Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Mothers’ Experiences with their Own Caregivers and Child’s Behaviors Attachment1 1 Support: Article derived from the Master’s dissertation of the first author under the supervision of the second, defended in 2015, in the Postgraduate Program in Psychology of the Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul.

Experiências Maternas com os Próprios Cuidadores e os Comportamentos de Apego do Filho

Experiencias Maternas con los propios Cuidadores y los Comportamientos de Apego del Hijo

Abstract

Early experiences of mothers with their own caregivers tend to influence later relationships, particularly with future children. Thus, the objective of this study was to investigate the mothers’ experiences with their own caregivers and the relation with the child’s current attachment behaviors. Participants were 48 mothers and their children (M = 28.1m; SD = 4.3). The mothers’ experience with their own caregivers was investigated through their perceptions of the care received from their caregivers (Parental Bonding Instrument), and by the representations of attachment (Attachment Script Assessment). The attachment behaviors of the child were investigated by Attachment Q-Sort. Analyzes revealed a reasonable consistency between the mothers’ perceptions about received care, their representations of attachment, and the attachment behaviors of their children. The results support the literature on the relation between mother’ experiences with their own caregivers and the attachment behaviors of the child and bring contributions when presenting evidences in a Brazilian sample.

Keywords:
attachment behavior; representation; psychic transmission between generations

Resumo

As experiências iniciais maternas com os próprios cuidadores tendem a influenciar as relações posteriores, particularmente com os futuros filhos. Dessa forma, o objetivo deste estudo foi investigar as experiências das mães com os próprios cuidadores e a relação com os comportamentos atuais de apego do filho. Participaram 48 mães e seus filhos/as (M = 28,1m; DP = 4,3). A experiência das mães com os próprios cuidadores foi investigada através das suas percepções sobre os cuidados recebidos de seus cuidadores (Parental Bonding Instrument), e pelas representações de apego (Attachment Script Assessment). Os comportamentos de apego da criança foram investigados pelo Attachment Q-Sort. Análises revelaram consistência razoável entre as percepções das mães sobre os cuidados recebidos, as suas representações de apego e os comportamentos de apego dos filhos. Os resultados apoiam a literatura sobre a relação entre experiências maternas com os próprios cuidadores e os comportamentos de apego do filho e traz contribuições ao apresentar evidências em uma amostra brasileira.

Palavras-chave:
comportamento de apego; representação; transmissão psíquica entre gerações

Resumen

Las experiencias iniciales maternas con sus propios cuidadores tienden a influir en las relaciones posteriores, particularmente con los futuros hijos. De esta forma, el objetivo de este estudio fue investigar las experiencias de las madres con los propios cuidadores y la relación con los comportamientos actuales de apego del hijo. Participaron 48 madres y sus hijos (M = 28,1m, DP = 4,3). La experiencia de las madres con los propios cuidadores fue investigada por medio de sus percepciones sobre los cuidados recibidos de sus cuidadores (Parental Bonding Instrument), y por las representaciones de apego (Attachment Script Assessment). Los comportamientos de apego del niño fueron investigados por el Attachment Q-Sort. Los análisis revelaron consistencia razonable entre las percepciones de las madres sobre los cuidados recibidos, sus representaciones de apego y los comportamientos de apego de los hijos. Los resultados apoyan la literatura sobre la relación entre experiencias maternas con sus propios cuidadores y los comportamientos de apego del hijo y aportan contribuciones al presentar evidencias en una muestra brasileña.

Palabras clave:
comportamiento de apego; representación; transmisión psíquica entre generaciones

Several theories, including the Attachment Theory (Bowlby, 1969/1990Bowlby, J. (1990). Apego e perda: Vol 1. Apego: A natureza do vínculo (A. Cabral, Trad., 2a ed.). São Paulo, SP: Martins Fontes. (Trabalho original publicado em 1969)), attribute a central importance to early experiences with caregivers and their influence on later relationships throughout development, such as with future children. Attachment refers to the affective bond that is constituted through the emotional and cognitive development of the child, as well as by the consistency of care received by the primary caregivers (Bowlby, 1979/1997Bowlby, J. (1997). Formação e rompimento dos laços afetivos (A. Cabral, Trad., 3a ed.). São Paulo, SP: Martins Fontes . (Trabalho original publicado em 1979)). In the relationship with the caregiver, the security and comfort experienced by the child in his/her presence would allow him/her to be used as a secure base, that is, a figure that would allow a balance between attachment and exploitation in the relation of the dyad (Ainsworth, 1989Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44(4), 709-716. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.44.4.709
https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.44.4.7...
).

Based on these concepts, Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters and Wall (1978Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. N. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.) defined three types of attachment: secure attachment, in which the child knows that his/her caregiver will be available to offer help in adverse situations and feels confident to explore the environment; the insecure avoidant attachment in which the child tends not to have confidence that when he/she looks for care he/she will have it and thus expects at some level to be rejected, tending to become emotionally sufficient, to interact little with his/her caregivers and showing little inhibited in interaction with strangers; and, the insecure ambivalent attachment in which the child tends to show uncertainty about the availability and possibility of receiving help from the caregiver, being able to present immature behaviors and little interest in the exploration, turning his attention to caregivers in a worried way. Subsequently, it was proposed a type called disorganized attachment (Main, 1991Main, M. (1991). Metacognitive knowledge, metacognitive monitoring and singular (coherent) vs multiple (incoherent) models of attachment: Findings and directions for future research. In C. M. Parkes, J. Stevenson-Hinde, & P. Marris (Eds.), Attachment across the life cicle (pp. 127-159). New York, NY: Routledge.), characterized by children with apparently disconnected behaviors. Such associations were corroborated in empirical studies, including Hsiao, Koren-Karie, Bailey and Moran (2015Hsiao, C., Koren-Karie, N., Bailey, H., & Moran, G. (2015). It takes two to talk: Longitudinal associations among infant-mother attachment, maternal attachment representations, and mother-child emotion dialogues. Attachment & Human Development, 17(1), 43-64. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2014.981671
https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2014.98...
), which emphasized the emotional dialogue of the dyads.

In addition to the caregiver-child relationships resulting in distinct attachment patterns, Bowlby (1969/1990)Bowlby, J. (1990). Apego e perda: Vol 1. Apego: A natureza do vínculo (A. Cabral, Trad., 2a ed.). São Paulo, SP: Martins Fontes. (Trabalho original publicado em 1969) suggested that these initial relationships were also related to the development of an internal psychological organization, involving representative models of self and attachment figures, called a model of internal functioning, which would tend to be stable throughout development and would also influence future romantic and parental relationships. For Waters, Rodrigues and Ridgeway (1998Waters, H. S., Rodrigues, L. M., & Ridgeway, D. (1998). Cognitive underpinnings of narrative attachment assessment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 71, 211-234.), this internal psychological organization would refer to the mental representations of attachment, in which attachment and the secure base phenomenon would be represented in the memory as a script. Scripts would be general representations of events experienced throughout development with primary attachment figures, which would tend to generalize, reach different levels of abstraction, and influence later relationships throughout development (Waters & Rodrigues-Doolabh, 2001Waters, H. S., & Rodrigues-Doolabh, L. (2001). Are attachment scripts the building blocks of attachment representations? Narrative assessment of representations and the AAI. In H. Waters & E. Waters (Chairs), Narrative Measures of Attachment for Adults. Poster symposium presented at the Biennial Meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, MN.).

Based on these theoretical assumptions and clinical practice, Bowlby (1979/1997)Bowlby, J. (1997). Formação e rompimento dos laços afetivos (A. Cabral, Trad., 3a ed.). São Paulo, SP: Martins Fontes . (Trabalho original publicado em 1979) and the followers of the Attachment Theory began to emphasize the stability of attachment patterns throughout child development and the existence of intergenerational transmission of attachment between parents and children. About the stability of attachment patterns, Ainsworth (1989Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44(4), 709-716. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.44.4.709
https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.44.4.7...
) pointed out that children with insecure attachment had the potential to eventually present safe standards from the experiences and changes in the relationships with their caregivers. At the same time, she pointed out that some children who had secure attachment had a risk of having insecure patterns later depending on their future experiences. In the same direction, for Main (2000Main, M. (2000). The organized categories of infant, child and adult attachment: Flexible vs. inflexible attention under attachment-related stress. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 48(4), 1055-1096. doi:10.1177/00030651000480041801
https://doi.org/10.1177/0003065100048004...
), attachment security would not be fully defined in childhood, since all children would be susceptible to experiencing a variety of favorable and unfavorable experiences that could alter the mental states of attachment. Thus, although the existence of a certain stability of the attachment pattern throughout development is accepted, it could be modified either by subsequent events, positive or negative, or by re-elaborations of the initial experiences. Thus, empirical studies suggest a certain continuous effect of the initial attachment experiences (Groh et al., 2014Groh, A. M., Roisman, G. I., Booth-LaForce, C., Fraley, R. C., Owen, M. T., Cox, M. J., & Burchinal, M. R. (2014). IV. Stability of attachment security from infancy to late adolescence. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 79(3), 51-66. doi:10.1111/mono.12113
https://doi.org/10.1111/mono.12113...
), while new environmental circumstances continue to play an important role (Booth-LaForce et al., 2014Booth-LaForce, C., Groh, A. M., Burchinal, M. R., Roisman, G. I., Owen, M. T., & Cox, M. J. (2014). V. Caregiving and contextual sources of continuity and change in attachment security from infancy to late adolescence. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 79(3), 67-84. doi:10.1111/mono.12114
https://doi.org/10.1111/mono.12114...
).

In addition to the question of stability, several studies have investigated the possibility of intergenerational transmission of attachment patterns (Schoenmakeret al., 2015Schoenmaker, C., Juffer, F., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Linting, M., van der Voort, A., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2015). From maternal sensitivity in infancy to adult attachment representations: A longitudinal adoption study with secure base scripts. Attachment & Human Development , 17(3), 241-256. doi:10.1080/14616734.2015.1037315
https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2015.10...
; Tambelli, Odorisio, & Simonini, 2013Tambelli, R., Odorisio, F., & Simonini, C. (2013). La trasmissione intergenerazionale dell’attaccamento nella prima infanzia: Uno studio sull’attaccamento madre, padre e bambino in una prospettiva interculturale. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 40(4), 785-811. doi:10.1421/76947
https://doi.org/10.1421/76947...
; Zaccagnino, Cussino, Borgi, Vianzone, & Carassaa, 2016Zaccagnino, M., Cussino, M., Borgi, S., Vianzone, S., & Carassaa, A. (2016). A longitudinal study of attachment and caregiving representations among Swiss mother-child dyads. Minerva Psichiatrica, 57(1), 10-21.). According to attachment theory, the intergenerational transmission of attachment would be interpreted quite specifically; that is, the roots of current attachment relationships in the parents’ minds were initially developed in the families of origin and would influence parental behavior and the development of their own child’s attachment patterns (Goodnow & Collins, 1990Goodnow, J. J., & Collins, W. A. (1990). Development according to parents: The nature, sources and consequences of parents’ ideas. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.). Corroborating this view, the van IJzendoorne Bakermans-Kranenburg (1997van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (1997). Intergenerational transmission of attachment: A move to the contextual level. In L. Atkinson & K. J. Zucker (Eds.), Attachment and psychopathology (pp. 135-170). New York, NY: Guilford.) meta-analysis evidenced strong associations between the parents’ attachment representations and the children’s sense of security, especially detached parents and children with insecure avoidant attachment (r = 0.45); autonomous parents and children with secure attachment (r = 0.47); worried parents and children with insecure ambivalent attachment (r = 0.42). A review of the most current literature corroborated intergenerational transmission, and when reviewing 95 studies (n = 4.819) analyses showed greater effects for transmission of secure attachment (r = 0.31) than for insecure attachment (r = 0.21), although with effects sizes significantly lower than two previous decades (Verhage et al., 2016Verhage, M. L., Schuengel, C., Madigan, S., Fearon, R. M. P., Oosterman, M., Cassibba, R., … van IJzendoorn, M. H (2016). Narrowing the transmission gap: A synthesis of three decades of research on intergenerational transmission of attachment. Psychological Bulletin, 142(4), 337-366. doi:10.1037/bul0000038
https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000038...
). Similar results were also found in the study by Fonagy, Steele, and Steele (1991Fonagy, P., Steele, H., & Steele, M. (1991). Maternal representations of attachment during pregnancy predict the organization of infant-mother attachment at one year of age. Child Development, 62(5), 891-905. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01578.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991...
), who prospectively investigated the relation between maternal attachment and baby attachment, so that 75% of the children had attachment patterns like those of their mothers. Thus, the literature identifies an association between parental attachment representations, parental care, and child attachment patterns (Behrens, Haltigan, & Bahm, 2016Behrens, K. Y., Haltigan, J. D., & Bahm, N. I. G. (2016). Infant attachment, adult attachment, and maternal sensitivity: Revisiting the intergenerational transmission gap. Attachment & Human Development, 18(4), 337-353. doi:10.1080/14616734.2016.1167095
https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2016.11...
; Schoenmaker et al., 2015Schoenmaker, C., Juffer, F., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Linting, M., van der Voort, A., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2015). From maternal sensitivity in infancy to adult attachment representations: A longitudinal adoption study with secure base scripts. Attachment & Human Development , 17(3), 241-256. doi:10.1080/14616734.2015.1037315
https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2015.10...
).

One of the possible explanations for the association of mother-child attachment patterns would be that mothers with securely-based attachment representation would tend to have a responsive history of their children’s signals as well as children who would use them as a secure base both in common and emergency environments. Likewise, mothers with representations of attachment without secure base content would tend to be less sensitive to their children’s signals, and they would not have their caregiver as a secure base (Pederson, Bailey, Tarabulsy, Bento, & Moran, 2014Pederson, D. R., Bailey, H. N., Tarabulsy, G. M., Bento, S., & Moran, G. (2014). Understanding sensitivity: Lessons learned from the legacy of Mary Ainsworth. Attachment & Human Development , 16(3), 261-270. doi:10.1080/14616734.2014.900094
https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2014.90...
; Quezada & Santelices, 2010Quezada, V., & Santelices, M. P. (2010). Attachment and mother’s psychopathology: Relation with baby attachment style in the first year of life. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 42(1), 53-61. Retrieved from http://www.scielo.org.co/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S0120-05342010000100005
http://www.scielo.org.co/scielo.php?scri...
). In this sense, the study by Kochanska and Kim (2013Kochanska, G., & Kim, S. (2013). Early attachment organization with both parents and future behavior problems: From infancy to middle childhood. Child Development, 84(1), 283-296. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01852.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012...
) showed that parents with insecure attachment patterns are considered as risk factors for the child’s attachment pattern. It is worth mentioning, as evidenced by Miljkovitch, Moss, Bernier, Pascuzzo and Sander (2015Miljkovitch, R., Moss, E., Bernier, A., Pascuzzo, K., & Sander, E. (2015). Refining the assessment of internal working models: The Attachment Multiple Model Interview. Attachment & Human Development , 17(5), 492-521. doi:10.1080/14616734.2015.1075561
https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2015.10...
), that both parents contribute to the individual’s emotional development.

In addition to attachment representations, Verhage et al. (2016Verhage, M. L., Schuengel, C., Madigan, S., Fearon, R. M. P., Oosterman, M., Cassibba, R., … van IJzendoorn, M. H (2016). Narrowing the transmission gap: A synthesis of three decades of research on intergenerational transmission of attachment. Psychological Bulletin, 142(4), 337-366. doi:10.1037/bul0000038
https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000038...
) pointed out that risk factors such as psychopathology in mothers, adolescent mothers, premature children, socioeconomic difficulties and domestic violence could hinder mothers’ sensitivity to their children, since they are absorbed by other issues in their lives that influence the relationship with the child. In this sense, the study by Kelly (2016Kelly, K. R. (2016). Mother-child conversations and child memory narratives: The roles of child gender and attachment. Psychology of Language and Communication, 20(1), 48-72. doi:10.1515/plc-2016-0003
https://doi.org/10.1515/plc-2016-0003...
) showed that mothers less attentive to the signs of their children in the context of conversations presented a conversational style that did not meet the child’s need and the child presented more possibility of developing an insecure attachment pattern. In this context, the study by Posada et al. (2016Posada, G., Trumbell, J., Noblega, M., Plata, S., Peña, P., Carbonell, O. A., & Lu, T. (2016). Maternal sensitivity and child secure base use in early childhood: Studies in different cultural contexts. Child Development, 87(1), 297-311. doi:10.1111/cdev.12454
https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12454...
) revealed that in all cultures maternal sensitivity and maternal care were determinant for the development of a certain pattern of attachment of the child.

On the possibility of stability or change in patterns of attachment and intergenerational transmission of these patterns, van IJzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg (1997van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (1997). Intergenerational transmission of attachment: A move to the contextual level. In L. Atkinson & K. J. Zucker (Eds.), Attachment and psychopathology (pp. 135-170). New York, NY: Guilford.) pointed out that past attachment experiences could be reworked by current relationships, influencing parental care and building new attachment relationships. The authors also pointed out that the current attachment representation would be formed based on the initial attachment experiences and on the current relationships. In this sense, a good friend, spouse or therapist could provide a “secure base” for re-elaborating adverse childhood experiences. Furthermore, for the authors, parental care would be influenced by the social context, since the social support network could moderate the effects of unfavorable circumstances; and that some characteristics of children could hinder more sensitive care of their parents due to severe physical disabilities or highly irritable temperament.

Although some studies have confirmed the relation between the mother’s initial experiences with their own caregivers and the child’s attachment behaviors (Kochanska & Kim, 2013Kochanska, G., & Kim, S. (2013). Early attachment organization with both parents and future behavior problems: From infancy to middle childhood. Child Development, 84(1), 283-296. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01852.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012...
; Miljkovitch et al., 2015Miljkovitch, R., Moss, E., Bernier, A., Pascuzzo, K., & Sander, E. (2015). Refining the assessment of internal working models: The Attachment Multiple Model Interview. Attachment & Human Development , 17(5), 492-521. doi:10.1080/14616734.2015.1075561
https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2015.10...
; Pederson et al., 2014Pederson, D. R., Bailey, H. N., Tarabulsy, G. M., Bento, S., & Moran, G. (2014). Understanding sensitivity: Lessons learned from the legacy of Mary Ainsworth. Attachment & Human Development , 16(3), 261-270. doi:10.1080/14616734.2014.900094
https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2014.90...
; Quezada & Santelices, 2010Quezada, V., & Santelices, M. P. (2010). Attachment and mother’s psychopathology: Relation with baby attachment style in the first year of life. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 42(1), 53-61. Retrieved from http://www.scielo.org.co/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S0120-05342010000100005
http://www.scielo.org.co/scielo.php?scri...
) no publications with this focus were found with the Brazilian population, nor based on the Scripts of the parents and the evaluation of the child’s attachment behaviors. Considering the above, the objective of this study was to investigate the mothers’ experiences with their own caregivers and the relation with the child’s current attachment behaviors. Based on the literature, it was hypothesized that the perception of the care received by the mother would be related to their representations of attachment, and that these would be related to the current behaviors of attachment of the child.

Method

Participants

Participating in this study were 48 mothers (33.3 years, SD = 4.5) and their children (M = 28.1 months, SD = 4.3, boys: 50.4%, girls: 49.6% with the majority attending daycare centers (63.1%). Most mothers had completed higher education (86.5%; 56.1% with postgraduate studies), married (91.5%, M = 6.7 years, SD = 3.4), worked outside the home (78.3%) and had only one child (74%). The family income was on average 10 minimum wages (SD = 6.7), characterizing the sample as having a high socioeconomic level.

Instruments

Family demographic data form (NUDIF/CRESCI, 2011), aims to obtain current demographic information from the family.

Parental Bonding Instrument (Parker, Tupling, & Brown, 1979Parker, G., Tupling, H., & Brown, L. B. (1979). A Parental Bonding Instrument. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 52(1), 1-10. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8341.1979.tb02487.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8341.1979...
), adapted to the Portuguese by Hauck et al. (2006Hauck, S., Schestatsky, S., Terra, L., Knijnik, L., Sanchez, P., & Ceitlin, L. H. F. (2006). Adaptação transcultural para o português brasileiro do Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI). Revista de Psiquiatria do Rio Grande do Sul, 28(2), 162-168. doi:10.1590/S0101-81082006000200008
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0101-8108200600...
), evaluates the respondents’ perception of care received from their parents. The instrument contemplates 25 items using a Likert scale. The analysis of the items allows to investigate affectivity (affection, warmth, availability, care, sensitivity versus coldness and rejection), and control or protection (control, intrusion versus encouragement of autonomy). Parents are classified into four categories: optimal care (affection scores high and protection scores low); affective control (affection and protection score high); control without affection (protection scores high and affection scores low); and negligent care (affection and protection score low).

Attachment Script Assessment (Waters & Rodrigues-Doolabh, 2001Waters, H. S., & Rodrigues-Doolabh, L. (2001). Are attachment scripts the building blocks of attachment representations? Narrative assessment of representations and the AAI. In H. Waters & E. Waters (Chairs), Narrative Measures of Attachment for Adults. Poster symposium presented at the Biennial Meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, MN.), evaluates whether the person has a script, or representation of secure base attachment. For application five sheets of paper are used, each containing a title and a list of words distributed in three columns and the person is asked to elaborate a narrative. Two of the stories are related to adult-child situations (Baby’s morning and Doctor’s office), two others are related to adult-adult stories (Joana and Pedro’s camp and Suzana’s accident) and a fifth, involves a situation considered neutral since it does not enter the analysis (The morning of purchases). Each narrative elaborated in relation to these situations receives a score of 1 (script with no apparent basic content) to 7 (script with secure content base with substantial elaboration) and the final score is the average of the four narratives. For the present study the codification of the narratives was carried out by the first author and by two other graduates of psychology trained in theory and practice by a specialist in the use of this instrument. The reliability among the narrative coders was: Baby’s morning (95.2%), Doctor’s office (93.7%), Joana and Pedro’s camp (88.9%) and Suzana’s accident (85.7%). The version of the instrument used in this study was translated into Portuguese by Semensato (2009Semensato, M. R. (2009). Relações entre scripts de apego individuais e compartilhados em casais com um filho com autismo (Dissertação de mestrado). Retrieved from https://www.lume.ufrgs.br/handle/10183/32001
https://www.lume.ufrgs.br/handle/10183/3...
) and presented good reliability and validity indexes (Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2006Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2006). Script-like attachment representations: Steps towards a secure base for further research. Attachment & Human Development, 8(3), 275-281. doi:10.1080/ 14616730600910037
https://doi.org/10.1080/ 146167306009100...
; T.E. Waters, Merrick, Treboux, Crowell, & Albersheim, 2015).

Attachment Q-Sort (E. Waters, 1987Waters, E. (1987). Attachment behavior Q-Set (Version 3.0). New York, NY: State University of New York.), assesses the child’s secure-based attachment behaviors toward the mother / caregiver. It consists of 90 items that characterize children’s behaviors and are distributed by the mother in stacks ranging from 9 (totally like) to 1 (totally different). The correlation between the scored results and the safe child prototype (criterion value established by specialists in the area) allows a child profile to be obtained in a continuum that can vary between -1 and +1 (perfectly negative or positive correlation with the safe child ideal) as detailed by E. Waters (1987)Waters, E. (1987). Attachment behavior Q-Set (Version 3.0). New York, NY: State University of New York.. For the present study the version translated into Portuguese by Barbian (1993Barbian, J. W. (1993). Qualidade do brinquedo simbólico na infância: Contribuições do apego mãe-criança (Tese de doutorado não publicada). Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, RS.) was used. The AQS presents adequate indicators of validity and reliability (van IJzendoorn, Vereijken, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Riksen-Walraven, 2004van IJzendoorn, M. H., Vereijken, C. M. J. L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Riksen-Walraven, J. M. (2004). Assessing attachment security with the Attachment Q-Sort: Meta-analytic evidence for the validity of the observer AQS. Child Development, 75(4), 1188-1213. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00733.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004...
).

Procedure

Data collection. All participants were enrolled in a larger study entitle Impact of daycare on socioemotional and cognitive development in children: longitudinal study of the sixth month of life of the baby at the end of the preschool years (CRESCI, 2010). This project aimed to analyze the impact of daycare on the socio-emotional and cognitive development of children from their sixth month until the end of the preschool years. More specifically, it sought to compare during this period the development of children who attended or not the daycare and relate it to the quality of institutional and family environments. The project started in 2011 with 77 families, among them 29 of the babies attending the daycare (Daycare Group) and 48 of the babies who were cared for by the mother or other caregivers, such as nanny (Non-Daycare Group). Families whose babies entered the daycare were recruited at two federal public daycare and the other families through media or referrals. Besides the families, the study also counted on the participation of 18 educators of the two daycares. The project involved six phases of data collection: 6th, 12th, 18th, 24th, 36th and 48th months of the child’s life. For the purposes of the present study, data collected and instruments were considered only when the children were around 28 months old or had completed the 24 months of age. Further information is detailed in Piccinini et al. (2012Piccinini, C. A., Lopes, R. C., Sperb, T., Gabriel, M., Polli, R., Becker, S. M. S, Martins, G. D. F., Bortolini, M, Cherer, E., Bossi, T. (2012). Impacto da creche no desenvolvimento socioemocional e cognitivo infantil: Estudo longitudinal do sexto mês de vida do bebê ao final dos anos pré-escolares - CRESCI, projeto não publicado.).

Following the procedures and data collection steps of CRESCI Project around the 28 months of the child, the families were contacted via telephone and / or e-mail, and two individual meetings with the mother were scheduled for approximately one hour and thirty minutes each, at the university or other location of her choice. All instruments were applied by staff members, including doctoral and master students.

Data analysis. To investigate the mother’s experience with her own caregivers and the relationship with the child’s attachment behaviors, a correlational design involving the variables was used: the mother’s experience with her caregivers (that is, the mother’s perception about the care received from the caregivers and the representations of attachment of the mother) and the attachment behavior of the child. Descriptive analyses, central distribution, data dispersion and correlations among all variables were performed. Subsequently, analysis of hierarchical grouping and clusters was performed (Field, 2009Field, A. (2009). Descobrindo a estatística utilizando o SPSS (L. Viale, Trad., 2a ed.). Porto Alegre, RS: Artmed.).

Ethical Considerations

The project was approved by the Research Ethics Committee (process no. 2010070) and by the Research Ethics Committee of the Institute of Psychology (process no. 100553).

Results

The hierarchical cluster analysis sought to identify the groups of mothers based on scores of variables: parenting styles of the mothers’ caregivers (if not lived with a caregiver the value of another caregiver was repeated) mother’s attachment representation (with and without secure base in mother/baby narratives, adult\adult and total score); and attachment behaviors of the child (security and dependence). For this analysis, all variables were transformed into Z scores. The result of the analysis (dendrogram) suggested two groups. Table 1 presents the distribution of the mothers in each group, based on the analysis of hierarchical groupings and scores of care styles, on attachment representations and attachment behaviors. To statistically test the differences in attachment behaviors, a Variance Analysis (ANOVA One-way) was performed. Post hoc analyzes (Bonferroni) indicated that the average of the security behaviors was higher in Group III than in group II (p < 0.001) and I (p < 0.001). Likewise, Group II had a higher average in these behaviors than Group I (p < 0.001).

Table 1
Distribution of mothers by group, based on analysis of groupings and scores of care styles, attachment representations and attachment behaviors (N = 48)

Table 2 presents the percentages and frequencies of the mothers allocated to each group by cluster analysis, considering the same variables. As can be seen, in Group 1, there were an expressive number of mothers who had maternal care styles characterized by negligence (37.0% mothers) and care without affection (44.4% mothers), which together totaled almost 81.4 %. In this group 1 also appear the styles of paternal care most characterized by control without affection (48.1%) and negligent (25.9%), totaling 74%. Still, in this group, mothers had a greater presence of attachment representations without a secure base in relation to their caregivers (mother-baby) (92.6% mothers) and even representations of attachment without a secure base in relation to love relationships (adult-adult) (55.6%). Regarding the child’s current attachment behaviors, many children with low (48.1%) and medium (33.3%) attachment behaviors in security can be observed (totaling74 and high (44.4% %) and medium (33.3%) in dependence (totaling 77.7%).

In Group 2, there was a rather different and opposite trend. The results revealed a greater number of mothers who had maternal parenting styles characterized by optimal care (38.1%) and control with affection (23.8%), totaling 61.9%. Parental parenting styles were also characterized by control with affection (38.1%) and optimal care (19.0%), totaling 57.1%. On the other hand, in this group 2 mothers had a greater presence of secure-based representations of attachment in relation to caregivers (76.2%), as were representations of attachment in relation to secure-based love relationships (90.5 %). In relation to current attachment behaviors, we observed more children with attachment behaviors with medium (47.6%) and high (38.1%) scores in security (totaling 85.7%), and low (42.9%) and medium (33.3%) scores in dependence, totaling 76.2%.

Table 2
Percentages and frequencies of the mothers allocated to each group by cluster analysis, considering the scores of parental styles of the caregivers, the representation of attachment of the mothers and the behaviors of attachment of the children (n = 48)

Discussion

The objective of the present study was to investigate the mothers’ experiences with their own caregivers and their current relationship with the child’s attachment behaviors. Based on the literature, the hypothesis of the study predicted relations between the maternal perceptions of their own caregivers, the representations of the mothers’ attachment, and the child’s attachment behaviors. Such a hypothesis would portray the trend of intergenerational transmission of attachment patterns (Schoenmaker et al., 2015Schoenmaker, C., Juffer, F., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Linting, M., van der Voort, A., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2015). From maternal sensitivity in infancy to adult attachment representations: A longitudinal adoption study with secure base scripts. Attachment & Human Development , 17(3), 241-256. doi:10.1080/14616734.2015.1037315
https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2015.10...
; Tambelli et al., 2013Tambelli, R., Odorisio, F., & Simonini, C. (2013). La trasmissione intergenerazionale dell’attaccamento nella prima infanzia: Uno studio sull’attaccamento madre, padre e bambino in una prospettiva interculturale. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 40(4), 785-811. doi:10.1421/76947
https://doi.org/10.1421/76947...
), that is, that the present representations of attachment relationships present in the parents’ minds have been developed in the families of origin and would influence parents’ behavior toward their child and the development of their own child’s attachment patterns (Goodnow & Collins, 1990Goodnow, J. J., & Collins, W. A. (1990). Development according to parents: The nature, sources and consequences of parents’ ideas. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.).

The results of the hierarchical clustering analysis revealed evidence supporting the association between maternal experiences with their caregivers and their child’s attachment behaviors. The analysis suggested two groups, one of mothers with more negative perceptions about care received from their caregivers, as well as representations of attachment without a secure base, and children with current behaviors of less secure and more dependent attachment. And another group of mothers with more positive perceptions about the care received from their caregivers as well as representations of secure-based attachment and children with current behaviors of more secure and less dependent attachment. These analyses show that there is a trend of association between the studied variables, corroborating the hypothesis of the study that suggests an intergenerational transmission of patterns of attachment between generations.

Specifically on the relationship between care styles of both mothers’ caregivers and mothers’ attachment representations, the results revealed that an expressive number of mothers who had maternal and paternal caregivers characterized by neglect and care with control and without affection; also had representations of attachment without a secure base in relation to their caregivers and, later, even, representations of attachment without a secure base in relation to love relationships. In addition, a considerable number of mothers who had styles of maternal and paternal care characterized as optimal and with control with affection had representations of attachment with a secure base in relation to their caregivers and, later, in relation to love relationships These results also support the hypothesis of intergenerational transmission of attachment patterns, in which the type of care received from caregivers would be related to the development of attachment representations (van IJzendoorn & Bakermans-Kranenburg, 1997van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (1997). Intergenerational transmission of attachment: A move to the contextual level. In L. Atkinson & K. J. Zucker (Eds.), Attachment and psychopathology (pp. 135-170). New York, NY: Guilford.).

However, the analyzes also allowed us to verify that the mothers’ representations of attachment to their caregivers did not always correspond to representations of romantic relationships [that mothers had], which is also evaluated by the attachment instrument. Some studies have already shown that childhood attachment patterns are only moderately related to love life, and may even be modified (Cowan & Cowan, 2001Cowan, P., & Cowan, C. P. (2001). A couple perspective on the transmission of attachment patterns. In C. Clulow (Ed.), Adult attachment and couple psychotherapy: The ‘secure base’ in practice and research (pp. 62-82). New York, NY: Routledge.; Kochendorfer & Kerns, 2017Kochendorfer, L. B., & Kerns, K. A. (2017). Perceptions of parent-child attachment relationships and friendship qualities: Predictors of romantic relationship involvement and quality in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 46(5), 1009-1021. doi:10.1007/s10964-017-0645-0
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-017-0645-...
). Thus, the results of the present study corroborate with other studies that important linkages in adulthood, such as loving, may allow the emergence of a new pattern of attachment of the mother, and therefore facilitate the development of a different attachment pattern in the child (van IJzendoorn & Bakermans-Kranenburg, 1997van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (1997). Intergenerational transmission of attachment: A move to the contextual level. In L. Atkinson & K. J. Zucker (Eds.), Attachment and psychopathology (pp. 135-170). New York, NY: Guilford.).

The results also showed that mothers with secure-based attachment representations had children with a higher score for secure attachment behaviors. These results corroborate with the literature, which suggests that mothers who present secure-based attachment content representation in the elaboration of their narratives tend to have a history of responsiveness sensitive to the signs of their children and have children who use them as a security in both common and emergency environments (Waters & Rodrigues-Doolabh, 2001Waters, H. S., & Rodrigues-Doolabh, L. (2001). Are attachment scripts the building blocks of attachment representations? Narrative assessment of representations and the AAI. In H. Waters & E. Waters (Chairs), Narrative Measures of Attachment for Adults. Poster symposium presented at the Biennial Meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, MN.).

One result that drew attention in the present study was the high number of mothers who revealed that the care they received from their caregivers was negligent and with control without affection. It is possible to think that 30 or 40 years ago, cultural and social issues may have contributed to a more rigid and controlling father-mother-children relationship (Moreira & Biasoli-Alves, 2008Moreira, L. V. C., & Biasoli-Alves, Z. M. M. (2008). Práticas educativas: A participação da mãe e da criança na determinação das atividades da rotina diária. Revista Brasileira de Crescimento e Desenvolvimento Humano, 18(1), 53-6. Retrieved from http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S0104-12822008000100008
http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?scr...
). This may have been particularly pronounced for girls, who have always suffered more from rigid cultural patterns. At any rate, it is striking how a negligent style of control without affection can have serious consequences in the child’s own attachment representations without secure base, and later in the third-generation attachment behaviors. In this sense, the study by Zaccagnino et al. (2016Zaccagnino, M., Cussino, M., Borgi, S., Vianzone, S., & Carassaa, A. (2016). A longitudinal study of attachment and caregiving representations among Swiss mother-child dyads. Minerva Psichiatrica, 57(1), 10-21.) also showed a high number of mothers with unsecure attachment representations in their nonclinical sample, emphasizing that previous relationships could be configured as risk factors for the mother-child relationship and the dyad well-being.

The results revealed that the variables investigated (mother’s perception of the care received, representation of attachment of the mothers, and behavior of attachment of the children) presented relations among themselves, as predicted in the hypothesis. However, in order to extend the evidence reported in the present study, it is suggested that future investigations consider the limitations of the present study, especially using other instruments to access the initial experiences of the mothers with their caregivers, given the complexity of investigating such phenomena so remote, at the same time so significant [for each person]. It is also suggested to investigate in more detail the events in the life of the mothers throughout their development that can contribute to re-elaborate their initial experiences, as well as to explain possible inconsistencies between what happened and what would be expected to happen to representations of attachment, as highlighted in the studies presented. [For example, in the present study, in the hierarchical grouping analysis, some inconsistencies were verified, for example: mothers with negligent caregiver perception, attachment representation without a secure base, but children with secure attachment behavior]. These deserve to be carefully investigated by future studies, as it would allow the understanding of the possible existence of protective factors in the mothers’ experiences, as well as a deeper understanding of the phenomenon of intergenerational transmission of attachment patterns. Another limitation would be the lack of triangulation of data with the use of other sources of research, so future studies could use other tools to investigate the attachment pattern of parents, children and parents with their own caregivers. In addition, another limitation of the present study was the absence of paternal research in the development of the child’s attachment patterns, thus, future studies suggest the inclusion of parents.

Investigating these inconsistencies, in other articles, the authors of the present study qualitatively investigated four dyads, two with a more secure attachment pattern (Bortolini & Piccinini, 2015Bortolini, M., & Piccinini, C. A. (2015). Transmissão intergeracional do apego seguro: Evidências a partir de dois casos. Psicologia em Estudo, 20(2), 247-259. doi:10.4025/psicolestud.v20i2.25246
https://doi.org/10.4025/psicolestud.v20i...
) and two with a less secure attachment pattern (Bortolini & Piccinini, in pressBortolini, M. & Piccinini, P. (no prelo). Mães com representações de apego sem base segura e o apego inseguro do filho. Estudos e Pesquisas em Psicologia, 17(3).) who were chosen among the participants of the present study. The findings revealed that the current quality of the parent-child relationship is essential for the establishment of attachment patterns, although it tends to be permeated by the mothers’ own representations of attachment related to their caregivers. Indeed, recent studies of this intergenerational transmission point out that some caregivers would be able to build attachment relationships with their children regardless of their representations of infant attachment, as some children might be more resistant to negative influences of caregivers and the environment (Verhage et al., 2016Verhage, M. L., Schuengel, C., Madigan, S., Fearon, R. M. P., Oosterman, M., Cassibba, R., … van IJzendoorn, M. H (2016). Narrowing the transmission gap: A synthesis of three decades of research on intergenerational transmission of attachment. Psychological Bulletin, 142(4), 337-366. doi:10.1037/bul0000038
https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000038...
).

The present study revealed empirical findings about the importance of the initial experiences of the mothers to the behaviors of attachment of the child. These findings endorse an extensive literature that has emphasized that infant experiences are central to attachment behaviors and to the formation of attachment patterns of individuals. In this sense, it is very important that the professionals involved with children be attentive to possible situations where the initial relationships are not fully meeting the children’s demands for affection and appropriate care. The absence of this tends to profoundly mark children and may extend through generations, as empirically revealed in the present study.

References

  • Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44(4), 709-716. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.44.4.709
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.44.4.709
  • Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. N. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Barbian, J. W. (1993). Qualidade do brinquedo simbólico na infância: Contribuições do apego mãe-criança (Tese de doutorado não publicada). Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, RS.
  • Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2006). Script-like attachment representations: Steps towards a secure base for further research. Attachment & Human Development, 8(3), 275-281. doi:10.1080/ 14616730600910037
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/ 14616730600910037
  • Behrens, K. Y., Haltigan, J. D., & Bahm, N. I. G. (2016). Infant attachment, adult attachment, and maternal sensitivity: Revisiting the intergenerational transmission gap. Attachment & Human Development, 18(4), 337-353. doi:10.1080/14616734.2016.1167095
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2016.1167095
  • Booth-LaForce, C., Groh, A. M., Burchinal, M. R., Roisman, G. I., Owen, M. T., & Cox, M. J. (2014). V. Caregiving and contextual sources of continuity and change in attachment security from infancy to late adolescence. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 79(3), 67-84. doi:10.1111/mono.12114
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/mono.12114
  • Bowlby, J. (1990). Apego e perda: Vol 1. Apego: A natureza do vínculo (A. Cabral, Trad., 2a ed.). São Paulo, SP: Martins Fontes. (Trabalho original publicado em 1969)
  • Bowlby, J. (1997). Formação e rompimento dos laços afetivos (A. Cabral, Trad., 3a ed.). São Paulo, SP: Martins Fontes . (Trabalho original publicado em 1979)
  • Bortolini, M., & Piccinini, C. A. (2015). Transmissão intergeracional do apego seguro: Evidências a partir de dois casos. Psicologia em Estudo, 20(2), 247-259. doi:10.4025/psicolestud.v20i2.25246
    » https://doi.org/10.4025/psicolestud.v20i2.25246
  • Bortolini, M. & Piccinini, P. (no prelo). Mães com representações de apego sem base segura e o apego inseguro do filho. Estudos e Pesquisas em Psicologia, 17(3).
  • Cowan, P., & Cowan, C. P. (2001). A couple perspective on the transmission of attachment patterns. In C. Clulow (Ed.), Adult attachment and couple psychotherapy: The ‘secure base’ in practice and research (pp. 62-82). New York, NY: Routledge.
  • Field, A. (2009). Descobrindo a estatística utilizando o SPSS (L. Viale, Trad., 2a ed.). Porto Alegre, RS: Artmed.
  • Fonagy, P., Steele, H., & Steele, M. (1991). Maternal representations of attachment during pregnancy predict the organization of infant-mother attachment at one year of age. Child Development, 62(5), 891-905. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01578.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01578.x
  • Goodnow, J. J., & Collins, W. A. (1990). Development according to parents: The nature, sources and consequences of parents’ ideas. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Groh, A. M., Roisman, G. I., Booth-LaForce, C., Fraley, R. C., Owen, M. T., Cox, M. J., & Burchinal, M. R. (2014). IV. Stability of attachment security from infancy to late adolescence. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 79(3), 51-66. doi:10.1111/mono.12113
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/mono.12113
  • Hauck, S., Schestatsky, S., Terra, L., Knijnik, L., Sanchez, P., & Ceitlin, L. H. F. (2006). Adaptação transcultural para o português brasileiro do Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI). Revista de Psiquiatria do Rio Grande do Sul, 28(2), 162-168. doi:10.1590/S0101-81082006000200008
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0101-81082006000200008
  • Hsiao, C., Koren-Karie, N., Bailey, H., & Moran, G. (2015). It takes two to talk: Longitudinal associations among infant-mother attachment, maternal attachment representations, and mother-child emotion dialogues. Attachment & Human Development, 17(1), 43-64. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2014.981671
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2014.981671
  • Kochendorfer, L. B., & Kerns, K. A. (2017). Perceptions of parent-child attachment relationships and friendship qualities: Predictors of romantic relationship involvement and quality in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 46(5), 1009-1021. doi:10.1007/s10964-017-0645-0
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-017-0645-0
  • Kelly, K. R. (2016). Mother-child conversations and child memory narratives: The roles of child gender and attachment. Psychology of Language and Communication, 20(1), 48-72. doi:10.1515/plc-2016-0003
    » https://doi.org/10.1515/plc-2016-0003
  • Kochanska, G., & Kim, S. (2013). Early attachment organization with both parents and future behavior problems: From infancy to middle childhood. Child Development, 84(1), 283-296. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01852.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01852.x
  • Main, M. (1991). Metacognitive knowledge, metacognitive monitoring and singular (coherent) vs multiple (incoherent) models of attachment: Findings and directions for future research. In C. M. Parkes, J. Stevenson-Hinde, & P. Marris (Eds.), Attachment across the life cicle (pp. 127-159). New York, NY: Routledge.
  • Main, M. (2000). The organized categories of infant, child and adult attachment: Flexible vs. inflexible attention under attachment-related stress. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 48(4), 1055-1096. doi:10.1177/00030651000480041801
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/00030651000480041801
  • Miljkovitch, R., Moss, E., Bernier, A., Pascuzzo, K., & Sander, E. (2015). Refining the assessment of internal working models: The Attachment Multiple Model Interview. Attachment & Human Development , 17(5), 492-521. doi:10.1080/14616734.2015.1075561
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2015.1075561
  • Moreira, L. V. C., & Biasoli-Alves, Z. M. M. (2008). Práticas educativas: A participação da mãe e da criança na determinação das atividades da rotina diária. Revista Brasileira de Crescimento e Desenvolvimento Humano, 18(1), 53-6. Retrieved from http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S0104-12822008000100008
    » http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S0104-12822008000100008
  • NUDIF/UFRGS (2012). Termo de Consentimento Livre e Esclarecido Informado (para o grupo não creche e para o grupo creche). Instrumento não publicado. Instituto de Psicologia. Universidade do Rio Grande do Sul.
  • NUDIF/UFRGS (2012). Ficha de dados demográficos da família. Instrumento não publicado. Instituto de Psicologia. Universidade do Rio Grande do Sul.
  • NUDIF/UFRGS (2012). Entrevista sobre a relação mãe-bebê. Instrumento não publicado. Instituto de Psicologia. Universidade do Rio Grande do Sul.
  • Pederson, D. R., Bailey, H. N., Tarabulsy, G. M., Bento, S., & Moran, G. (2014). Understanding sensitivity: Lessons learned from the legacy of Mary Ainsworth. Attachment & Human Development , 16(3), 261-270. doi:10.1080/14616734.2014.900094
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2014.900094
  • Posada, G., Trumbell, J., Noblega, M., Plata, S., Peña, P., Carbonell, O. A., & Lu, T. (2016). Maternal sensitivity and child secure base use in early childhood: Studies in different cultural contexts. Child Development, 87(1), 297-311. doi:10.1111/cdev.12454
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12454
  • Parker, G., Tupling, H., & Brown, L. B. (1979). A Parental Bonding Instrument. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 52(1), 1-10. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8341.1979.tb02487.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8341.1979.tb02487.x
  • Piccinini, C. A., Lopes, R. C., Sperb, T., Gabriel, M., Polli, R., Becker, S. M. S, Martins, G. D. F., Bortolini, M, Cherer, E., Bossi, T. (2012). Impacto da creche no desenvolvimento socioemocional e cognitivo infantil: Estudo longitudinal do sexto mês de vida do bebê ao final dos anos pré-escolares - CRESCI, projeto não publicado.
  • Quezada, V., & Santelices, M. P. (2010). Attachment and mother’s psychopathology: Relation with baby attachment style in the first year of life. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 42(1), 53-61. Retrieved from http://www.scielo.org.co/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S0120-05342010000100005
    » http://www.scielo.org.co/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S0120-05342010000100005
  • Semensato, M. R. (2009). Relações entre scripts de apego individuais e compartilhados em casais com um filho com autismo (Dissertação de mestrado). Retrieved from https://www.lume.ufrgs.br/handle/10183/32001
    » https://www.lume.ufrgs.br/handle/10183/32001
  • Schoenmaker, C., Juffer, F., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Linting, M., van der Voort, A., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2015). From maternal sensitivity in infancy to adult attachment representations: A longitudinal adoption study with secure base scripts. Attachment & Human Development , 17(3), 241-256. doi:10.1080/14616734.2015.1037315
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2015.1037315
  • Tambelli, R., Odorisio, F., & Simonini, C. (2013). La trasmissione intergenerazionale dell’attaccamento nella prima infanzia: Uno studio sull’attaccamento madre, padre e bambino in una prospettiva interculturale. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 40(4), 785-811. doi:10.1421/76947
    » https://doi.org/10.1421/76947
  • van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (1997). Intergenerational transmission of attachment: A move to the contextual level. In L. Atkinson & K. J. Zucker (Eds.), Attachment and psychopathology (pp. 135-170). New York, NY: Guilford.
  • van IJzendoorn, M. H., Vereijken, C. M. J. L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Riksen-Walraven, J. M. (2004). Assessing attachment security with the Attachment Q-Sort: Meta-analytic evidence for the validity of the observer AQS. Child Development, 75(4), 1188-1213. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00733.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00733.x
  • Verhage, M. L., Schuengel, C., Madigan, S., Fearon, R. M. P., Oosterman, M., Cassibba, R., … van IJzendoorn, M. H (2016). Narrowing the transmission gap: A synthesis of three decades of research on intergenerational transmission of attachment. Psychological Bulletin, 142(4), 337-366. doi:10.1037/bul0000038
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000038
  • Waters, E. (1987). Attachment behavior Q-Set (Version 3.0). New York, NY: State University of New York.
  • Waters, H. S., Rodrigues, L. M., & Ridgeway, D. (1998). Cognitive underpinnings of narrative attachment assessment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 71, 211-234.
  • Waters, H. S., & Rodrigues-Doolabh, L. (2001). Are attachment scripts the building blocks of attachment representations? Narrative assessment of representations and the AAI. In H. Waters & E. Waters (Chairs), Narrative Measures of Attachment for Adults. Poster symposium presented at the Biennial Meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, MN.
  • Waters, T. E., Fraley, R. C., Groh, A. M., Steele, R. D., Vaughn, B. E., Bost, K. K., … Roisman, G. I. (2015). The latent structure of secure base script knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 51(6), 823-830. doi:10.1037/dev0000012
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000012
  • Zaccagnino, M., Cussino, M., Borgi, S., Vianzone, S., & Carassaa, A. (2016). A longitudinal study of attachment and caregiving representations among Swiss mother-child dyads. Minerva Psichiatrica, 57(1), 10-21.
  • 1
    Support: Article derived from the Master’s dissertation of the first author under the supervision of the second, defended in 2015, in the Postgraduate Program in Psychology of the Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    31 Jan 2019
  • Date of issue
    2018

History

  • Received
    30 July 2017
  • Reviewed
    28 Dec 2017
  • Accepted
    05 Apr 2018
Universidade de São Paulo, Faculdade de Filosofia Ciências e Letras de Ribeirão Preto, Programa de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia Av.Bandeirantes 3900 - Monte Alegre, 14040-901 Ribeirão Preto - São Paulo - Brasil, Tel.: (55 16) 3315-3829 - Ribeirão Preto - SP - Brazil
E-mail: paideia@usp.br