Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Revised Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET) - Brazilian version

Abstract

Objective:

To translate and adapt to Brazilian Portuguese the Revised Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET), in both paper-and-pencil and computerized versions. The RMET is a well-accepted instrument for assessment of Theory of Mind (ToM), an important component of social cognition.

Methods:

Following a guideline for translation of material for clinical populations, this study had three main phases: 1) formal translation and semantic adaptation to Brazilian Portuguese; 2) an acceptability trial with health professionals as judges evaluating picture-word matching; and 3) a trial using the paper-and-pencil and computerized versions (experiments built in E-Prime 2.0.10 software) with healthy participants to test whether the instrument has similar outputs to those expected in versions in other languages.

Results:

RMET was adequately adapted to Brazilian Portuguese. This version showed acceptability and outputs similar to versions of the instrument in other languages, including the original one. We kept the same number of images as the original English version.

Conclusions:

Considering the scarcity of cognitive assessment instruments adequately adapted to Portuguese and the importance of social cognition in many psychiatric disorders, this work adds an important resource to Brazilian research and is administrable in both paper-and-pencil and computerized versions.

Mentalizing; Theory of Mind; autism; schizophrenia; social cognition; tests


Introduction

Early cognitive psychological findings11. Beck AT. Thinking and depression. I. Idiosyncratic content and cognitive distortions. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1963;9:324-33. regarding mental disorders support that patients have personal beliefs about the world and themselves, which are expressed behaviorally through symptoms (e.g., aggressiveness, delusions, liability). Moreover, there is evidence suggesting links between self-schemas and cognitive background,22. Shadel WG, Cervone D. Evaluating social-cognitive mechanisms that regulate self-efficacy in response to provocative smoking cues: an experimental investigation. Psychol Addict Behav. 2006;20:91-6. which is also related to several psychiatric conditions.33. Millan MJ, Agid Y, Brüne M, Bullmore ET, Carter CS, Clayton NS, et al. Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nat Rev Drug Discov. 2012;11:141-68. In this sense, the role of cognition has been highlighted in psychiatry, comprising not only classical functions (e.g., memory, language), but also those regarding handling of social environmental stimuli (e.g., face emotion recognition, Theory of Mind [ToM], moral judgment).44. Frith CD, Friston KJ, Liddle PF, Frackowiak RS. PET imaging and cognition in schizophrenia. J R Soc Med. 1992;85:222-4. This field of cognitive science is known as social cognition, which has been considered a new target for therapeutic interventions.55. Veltro F, Mazza M, Vendittelli N, Alberti M, Casacchia M, Roncone R. A comparison of the effectiveness of problem solving training and of cognitive-emotional rehabilitation on neurocognition, social cognition and social functioning in people with schizophrenia. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health. 2011;7:123-32.

One of the major issues that has been addressed in social cognition, alongside recognition of facial emotions, is ToM. ToM is the ability to “mentalize” - to reason about our own thoughts, feelings, and intentions, as well as those of others. Studies of ToM are now widely conducted in humans, although the term originated in primates research.66. Premack D, Woodruff G. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci. 1978;1:515-26. At the ages of 4 to 6, children are able to depict mental states in false-belief stories (an assessment paradigm of ToM); at the age of 7, they are able to understand metaphors; and at the age of 9 to 11 years, they would perform ToM similarly to adults. However, the course of ToM is not dependent on other cognitive abilities, as patients with Down syndrome or Williams syndrome do not exhibit impairments in social cognition.77. Brüne M, Brüne-Cohrs U. Theory of mind: evolution, ontogeny, brain mechanisms and psychopathology. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30:437-55.

In adults, psychiatric conditions are related to impairments in ToM. For example, comparisons between participants with mood disorders,88. Cusi AM, Nazarov A, Holshausen K, Macqueen GM, McKinnon MC. Systematic review of the neural basis of social cognition in patients with mood disorders. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2012;37:154-69. alcohol abusers,99. Uekermann J, Channon S, Winkel K, Schlebusch P, Daum I. Theory of mind, humour processing and executive functioning in alcoholism. Addiction. 2007;102:232-40. and dementia1010. Gleichgerrcht E, Torralva T, Roca M, Pose M, Manes F. The role of social cognition in moral judgment in frontotemporal dementia. Soc Neurosci. 2011;6:113-22. and healthy controls showed differences in favor of the controls. However, the conditions with the strongest evidence regarding ToM impairments are schizophrenia1111. Sprong M, Schothorst P, Vos E, Hox J, van Engeland H. Theory of mind in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. 2007;191:5-13. and autism.1212. Di Martino A, Ross K, Uddin LQ, Sklar AB, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. Functional brain correlates of social and nonsocial processes in autism spectrum disorders: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65:63-74. Considering schizophrenia and its associations with ToM performance, Frith argued that disease leads to an impairment in ToM capacity, which could make false beliefs lead to false conclusions, thus increasing delusional symptoms.1313. Frith CD, Corcoran R. Exploring ‘theory of mind’ in people with schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 1996;26:521-30. However, data about impairments in ToM are also available in people at high risk of psychosis.1414. Stanford AD, Messinger J, Malaspina D, Corcoran CM. Theory of Mind in patients at clinical high risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2011;131:11-7. Hence, it is still unclear whether ToM functioning could be a trait or state of schizophrenia. Moreover, it is also unknown whether ToM can be a “social-endophenotype” for psychosis or a social-cognitive side effect of psychosis.1515. Penn DL, Sanna LJ, Roberts DL. Social cognition in schizophrenia: an overview. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:408-11.

The first methodological strategy used to assess ToM in schizophrenia research was the false-belief paradigm, based on vignettes that provided contextual information and required the participant to answer what the characters in the story were thinking.1616. Shryane NM, Corcoran R, Rowse G, Moore R, Cummins S, Blackwood N, et al. Deception and false belief in paranoia: modelling theory of mind stories. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2008;13:8-32. This method is still used, but the influence of another method is now being felt and enriching results: the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET). The RMET assesses one's ability to read the intentions/feelings/thoughts of others through facial expressions, specifically in the region of the eyes.1717. Emery NJ, Clayton NS. Comparative social cognition. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60:87-113.,1818. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42:241-51.

The RMET was first described by Baron-Cohen et al.,1919. Baron-Cohen S, Jolliffe T, Mortimore C, Robertson M. Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or asperger syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38:813-22. who realized that information about context alone is not enough to figure out other peoples' intentions. They argued that people use resources from facial expressions to construct ideas about their feelings and desires. The interpretation of such signs should be mentally represented according to individual characteristics, and this mental representation would characterize the mental states of others. Findings such as these led to the present understanding of ToM, which combines appropriate input (catching several stimuli), recognition, and representation, thus entailing adoption of resources from previous knowledge.2020. Ahmed FS, Stephen Miller L. Executive function mechanisms of theory of mind. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011;41:667-78. However, the full apparatus can be abbreviated if an immediate response is required; therefore, ToM probably works in two modes - one controlled and one implicit.2121. Low J, Perner J. Implicit and explicit theory of mind: state of the art. Br J Dev Psychol. 2012;30:1-13.

The first version of the RMET contains pictures from the eye region and two options; the participant must then choose the option that better represents the feeling expressed. Later, Baron-Cohen et al.1919. Baron-Cohen S, Jolliffe T, Mortimore C, Robertson M. Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or asperger syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38:813-22. upgraded the original version, adding two options to each item. Results from the use of this method supported their theory about the inability of an autistic person to capture subtle differences in emotion expression in this region of the face.1818. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42:241-51. This new version (Revised RMET) facilitated advances in psychiatric research, as it made it possible to combine the RMET with imaging techniques. Now, results are discussed with regard to behavioral consequences. In autism, for example, the difficulties involved in accurately understanding the feelings of others may cause isolation effects and the maintenance of avoidance of eye contact with others.2222. Baron-Cohen S. Empathizing, systemizing, and the extreme male brain theory of autism. Prog Brain Res. 2010;186:167-75.

The RMET can be completed either using paper and pencil, or as a computerized version. It is simple and its administration is not restricted to specialized professionals. The original revised version contains 36 pictures, which are all black-and-white and of the same region of the face (midway along the nose to just above the eyebrow), plus one to be used in a training condition. Each image is surrounded by four words regarding mental states. The participants must choose the word that correctly depicts the mental state expressed in the picture. The test also includes a glossary, which is a list of all words the test contains, appropriate synonyms for each, and an example of their use in a phrase. The glossary must be presented before the test starts, a procedure adopted to prevent language biases. The RMET is often interpreted in terms a total score; however, some studies have added subscales to the original version. The most common subdivision consists of splitting the items into positive, negative, and neutral emotions,2323. Maurage P, Grynberg D, Noël X, Joassin F, Hanak C, Verbanck P, et al. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test as a new way to explore complex emotions decoding in alcohol dependence. Psychiatry Res. 2011;190:375-8. although the psychometric properties of this procedure have not been officially recognized.2424. Vellante M, Baron-Cohen S, Melis M, Marrone M, Petretto DR, Masala C, et al. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test: systematic review of psychometric properties and a validation study in Italy. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2013;18:326-54.

The RMET has been translated into several languages, including French and German (see ARC website for more: http://www.autismresearchcentre.com/arc_tests), and has a documented validation process for the Turkish2525. Yildirim EA, Kasar M, Güdük M, Ates E, Kuçukparlak I, Ozalmete EO. Investigation of the reliability of “Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test” in a Turkish population. Turk Psikiyatri Derg.. 2011;22:177-86. and Italian2424. Vellante M, Baron-Cohen S, Melis M, Marrone M, Petretto DR, Masala C, et al. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test: systematic review of psychometric properties and a validation study in Italy. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2013;18:326-54. versions, with adequate internal and external consistency. Nevertheless, it is important to note discrepancies in cultural comparisons, which may produce a familiarity effect due to ethnicity issues within actors in the pictures. For example, when the performance of American subjects was compared to the performance of Japanese participants in two versions of the RMET (the original version and a version with pictures depicting Asian actors), the authors found an important familiarity effect.2626. Adams RB Jr, Rule NO, Franklin RG Jr, Wang E, Stevenson MT, Yoshikawa S, et al. Cross-cultural reading the mind in the eyes: an fMRI investigation. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;22:97-108.

Considering the strengths of the RMET and the lack of Brazilian Portuguese instruments for ToM assessment, we sought to translate and adapt the original English RMET into Brazilian Portuguese and for the Brazilian reality. Although an early version of the RMET was previously translated into Brazilian Portuguese by one of the authors of this paper (H.A.T.), this first version did not include all the semantic validation steps, and was thus beset with uncontrolled language issues. To overcome these issues, we conducted the translation and content validation of RMET to Brazilian Portuguese in both paper-and-pencil and computerized versions.

Method

The Brazilian version of the RMET includes 37 items, as did the original test.1818. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42:241-51. Regarding material and legal matters, we contacted the Autism Research Centre (Autism Research Center/ARC, Cambridge, United Kingdom) and obtained their formal authorization to conduct the adaptation of the instrument to Brazilian Portuguese.

The final version was organized through standardized translation procedures. Special attention was paid to a valid semantic adaptation to the Brazilian sociolinguistic reality.2727. Guillemin F, Bombardier C, Beaton D. Cross-cultural adaptation of health-related quality of life measures: literature review and proposed guidelines. J Clin Epidemiol. 1993;46:1417-32. The first step consisted of semantic and conceptual translations and back-translation. The second step sought to ascertain whether the translated Brazilian Portuguese target-words were correctly matched with the facial expressions in the pictures (acceptability trial). Finally, the third step was a pilot study, as recommended by the ARC. Each procedure is further illustrated in Figure 1.

Figure 1
Flowchart of methodological steps of adaptation. BT = back-translation; FV = final version; O = original version; T = translation by two researchers; V1 = version 1; V2 = version 2.

Semantic and conceptual translation

Two independent language professionals translated the original version of the test into Brazilian Portuguese. The two versions were submitted for semantic and conceptual evaluation by a psychology researcher to verify the general meaning of the terms in light of the theoretical model.2727. Guillemin F, Bombardier C, Beaton D. Cross-cultural adaptation of health-related quality of life measures: literature review and proposed guidelines. J Clin Epidemiol. 1993;46:1417-32. These procedures led to the construction of a consensus version by the authors (version 1).

Following ARC recommendations and guidelines for test adaptations,2727. Guillemin F, Bombardier C, Beaton D. Cross-cultural adaptation of health-related quality of life measures: literature review and proposed guidelines. J Clin Epidemiol. 1993;46:1417-32. a back-translation of version 1 was performed by a bilingual native English speaker. Later, to avoid repeated words and perform semantic validation, a professional linguist screened both version 1 and the back-translation. Any differences were analyzed and corrected; there were no conceptual conflicts. Despite these strict methodological procedures, some accuracy issues might still be a problem (for instance, some pictures could not correspond to target words). To avoid this, the authors conducted a judging phase to test the acceptability of the final target words and their matching with the corresponding pictures.

Acceptability procedures

To test target-word vs. picture acceptability, an acceptability trial was conducted. The trial was computerized, performed on a 15-inch color monitor using E-Prime software version 2.0.2828. Schineider W, Eschman A, Zuccolotto A. E-Prime Reference Guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools; 2002. Participants were seated in front of the computer screen so that their eyes were approximately 50 cm from the display and all response keys were located on a standard QWERTY keyboard. The same pictures from Baron-Cohen's final version of the RMET were presented in the center of the screen. Each picture remained on the screen until the participant responded. The presentation of options was randomized to avoid laterality-biased answers.

Ten raters (six female, four male), all mental health professionals, were invited to take part in the acceptability trial. This method is based on choosing between two words: the aim-word and a foil-word. Foil words were selected from the glossary list, with the sole criterion of having a different meaning from the target word. The acceptability trial presented herein is similar to that adopted for the original version of the RMET, which had only two word options.1919. Baron-Cohen S, Jolliffe T, Mortimore C, Robertson M. Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or asperger syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38:813-22. The results were analyzed for descriptiveness, looking for mistakes committed twice or more times. If the raters still disagreed with the target-word vs. picture match after the mistake was presented, the incorrect items were revised.

Once the acceptability trial was complete and all corrections had been made, the authors conducted a pilot study to look for problems in the RMET behavioral outcomes in a non-clinical sample. The pilot study was also an ARC recommendation, as previously described.

Pilot study

The final version of the RMET consisted of one block with 37 items (one item being the training trial) programmed on E-Prime 2.0 software. Each item began with a 500-ms presentation of the “+” sign in the center of the screen, which served as a fixation cue. Immediately following the termination of this display, the image stimuli (measuring 23.5 x 9.4 centimeters) were presented and kept on the screen until the participant responded (Figure 2). Unlike in the acceptability trial, the response was obtained using the DoHit Test function of E-Prime, which is a procedure to obtain responses with the mouse.

Figure 2
Stimuli presentation in computer version of the RMET

The pilot study was conducted with a sample of 10 individuals (five male, five female), whose mean age was 26.19 (±3.02) years, mean educational attainment was 14.53 (±2.86) years, and mean monthly household income was US$5206.77 (±2110.07), which, in the Brazilian reality, is considered a high socioeconomic class. In the pilot study, pictures were presented with four options, one in each corner of the picture (one being the target word and the others being foils), as illustrated in Figure 2. Foil words were selected to be the correspondent ones from original version. Thus, the participant had to choose one option with the mouse.

The correction of this step was based on the Turkish adaptation of the RMET.2525. Yildirim EA, Kasar M, Güdük M, Ates E, Kuçukparlak I, Ozalmete EO. Investigation of the reliability of “Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test” in a Turkish population. Turk Psikiyatri Derg.. 2011;22:177-86. As in this study, items that more than 50% of the sample had mistaken or those for which 25% of subjects or more had incorrectly selected the same foil option were corrected. Remaining conflicts were re-evaluated and re-tested using the whole sample.

Results

Semantic and conceptual translation

The previously planned steps were all completed, as presented in Table 1, which shows the original words, version 1, the back-translation, version 2, and the final version (completed after the acceptability trial and pilot study).

Table 1
Main changes to instrument terms through the translation and adaptation process

The process, however, was conducted with some difficulties. The major issue was the need to maintain the same number of words, which was critical in some cases. Faced with this situation, the authors had two options: a) to use words which were not so common in standard Brazilian Portuguese; or b) to use fewer words than the original version of the RMET. Since the instructions permitted the respondent to use a glossary to recognize words clearly, the authors decided on option “a.” However, exceptions remained due to the limited number of synonyms in Portuguese. Therefore, eight words were repeated in version 1, three in version 2, and four in the final version.

After back-translation, a professional linguist compared the translated and original versions. This process revealed some mismatching terms. Such terms were revised in the Portuguese form, for example, “dispirited” and “encouraging,” which matched “humiliated” and “animated” in the back-translation version, respectively. The corrections were completed and the authors established version 2, which was used to conduct the acceptability and pilot trials.

Acceptability procedures

This procedure revealed a number of patterns regarding mistakes in some items. Among the 37 items, two raters got six items wrong (1, 4, 17, 25, 31, 36), three raters for one item wrong (10), and more than five raters got three items wrong (7, 9, 13).

Results from the acceptability trial alerted the authors to other issues; therefore, items 7, 9, and 13 were revised, changing the words “inquieto” to “apreensivo” (7), “nervoso” to “preocupado” (9), and “torcedor” to “aflito” (13). The remaining mistakes were analyzed together with the raters who conducted the trial. In these cases, raters agreed that errors were their fault, but once they knew the correct option, they were able to recognize it. Therefore, the authors and raters could reject the hypothesis that mistakes occurred because the word did not fit the picture properly or because there were too many confusing options. Once this phase was completed, the last procedure to be carried out was a pilot trial.

Pilot trial

The trial run detected few errors; therefore, items 8, 9, 10, 12, 17, 23 and 32 were run a second time, as the first trial detected more than 25% of mistakes regarding the same foil word. For this reason, these items were analyzed and modifications were made, especially to the foil words. For example, in item 9, the word “horrified” (translated to “amedrontado”) was conflicting with the right answer (“preoccupied” translated as “preocupado”). In item 9, the foil word was substituted for another foil word, “relieved,” translated as “aliviado.”

Table 2 presents the frequency of responses in the final application of the RMET for each option of the task. The changes are described in Table 1, with the final version in the right hand column. The retest was run with the whole first sample, and this time, the results showed no mistakes in items 10, 17, 23, 32; however, items 8 and 12 still contained errors, which did not reach 50% of the sample, nor 25% in the same foil word option. Hence, this was considered the final version of the Brazilian adaptation of the Revised RMET. Of the 37 items, five reached 100% agreement, 15 had 90%, eight had 80%, seven had 70%, and two had 60%.

Table 2
Item option frequency in the pilot study (n=10)

Discussion

This project sought to construct a Brazilian version of the RMET, following recommended methodological steps. All procedures were successfully completed, resulting in a final Brazilian version of the Revised RMET. It adds an advanced method to social cognition research.2929. Combs DR, Adams SD, Penn DL, Roberts D, Tiegreen J, Stem P. Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) for inpatients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders: preliminary findings. Schizophr Res. 2007;91:112-6.

30. Maat A, Fett AK, Derks E, GROUP Investigators. Social cognition and quality of life in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2012;137:212-8.
-3131. Kluwe-Schiavon B, Sanvicente-Vieira B, Kristensen CH, Grassi-Oliveira R. Executive functions rehabilitation for schizophrenia: a critical systematic review. J Psychiatr Res. 2013;47:91-104. Hence, the main contribution of this article is to add criteria for the translation and adaptation of the test. Furthermore, additional adaptations were made to the instrument to enable its administration using computer resources. Here, the translated methods were updated and an effort was made to follow translation guidelines2727. Guillemin F, Bombardier C, Beaton D. Cross-cultural adaptation of health-related quality of life measures: literature review and proposed guidelines. J Clin Epidemiol. 1993;46:1417-32. and ARC recommendations (pilot running), to maintain the properties of the test.

As noted above, our adaptation of the RMET employed computerized methods. We stress that the use of computers allowed us to obtain more accurate results, in addition to other outcomes (e.g., reaction time, response time, bias). Computerized assessment is also an advanced method in neuroimaging studies investigating ToM functioning.3232. Rabin JS, Rosenbaum RS. Familiarity modulates the functional relationship between theory of mind and autobiographical memory. Neuroimage. 2012;62:520-9.,3333. Malhi GS, Lagopoulos J, Das P, Moss K, Berk M, Coulston CM. A functional MRI study of Theory of Mind in euthymic bipolar disorder patients. Bipolar Disord. 2008;10:943-56. However, the test can also be administered using paper and pencil, as the development of this Brazilian version was based on standardized procedures for translation of instruments and scales,2727. Guillemin F, Bombardier C, Beaton D. Cross-cultural adaptation of health-related quality of life measures: literature review and proposed guidelines. J Clin Epidemiol. 1993;46:1417-32. including additional linguistic adaptation methods.

The revised Brazilian version of the RMET has 36 items plus the training item, preserving the format of the original English version. The instructions for administration are accessible to any trained health professional and consist of a glossary, which includes all the words used in the test. After presenting the glossary, the evaluator provides instructions concerning what must be answered and the need to accurately reason on the mental state depicted by the eyes in each photo. One answer out of four options is presented together with the picture. In this study, the answers were given by mouse response, although the response method can be adapted to the needs of the investigator.

The glossary presentation sought to diminish vocabulary limitation bias in the test outcomes. The words in the glossary are accompanied by their meanings, as well as synonyms and examples provided as statements (the glossary is available from the authors on request). In the glossary, we adapted some statements (e.g., character names) to correspond to Brazilian reality. Our glossary only contains the words we used, omitting duplicate translations, such as “annoyed” and “irritated” (both of which were rendered as the Portuguese word “irritado” in the final version).

All the steps followed in our adaptation aimed to achieve the most appropriate meaning for the translated words to ensure adequate fit to the corresponding picture. Furthermore, we sought to avoid conflicting words that could confuse respondents; the pilot study was conducted to identify such cases. In such situations, the authors had to conduct a theoretical analysis of the mixed outputs. These analyses always followed the search for emotions with a similar valence (e.g., “preocupado” and “nervoso,” both of which refer to anxiety states).

During the translation, limitations arose because the ranges of synonyms are different in different languages and, in some cases, because of incompatibility between the grammatical classes of words. Some of the original words, out of context, could be considered nouns or even verbs in Portuguese (e.g., “comforting” and “disappointed”). The first translation committed this type of error, which was caught early on during back-translation and avoided in the final version. This observation revealed the need to restrict the options to adjectives. This recommendation was fully accepted, as the task consisted of providing the right definition for the emotions depicted by the photos, thus adjectiving the eyes in each picture.

The acceptability trial presented in this study recreates the original paper by Baron-Cohen et al.,1919. Baron-Cohen S, Jolliffe T, Mortimore C, Robertson M. Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or asperger syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38:813-22. where the respondent may choose between two options to describe the expression in the picture. In the present study, the respondent has unlimited time to respond to each item in the acceptability trial, whereas in the original work, the pictures were displayed for 3 seconds, after which they disappeared and the participant was forced to choose between the two options. The infinite response time was selected in this phase of the adaptation because the participant needs to use other resources, such as working memory and attention, to provide a correct answer; therefore, limiting the response time to 3 seconds is restrictive. As the image should remain in the mind while the participant selects an option,3434. Baddeley A. Working memory. In: Baddeley A, Anderson M, Eysenck M, editors. Memory. Hove: Psychology Press; 2009. the outcomes could be biased. The authors wanted to avoid any potential bias (especially because it was done in an acceptability trial setting) by diminishing interferences from other cognitive processes, as previous research has already found significant relations between complex cognitive functions and ToM.2020. Ahmed FS, Stephen Miller L. Executive function mechanisms of theory of mind. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011;41:667-78.

Having detailed the strengths of the methodology of this study, some limitations of relevance to the interpretation of this work must now be noted. The sample had a high level of educational attainment, which is not representative of Brazilian reality. Therefore, the possibility of unfamiliarity with some expressions must be considered, although the glossary tried to minimize this effect. Caution should be exercised when interpreting the results due to the lack of an evaluation of the back-translation by a native English-speaking health professional. However, a native English speaker adjusted the translation to make corrections as necessary, which helped improve linguistic control of the stimuli.

In conclusion, future studies should aim to validate the REMT among the Brazilian population. Furthermore, photos of Brazilians should be used in the test, as familiarity effects driven by the culture have already been recorded in previous research.2626. Adams RB Jr, Rule NO, Franklin RG Jr, Wang E, Stevenson MT, Yoshikawa S, et al. Cross-cultural reading the mind in the eyes: an fMRI investigation. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;22:97-108. Validation should be conducted in larger samples, together with clinical samples that are known to reflect ToM impairments (e.g., autism, schizophrenia), so as to compare Brazilian RMET results with those observed in foreign data. After validation, ToM screening could be added to neuropsychological clinical evaluations. The Revised Brazilian RMET is a version of an established, elegant, and sophisticated method for assessment of social cognition. This subject, which is neglected in most psychiatric studies that focus on cognition conducted in our country, is an exception to theoretical works3535. Caixeta L, Nitrini R. Teoria da mente: uma revisão com enfoque na sua incorporação pela psicologia médica. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2002;15:105-12. and reviews.3636. Tonelli HA. [Cognitive “Theory of Mind” processing in bipolar disorder]. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2009;31:369-74. Furthermore, to our knowledge, there is a work by Jou and Sperb,3737. Jou GI, Sperb TM. O contexto experimental e a teoria da mente. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2004;17:167-76. who developed an instrument (using the false-belief paradigm) for the assessment of children. In addition, a translation of False Belief tests was recently carried out by our own group.3838. Sanvicente-Vieira B, Brietzke E, Grassi-Oliveira R. Translation and adaptation of theory of mind tasks into Brazilian portuguese. Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 2012;34:178-85.

Finally, considering future research, ToM instruments must continue to be developed and adapted to the Brazilian reality to enable a deeper understanding misperceptions about the social environment and its relation to symptoms. Instruments addressing theories other than ToM must also be considered, as well as tasks that assess other social cognitive abilities, such as emotion recognition, social-value attribution, and social rules. In this sense, a common instrument in recent research, which has not yet been adapted to Portuguese, is The Awareness of Social Inference Test Revised (TASIT). This test has excellent ecological validity and comprises visual, verbal, and non-verbal issues, which other tasks/tests fail to investigate.3939. McDonald S, Bornhofen C, Shum D, Long E, Saunders C, Neulinger K. Reliability and validity of The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT): a clinical test of social perception. Disabil Rehabil. 2006;28:1529-42. TASIT has three parts, the third of which is considered to assess ToM through video presentations. Continued advancement in methods for the evaluation of social cognition is required to shed light on new perspectives that are increasingly being considered as crucial to changes in psychiatry and, consequently, to improvement of pharmacological and non-pharmacological treatments and even avoidance strategies for mental disorders.4040. van Os J, Kenis G, Rutten BP. The environment and schizophrenia. Nature. 2010;468:203-12.

Authors would like to thank Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq) and Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES) for their support.

References

  • 1
    Beck AT. Thinking and depression. I. Idiosyncratic content and cognitive distortions. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1963;9:324-33.
  • 2
    Shadel WG, Cervone D. Evaluating social-cognitive mechanisms that regulate self-efficacy in response to provocative smoking cues: an experimental investigation. Psychol Addict Behav. 2006;20:91-6.
  • 3
    Millan MJ, Agid Y, Brüne M, Bullmore ET, Carter CS, Clayton NS, et al. Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nat Rev Drug Discov. 2012;11:141-68.
  • 4
    Frith CD, Friston KJ, Liddle PF, Frackowiak RS. PET imaging and cognition in schizophrenia. J R Soc Med. 1992;85:222-4.
  • 5
    Veltro F, Mazza M, Vendittelli N, Alberti M, Casacchia M, Roncone R. A comparison of the effectiveness of problem solving training and of cognitive-emotional rehabilitation on neurocognition, social cognition and social functioning in people with schizophrenia. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health. 2011;7:123-32.
  • 6
    Premack D, Woodruff G. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci. 1978;1:515-26.
  • 7
    Brüne M, Brüne-Cohrs U. Theory of mind: evolution, ontogeny, brain mechanisms and psychopathology. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30:437-55.
  • 8
    Cusi AM, Nazarov A, Holshausen K, Macqueen GM, McKinnon MC. Systematic review of the neural basis of social cognition in patients with mood disorders. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2012;37:154-69.
  • 9
    Uekermann J, Channon S, Winkel K, Schlebusch P, Daum I. Theory of mind, humour processing and executive functioning in alcoholism. Addiction. 2007;102:232-40.
  • 10
    Gleichgerrcht E, Torralva T, Roca M, Pose M, Manes F. The role of social cognition in moral judgment in frontotemporal dementia. Soc Neurosci. 2011;6:113-22.
  • 11
    Sprong M, Schothorst P, Vos E, Hox J, van Engeland H. Theory of mind in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. 2007;191:5-13.
  • 12
    Di Martino A, Ross K, Uddin LQ, Sklar AB, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. Functional brain correlates of social and nonsocial processes in autism spectrum disorders: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65:63-74.
  • 13
    Frith CD, Corcoran R. Exploring ‘theory of mind’ in people with schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 1996;26:521-30.
  • 14
    Stanford AD, Messinger J, Malaspina D, Corcoran CM. Theory of Mind in patients at clinical high risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2011;131:11-7.
  • 15
    Penn DL, Sanna LJ, Roberts DL. Social cognition in schizophrenia: an overview. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:408-11.
  • 16
    Shryane NM, Corcoran R, Rowse G, Moore R, Cummins S, Blackwood N, et al. Deception and false belief in paranoia: modelling theory of mind stories. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2008;13:8-32.
  • 17
    Emery NJ, Clayton NS. Comparative social cognition. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60:87-113.
  • 18
    Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42:241-51.
  • 19
    Baron-Cohen S, Jolliffe T, Mortimore C, Robertson M. Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or asperger syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38:813-22.
  • 20
    Ahmed FS, Stephen Miller L. Executive function mechanisms of theory of mind. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011;41:667-78.
  • 21
    Low J, Perner J. Implicit and explicit theory of mind: state of the art. Br J Dev Psychol. 2012;30:1-13.
  • 22
    Baron-Cohen S. Empathizing, systemizing, and the extreme male brain theory of autism. Prog Brain Res. 2010;186:167-75.
  • 23
    Maurage P, Grynberg D, Noël X, Joassin F, Hanak C, Verbanck P, et al. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test as a new way to explore complex emotions decoding in alcohol dependence. Psychiatry Res. 2011;190:375-8.
  • 24
    Vellante M, Baron-Cohen S, Melis M, Marrone M, Petretto DR, Masala C, et al. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test: systematic review of psychometric properties and a validation study in Italy. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2013;18:326-54.
  • 25
    Yildirim EA, Kasar M, Güdük M, Ates E, Kuçukparlak I, Ozalmete EO. Investigation of the reliability of “Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test” in a Turkish population. Turk Psikiyatri Derg.. 2011;22:177-86.
  • 26
    Adams RB Jr, Rule NO, Franklin RG Jr, Wang E, Stevenson MT, Yoshikawa S, et al. Cross-cultural reading the mind in the eyes: an fMRI investigation. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;22:97-108.
  • 27
    Guillemin F, Bombardier C, Beaton D. Cross-cultural adaptation of health-related quality of life measures: literature review and proposed guidelines. J Clin Epidemiol. 1993;46:1417-32.
  • 28
    Schineider W, Eschman A, Zuccolotto A. E-Prime Reference Guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools; 2002.
  • 29
    Combs DR, Adams SD, Penn DL, Roberts D, Tiegreen J, Stem P. Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) for inpatients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders: preliminary findings. Schizophr Res. 2007;91:112-6.
  • 30
    Maat A, Fett AK, Derks E, GROUP Investigators. Social cognition and quality of life in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2012;137:212-8.
  • 31
    Kluwe-Schiavon B, Sanvicente-Vieira B, Kristensen CH, Grassi-Oliveira R. Executive functions rehabilitation for schizophrenia: a critical systematic review. J Psychiatr Res. 2013;47:91-104.
  • 32
    Rabin JS, Rosenbaum RS. Familiarity modulates the functional relationship between theory of mind and autobiographical memory. Neuroimage. 2012;62:520-9.
  • 33
    Malhi GS, Lagopoulos J, Das P, Moss K, Berk M, Coulston CM. A functional MRI study of Theory of Mind in euthymic bipolar disorder patients. Bipolar Disord. 2008;10:943-56.
  • 34
    Baddeley A. Working memory. In: Baddeley A, Anderson M, Eysenck M, editors. Memory. Hove: Psychology Press; 2009.
  • 35
    Caixeta L, Nitrini R. Teoria da mente: uma revisão com enfoque na sua incorporação pela psicologia médica. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2002;15:105-12.
  • 36
    Tonelli HA. [Cognitive “Theory of Mind” processing in bipolar disorder]. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2009;31:369-74.
  • 37
    Jou GI, Sperb TM. O contexto experimental e a teoria da mente. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2004;17:167-76.
  • 38
    Sanvicente-Vieira B, Brietzke E, Grassi-Oliveira R. Translation and adaptation of theory of mind tasks into Brazilian portuguese. Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 2012;34:178-85.
  • 39
    McDonald S, Bornhofen C, Shum D, Long E, Saunders C, Neulinger K. Reliability and validity of The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT): a clinical test of social perception. Disabil Rehabil. 2006;28:1529-42.
  • 40
    van Os J, Kenis G, Rutten BP. The environment and schizophrenia. Nature. 2010;468:203-12.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    17 Dec 2013
  • Date of issue
    Jan-Mar 2014

History

  • Received
    4 May 2013
  • Accepted
    16 June 2013
Associação Brasileira de Psiquiatria Rua Pedro de Toledo, 967 - casa 1, 04039-032 São Paulo SP Brazil, Tel.: +55 11 5081-6799, Fax: +55 11 3384-6799, Fax: +55 11 5579-6210 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: editorial@abp.org.br