchat-Fichman H, Carthert MT, et al. Performance of illiterate and literate nondemented elderly subjects in two tests of long-term memory. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10(4):634-8. , 1414. Sahadevan S, Tan NJ, Tan T, Tan S. Cognitive testing of elderly Chinese people in Singapore: influence of education and age on normative scores. Age Ageing. 1997;26(6):481-6.

15. Dansilio S, Charamelo A. Constructional functions and figure copying in illiterates or low-schooled Hispanics. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2005;20(8):1105-12.

16. Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Herrera Júnior E, Charchat-Fichman H, Porto CS. Performance in Luria's fist-edge-palm test according to educational level. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005;18(4):211-4.

17. Hong YJ, Yoon B, Shim YS, Cho AH, Lee ES, Kim YI, et al. Effect of literacy and education on the visuoconstructional ability of non-demented elderly individuals. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17(5):934-9.

18. Kim H, Chey J. Effects of education, literacy, and dementia on the Clock Drawing Test performance. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16(6):1138-46.

19. Ashendorf L, Vanderslice-Barr JL, McCaffrey RJ. Motor tests and cognition in healthy older adults. Appl Neuropsychol. 2009;16(3):171-6.

20. Neves ET. Aprendizagem de movimentos seqüenciais de dedos em idosos saudáveis: efeito da escolaridade [Dissertação de Mestrado]. São Paulo: Instituto de Psicologia, Neurociências e Comportamento da Universidade de São Paulo; 2008. 92 p.

21. Bramão I, Mendonça A, Faísca L, Ingvar M, Petersson KM, Reis A. The impact of reading and writing skills on a visuo-motor integration task: a comparison between illiterate and literate subjects. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2007;13(2):359-64.

22. Cavalcante KR. Avaliação do desempenho de idosos normais em um protocolo de produção e reconhecimento de gestos: influência do sexo, da idade e da escolaridade no perfil de normalidade [Dissertação de Mestrado]. São Paulo: Departamento de Neurologia da Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de São Paulo; 2004. 88 p.

23. Butler SM, Ashford JW, Snowdon DA. Age, education, and changes in the Mini-Mental State Exam scores of older women: findings from the Nun Study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1996;44(6):675-81.
- 2424. Machado MSA, Voos MC, Teixeira PPPS, Piemonte MEP, Ribeiro-do-Valle LE. The impact of educational status on dual-task performance. Braz J Motor Behav. 2011;6(1):32-8.. In general, it was observed a greater difficulty of individuals with lower educational status on learning new movements2020. Neves ET. Aprendizagem de movimentos seqüenciais de dedos em idosos saudáveis: efeito da escolaridade [Dissertação de Mestrado]. São Paulo: Instituto de Psicologia, Neurociências e Comportamento da Universidade de São Paulo; 2008. 92 p. . Besides that, the studies reported that individuals with low schooling level showed precocious signs of cognitive and motor aging. They also described lower capacity of less educated individuals with in expressing their ideas both orally and in writing.

In general, less educated elderly had lower functional independence and less coordination and grip 2323. Butler SM, Ashford JW, Snowdon DA. Age, education, and changes in the Mini-Mental State Exam scores of older women: findings from the Nun Study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1996;44(6):675-81.. The studies also reported higher prevalence of alterations on the executive function2525. Van der Elst W, Van Boxtel MPJ, Van Breukelen GJP, Jolles J. The Stroop color-word test: influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment. 2006;13(1):62-79. and dementia among less educated elderly2626. Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA, Evans DA, Mendes de Leon CF, Arnold SE, et al. Education modifies the relation of AD pathology to level of cognitive function in older persons. Neurology. 2003;60(12):1909-15.

27. Alley D, Suthers K, Crimmins E. Education and cognitive decline in older Americans: results from the AHEAD sample. Res Aging. 2007;29(1):73-94.

28. Paradise M, Cooper C, Livingston G. Systematic review of the effect of education on survival in Alzheimer's disease. Int Psychogeriatr. 2009;21(1):25-32.

29. Tun PA, Lachman ME. Age differences in reaction time and attention in a national telephone sample of adults: education, sex, and task complexity matter. Dev Psychol. 2008;44(5):1421-9.

30. Scazufca M, Almeida OP, Menezes PR. The role of literacy, occupation and income in dementia prevention: the São Paulo Ageing & Health Study (SPAH). Int Psychogeriatr. 2010;22(8):1209-15.
- 3131. Roe CM, Xiong C, Miller JP, Morris JC. Education and Alzheimer disease without dementia: support for the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Neurology. 2007;68(3):223-8.. The negative influence of low schooling was also described for the performance and cognitive and motor recovery of other diseases, such as head trauma3232. Walker AJ, Batchelor J, Shores EA, Jones M. Diagnostic efficiency of demographically corrected Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III and Wechsler Memory Scale-III indices in moderate to severe traumatic brain injury and lower education levels. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15(6):938-50., Parkinson's disease3333. Homann CN, Quehenberger F, Petrovic K, Hartung HP, Ruzincka E, Homann B, et al. Influence of age, gender, education and dexterity on upper limb motor performance in Parkinsonian patients and healthy controls. J Neural Transm. 2003;110(8):885-97. , 3434. Souza CO, Voos MC, Francato DV, Chien HF, Barbosa ER. Influence of educational status on executive function and functional balance in individuals with Parkinson disease. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2013;26(1):6-13.. The influence of schooling on therapeutic approaches for the prevention of falls among healthy elderly was also mentioned3535. Voos MC, Custódio EB, Malaquias Junior J. Relationship of executive function and educational status with functional balance in older adults. J Geriatr Phys Ther. 2011;34(1):11-8..

DISCUSSION

The influence of low school education on motor performance and learning

The low education results in the decrease of visual and motor abilities, poorer dexterity and praxis ability. Individuals with low educational level have worse performance in the task of building replicas with cubes, from figures drawn1414. Sahadevan S, Tan NJ, Tan T, Tan S. Cognitive testing of elderly Chinese people in Singapore: influence of education and age on normative scores. Age Ageing. 1997;26(6):481-6., copies of figures1515. Dansilio S, Charamelo A. Constructional functions and figure copying in illiterates or low-schooled Hispanics. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2005;20(8):1105-12. , 1717. Hong YJ, Yoon B, Shim YS, Cho AH, Lee ES, Kim YI, et al. Effect of literacy and education on the visuoconstructional ability of non-demented elderly individuals. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17(5):934-9. and clock drawing99. Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Herrera Júnior E, Porto CS, Charchat-Fichman H, Carthert MT, et al. Performance of illiterate and literate nondemented elderly subjects in two tests of long-term memory. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10(4):634-8. , 1818. Kim H, Chey J. Effects of education, literacy, and dementia on the Clock Drawing Test performance. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16(6):1138-46.. They also present worse performance in manual movements, such as imitating gestures, pushing buttons and placing opposing fingers1616. Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Herrera Júnior E, Charchat-Fichman H, Porto CS. Performance in Luria's fist-edge-palm test according to educational level. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005;18(4):211-4. , 1919. Ashendorf L, Vanderslice-Barr JL, McCaffrey RJ. Motor tests and cognition in healthy older adults. Appl Neuropsychol. 2009;16(3):171-6.

20. Neves ET. Aprendizagem de movimentos seqüenciais de dedos em idosos saudáveis: efeito da escolaridade [Dissertação de Mestrado]. São Paulo: Instituto de Psicologia, Neurociências e Comportamento da Universidade de São Paulo; 2008. 92 p.

21. Bramão I, Mendonça A, Faísca L, Ingvar M, Petersson KM, Reis A. The impact of reading and writing skills on a visuo-motor integration task: a comparison between illiterate and literate subjects. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2007;13(2):359-64.
- 2222. Cavalcante KR. Avaliação do desempenho de idosos normais em um protocolo de produção e reconhecimento de gestos: influência do sexo, da idade e da escolaridade no perfil de normalidade [Dissertação de Mestrado]. São Paulo: Departamento de Neurologia da Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de São Paulo; 2004. 88 p. .

Individuals with low education seem to adopt different strategies for execution and learning. Two strategies are used in order to help developing a movement. The first one is based on the visual analysis of the movement (sensorial), with the transformation of the visual information into a motor representation. The second one is based on the verbal interpretation of gesture (semantics) and it occurs when, for example, a verbal command is given for the movement. Individuals with high schooling level use both strategies when performing movements. However, the semantic strategy is more poorly elaborated among individuals with low schooling level, which makes them more dependent of visual strategies, resulting in flaws and lower scores in tests1616. Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Herrera Júnior E, Charchat-Fichman H, Porto CS. Performance in Luria's fist-edge-palm test according to educational level. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005;18(4):211-4..

In an experiment which associated an identification visual task and comparison of pictures to a motor task of alternance in steps from the ground to a platform, individuals with low schooling had more difficulties on isolated tasks and more mistakes on the visual task, when this was performed simultaneously to the motor task2424. Machado MSA, Voos MC, Teixeira PPPS, Piemonte MEP, Ribeiro-do-Valle LE. The impact of educational status on dual-task performance. Braz J Motor Behav. 2011;6(1):32-8.. They also had more difficulties in learning a sequence of finer oppositions movements, with more mistakes and lower speed in the trained sequence and more difficulty of learning generalization for a untrained task2020. Neves ET. Aprendizagem de movimentos seqüenciais de dedos em idosos saudáveis: efeito da escolaridade [Dissertação de Mestrado]. São Paulo: Instituto de Psicologia, Neurociências e Comportamento da Universidade de São Paulo; 2008. 92 p. .

The influence of low school education on aging

Education has a protective effect against the loss of cognitive-motor performance by aging2525. Van der Elst W, Van Boxtel MPJ, Van Breukelen GJP, Jolles J. The Stroop color-word test: influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment. 2006;13(1):62-79. , 2626. Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA, Evans DA, Mendes de Leon CF, Arnold SE, et al. Education modifies the relation of AD pathology to level of cognitive function in older persons. Neurology. 2003;60(12):1909-15.. Individuals with low schooling present early cognitive aging. Women with different schooling level participated in a series of studies known as "the nun study", but only if they became nuns since they were young and kept similar living habits. Throughout the aging process, nuns with low schooling level had earlier worsening of coordination and manual grip strength, visual acuity, ability to express written ideas and functional independency2323. Butler SM, Ashford JW, Snowdon DA. Age, education, and changes in the Mini-Mental State Exam scores of older women: findings from the Nun Study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1996;44(6):675-81..

Higher school education compensates the progression of the Alzheimer's disease and delays its clinical manifestations. The cognitive reserve theory proposes that education provides a cognitive and neurologic reservation, through neuronal changes or increase in efficiency of processing networks, thus, the clinical symptoms of degeneration due to Alzheimer's appear just later on2727. Alley D, Suthers K, Crimmins E. Education and cognitive decline in older Americans: results from the AHEAD sample. Res Aging. 2007;29(1):73-94.

28. Paradise M, Cooper C, Livingston G. Systematic review of the effect of education on survival in Alzheimer's disease. Int Psychogeriatr. 2009;21(1):25-32.
- 2929. Tun PA, Lachman ME. Age differences in reaction time and attention in a national telephone sample of adults: education, sex, and task complexity matter. Dev Psychol. 2008;44(5):1421-9.. There is a higher prevalence of dementia among elderly with low educational status3030. Scazufca M, Almeida OP, Menezes PR. The role of literacy, occupation and income in dementia prevention: the São Paulo Ageing & Health Study (SPAH). Int Psychogeriatr. 2010;22(8):1209-15..

The ability to inhibit a motor response, assessed by the Stoop test, worsens with aging in a more pronounced way among individuals with low schooling level2525. Van der Elst W, Van Boxtel MPJ, Van Breukelen GJP, Jolles J. The Stroop color-word test: influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment. 2006;13(1):62-79.. Individuals with eight or more years of study have higher protection against the reduction of cognitive response55. Stern Y, Habeck C, Moeller J, Scarmeas N, Anderson KE, Hilton HJ, et al. Brain networks associated with cognitive reserve in healthy young and old adults. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15(4):394-402. , 77. Chiu NT, Lee BF, Hsiao S, Pai MC. Educational level influences regional cerebral blood flow in patients with Alzheimer's disease. J Nucl Med. 2004;45(11):1860-3. , 2626. Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA, Evans DA, Mendes de Leon CF, Arnold SE, et al. Education modifies the relation of AD pathology to level of cognitive function in older persons. Neurology. 2003;60(12):1909-15.. These data were confirmed by studies with neuroimaging33. Dehaene S, Pegado F, Braga LW, Ventura P, Nunes Filho G, Jobert A, et al. How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science. 2010;330(6009):1359-64.

4. Angel L, Fay S, Bouazzaoui B, Baudouin A, Isingrini M. Protective role of educational level on episodic memory aging: an event-related potential study. Brain Cogn. 2010;74(3):312-23.

5. Stern Y, Habeck C, Moeller J, Scarmeas N, Anderson KE, Hilton HJ, et al. Brain networks associated with cognitive reserve in healthy young and old adults. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15(4):394-402.

6. Eisenberg DP, London ED, Matochik JA, Derbyshire S, Cohen LJ, Steinfeld M, et al. Education-associated cortical glucose metabolism during sustained attention. Neuroreport. 2005;16(13):1473-6.
- 77. Chiu NT, Lee BF, Hsiao S, Pai MC. Educational level influences regional cerebral blood flow in patients with Alzheimer's disease. J Nucl Med. 2004;45(11):1860-3. and post-mortem 88. Bennet DA, Schneider JA, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Arnold SE. Education modifies the association of amyloid but not tangles with cognitive function. Neurology. 2005;65(6):953-5. , 2626. Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA, Evans DA, Mendes de Leon CF, Arnold SE, et al. Education modifies the relation of AD pathology to level of cognitive function in older persons. Neurology. 2003;60(12):1909-15. , 3131. Roe CM, Xiong C, Miller JP, Morris JC. Education and Alzheimer disease without dementia: support for the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Neurology. 2007;68(3):223-8., which demonstrated that, among individuals with dementia with the same clinical impairment and the same severity classification for the disease had lower number of neuropathological findings. The hypothesis of compensation suggests that high schooling level may diminish cognitive decline up to a point at which basic skills Begin deteriorating with aging. At this moment, individuals with higher school education may present a faster progression of cognitive decline2727. Alley D, Suthers K, Crimmins E. Education and cognitive decline in older Americans: results from the AHEAD sample. Res Aging. 2007;29(1):73-94. , 2929. Tun PA, Lachman ME. Age differences in reaction time and attention in a national telephone sample of adults: education, sex, and task complexity matter. Dev Psychol. 2008;44(5):1421-9..

Besides studies on dementia, other studies approached the influence of low educational status on performance and on the recovery of other diseases. Walker et al.3232. Walker AJ, Batchelor J, Shores EA, Jones M. Diagnostic efficiency of demographically corrected Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III and Wechsler Memory Scale-III indices in moderate to severe traumatic brain injury and lower education levels. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15(6):938-50. assessed individuals with head trauma sequelae performing tasks of completing figures, codes, cubes, matrix reasoning, figures' arrangement, searching for symbols and setting up objects. There was an effect of age and school education level, with poorer performance among the elderly and among low schooling level3232. Walker AJ, Batchelor J, Shores EA, Jones M. Diagnostic efficiency of demographically corrected Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III and Wechsler Memory Scale-III indices in moderate to severe traumatic brain injury and lower education levels. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15(6):938-50.. Homann et al.3333. Homann CN, Quehenberger F, Petrovic K, Hartung HP, Ruzincka E, Homann B, et al. Influence of age, gender, education and dexterity on upper limb motor performance in Parkinsonian patients and healthy controls. J Neural Transm. 2003;110(8):885-97. assessed individuals with Parkinson's disease in a task of pushing buttons in a keyboard as fast as possible, but without compromising precision, and they observed that elderly and low educated individuals presented slower speeds.

The reduction of functional balance and the increase of fall risk, which occur with aging, are aggravated by low schooling levels. Low educated elderly tend to have lower scores in Berg's balance scale and in the Timed Up and Go test. Also, individuals with higher scores in Berg's scale have better performance in the trail making test, which suggests a relation between balance and the executive function3434. Souza CO, Voos MC, Francato DV, Chien HF, Barbosa ER. Influence of educational status on executive function and functional balance in individuals with Parkinson disease. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2013;26(1):6-13. , 3535. Voos MC, Custódio EB, Malaquias Junior J. Relationship of executive function and educational status with functional balance in older adults. J Geriatr Phys Ther. 2011;34(1):11-8..

Hester et al.3636. Hester RL, Kinsella GJ, Ong B, McGregor J. Demographic influences on baseline and derived scores from the trail making test in healthy older Australian adults. Clin Neuropsychol. 2005;19(1):45-54., Tombaugh3737. Tombaugh TN. Trail Making Test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;19(2):203-14. and Barnes et al.3838. Barnes DE, Tager IB, Satariano WA, Yaffe K. The relationship between literacy and cognition in well-educated elders. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2004;59(4):390-5. studied the influence of age and school education on the trail making test. The interference of age and school education was not the same in parts A and B of the test. In part A, the simpler one, the performance worsened with the growing age, but did not suffer interference from education. In part B, the most complex one, the individuals suffered both the influence of age and schooling. Therefore, for individuals with low schooling level, there was a most pronounced decline of performance each decade of life3636. Hester RL, Kinsella GJ, Ong B, McGregor J. Demographic influences on baseline and derived scores from the trail making test in healthy older Australian adults. Clin Neuropsychol. 2005;19(1):45-54.

37. Tombaugh TN. Trail Making Test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;19(2):203-14.
- 3838. Barnes DE, Tager IB, Satariano WA, Yaffe K. The relationship between literacy and cognition in well-educated elders. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2004;59(4):390-5.. Another study adapted the trail making test to be performed on wandering mats, instead of tracing the path on paper with a pencil. They observed that there was the same effect of age and schooling: elderly and lower educated individuals presented lower speed, especially in part B3939. Voos, MC. A influência da idade e da escolaridade na execução e no aprendizado de uma tarefa cognitivo-motora [Tese de Doutorado]. São Paulo: Instituto de Psicologia; 2010. 99 p..

Gitlin et al.4040. Gitlin LN, Winter L, Dennis MP, Hauck WW. Variation in response to a home intervention to support daily function by age, race, sex, and education. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2008;63(7):745-50. investigated whether the differences in schooling level among elderly could result in distinct gain after a guidance program which aimed at minimizing disparities between physical capacity and environmental demand, with exercises of balance and muslce strengthening, protection reaction training and raising safely from the ground, in case of falls, ways of using tools, energy conservation and modifications in the house for greater safety. The performance was assessed after 6 and 12 months of intervention. The group with lower schooling level presented higher improvement over the group with higher school education. A possible explanation to this fact is the lower accessibility to this kind of information, as well as to resources on assisting locomotion and lesser knowledge about compensatory strategies4040. Gitlin LN, Winter L, Dennis MP, Hauck WW. Variation in response to a home intervention to support daily function by age, race, sex, and education. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2008;63(7):745-50..

Higher schooling levels are associated to better executive eficiency1313. Ardila A, Ostrosky-Solis F, Rosseli M, Gómez C. Age-related cognitive decline during normal aging: the complex effect of education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2000;15(6):495-513. , 2525. Van der Elst W, Van Boxtel MPJ, Van Breukelen GJP, Jolles J. The Stroop color-word test: influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment. 2006;13(1):62-79. , 2929. Tun PA, Lachman ME. Age differences in reaction time and attention in a national telephone sample of adults: education, sex, and task complexity matter. Dev Psychol. 2008;44(5):1421-9.. In a task which would involve inhibitory control and standard alternance to verbal response to sound stimulation, individuals with higher education presented performances similar to the ones of less diminish the influence of aging in cognitive and motor tasks.

Implications for Physical Therapy

In Physical Therapy practice and in clinical research, it is important to consider that individuals with low schooling level may become more tense during assessments1313. Ardila A, Ostrosky-Solis F, Rosseli M, Gómez C. Age-related cognitive decline during normal aging: the complex effect of education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2000;15(6):495-513. and thus reduce the speed in the assessed task due to greater fear of making mistakes2020. Neves ET. Aprendizagem de movimentos seqüenciais de dedos em idosos saudáveis: efeito da escolaridade [Dissertação de Mestrado]. São Paulo: Instituto de Psicologia, Neurociências e Comportamento da Universidade de São Paulo; 2008. 92 p. , 2323. Butler SM, Ashford JW, Snowdon DA. Age, education, and changes in the Mini-Mental State Exam scores of older women: findings from the Nun Study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1996;44(6):675-81., for not being so familiar with testing situations, which are frequent in school activities. Since verbal fluency and memory may be worse in individuals with low schooling levels99. Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Herrera Júnior E, Porto CS, Charchat-Fichman H, Carthert MT, et al. Performance of illiterate and literate nondemented elderly subjects in two tests of long-term memory. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10(4):634-8. , 1212. Kawano N, Umegaki H, Suzuki Y, Yamamoto S, Mogi N, Iguchi A. Effects of educational background on verbal fluency task performance in older adults with Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Int Psychogeriatr. 2010;22(6):995-1002., the anamnesis and guidance using visual information associated to verbal one may have more positive results than the use of exclusively verbal resources. There are studies showing better perception of individuals with low schooling level in the face of real objects or pictures rather than with two-dimensional schematics1010. Reis A, Petersson KM, Castro-Caldas A, Ingvar M. Formal schooling influences two- but not three-dimensional naming skills. Brain Cogn. 2001;47(3):397-411., thus, it is interesting, when transmitting orientation, to use this kind of resource.

When reproducing sequential movements, low educated people may have more difficulty, which could justify the solicitation of simplified motor sequences, or subdivided ones. Despite having difficulty in the execution, learning and generalization of motor tasks, low educated individuals improve their performance with training2020. Neves ET. Aprendizagem de movimentos seqüenciais de dedos em idosos saudáveis: efeito da escolaridade [Dissertação de Mestrado]. São Paulo: Instituto de Psicologia, Neurociências e Comportamento da Universidade de São Paulo; 2008. 92 p. , 2424. Machado MSA, Voos MC, Teixeira PPPS, Piemonte MEP, Ribeiro-do-Valle LE. The impact of educational status on dual-task performance. Braz J Motor Behav. 2011;6(1):32-8. , 3838. Barnes DE, Tager IB, Satariano WA, Yaffe K. The relationship between literacy and cognition in well-educated elders. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2004;59(4):390-5., therefore, it is interesting to train these activities.

Considering the physiological process of aging, it is important to consider that individuals with low educational level may be more dependent on daily life activities, have worse balance and lower locomotion speed2626. Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA, Evans DA, Mendes de Leon CF, Arnold SE, et al. Education modifies the relation of AD pathology to level of cognitive function in older persons. Neurology. 2003;60(12):1909-15. , 3535. Voos MC, Custódio EB, Malaquias Junior J. Relationship of executive function and educational status with functional balance in older adults. J Geriatr Phys Ther. 2011;34(1):11-8. , 4141. Yong V, Saito Y. Are there education differentials in disability and mortality transitions and active life expectancy among Japanese older adults? Findings from a 10-year prospective cohort study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2012;67(3):343-53.

42. Gregory PC, Szanton SL, Xue QL, Tian J, Thorpe RJ, Fried LP. Education predicts incidence of preclinical mobility disability in initially high-functioning older women. The Women's Health and Aging Study II. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2011;66(5):577-81.

43. Jagger C, Matthews R, Melzer D, Matthews F, Brayne C; MRC CFAS. Educational differences in the dynamics of disability incidence, recovery and mortality: findings from the MRC Cognitive Function and Ageing Study (MRC CFAS). Int J Epidemiol. 2007;36(2):358-65.

44. Camargos MC, Machado CJ, Rodrigues RN. Disability life expectancy for the elderly, city of São Paulo, Brazil, 2000: gender and educational differences. J Biosoc Sci. 2007;39(3):455-63.
- 4545. Meijer WA, van Boxtel MP, Van Gerven PW, van Hooren SA, Jolles J. Interaction effects of education and health status on cognitive change: a 6-year follow-up of the Maastricht Aging Study. Aging Ment Health. 2009;13(4):521-9.. Elderly individuals with low schooling have higher dependence of help to perform functional activities4141. Yong V, Saito Y. Are there education differentials in disability and mortality transitions and active life expectancy among Japanese older adults? Findings from a 10-year prospective cohort study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2012;67(3):343-53.. According to Gregory et al.4242. Gregory PC, Szanton SL, Xue QL, Tian J, Thorpe RJ, Fried LP. Education predicts incidence of preclinical mobility disability in initially high-functioning older women. The Women's Health and Aging Study II. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2011;66(5):577-81., low schooling is an independent predictor of the incidence of difficulties on mobility at pre-clinical level. It is important to monitor these individuals, verifying the demands on multiprofessional intervention that they may present. This group tends to have less Access to health treatments and orientations, as well as to resources of assistance to locomotion and less knowledge of compensatory strategies4040. Gitlin LN, Winter L, Dennis MP, Hauck WW. Variation in response to a home intervention to support daily function by age, race, sex, and education. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2008;63(7):745-50., therefore, they will be the individuals who will probably benefit the most from these interventions.

Finally, in the rehabilitation o f individuals with dementia55. Stern Y, Habeck C, Moeller J, Scarmeas N, Anderson KE, Hilton HJ, et al. Brain networks associated with cognitive reserve in healthy young and old adults. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15(4):394-402. , 77. Chiu NT, Lee BF, Hsiao S, Pai MC. Educational level influences regional cerebral blood flow in patients with Alzheimer's disease. J Nucl Med. 2004;45(11):1860-3. , 99. Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Herrera Júnior E, Porto CS, Charchat-Fichman H, Carthert MT, et al. Performance of illiterate and literate nondemented elderly subjects in two tests of long-term memory. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10(4):634-8. , 1212. Kawano N, Umegaki H, Suzuki Y, Yamamoto S, Mogi N, Iguchi A. Effects of educational background on verbal fluency task performance in older adults with Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Int Psychogeriatr. 2010;22(6):995-1002. , 1818. Kim H, Chey J. Effects of education, literacy, and dementia on the Clock Drawing Test performance. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16(6):1138-46. , 2626. Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA, Evans DA, Mendes de Leon CF, Arnold SE, et al. Education modifies the relation of AD pathology to level of cognitive function in older persons. Neurology. 2003;60(12):1909-15.

27. Alley D, Suthers K, Crimmins E. Education and cognitive decline in older Americans: results from the AHEAD sample. Res Aging. 2007;29(1):73-94.

28. Paradise M, Cooper C, Livingston G. Systematic review of the effect of education on survival in Alzheimer's disease. Int Psychogeriatr. 2009;21(1):25-32.

29. Tun PA, Lachman ME. Age differences in reaction time and attention in a national telephone sample of adults: education, sex, and task complexity matter. Dev Psychol. 2008;44(5):1421-9.
- 3030. Scazufca M, Almeida OP, Menezes PR. The role of literacy, occupation and income in dementia prevention: the São Paulo Ageing & Health Study (SPAH). Int Psychogeriatr. 2010;22(8):1209-15., Parkinson's disease3333. Homann CN, Quehenberger F, Petrovic K, Hartung HP, Ruzincka E, Homann B, et al. Influence of age, gender, education and dexterity on upper limb motor performance in Parkinsonian patients and healthy controls. J Neural Transm. 2003;110(8):885-97. , 3434. Souza CO, Voos MC, Francato DV, Chien HF, Barbosa ER. Influence of educational status on executive function and functional balance in individuals with Parkinson disease. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2013;26(1):6-13., head trauma3232. Walker AJ, Batchelor J, Shores EA, Jones M. Diagnostic efficiency of demographically corrected Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III and Wechsler Memory Scale-III indices in moderate to severe traumatic brain injury and lower education levels. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15(6):938-50. and stroke4646. Ojala-Oksala J, Jokinen H, Kopsi V, Lehtonen K, Luukkonen L, Paukkunen A, et al. Educational history is an independent predictor of cognitive deficits and long-term survival in postacute patients with mild to moderate ischemic stroke. Stroke. 2012;43(11):2931-5. it is important to consider the individual's educational level in order to choose the best assessment strategy, for several studies showed that the impact f brain injuries in functional independency is higher among individuals with low schooling level.

The overview of the work presented in this review is observed in Chart 1.

Chart 1
Main studies investigating the influence of schooling on motor performance G1: studied group 1; G2: studied group 2; G3: studied group 3; IL: illiterate; L: literate; LSE: Low school education

CONCLUSION

The educational level of patients must be considered by physical therapists in experimental and clinical situations, for several studies showed its influence on the assessment and treatment of youngsters and elderlies.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Alves LC, Leite IC, Machado CJ. Conceituando e mensurando a incapacidade funcional da população idosa: uma revisão de literatura. Ciênc Saúde Coletiva. 2008;13(4):1199-207.
  • 2
    Castro-Caldas A, Petersson KM, Reis A, Stone-Elander S, Ingvar M. The illiterate brain: learning to read and write during childhood influences the functional organization of the adult brain. Brain. 1998;121(6):1053-63.
  • 3
    Dehaene S, Pegado F, Braga LW, Ventura P, Nunes Filho G, Jobert A, et al. How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science. 2010;330(6009):1359-64.
  • 4
    Angel L, Fay S, Bouazzaoui B, Baudouin A, Isingrini M. Protective role of educational level on episodic memory aging: an event-related potential study. Brain Cogn. 2010;74(3):312-23.
  • 5
    Stern Y, Habeck C, Moeller J, Scarmeas N, Anderson KE, Hilton HJ, et al. Brain networks associated with cognitive reserve in healthy young and old adults. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15(4):394-402.
  • 6
    Eisenberg DP, London ED, Matochik JA, Derbyshire S, Cohen LJ, Steinfeld M, et al. Education-associated cortical glucose metabolism during sustained attention. Neuroreport. 2005;16(13):1473-6.
  • 7
    Chiu NT, Lee BF, Hsiao S, Pai MC. Educational level influences regional cerebral blood flow in patients with Alzheimer's disease. J Nucl Med. 2004;45(11):1860-3.
  • 8
    Bennet DA, Schneider JA, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Arnold SE. Education modifies the association of amyloid but not tangles with cognitive function. Neurology. 2005;65(6):953-5.
  • 9
    Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Herrera Júnior E, Porto CS, Charchat-Fichman H, Carthert MT, et al. Performance of illiterate and literate nondemented elderly subjects in two tests of long-term memory. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10(4):634-8.
  • 10
    Reis A, Petersson KM, Castro-Caldas A, Ingvar M. Formal schooling influences two- but not three-dimensional naming skills. Brain Cogn. 2001;47(3):397-411.
  • 11
    Brucki SM, Nitrini, R. Cancellation task in very low educated people. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2008;23(2):139-47.
  • 12
    Kawano N, Umegaki H, Suzuki Y, Yamamoto S, Mogi N, Iguchi A. Effects of educational background on verbal fluency task performance in older adults with Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Int Psychogeriatr. 2010;22(6):995-1002.
  • 13
    Ardila A, Ostrosky-Solis F, Rosseli M, Gómez C. Age-related cognitive decline during normal aging: the complex effect of education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2000;15(6):495-513.
  • 14
    Sahadevan S, Tan NJ, Tan T, Tan S. Cognitive testing of elderly Chinese people in Singapore: influence of education and age on normative scores. Age Ageing. 1997;26(6):481-6.
  • 15
    Dansilio S, Charamelo A. Constructional functions and figure copying in illiterates or low-schooled Hispanics. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2005;20(8):1105-12.
  • 16
    Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Herrera Júnior E, Charchat-Fichman H, Porto CS. Performance in Luria's fist-edge-palm test according to educational level. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005;18(4):211-4.
  • 17
    Hong YJ, Yoon B, Shim YS, Cho AH, Lee ES, Kim YI, et al. Effect of literacy and education on the visuoconstructional ability of non-demented elderly individuals. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17(5):934-9.
  • 18
    Kim H, Chey J. Effects of education, literacy, and dementia on the Clock Drawing Test performance. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16(6):1138-46.
  • 19
    Ashendorf L, Vanderslice-Barr JL, McCaffrey RJ. Motor tests and cognition in healthy older adults. Appl Neuropsychol. 2009;16(3):171-6.
  • 20
    Neves ET. Aprendizagem de movimentos seqüenciais de dedos em idosos saudáveis: efeito da escolaridade [Dissertação de Mestrado]. São Paulo: Instituto de Psicologia, Neurociências e Comportamento da Universidade de São Paulo; 2008. 92 p.
  • 21
    Bramão I, Mendonça A, Faísca L, Ingvar M, Petersson KM, Reis A. The impact of reading and writing skills on a visuo-motor integration task: a comparison between illiterate and literate subjects. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2007;13(2):359-64.
  • 22
    Cavalcante KR. Avaliação do desempenho de idosos normais em um protocolo de produção e reconhecimento de gestos: influência do sexo, da idade e da escolaridade no perfil de normalidade [Dissertação de Mestrado]. São Paulo: Departamento de Neurologia da Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de São Paulo; 2004. 88 p.
  • 23
    Butler SM, Ashford JW, Snowdon DA. Age, education, and changes in the Mini-Mental State Exam scores of older women: findings from the Nun Study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1996;44(6):675-81.
  • 24
    Machado MSA, Voos MC, Teixeira PPPS, Piemonte MEP, Ribeiro-do-Valle LE. The impact of educational status on dual-task performance. Braz J Motor Behav. 2011;6(1):32-8.
  • 25
    Van der Elst W, Van Boxtel MPJ, Van Breukelen GJP, Jolles J. The Stroop color-word test: influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment. 2006;13(1):62-79.
  • 26
    Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA, Evans DA, Mendes de Leon CF, Arnold SE, et al. Education modifies the relation of AD pathology to level of cognitive function in older persons. Neurology. 2003;60(12):1909-15.
  • 27
    Alley D, Suthers K, Crimmins E. Education and cognitive decline in older Americans: results from the AHEAD sample. Res Aging. 2007;29(1):73-94.
  • 28
    Paradise M, Cooper C, Livingston G. Systematic review of the effect of education on survival in Alzheimer's disease. Int Psychogeriatr. 2009;21(1):25-32.
  • 29
    Tun PA, Lachman ME. Age differences in reaction time and attention in a national telephone sample of adults: education, sex, and task complexity matter. Dev Psychol. 2008;44(5):1421-9.
  • 30
    Scazufca M, Almeida OP, Menezes PR. The role of literacy, occupation and income in dementia prevention: the São Paulo Ageing & Health Study (SPAH). Int Psychogeriatr. 2010;22(8):1209-15.
  • 31
    Roe CM, Xiong C, Miller JP, Morris JC. Education and Alzheimer disease without dementia: support for the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Neurology. 2007;68(3):223-8.
  • 32
    Walker AJ, Batchelor J, Shores EA, Jones M. Diagnostic efficiency of demographically corrected Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III and Wechsler Memory Scale-III indices in moderate to severe traumatic brain injury and lower education levels. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15(6):938-50.
  • 33
    Homann CN, Quehenberger F, Petrovic K, Hartung HP, Ruzincka E, Homann B, et al. Influence of age, gender, education and dexterity on upper limb motor performance in Parkinsonian patients and healthy controls. J Neural Transm. 2003;110(8):885-97.
  • 34
    Souza CO, Voos MC, Francato DV, Chien HF, Barbosa ER. Influence of educational status on executive function and functional balance in individuals with Parkinson disease. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2013;26(1):6-13.
  • 35
    Voos MC, Custódio EB, Malaquias Junior J. Relationship of executive function and educational status with functional balance in older adults. J Geriatr Phys Ther. 2011;34(1):11-8.
  • 36
    Hester RL, Kinsella GJ, Ong B, McGregor J. Demographic influences on baseline and derived scores from the trail making test in healthy older Australian adults. Clin Neuropsychol. 2005;19(1):45-54.
  • 37
    Tombaugh TN. Trail Making Test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;19(2):203-14.
  • 38
    Barnes DE, Tager IB, Satariano WA, Yaffe K. The relationship between literacy and cognition in well-educated elders. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2004;59(4):390-5.
  • 39
    Voos, MC. A influência da idade e da escolaridade na execução e no aprendizado de uma tarefa cognitivo-motora [Tese de Doutorado]. São Paulo: Instituto de Psicologia; 2010. 99 p.
  • 40
    Gitlin LN, Winter L, Dennis MP, Hauck WW. Variation in response to a home intervention to support daily function by age, race, sex, and education. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2008;63(7):745-50.
  • 41
    Yong V, Saito Y. Are there education differentials in disability and mortality transitions and active life expectancy among Japanese older adults? Findings from a 10-year prospective cohort study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2012;67(3):343-53.
  • 42
    Gregory PC, Szanton SL, Xue QL, Tian J, Thorpe RJ, Fried LP. Education predicts incidence of preclinical mobility disability in initially high-functioning older women. The Women's Health and Aging Study II. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2011;66(5):577-81.
  • 43
    Jagger C, Matthews R, Melzer D, Matthews F, Brayne C; MRC CFAS. Educational differences in the dynamics of disability incidence, recovery and mortality: findings from the MRC Cognitive Function and Ageing Study (MRC CFAS). Int J Epidemiol. 2007;36(2):358-65.
  • 44
    Camargos MC, Machado CJ, Rodrigues RN. Disability life expectancy for the elderly, city of São Paulo, Brazil, 2000: gender and educational differences. J Biosoc Sci. 2007;39(3):455-63.
  • 45
    Meijer WA, van Boxtel MP, Van Gerven PW, van Hooren SA, Jolles J. Interaction effects of education and health status on cognitive change: a 6-year follow-up of the Maastricht Aging Study. Aging Ment Health. 2009;13(4):521-9.
  • 46
    Ojala-Oksala J, Jokinen H, Kopsi V, Lehtonen K, Luukkonen L, Paukkunen A, et al. Educational history is an independent predictor of cognitive deficits and long-term survival in postacute patients with mild to moderate ischemic stroke. Stroke. 2012;43(11):2931-5.
  • Financing source: none
  • Study conducted out at the Department of Phisiology, Speech Language and Audiology and Occupational Therapy at School Medicine of Universidade de São Paulo (USP) - São Paulo (SP), Brazil

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Jul-Sep 2014

History

  • Received
    Sept 2013
  • Accepted
    July 2014
Universidade de São Paulo Rua Ovídio Pires de Campos, 225 2° andar. , 05403-010 São Paulo SP / Brasil, Tel: 55 11 2661-7703, Fax 55 11 3743-7462 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revfisio@usp.br