Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Influence of schooling on lingustic-cognitive skills

Abstracts

PURPOSE:

to verify the influence of schooling on Phonemic Awareness and Rapid Automatized Naming, and also the possible correlation between such skills, in students from the 3rd and 4th year of elementary school.

METHODS:

as sample, this study counted on 29 students from the 3rd (Group 1) and 28 from the 4th (Group 2) year of elementary school. All the participants were selected through standardized tests to Brazilian Portuguese regarding Phonology (Oral Language) and further reading, writing and arithmetic (Written Language). To asses Phonological Awareness it was applied the second part of a Phonological Awareness Test - Consciência Fonológica - Instrumento de Avaliação Sequencial (CONFIAS); the assessment of Rapid Automatized Naming was performed by the Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing in its Brazilian Portuguese version.

RESULTS:

students from Group 1 showed lower performance in Phonological Awareness than Group 2, however with no significant difference. In relation to Rapid Automatized Naming, the time spent to naming and also the errors pattern was similar in both groups. There was difference only in the time spent to naming and in the errors pattern of numbers.

CONCLUSION:

there is influence of schooling, even slightly, on Phonological Awareness and Rapid Automatized Naming once students from Group 2 showed best results than Group 1 in both tests. Nevertheless no significant difference was found.

Evaluation; Language; Reading; Learning; Education


OBJETIVO:

verificar a influência da escolaridade na Consciência Fonêmica e Nomeação Automática Rápida, além da possível correlação entre tais habilidades em escolares do 3º e 4º ano do Ensino Fundamental.

MÉTODOS:

participaram deste estudo 29 escolares do 3º ano (Grupo 1) e 28 escolares do 4º ano (Grupo 2) do Ensino Fundamental. Os sujeitos foram selecionados por meio de testes padronizados: um de fonologia (Linguagem Oral) e um de leitura, escrita e aritmética (Linguagem Escrita). Para a avaliação da Consciência Fonêmica foi aplicada a segunda parte da prova de Consciência Fonológica - Instrumento de Avaliação Sequencial. A avaliação da Nomeação Automática Rápida foi realizada por meio dos testes de Nomeação Rápida de números, cores, objetos e letras do "Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing" adaptado para o Português Brasileiro.

RESULTADOS:

os escolares do Grupo 1 apresentaram desempenho inferior em Consciência Fonêmica quando comparados ao Grupo 2, porém esta diferença não foi significante. A observação do desempenho entre os escolares do Grupo 1 e Grupo 2 em Nomeação Automática Rápida demonstrou que o padrão de tempo e de erros foi semelhante entre os grupos. Houve diferença apenas no tempo de nomeação e na ocorrência de erros na tarefa relacionada aos números.

CONCLUSÃO:

há influência da escolaridade, ainda que modestamente, em Consciência Fonêmica e Nomeação Automática Rápida, uma vez que escolares do Grupo 2 apresentaram melhores resultados do que Grupo 1, em ambos os testes, contudo sem diferença significante entre os grupos.

Avaliação; Linguagem; Leitura; Aprendizagem; Educação


Introduction

Several studies11. Torgesen JK, Morgan ST, Davis C. Effects of two types of phonological awareness training on word learning in kindergarten children. J Educ Psychol.1992;84(3):364-70. , 22. Mousinho R, Correa J. Habilidades linguístico-cognitivas em leitores e não-leitores. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2009;21(2):113-8. have claimed that linguistic-cognitive skills such as phonemic awareness (PA) and rapid automatized naming (RAN) are essential to both acquisition and development of reading abilities.

PA is the capacity of manipulating the phonemes of the language33. Wocadlo C, Rieger I. Phonology, rapid naming and academic achievement in very preterm children at eight years of age. Early Hum Dev. 2009;83(6):367-77. , 44. Nunes C, Frota S, Mousinho R. Consciência fonológica e o processo de aprendizagem de leitura e escrita: implicações teóricas para o embasamento da prática fonoaudiológica. Rev CEFAC. 2009;11(2):207-12. in tasks of blending, splitting and manipulation of speech sounds. Such skill is developed in parallel to reading and writing acquisition and also foster literacy development, that is, they are connected in a reciprocal way. Researches have shown that PA skills are more linked to the literacy process than syllabic tasks55. Salgado CA, Capellini SA. Programa de remediação fonológica em escolares com dislexia do desenvolvimento. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2008; 20(6):31-6. , 66. Ukrainetz TA, Nuspl JJ, Wilkerson K, Beddes SR. The effects of syllable instruction on phonemic awareness in preschoolers. Early Child Res Q. 2011;26 (2):50-60..

Meanwhile PA have been related to decoding skills and also letter-to-sound proficiency77. Soares AJC, Cárnio MS. Consciência fonêmica em escolares antes e após oficinas de linguagem. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2012;24(1):69-75., further variables are associated to word recognition and, consequently to reading fluency such as RAN88. Yesil-Dagli U. Predicting ELL students' beginning first grade English oral reading fluency from initial kindergarten: Reading, letter naming, and phonological awareness skills. Early Child Res. 2011;26(2):15-29., one of the most important skills. A wide range of researches have stated that RAN may predict reading fluency 99. Powell D, Stainthorp R, Stuart M, Garwood H, Quinlan P. An experimental comparison between rival theories of rapid automatized naming performance and its relationship to reading. J Exp Child Psychol. 2007;98(1):46-68.

10. Wakamiya E, Okumura T, Nakanishi M, Takeshita T, Mizuta T, Kurimoto N, Tamai H. Effects of sequential and discrete rapid naming on reading in Japanese children with reading difficulty. Brain Dev. 2011;12(3):356-68.
- 1111. Castles A, Coltheart M, Wilson K, Valpied J. The genesis of reading ability: what helps children learn letter-sound correspondences? J Exp Child Psychol. 2009;104(1): 68-88..

When children master phonological awareness and know the name of the letters, it leads them to quickly access phonological information and drive their attention to the letter sequences, which may favor the orthographic processing. Furthermore, the fast processing in each level of reading leads to the quick word recognition1212. Navas ALGP, Pinto JCBR, Dellisa PRR. Avanços no conhecimento do processamento da fluência em leitura: da palavra ao texto. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2009;14(4):553-9.. Learning to read and write in an alphabetic orthography requires the ability of quickly process visual symbols1313. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashote CA. Comprehensive test of phonological processing (CTOPP). Proed - an international publisher. Austin: Texas; 1999..

Various studies88. Yesil-Dagli U. Predicting ELL students' beginning first grade English oral reading fluency from initial kindergarten: Reading, letter naming, and phonological awareness skills. Early Child Res. 2011;26(2):15-29. , 99. Powell D, Stainthorp R, Stuart M, Garwood H, Quinlan P. An experimental comparison between rival theories of rapid automatized naming performance and its relationship to reading. J Exp Child Psychol. 2007;98(1):46-68. have suggested that PA is more deeply related to the development of decoding skills, that is, the ability to establish letter-to-sound associations. In the other hand, orthographic memory is associated to quickly process visual symbols and favor children to read and interpret texts efficiently99. Powell D, Stainthorp R, Stuart M, Garwood H, Quinlan P. An experimental comparison between rival theories of rapid automatized naming performance and its relationship to reading. J Exp Child Psychol. 2007;98(1):46-68.

10. Wakamiya E, Okumura T, Nakanishi M, Takeshita T, Mizuta T, Kurimoto N, Tamai H. Effects of sequential and discrete rapid naming on reading in Japanese children with reading difficulty. Brain Dev. 2011;12(3):356-68.
- 1111. Castles A, Coltheart M, Wilson K, Valpied J. The genesis of reading ability: what helps children learn letter-sound correspondences? J Exp Child Psychol. 2009;104(1): 68-88.. Hence, is undeniable the importance of such skills to the literacy in alphabetic languages such as the Brazilian Portuguese.

Several researches88. Yesil-Dagli U. Predicting ELL students' beginning first grade English oral reading fluency from initial kindergarten: Reading, letter naming, and phonological awareness skills. Early Child Res. 2011;26(2):15-29.

9. Powell D, Stainthorp R, Stuart M, Garwood H, Quinlan P. An experimental comparison between rival theories of rapid automatized naming performance and its relationship to reading. J Exp Child Psychol. 2007;98(1):46-68.
- 1010. Wakamiya E, Okumura T, Nakanishi M, Takeshita T, Mizuta T, Kurimoto N, Tamai H. Effects of sequential and discrete rapid naming on reading in Japanese children with reading difficulty. Brain Dev. 2011;12(3):356-68. , 1313. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashote CA. Comprehensive test of phonological processing (CTOPP). Proed - an international publisher. Austin: Texas; 1999. have investigated the relation between PA, RAN and written language, showing the importance of then in the schooling process. Nevertheless, studies which investigated the opposite relation, that is, whether the schooling process has influence on linguistic-cognitive skills and also if there is reciprocity between them, are scarce. Relying on this basis, investigate the influence of schooling on PA and RAN may provide parameters to best understand the relation between them.

Thus, this study aimed to verify the influence of schooling on PA and RAN and also the possible correlation between them in students from the 3rd and 4th year of elementary school.

Methods

This study, prospective and transversal, was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Institution under nº 214/11. All parents/caretakers signed the Free Informed Consent Term to allow their children to participate in this research.

The study included 29 elementary school children from 3rd grade (G1) and 28 from 4th grade (G2), for a total of 57 children. The mean age of the children in G1 was 8:5 years, and the group included 16 male and 13 female children; G2 consisted of 12 female and 16 male children with a mean age of 9:3 years.

To compose the sample, the following inclusion criteria were considered: lack of hearing, cognitive or visual impairment; absence of previous communication disorders in either oral or written language; lack of concerns regarding the child's learning process or current academic performance.

Procedures for selecting subjects

All parents/caretakers signed the Free Informed Consent Term to allow their children to participate in this research.

In order to guarantee the inclusion criteria an oral phonological assessment standardized to Brazilian Portuguese1414. Wertzner HF. Fonologia. In: Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes D, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner HF. ABFW - Teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. Carapicuíba: Pró-Fono; 2004. P. 5-40. was performed to verify the existence of phonological disorders. Regarding written language it was used the School Performance Test1515. Stein LM. Teste de Desempenho Escolar (TDE). São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 1994. which assesses writing, reading and arithmetic skills. All tests were performed in scheduled days according to the availability of the school. All analysis of both tests followed the guidelines presented in each test. Students who showed any type of disorders were referred to specific health services.

Experimental Proofs

All children underwent the second part of CONFIAS (Consciência Fonológica: Instrumento de Avaliação Sequencial) 1616. Moojen S, Lamprecht RR, Santos RM, Freitas GM, Brodacz R, Siqueira M, Correa A, Guarda E. Consciência Fonológica - Instrumento de Avaliação Sequencial (CONFIAS). São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2003. . According to its instructions the children were supposed to perform tasks presented in a increasing level of challenge, including producing a word that starts with a defined phoneme, identifying a word's initial phoneme, phoneme exclusion, and split and mixed phonemes. All children were previously trained with two examples for each task. The test comprises 30 items in which each correct answer is equivalent to one point. Consequently 30 points is the maximum score of the test.

After PA assessment, RAN was investigated through the Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP) 1313. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashote CA. Comprehensive test of phonological processing (CTOPP). Proed - an international publisher. Austin: Texas; 1999., according to the instructions of the Brazilian Portuguese version1717. Simões VF. Estudo do desempenho de crianças das séries iniciais: do ensino fundamental I em testes de leitura, escrita e nomeação rápida [tese]. São Paulo (SP): Universidade de São Paulo; 2006. , 1818. Rosal CAR. Habilidades de segmentação fonêmica em crianças normais de primeira, segunda e terceira séries do ensino fundamental [dissertação]. São Paulo (SP): Universidade de São Paulo; 2002.. All subjects were trained in order to guarantee their knowledge of the items to be named. Sequentially, they were instructed to name the stimuli presented as fast as possible, speaking intelligibly and audibly in each item. All the stimuli were distributed and randomly repeated in four lines and nine columns divided into two parts (A and B). Analysis of the performance considered the time taken for naming in each category of the test as well as the amount of errors in all subtests.

All data was tabulated and statistically analyzed in SPPS 18. Initially, a descriptive analysis of all parameters was performed and also the distribution of data was investigated. Mann-Whitney and Spearman correlation coefficient were used to analyze the relation between the performances of the children in both tests. A significance level of 5% was adopted for hypotheses tests.

Results

Students from G1 showed lower means than G2 with no significant difference. However, the value of p was very close to 0,005 (Table 1).

Table1: Performance
of Groups 1 and 2 in phonemic awareness

Descriptive analysis of time spent to each task indicated that students from both groups showed greater means naming objects and letters (Tables 2 and 3).

Table 2:
Performance of Group 1 in Rapid Automatized Naming

In relation to the amount of errors in RAN, descriptive analysis indicated that students from G1 and G2 presented errors mostly in naming colors (Tables 2 and 3).

Table 3:
Performance of Group 2 in Rapid Automatized Naming

The comparison of the performance of students from G1 and G2 demonstrated that their errors patterns were similar. There was found difference between the groups only in RAN of numbers both in the time spent to name and in the amount of errors (Table 4).

Table 4:
Comparison between the performance of groups in Rapid Automatized Naming

To verify whether the performance in PA is correlated to RAN tasks, the Spearman correlation coefficient was used. Such investigation was restricted to the time spent to name since the number of errors was low in all tasks. It was noticed that no correlation between PA and RAN was found in G1. Concerning G2, negative correlation between PA and time spent to name letters and numbers was found, suggesting that the better the performance in PA, the lower is the time spent to name those items (Table 5).

Table 5:
Spearman correlation coefficient of Phonemic Awareness and time of Rapid Automatized Naming in Groups 1 and 2

Discussion

The results of PA showed similar means and standard deviations for both groups, assuring a steady pattern of scores for the children. Because the children in G1 and G2 were at different schooling levels, we expected to find significant differences in their scores, as several authors have previously claimed1919. Schuele CM, Boudreau D. Phonological awareness intervention: Beyond the basics. Lang Speech Hear Ser. 2008;39(1):3-20. , 2020. Furnes B, Samuelsson S. Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming predicting early development in reading e spelling: Results from a cross-linguistic longitudinal study. Learn Individ Differ. 2011;21(2):85-95.. However, the statistical analysis shows a tendency toward significance that might have occurred whether the sample had been larger. Data indicated, even slightly, a best performance of G1 in PA suggesting the influence of schooling in such skill.

A study2121. Capellini SA, Lanza SC. Desempenho de escolares em consciência fonológica, nomeação rápida, leitura e escrita. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2010;22(3):239-44. in which children from 3rd and 4th year were subjects found results to those from the present study. The authors indicated that the knowledge of the children regarding orthography might have led them to rely on the written form of the words, even though they were instructed to not consider it. Thus, it is possible to affirm that the same process may have occurred to the children from this research.

Furthermore, it is important to note that in most countries where studies concerning PA are conducted, the literacy process is based on letter-sound tasks, which are essential to the proper evolution of literacy; however, such skills are not commonly taught in Brazilian schools. Hence, this may be an explanation of why children from different levels of schooling achieved similar performance in PA.

Specifically in the school where this study was performed, teachers emphasize syllable activities that do not improve phonemic knowledge, an essential issue to languages that rely on the alphabetic principle, such as Brazilian Portuguese2222. Santos MTM, Navas ALP. Terapia da linguagem escrita. In: Santos MTM, Navas ALP. Distúrbios de leitura e escrita: teoria e prática. 1ª Ed. Barueri: Ed. Manole ; 2002. P.191-223.. On this basis, it is possible to speculate that schooling was not a decisive factor to the statistical difference in PA due to the way that Portuguese is taught in Brazilian schools.

Data regarding RAN showed that children in G2 required less time to name the stimuli and made fewer errors; however, no statistically significant difference was found. Both groups required more time to name colors and objects than letters and numbers. Such stimuli demand the use of attentional, perceptive and visual process to retrieve the larger lexicon structure, which requires more time for naming them rapid and sequentially 55. Salgado CA, Capellini SA. Programa de remediação fonológica em escolares com dislexia do desenvolvimento. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2008; 20(6):31-6. , 2323. Denckla MB, Rudel RG. Rapid automatized naming of pictures objects, colors, letters e numbers by normal children. Cortex. 1974;10(4):186-202. , 2424. Albuquerque CP. Rapid naming contributions to reading and writing acquisition of European Portuguese. Read Writ. 2012;25(2):775-97..

Regarding the similarity in the performance in RAN of children from the present study, it might have occurred due to the nearness of grade school between the groups and also because they have started to learn the orthographic patterns of Brazilian Portuguese. A research2525. Bicalho LGR, Alves LM. A nomeação seriada rápida em escolares com e sem queixas de problemas de aprendizagem em escola pública e particular. Rev CEFAC. 2010;12(2):608-16. investigated RAN in students from the cycle I of elementary school and found similar means between 3rd and 4th year children, the same level of schooling of students from the present study. Another Brazilian study2626. Germano GD, Pinheiro FH, Padula NAMR, Lorencetti MD, Capellini SA. Desempenho em consciência fonológica, nomeação rápida, leitura e escrita em escolares com dislexia secundária a retardo mental e com bom desempenho acadêmico. Rev CEFAC. 2011;6(2):236-48., which verified the performance of students with and without cognitive impairment, also reported similar performance between the groups.

It is important to mention that several authors affirmed that in the early literacy, items such as letters and numbers are more quickly named than colors and objects due to the multiplicity of semantic structure that the formers require2727. Närhi V, Ahonen T, Aro M, Leppäsaari T, Korhonen TT, Tolvanen A, Lyytinen H. Rapid serial naming: relations between different stimuli e neuropsychological factors. Brain Lang. 2005;92(3):45-57. , 2828. Savage R, Frederickson N. Evidence of a highly specific relationship between rapid automatic naming of digits and text-reading speed. Brain Lang. 2005;93(2):152-9.. Since objects and colors involve lexical access, they might involve more than one retrieval feature, for example, the color blue may have various gradation of tones2727. Närhi V, Ahonen T, Aro M, Leppäsaari T, Korhonen TT, Tolvanen A, Lyytinen H. Rapid serial naming: relations between different stimuli e neuropsychological factors. Brain Lang. 2005;92(3):45-57..

An international study2929. Brizzolara D, Chilosi A, Cipriani P, Di Filippo G, Gasperini F, Mazzotti S. Do phonologic e rapid automatized naming deficits differentially affect yslexic children with e without a history of language delay? A study of Italian dyslexic children. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2006;12(5):141-9. stated that although the performance in colors and objects may predict reading proficiency in preschool children, number and letters have more correlation with the reading of children in early literacy. Therefore, we may speculate that children from the different groups, even though in different levels of schooling, are both in early literacy. However, it is important to highlight that the statistical difference found in the time spent to name numbers and in the amount of errors in it, indicate a best performance of G2 and suggest that they may be more advanced than G1 in decoding skills.

Regarding the negative correlation between PA and RAN of letters and numbers for children from G2, results from others researches were confirmed2727. Närhi V, Ahonen T, Aro M, Leppäsaari T, Korhonen TT, Tolvanen A, Lyytinen H. Rapid serial naming: relations between different stimuli e neuropsychological factors. Brain Lang. 2005;92(3):45-57. , 2828. Savage R, Frederickson N. Evidence of a highly specific relationship between rapid automatic naming of digits and text-reading speed. Brain Lang. 2005;93(2):152-9.; those studies pointed that the better the performance in PA, the fewer is the time spent in RAN, and they both increase as the schooling advancement. Such correlation did not occur in RAN of colors and objects. A possible explanation for that relies on the fact that both skills are related to decoding tasks, suggesting that G2 may be more advanced in such skill.

A recent study3030. Soares AJC. Consciência fonêmica, nomeação automática rápida e velocidade de leitura em escolares do ensino fundamental [dissertação]. São Paulo: Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo; 2013. noticed that PA is related no only to early literacy but also to reading proficiency and may provide more accuracy in text reading activities. Thus, is possible to speculate that G2 children may present, even slightly, better performance in decoding tasks, variable which were not verified in the present research.

Hence, the present study provides relevant contributions showing that schooling has an important role on the development of linguistic-cognitive skills. Such data have been already known concerning PA22. Mousinho R, Correa J. Habilidades linguístico-cognitivas em leitores e não-leitores. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2009;21(2):113-8. , 44. Nunes C, Frota S, Mousinho R. Consciência fonológica e o processo de aprendizagem de leitura e escrita: implicações teóricas para o embasamento da prática fonoaudiológica. Rev CEFAC. 2009;11(2):207-12. , 66. Ukrainetz TA, Nuspl JJ, Wilkerson K, Beddes SR. The effects of syllable instruction on phonemic awareness in preschoolers. Early Child Res Q. 2011;26 (2):50-60.

7. Soares AJC, Cárnio MS. Consciência fonêmica em escolares antes e após oficinas de linguagem. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2012;24(1):69-75.
- 88. Yesil-Dagli U. Predicting ELL students' beginning first grade English oral reading fluency from initial kindergarten: Reading, letter naming, and phonological awareness skills. Early Child Res. 2011;26(2):15-29. , 1616. Moojen S, Lamprecht RR, Santos RM, Freitas GM, Brodacz R, Siqueira M, Correa A, Guarda E. Consciência Fonológica - Instrumento de Avaliação Sequencial (CONFIAS). São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2003., however they are scarce in relation to RAN. Thus, data from the present study indicate reciprocity between literacy and consolidation of PA and RAN.

It is important to emphasize that data from the present research highlight an important area of investigation in reading and writing field, pointing out the need of a better understanding regarding the reciprocity between the skills investigated in this research. Thus, a larger sample and tests involving reading skills may provide advancement in the knowledge of the relation between the variables investigated in this study.

Conclusion

There is, even slightly, influence of schooling on PA and RAN once G2 children showed better results than G1 in both tests, even though with no statistical difference.

Also, negative correlation between PA and RAN of letters and number is noticed only for students with higher level of schooling, indicating that the better performance in PA, the fewer time spent in RAN.

The present study raises important contributions concerning the development of linguistic-cognitive skills and its relation with schooling advancement, indicating reciprocity between them, and also the need of investigating a larger sample and different levels of schooling.

Acknowledgments

We thank Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo for sponsoring this research through a scholarship scientific initiation under process nº 2011/080928-7

  • 1
    Torgesen JK, Morgan ST, Davis C. Effects of two types of phonological awareness training on word learning in kindergarten children. J Educ Psychol.1992;84(3):364-70.
  • 2
    Mousinho R, Correa J. Habilidades linguístico-cognitivas em leitores e não-leitores. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2009;21(2):113-8.
  • 3
    Wocadlo C, Rieger I. Phonology, rapid naming and academic achievement in very preterm children at eight years of age. Early Hum Dev. 2009;83(6):367-77.
  • 4
    Nunes C, Frota S, Mousinho R. Consciência fonológica e o processo de aprendizagem de leitura e escrita: implicações teóricas para o embasamento da prática fonoaudiológica. Rev CEFAC. 2009;11(2):207-12.
  • 5
    Salgado CA, Capellini SA. Programa de remediação fonológica em escolares com dislexia do desenvolvimento. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2008; 20(6):31-6.
  • 6
    Ukrainetz TA, Nuspl JJ, Wilkerson K, Beddes SR. The effects of syllable instruction on phonemic awareness in preschoolers. Early Child Res Q. 2011;26 (2):50-60.
  • 7
    Soares AJC, Cárnio MS. Consciência fonêmica em escolares antes e após oficinas de linguagem. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2012;24(1):69-75.
  • 8
    Yesil-Dagli U. Predicting ELL students' beginning first grade English oral reading fluency from initial kindergarten: Reading, letter naming, and phonological awareness skills. Early Child Res. 2011;26(2):15-29.
  • 9
    Powell D, Stainthorp R, Stuart M, Garwood H, Quinlan P. An experimental comparison between rival theories of rapid automatized naming performance and its relationship to reading. J Exp Child Psychol. 2007;98(1):46-68.
  • 10
    Wakamiya E, Okumura T, Nakanishi M, Takeshita T, Mizuta T, Kurimoto N, Tamai H. Effects of sequential and discrete rapid naming on reading in Japanese children with reading difficulty. Brain Dev. 2011;12(3):356-68.
  • 11
    Castles A, Coltheart M, Wilson K, Valpied J. The genesis of reading ability: what helps children learn letter-sound correspondences? J Exp Child Psychol. 2009;104(1): 68-88.
  • 12
    Navas ALGP, Pinto JCBR, Dellisa PRR. Avanços no conhecimento do processamento da fluência em leitura: da palavra ao texto. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2009;14(4):553-9.
  • 13
    Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashote CA. Comprehensive test of phonological processing (CTOPP). Proed - an international publisher. Austin: Texas; 1999.
  • 14
    Wertzner HF. Fonologia. In: Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes D, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner HF. ABFW - Teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. Carapicuíba: Pró-Fono; 2004. P. 5-40.
  • 15
    Stein LM. Teste de Desempenho Escolar (TDE). São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 1994.
  • 16
    Moojen S, Lamprecht RR, Santos RM, Freitas GM, Brodacz R, Siqueira M, Correa A, Guarda E. Consciência Fonológica - Instrumento de Avaliação Sequencial (CONFIAS). São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2003.
  • 17
    Simões VF. Estudo do desempenho de crianças das séries iniciais: do ensino fundamental I em testes de leitura, escrita e nomeação rápida [tese]. São Paulo (SP): Universidade de São Paulo; 2006.
  • 18
    Rosal CAR. Habilidades de segmentação fonêmica em crianças normais de primeira, segunda e terceira séries do ensino fundamental [dissertação]. São Paulo (SP): Universidade de São Paulo; 2002.
  • 19
    Schuele CM, Boudreau D. Phonological awareness intervention: Beyond the basics. Lang Speech Hear Ser. 2008;39(1):3-20.
  • 20
    Furnes B, Samuelsson S. Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming predicting early development in reading e spelling: Results from a cross-linguistic longitudinal study. Learn Individ Differ. 2011;21(2):85-95.
  • 21
    Capellini SA, Lanza SC. Desempenho de escolares em consciência fonológica, nomeação rápida, leitura e escrita. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2010;22(3):239-44.
  • 22
    Santos MTM, Navas ALP. Terapia da linguagem escrita. In: Santos MTM, Navas ALP. Distúrbios de leitura e escrita: teoria e prática. 1ª Ed. Barueri: Ed. Manole ; 2002. P.191-223.
  • 23
    Denckla MB, Rudel RG. Rapid automatized naming of pictures objects, colors, letters e numbers by normal children. Cortex. 1974;10(4):186-202.
  • 24
    Albuquerque CP. Rapid naming contributions to reading and writing acquisition of European Portuguese. Read Writ. 2012;25(2):775-97.
  • 25
    Bicalho LGR, Alves LM. A nomeação seriada rápida em escolares com e sem queixas de problemas de aprendizagem em escola pública e particular. Rev CEFAC. 2010;12(2):608-16.
  • 26
    Germano GD, Pinheiro FH, Padula NAMR, Lorencetti MD, Capellini SA. Desempenho em consciência fonológica, nomeação rápida, leitura e escrita em escolares com dislexia secundária a retardo mental e com bom desempenho acadêmico. Rev CEFAC. 2011;6(2):236-48.
  • 27
    Närhi V, Ahonen T, Aro M, Leppäsaari T, Korhonen TT, Tolvanen A, Lyytinen H. Rapid serial naming: relations between different stimuli e neuropsychological factors. Brain Lang. 2005;92(3):45-57.
  • 28
    Savage R, Frederickson N. Evidence of a highly specific relationship between rapid automatic naming of digits and text-reading speed. Brain Lang. 2005;93(2):152-9.
  • 29
    Brizzolara D, Chilosi A, Cipriani P, Di Filippo G, Gasperini F, Mazzotti S. Do phonologic e rapid automatized naming deficits differentially affect yslexic children with e without a history of language delay? A study of Italian dyslexic children. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2006;12(5):141-9.
  • 30
    Soares AJC. Consciência fonêmica, nomeação automática rápida e velocidade de leitura em escolares do ensino fundamental [dissertação]. São Paulo: Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo; 2013.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Feb 2015

History

  • Received
    19 Feb 2014
  • Accepted
    01 June 2014
ABRAMO Associação Brasileira de Motricidade Orofacial Rua Uruguaiana, 516, Cep 13026-001 Campinas SP Brasil, Tel.: +55 19 3254-0342 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistacefac@cefac.br