Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

The Structural Approach to Social Representations: Bridges between Theory and Methods

A Abordagem Estrutural das Representações Sociais: Pontes entre Teoria e Método

El Abordaje Estructural de las Representaciones Sociales: Puentes entre Teoría y Método

Abstract

Since the publication of the works of Jean-Claude Abric and Celso Sá, the structural approach to social representation has become widely diffused. There is often a lack of congruency between theoretical aspects of the structural approach and technical characteristics of the different methods used. This paper aims at making explicit the structural characteristics that are studied by the different structural approach techniques. These characteristics are: associative power of the elements, consensual aspects of thought and object essence. With these characteristics it is possible to elaborate a classification of the different techniques of the structural approach to social representations. The conclusion focuses on the absence of the social representation dynamics on a technical level despite being a central theoretical point for a better understanding of the socio representational phenomenon.

Keywords:
Structural approach; social representations; methods; theory

Resumo

A abordagem estrutural das representações sociais é extremamente difundida no campo das representações sociais, notadamente após os livros de Abric em 1994 e de Celso Sá em 1996. Há frequentemente nas pesquisas empíricas uma falta de congruência entre os aspectos teóricos da abordagem estrutural e as características técnicas dos diferentes métodos usados. Neste artigo explicitamos as características da estrutura estudadas pelas diferentes técnicas, mais precisamente: o poder associativo dos elementos, a consensualidade do que é pensado e por fim a essência do objeto. Após essas reflexões é apresentada uma classificação das grandes técnicas da abordagem estrutural a partir dessas três características. Na conclusão, é levantada a questão da dinâmica representacional que fica deixada de lado pelas diferentes técnicas e deveria ser mais estudada para uma melhor apreensão do fenômeno representacional.

Palavras-chave:
abordagem estrutural; representações sociais; método; teoria

Resumen

El abordaje estructural de las representaciones sociales ha sido ampliamente difundido, especialmente a partir de la publicación de los libros de Abric en 1994 y de Celso Sá en 1996. Existe con frecuencia en las investigaciones empíricas una incongruencia entre los aspectos teóricos del abordaje estructural y las características técnicas de los diferentes métodos utilizados. En este artículo quedan explícitas las características de estructura estudiadas por las diferentes técnicas, especialmente: poder de asociación de los elementos, consenso de lo que se piensa y finalmente esencia del objeto. Después de estas reflexiones se presenta una clasificación de las grandes técnicas de abordaje estructural a partir de estas tres características. Como conclusión se plantea el tema de la dinámica representativa que es dejada de lado por las diferentes técnicas y debería ser más estudiada para una mejor aprehensión del fenómeno representativo.

Palabras-clave:
Abordaje estructural; representaciones sociales; método; teoría Introduction

Two decades ago Celso Sá (1996Sá, C. P. (1996). Núcleo central das representações sociais. Petrópolis: Vozes.) published “the central nucleus of social representations,” which covers the fundamental concepts of the theory of social representations in general and of the structural approach in particular. Since then, several studies have been based on this book (Tura, 1998Tura, L. F. R. (1998). Aids e estudantes: A estrutura das representações sociais. In D. Jodelet & M. Madeira (Orgs.), Aids e representações sociais: A busca de sentidos (pp. 121-154). Natal: Ed. UFRN.; Campos & Rouquette, 2003Campos, P., & Rouquette, M. (2003). Abordagem estrutural e componente afetivo das representações sociais. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 16 (3), 435-445.; Pecora & Sá, 2008Pecora, A. R. & Sá, C. P. (2008). Memórias e representações sociais da cidade de Cuiabá, ao longo de três gerações. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 21(2), 319-325.; Wachelke, 2013Wachelke, J. (2013). Beyond social representations: A review of the structural approach on social thinking. Revista Interamericana de Psicologia/Interamerican Journal of Psychology, 47(1).; Wolter, 2008Wolter, R. P. (2008). Pensée sociale et situations de crise: le rôle des nexus dans l’implication personnelle et les modes de raisonnement. Tese de Doutorado, Université Paris Descartes, Paris., 2016Wolter, R. P., & Sá. C. P. (2013). As relações entre representações e práticas: o caminho esquecido. Revista Internacional de Ciencias Sociales y Humanidades, 33 (2), 87-105. Recuperado de: https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Rafael_Wolter/publication/275889356_AS_RELAES_ENTRE_REPRESENTAES_E_PRTICAS_O_CAMINHO_ESQUECIDO/links/5549308a0cf25a87816ab2aa.pdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Raf...
; Wolter & Wachelke, 2013). Regarding the relation between method and theory, Bourdieu (1992Bourdieu, P. (1992). Um progrès de la réflexivité. In W. Doise, A. Clemence, F. Lorenzi-Cioldi (Orgs.). Représentations sociales et analyses des données (pp. 7-8). Grenoble: PUG., p. 7) reminds us that “reflection on the method of analyzing empirical data [...] is not separate from the reflection between objects and objectives of research.” He adds that the established boundary between methodology and theory is baleful. In the specific case of the structural approach to social representations there are a number of specific techniques that are inseparable from theoretical issues. In this article we seek to explain these relations based on a reflection upon the theoretical characteristics studied by the most usual techniques. All techniques have theoretical assumptions, often implicit, that need to be apprehended. More specifically, in this article we elaborate a characterization (Figure 1) of the different ways of studying the central nucleus that explains these bridges between theoretical and methodological aspects.

Figure 1
Techniques of the structural approach and its characteristics

1. Some preliminary elements on the concept of structure and the central nucleus theory

Frequently used in human and social sciences, the notion of social representation has become banal, implying a certain conceptual imprecision. As Flament and Rouquette (2003Flament, C. & Rouquette, M.-L. (2003). Anatomie des idées ordinaires. Comment étudier les représentations sociales. Paris: Armand Colin.) have often pointed out, the notion is vaguely a synonym for a more or less shared “conception” or for a simple general idea about anything.

The preceding paragraph refers to a simple and repeatedly expressed idea: each human group, at a given time, codifies and decodes its experience of the world in a specific way that carries the mark of social positions and relations (Clemence, Doise & Lorenzi-Cioldi, 1994Clémence, A., Doise, W. & Lorenzi-Cioldi, F. (1994). Prises de position et principes organisateurs des représentations sociales, In: C. Guimelli (Ed.), Structures et transformations des représentations sociales (pp. 119-152). Neuchâtel: Delachaux et Niestlé.). Moscovici (1976Moscovici, S. (1976). La psychanalyse, son image et son public. Paris: PUF.) developed the theory of social representations to fill a theoretical gap left by sociologists and psychologists. On the one hand, Émile Durkheim (1898Durkheim, E. (1898). Représentations individuelles et représentations collectives. Revue de métaphysique et de morale, VI, 273-302. Recuperado de: http://classiques.uqac.ca/classiques/Durkheim_emile/Socio_et_philo/ch_1_representations/representations.html
http://classiques.uqac.ca/classiques/Dur...
), in his sociology of knowledge, while formulating the concept of collective representations, covered the myths, language, and beliefs that sustain society. On the other hand, the concept of attitudes took a more individualizing course, notably in the works of Gordon Allport (1954Allport, G. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley). This view of attitudes can be illustrated in the definition by Eagly and Chaiken (1998Eagly, A. H. & Chaiken. S. (1998). Attitude, Structure and Function. In. D.T. Gilbert, Susan T. Fisk, & G. Lindsey (Orgs.). Handbook of Social Psychology (pp.269-322). New York: McGowan-Hill.) as a psychological tendency that shows itself in the evaluation, with some degree of favorability or unfavorability, of specific objects.

In his doctoral thesis, Moscovici (1976Moscovici, S. (1976). La psychanalyse, son image et son public. Paris: PUF.) studied how Communists and Catholics of the 1950s thought psychoanalysis, occupying the theoretical gap between Durkheimian sociology and the field of attitudes. On the one hand, he studied the inner dynamics of society with a group approach, such that Durkheim’s sociology of knowledge and the theory of collective representations with its societal approach did not fully apprehend, because they focused on society as a whole. On the other hand, the concept of attitudes proved insufficient to understand the differences between Catholics and Communists in relation to the object (psychoanalysis) because they neglected extra-individual reasons for rejecting the object. More precisely, both groups rejected psychoanalysis, but conceived it quite distinctly, that is, they had different social representations (SR).

Before characterizing SRs, it should be pointed out, according to Flament and Rouquette (2003Flament, C. & Rouquette, M.-L. (2003). Anatomie des idées ordinaires. Comment étudier les représentations sociales. Paris: Armand Colin. p. 13) that they

“are cognitive formations, socially constructed and consequently differentiated. Two general and distinct principles preside over the investigation of the notion: the need for a sociological reference (and not strictly psychological) to the genesis and functioning of SRs; (...); and the need for comparisons (synchronic and diachronic) between populations”.

The authors add that this view of the world shared by groups cannot be satisfactorily grasped in individualizing studies, because it is a social fact.

1.1 Structure, thinking structure

Studying the myths of indigenous populations, Lévi-Strauss came to contradictory conclusions, sometimes describing myths as stable, sometimes as unstable, sometimes as a construct that follows a rationale, and sometimes as something unruly. He stated that

“anything is likely to happen in myth; it seems that the sequence of events follows no logical or continuity rule. Any characteristic can be attributed to any subject; every conceivable relationship can be met. But despite this apparent arbitrariness, myths reproduce themselves with the same characteristics, and often with the same details, in different regions of the world” (1955, p. 432).

He concluded that the meaning of the myths is not in the isolated elements, but in the way the different isolated elements relate. Myths vary in content, but they share the same structure.

Jean Piaget described the general and necessary characteristics to distinguish one structure from any other system:

“A structure is a system of transformations which, in its system quality, implies the existence of laws (opposed to the properties of each element), and which preserves itself or develops by means of its own transformations, without taking them outside their boundaries or using external elements. In a word, a structure has three characteristics; wholeness, transformation and self-adjustability” ( Piaget, 1968 Piaget, J. (1968). Le structuralisme. Paris: PUF. , p.7).

Every structure is by definition, an artifice, a theoretical construction, and is not provided by any research subject. Wolter et al. (2015Wolter, R. P., Wachelke, J., Sá, C., Passeri, C. & Naiff, D. (2015). Temporalidade e representações sociais: estabilidade e dinâmica dos elementos ativados pelo Regime Militar brasileiro. Psychologica (Coimbra), 28, (1), 107-125. doi: 10.14195/1647-8606_58-1_6.
https://doi.org/10.14195/1647-8606_58-1_...
) illustrate this point, reminding us that no child in the experiments of Piaget and no indigenous in the work of Lévi-Strauss was able to provide the structures that governed, respectively, the development and family relationships. Piaget (1968) and Lévi-Strauss (1958Lévi-Strauss, C. (1955). The Structural Study of Myth. Journal of American Folklore, 78 (270), 428-444.) where the authors who, from a theoretical and empirical work, developed and defined the structure of the phenomena they studied.

Abric (1994Abric, J.-C. (1994). Représentations sociales: aspects théoriques. In J.-C. Abric (Org.). Pratiques sociales et representations (pp. 11-36). Paris: PUF., p.19) believes that the SR has a structure because it is constituted by a set of cognemes that are organized and have differentiated status. More specifically, for the author, “the elements that constitute a representation are hierarchized, weighted, and maintain, between themselves, relations that determine the meaning, and the place they occupy in the representational system.” The social representation can, therefore, be conceived as a set of ideas, which are related to one another, thought by a group about an object.

From the moment that, when studying a social representation, the researcher seeks to apprehend not only the content of representation, but also the relations between the contents and the dynamics of the whole, it can be said that the research follows a structural perspective. Within this logic, Flament and Rouquette (2003Flament, C. & Rouquette, M.-L. (2003). Anatomie des idées ordinaires. Comment étudier les représentations sociales. Paris: Armand Colin., p.13) gave an operational definition of representation: “a SR can be characterized as a set of cognitive elements linked by relations. These elements and these relations are attested in certain groups.” That is, not only the elements vary from group to group but also the relationships between the elements. Within a structural approach, both the relations between different cognemes and the elements (thought contents) themselves are highly important. This emphasis on the connections between the elements underlies a set of techniques that focus on the associative power that we will describe next.

1.2 The structure of social representation: central core and periphery

In more technical terms, the social representation is composed of cognemes that are related to each other and form a set that is transformed based on rules that preserve the totality. According to Codol (1969Codol, J.-P. (1969). Note terminologique sur l’emploi de quelques expressions concernant lês activités et processus cognitifs em psychologie social. Bulletin de Psychologie, 23, 63-71.), a cogneme is the most basic cognitive element, that is, the smallest unit of cognition at this level of analysis. A representation is then composed of ideas (or cognemes or elements) that are activated when a group thinks of an object. When thinking about the object many ideas are activated but not all are equivalent, as Abric (1994Abric, J.-C. (1994). Représentations sociales: aspects théoriques. In J.-C. Abric (Org.). Pratiques sociales et representations (pp. 11-36). Paris: PUF.) says, some are more valuable than others. He also states that sharing a representation with other people then means sharing the core values relative to the object in question.

It was Abric (1987Abric, J.-C. (1987). Coopération, competition et representations sociales. Cousset: Del Val., 1994) who introduced the central core theory (CC) in the study of social representations. According to Sá (1996Sá, C. P. (1998). A construção do objeto de pesquisa em representações sociais. Rio de Janeiro: EdUERJ.) the central core theory is a complementary approach to the “great theory” elaborated by Moscovici (1976Moscovici, S. (1976). La psychanalyse, son image et son public. Paris: PUF.). Also according to Sá (1996Sá. C. P., Castro, R. V., Möller, R. C., Perez, J. A., & Bezerra, F. C. C. (2008). A memória histórica do Regime Militar em três gerações: conteúdos factuais e juízos críticos. Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 10(1), 36-51. Recuperado de: http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1516-36872008000100004
http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?scr...
), the CC theory is not intended to replace the great theory and is more specific and heuristic.

For Abric the central core is “the fundamental element of the representation, since it determines both the signification and the organization of the representation (1994, p.21). According to him (1994, p. 22):

“the central core provides two essential functions: A generative function: it is through the central core that other elements in the representational field acquire meaning and specific value. An organizational function: it is the central core that determines the nature of the relationships that these elements maintain with each other. It is, in this sense, the unifying and stabilizing element of the representation”.

2. The characteristics of the central core and the periphery

The elements of the CC have a number of characteristics that distinguish it from the peripheral system. These are essential for the empirical study within the structural approach because the different techniques aim to study these attributes to differentiate the status of representational elements: central or peripheral (Sá, 1996Sá, C. P. (1996). Núcleo central das representações sociais. Petrópolis: Vozes.).

2.1 The associative power

The first great characteristic of central elements is their associative power. Elements that are associated with fewer ones can hardly be considered central. Since the meaning of the peripheral elements derives from the CC, it is natural for the central elements to connect to a large number of other cognemes. The central element is not an isolated element and it lies at the confluence of a wide range of ideas that arise about the object. It should be noted that the central elements often relate to other central elements. As these central cognemes connect they form a system, the central core, around which another system, the peripheral, circulates. These strong connections between the different central elements of a SR make the central system cohesive and hardly a central element opposes another central one. More specifically, a central element does not contradict another central element; oppositions, contradictions and incongruities are situated in the peripheral system. This peripheral system is not secondary in the representation and acts as a bumper (Flament, 1987Flament, C. (1987). Pratiques et representations sociales. In J.-L. Beauvois, R. V. Joule et J. M. Monteil (Orgs.). Perspectives cognitive et conduits sociales. 1. Théorie simplicites et conflits cognitifs (pp.143-150). Cousset: Del Val.), by allowing an adaptation of the group’s thinking to everyday reality, to the different contingencies external to the representation, and to the peculiarities internal to individuals. Without the peripheral system, the SR would be a rigid and abstract thinking system little adaptable to the vicissitudes of everyday life and the uniqueness of some situations. For these reasons the studies of the internal connections to the central system are as important as the study of the relations between central and the peripheral systems.

2.2 Consensuality

Another great feature of the CC elements is consensuality within a group. These central elements are commonly thought and evoked within a given population for a given object. For example, in the case of AIDS, the elements of illness and death are thought of in a recurrent way by young people. In other words, the central elements are not thought of by a dissident minority within the group but by a good part of the group. This consensuality allows the cohesion of group thinking, something necessary for intragroup communication and for sociability to remain in social relations. According to Abric (1994Abric, J.-C. (1994). Représentations sociales: aspects théoriques. In J.-C. Abric (Org.). Pratiques sociales et representations (pp. 11-36). Paris: PUF., p.28) the CC corresponds to a

“common social and collective basis that defines the homogeneity of a group (...). It plays a key role in the stability and cohesion of representation (...). Moreover, it is relatively independent from the immediate context in which the individual uses or verbalizes his representations; its origin is elsewhere: in the historical, social, ideological global context - which defines the norms and values of individuals and groups within a given social system.”

It should be noted that the notion of consensuality in no way means that the group will fully share the central ideas because unanimity is a rare and special case of consensuality. In most cases, central elements are shared by a large part of the group in most situations, which is not equivalent to being activated every time by all members of the group in all situations.

2.3 stability

Another characteristic of the central core is its stability (Wolter et al., 2015Wolter, R. P., Wachelke, J., Sá, C., Passeri, C. & Naiff, D. (2015). Temporalidade e representações sociais: estabilidade e dinâmica dos elementos ativados pelo Regime Militar brasileiro. Psychologica (Coimbra), 28, (1), 107-125. doi: 10.14195/1647-8606_58-1_6.
https://doi.org/10.14195/1647-8606_58-1_...
). The stability of the SR can be seen in two aspects, synchronic and diachronic. Synchronic stability corresponds to the fact that at a given moment, regardless of context, the central element will be activated by members of the group when thinking about the object in question. In other words, at some point, no matter the context, some elements remain extremely activated.

The peripheral elements of the SR, as opposed to central ones, are extremely contextual. When circumstances change, the activation of the peripheral elements may also be affected. The contingencies of the moment - such as a more salient issue in the media, purpose of the situation, personal interest of the moment - can cause peripheral cognemes to arise or disappear.

Diachronic stability, in turn, corresponds to the persistence of the central elements to remain as such over time (Wolter et al., 2015Wolter, R. P., Wachelke, J., Sá, C., Passeri, C. & Naiff, D. (2015). Temporalidade e representações sociais: estabilidade e dinâmica dos elementos ativados pelo Regime Militar brasileiro. Psychologica (Coimbra), 28, (1), 107-125. doi: 10.14195/1647-8606_58-1_6.
https://doi.org/10.14195/1647-8606_58-1_...
). According to Rouquette (1994Rouquette, M-L. (1994). Sur La connaissance des masses. Grenoble: Presses Universitaires de Grenoble.), every SR has an earlier state, a current state, and a later state; we could add that in the transition from one state to another, the peripheral elements are more susceptible to changes compared to the central ones. This lower susceptibility to change over time corresponds to the diachronic dimension of stability. Because of the diachronic stability it is possible to foresee the group’s thinking about an object, because we will know that some elements will be thought and they are the ones that give the meaning of the SR. For example, studies on the social representation of young people about the Military Regime (Sá et. al. 2008; Wolter et. al. 2015) indicated that censorship, dictatorship and repression were central in three data collections (2005, 2010 and 2011) with different techniques. It is possible to affirm that a possible collection next year will also indicate, after due analysis and if no external factor generates a strong contextual change, that these elements are central. Peripheral elements, on the other hand, are not necessarily stable and can quickly disappear from the group’s thought.

2.4 Conditionality

Superficially, it would be possible to state that the central and peripheral elements are quantitatively distinguished: number of ideas with which the element connects, number of people sharing the cogneme, number of situations to which the element resists, or even amount of time the element remains in the group. However, the differences are not limited in any way to quantifications and a great distinction between central and peripheral elements is, according to Sá (1996), of a qualitative nature. According to Moliner (1994Moliner, P. (1994). Les méthodes de répérage et d’identification du noyau des representations sociales. In C. Guimelli (Org.). Structures et transformations des representations sociales (pp. 199-232). Lausanne: Delachaux et Niestlé.), the central core entertains “a privileged bond with the object of representation. This bond is symbolic and results from the historical and social conditions that presided over the birth of the representation” (p. 202).

As described by Flament (1994Flament, C. (1994). Structure, dynamique et transformation des representations sociales. In J. C. Abric (Org.): Pratiques sociales et representations (pp. 37-57). Paris: PUF.), the central elements of the SR are absolute while the peripheral elements are conditional. The studies on this question have their origin in the findings of Abric et al. (1967Abric, J.-C., Faucheux, C., Moscovici, S., & Plon, M. (1967). Rôle de l’image du partenaire sur la coopération en situation de jeu. Psychologie Française, 12, 267-275.), and are perfectly illustrated in the studies of Moliner (1989Moliner, P. (1989). Validation expérimentale de l’hypothèse du noyau central des représentations sociales. Bulletin de Psychologie, 41 (387), 759-762.), Rateau (1995Rateau, P (1995). Le noyau central des representations socials comme système hiérarchisé. Une etudes sur la représentation du groupe. Les Cahiers Internationaux de Psychologie Sociale, 26(2), 29-52.) or more recently Lheureux, Rateau and Guimelli (2008Lheureux, F., Rateau, P., & Guimelli, C. (2008). Hiérarchie structurale, condionnalité et normativité des représentations sociales. Cahiers Internationaux de Psychologie Sociale, 77, 41-55. doi: 10.3917/cips.077.0041
https://doi.org/10.3917/cips.077.0041...
) on the ideal group. All these studies focused on the SR of the ideal group for young French and showed that four cognemes were often thought of: common opinion, friendship, equality (absence of hierarchy) and hanging out together. This meant that for these young French people the ideal group is composed of friendly people who share common opinions, where there is no hierarchy and who get together. That is, subjects cannot think of the ideal group without the cognemes friendship and equality, which are absolute, in turn, they can discard the cognemes having a common opinion and hanging out together, which are conditional.

3. Normative, practical and descriptive dimensions: evaluative, functional and denotative elements

Social representations have been described as linked to practices and judgments since the princeps work by Serge Moscovici (1976Moscovici, S. (1976). La psychanalyse, son image et son public. Paris: PUF.). Social groups often judge when thinking of an object. For example, the studies on the Gypsy SRs in several countries bring ideas that reflect the judgment of this social group. The works of Guimelli and Deschamps (2000Guimelli, C. & Deschamps, J.C. (2000). Effets de contexte sur la production d’associations verbales. Le cas des représentations sociales des Gitans. Cahiers Internationaux de Psychologie Sociale, 47 (4), 44-54.) show that when thinking of gypsies, young French people quickly have in mind the idea of theft. In Brazil a recent work (Bonomo, Faria, Souza & Brazil, 2013Bonomo, M., Faria, M. G. & Souza, L. d., Brasil, J. A. (2012). Temor e fascínio: dimensão afetiva e representações sociais de cigano entre população não cigana. Psicologia e Saber Social, 1 (2), 245-264.) showed that non-gypsies activate the deception cogneme when thinking of gypsies. Both elements, theft and deception, translate that when thinking about gypsies the normative dimension is activated.

3.1 The Normative Dimension

The normative dimension corresponds to a part of the SR that forms a set of affective and evaluating ideas, which are related to each other, thought by a group about a particular social object. This normative dimension composed of evaluative elements is, according to Guimelli (2003Guimelli, C. (2003). Le modèle des schèmes cognitifs de base: méthodes et applications. In J.-C. Abric (Org.). Méthodes d’étude des représentations sociales (pp. 119-143). Ramonville Saint-Agne: Érès., p.136), “linked to values, norms or strongly salient stereotypes in the group; it allows the group to make judgments about the object. This dimension is probably marked by ideological and historical factors.” It corresponds to the normative register of cognitions.

The normative dimension contains the representational elements that express affection in relation to the object, the fact of liking, disliking, detesting, rejecting, and accepting. Naturally, in situations of social pressure, this dimension, when opposed to the norms prevailing in the situation, is partially masked in what has been called the silent zone of social representations (Abric 2003Abric, J.-C. (2003). La recherche du noyau central et la zone muette des representations sociales. In J. C. Abric (Org.) Méthodes d’études des representations sociales (pp. 60-80). Ramonville Saint-Agne: Érès.).

Objects linked to ideologies, governed by many social norms, tend to be thought of with many evaluative elements. Abortion, for example, is an extremely ideological object and, depending on the ideology in question, it will be thought with positive or negative judgments, however, in both cases, it will be thought of through judgments, therefore it will have evaluative elements.

When the evaluative elements are central to the SR, it is acceptable to assume that social norms strongly influence the thinking of the group in question. Naturally, the activation of these elements is influenced by the social context (Abric & Guimelli, 1998Abric, J.C. & Guimelli, C. (1998). Représentations sociales et effets de contexte. Connexions. N° spécial: Logiques sociales de la connaissance, 72 (2), 23-38.) as well as the contingencies of the moment, and in situations of conflict over the object, the evaluative elements tend to over-activate. For example, in the full debate about the legalization of hunting in a given municipality, it is natural that the evaluation elements have a strong activation compared to situations without debate.

3.2 The functional dimension

Social groups often judge and evaluate when thinking about an object, but this is not the only way of thinking. Several findings have shown that thought can be practical, or in other words, functional. Many studies of the structural approach focused on the question of the relations between practices and representations (Abric, 1994Abric, J.-C. (1994). Représentations sociales: aspects théoriques. In J.-C. Abric (Org.). Pratiques sociales et representations (pp. 11-36). Paris: PUF.; Flament, 1994Flament, C. (1994). Structure, dynamique et transformation des representations sociales. In J. C. Abric (Org.): Pratiques sociales et representations (pp. 37-57). Paris: PUF.; Rouquette, 2000Rouquette, M. L. (2000). Representações e práticas sociais: alguns elementos teóricos. In: A.S.P. Moreira e D.C. Oliveira (Orgs.). Estudos interdisciplinares de representação social (pp. 39-46). Goiânia: AB.; Guimelli, 1994Guimelli, C. (1994). Transformations et représentations sociales, pratiques nouvelles et schèmes cognitifs de base. In: C. Guimelli (Org.). Structures et transformations des représentations sociales (pp. 171- 198). Lausanne: Delachaux et Niestlé.; Wolter & Sá, 2013Wolter, R. P. & Wachelke, J. (2013). Índices complementares para o estudo de uma representação social a partir de evocações livres: raridade, diversidade e comunidade. Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 15(2), 119-129.). To understand this relation it is necessary to know which representational elements are the interface with the action and have a prescriptive character. Flament explains that prescriber has the meaning of

“all the modalities in which an action is susceptible of being affected: ‘one must do...; ‘one can do...’; ‘it is desirable to...’; ‘one cannot...’; ‘one should not...’ etc. The prescriptive aspect of a cognition is the fundamental bond between cognition and the behaviors that are supposed to correspond to it” ( Flament, 1994 Flament, C. (1994). Structure, dynamique et transformation des representations sociales. In J. C. Abric (Org.): Pratiques sociales et representations (pp. 37-57). Paris: PUF. , p.38).

Therefore, the functional elements of social representations have this prescriptive feature. For this reason, in contexts where new practices emerge, these elements are affected and may, in some cases, disappear from the representation and/or give way to another functional element that best adapts to the new practice (Flament, 1994Flament, C. (1994). Structure, dynamique et transformation des representations sociales. In J. C. Abric (Org.): Pratiques sociales et representations (pp. 37-57). Paris: PUF.). The set of functional elements, in case they are interconnected, forms the practical dimensions of the SR.

3.3 The descriptive dimension

Finally, the descriptive dimension, as its name, brings elements that denote the object. They are often cognemes that present some characteristic of the object, for example, when representing soccer many groups may think in grass, ball or goal post. These elements describe and characterize the object for the group, not necessarily serve to judge or to guide practices, but to say what it is. The three dimensions are not mutually exclusive and can be studied from the techniques derived from the basic cognitive schemes model (Guimelli & Rouquette, 1992Guimelli, C. & Rouquette, M.-L. (1992). Contributions du modèle associatif des schemes cognitifs de base à l’analyse structurale des representations sociales. Bulletin de psychologie, XLV, 405, 196-202.).

4. The techniques of study of the central core

In the last decades five techniques have been extremely used in the structural approach studies: prototypical analysis (Vergès, 1992Vergès, P. (1992). L’évocation de l’argent: une méthode pour la définition du noyau central d’une représentation. Bulletin de psychologie, XLV (405), 203-209.); mise-en-cause (Moliner, 1989Moliner, P. (1989). Validation expérimentale de l’hypothèse du noyau central des représentations sociales. Bulletin de Psychologie, 41 (387), 759-762.) and a more recent and less spread adaption of this technique, the context independence test (Lo Monaco et al., 2008Lo Monaco, G., Lheureux, F. & Halimi-Falkowicz, S. (2008). Test d’Indépendance au Contexte (TIC) et structure des représentations sociales. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 67(2), 119-123.); similarity analysis (Bouriche, 2003); induction by ambiguous scenario (Moliner, 1993); and basic cognitive schemes (Guimelli & Rouquette, 1992Guimelli, C. & Rouquette, M.-L. (1992). Contributions du modèle associatif des schemes cognitifs de base à l’analyse structurale des representations sociales. Bulletin de psychologie, XLV, 405, 196-202.).

4.1 The prototypical analysis

The prototypical analysis is a survey of what the different research participants (usually more than a hundred) evoke in relation to the representational object, to then classify the elements into two coordinates (Wachelke & Wolter, 2001Wachelke, J., & Wolter, R. (2011). Critérios de construção e relato da análise prototípica para representações sociais. Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa, 27(4),521-526. Doi: 1590/S0102-37722011000400017
https://doi.org/1590/S0102-3772201100040...
; Wolter & Wachelke, 2013). The first coordinate is taken from the average order of recall, where the terms readily evoked are at the first extreme, and at the other, the terms later evoked. The second coordinate opposes the extremely quoted and frequent terms to the least evoked terms. As a result, the evoked elements are located, based on these two coordinates, in one of the four possible places (very often and readily evoked; very often and late evoked; rarely and readily evoked; and rarely and late evoked).

4.2 The questioning technique

The questioning technique, or mise-en-cause, analyses centrality under another aspect: that of the negotiability of the cogneme for the recognition of the representational object. In this case the researcher must present the object of study (e.g. Family) without the element tested (e.g. love). If most participants agree that without love it is not a family, then the love element will be central. Now, if most participants accept that there may be a family without love, then the love element will be seen as peripheral because the group can think of the object without this element.

4.3 The similarity analysis

The similarity analysis (Flament, 1981Flament, C. (1981). L’analyse de similitude: une technique pour les recherché sur les representations sociales. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 4, 357-396.), in turn, studies the distances between the different representational elements (cognemes). These distances are typically presented in tree shapes where the edges translate the distances and the poles are the representational elements. The elements that are close to many other elements tend to be considered central. In turn, the elements close to few other elements tend to be considered peripheral.

4.4 The basic cognitive schemes

The basic cognitive schemes (Guimelli e Rouquette, 1992Guimelli, C. & Rouquette, M.-L. (1992). Contributions du modèle associatif des schemes cognitifs de base à l’analyse structurale des representations sociales. Bulletin de psychologie, XLV, 405, 196-202.; Wolter et al., 2016Wolter, R. P., Wachelke, J., & Naiff, D. (2016). A abordagem estrutural das representações sociais e o modelo dos esquemas cognitivos de base: perspectivas teóricas e utilização empírica. Temas em Psicologia, 24(3), 1139-1152. doi: 10.9788/TP2016.3-18
https://doi.org/10.9788/TP2016.3-18...
) study the types of connectors activated by the elements that are candidates to the central core. The data collection consists of presenting the candidates to the subjects, with 28 connectors connecting them to the other representational elements. We can understand these relationships as cognitive paths that lead from one element to another. If, for example, a group, when thinking about soccer, thinks of the ‘Manchester United’ cogneme, which in turn brings the ‘club’ cogneme, several paths may have been traversed to relate these two cognemes:

  • Manchester United is a type of a club, which translates a relation of class included;

  • Manchester United can be defined as a club, which translates as a defining relation;

  • Manchester United uses its club status (for example, to raise funds for the club), which translates into a relation of use;

  • Manchester United is always characterized by being a club, which translates a relationship of characterization.

These 28 connectors are composed of three families, or rather, meta-schemes of connectors: 7 evaluative connectors (which translate the normative dimension), 12 functional/prescriptive connectors (which translate the practical dimension) and 9 descriptive connectors (which are related to the descriptive dimension of the object). Participants must say which of the 28 connectors can connect the candidate to the other representational elements. If the element is central, it will mass activate the functional and evaluative connectors to relate to the other representational elements. This activation is studied from an algorithm that studies both the connectivity of the element and the balance in the activation of practical and evaluative elements.

4.5 Induction by ambiguous scenario

In the case of induction by ambiguous scenario (Moliner, 1993Moliner, P. (1993). Cinq questions à propos des représentations sociales. Cahiers Internationaux de Psychologie Sociale, 20, 5-52.) the researcher presents the object without the main cognemes and asks the subjects to bring the cognemes that will allow a better description of the object. The idea is that the central elements will be spontaneously presented by the subjects to reduce the ambiguity of the description of the object.

5. Discussion: the types of relationship studied by structural approach techniques and their general and specific characteristics

It is important to emphasize that all the techniques presented above have as a characteristic the possibility of studying the SR based on the central core theory and distinguishing the central elements from the peripherals. Naturally, some techniques are more restrictive than others: prototypical analysis, for example, is less restrictive in distinguishing elements compared to basic cognitive schemes. However, we can separate these techniques into two groups: those that study the relations between the object and the elements; and those that study the relations between elements of the representation.

5.1 The relation between the object and the representational element

An object of representation activates a set of elements or cognemes within certain social groups. If the elements are activated within the population it is because there is a relation between them that we will call it object-element relation. It can be illustrated by a simple task of free evocation, where an object, for example AIDS, can activate several elements, such as death, illness, suffering and sadness (Costa, Oliveira and Fomozo, 2012).

This relationship has several characteristics that are studied by the techniques. The first characteristic studied is the degree of cogneme sharing when the group thinks about it as it comes across the object. For example, in the case of the object AIDS, several studies show that illness and death are extremely shared by young people (Camargo, Barbará & Bertoldo, 2007Camargo, B. V., Barbará, A., & Bertoldo, R. B. (2007). Concepção pragmática e científica dos adolescentes sobre a aids. Psicologia em Estudo, 12 (2), 277-284. Recuperado de: http://www.scielo.br/pdf/pe/v12n2/v12n2a08
http://www.scielo.br/pdf/pe/v12n2/v12n2a...
; Costa, Oliveira & Fomozo, 2012). That is, in thinking about the object, many members of the group have in mind the same cogneme. The degree of sharing is directly or indirectly taken into account in all structural approach techniques.

The second characteristic studied is related to the readiness to access to the cogneme when the object is thought. When thinking about an object, some elements emerge more quickly than others. In this same example of AIDS, some studies (e.g. Camargo, Barbará & Bertoldo, 2007Camargo, B. V., Barbará, A., & Bertoldo, R. B. (2007). Concepção pragmática e científica dos adolescentes sobre a aids. Psicologia em Estudo, 12 (2), 277-284. Recuperado de: http://www.scielo.br/pdf/pe/v12n2/v12n2a08
http://www.scielo.br/pdf/pe/v12n2/v12n2a...
; Costa, Oliveira & Fomozo, 2012) show that death and disease are among the first terms that come in mind, soon they are accessible. Accessibility is studied with the prototypical analysis developed by Vergès (1992Vergès, P. (1992). L’évocation de l’argent: une méthode pour la définition du noyau central d’une représentation. Bulletin de psychologie, XLV (405), 203-209.).

The third feature of the object-element relationship studied is conditionality, that is, the fact that, without the presence of the element the members of the group are not able to recognize the object. The studies on the ideal group (Moliner, 1993Moliner, P. (1993). Cinq questions à propos des représentations sociales. Cahiers Internationaux de Psychologie Sociale, 20, 5-52.) illustrate well this point, when presenting a group without the cogneme friendship, most participants were not able to recognize the group presented as being an ideal group (object). For example, several studies have shown that the SR of the Military Regime for university students is structured around the dictatorship element (cf. Wolter et al. 2015Wolter, R. P., Wachelke, J., Sá, C., Passeri, C. & Naiff, D. (2015). Temporalidade e representações sociais: estabilidade e dinâmica dos elementos ativados pelo Regime Militar brasileiro. Psychologica (Coimbra), 28, (1), 107-125. doi: 10.14195/1647-8606_58-1_6.
https://doi.org/10.14195/1647-8606_58-1_...
for the presentation of the several studies). Hardly would this same group accept the idea that the Military Regime does not have this characteristic of having been dictatorial. That is, the idea of a dictatorship, for these young people, is absolute in thinking the Military Regime, just as friendship is unconditional and necessary for young people to think of the ideal group. In opposition to the absolute elements, other elements are conditional, and the group is able to think the object without them. Moliner’s study demonstrated that young people were able to recognize a group where members hold different opinions. In other words, the fact of having a common opinion is not absolute but rather conditional to think the object ideal group. Conditionality is present in the mise-en-cause technique (Moliner, 1989).

The fourth characteristic of the techniques that focus on the object-element relationship is the degree of exclusiveness of the cogneme in relation to nearby objects. Abric (2003Abric, J.-C. (2003). La recherche du noyau central et la zone muette des representations sociales. In J. C. Abric (Org.) Méthodes d’études des representations sociales (pp. 60-80). Ramonville Saint-Agne: Érès., p.71) summarized the quest for exclusiveness by stating that “it rests on the idea that representation is an active process of construction of reality. We, therefore, seek the elements that the subject needs to recognize the object of representation and differentiate it from other objects nearby.” This feature is present in the induction by ambiguous scenario (Moliner, 1993Moliner, P. (1993). Cinq questions à propos des représentations sociales. Cahiers Internationaux de Psychologie Sociale, 20, 5-52.), where the researcher seeks to know which elements are essential and exclusive to the object, that is, they are not activated by nearby objects. The example of the profit element illustrates well this idea of exclusiveness, since many elements are common to both ‘business’ and ‘association’, and very activated by both. However, the profit element is exclusive of the ‘business’ object and according to Moliner (1994), it is central. In turn, ‘workplace’ is common to both objects, but because it does not differentiate these two close objects it would not be central.

These four characteristics, degree of sharing, readiness of access, conditionality and exclusiveness serve to distinguish the central elements from peripherals by studying the relation between the object and what it activates. In the first case the central cogneme is activated within the population when thinking about an object among many people. In the second case, the cogneme is very available to many people in the group studied. Marbe’s law lists the first two characteristics in stating that the terms frequently evoked by a population tend to be more readily evoked than the infrequent terms (Vergès, 1992Vergès, P. (1992). L’évocation de l’argent: une méthode pour la définition du noyau central d’une représentation. Bulletin de psychologie, XLV (405), 203-209., Osgood, 1953Osgood, C. E. (1953). Method and theory in experimental psychology. New-York: Oxford University Press.). That is, degree of sharing and readiness of access are characteristics that are correlated and empirically are often studied together. In fact, both study two aspects of the accessibility of the element in the population when thinking the object. Conditionality and exclusiveness, in turn, translate the existence of more qualitative characteristics of the central core and both translate characteristics of essence of the object.

5.2 The relation between representational elements

Another way of studying the social representation within the structural approach is to focus on the relationship between the different elements activated by the object. This form is perhaps the one that best corresponds to the different definitions of structure presented by Lévi-Strauss and Piaget, cited at the beginning of this article. The fundamental idea for the study of the relation between elements is that the central element has a privileged relationship with the other representational elements. Abric (1994Abric, J.-C. (1994). Représentations sociales: aspects théoriques. In J.-C. Abric (Org.). Pratiques sociales et representations (pp. 11-36). Paris: PUF.) defined the central core as the part of the social representation that gives meaning to the whole. The great majority of representational elements take their meaning from the few elements of the central core. Claude Flament, in 1981Flament, C. (1981). L’analyse de similitude: une technique pour les recherché sur les representations sociales. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 4, 357-396., emphasized the importance of studying the relations between elements, and adapted the analysis of similarity to the study of social representations. This analysis focuses on the distance from the representational elements. When an element is close to many other elements (i.e., in the population, both terms often “go together”) it can be considered a good candidate for the CC; on the other hand, elements that are only close to one other can be considered peripheral. The basic cognitive schemes developed by Guimelli and Rouquette (1992Guimelli, C. & Rouquette, M.-L. (1992). Contributions du modèle associatif des schemes cognitifs de base à l’analyse structurale des representations sociales. Bulletin de psychologie, XLV, 405, 196-202.) study the amount of connectors activated that connect one representational element to another. The idea behind this method is that the elements of CC activate many connectors, primarily evaluative and functional, to relate to other elements. If the element has few evaluative and functional connections, it is hardly central. In turn, the peripheral elements have comparatively fewer connections. Both the techniques of similarity and basic cognitive schemes focus on the fifth and final characteristic, associative power.

In short, the bridge between the different techniques and their theoretical aspects studied is summarized in Figure 1. There, it is possible to see that the techniques of the structural approach are divided according to: the type of relation studied; the general characteristic of centrality in question; and the specific characteristic in question.

Conclusion

The structural approach to social thought in recent decades presented a number of theoretical innovations (Wachelke, 2013Wachelke, J. (2013). Beyond social representations: A review of the structural approach on social thinking. Revista Interamericana de Psicologia/Interamerican Journal of Psychology, 47(1).) and five major techniques (presented above). The advantages and disadvantages of each technique are varied and there is a wide literature on the subject (e.g., Abric, 2003Abric, J.-C. (2003). La recherche du noyau central et la zone muette des representations sociales. In J. C. Abric (Org.) Méthodes d’études des representations sociales (pp. 60-80). Ramonville Saint-Agne: Érès.); however, it should be emphasized that all techniques are, to a lesser or greater extent, approximations of the phenomenon. As we explained before, each technique studies particular characteristics of the CC and periphery and the approaches have specificities, some focus on the associative power, others on accessibility or even on the essence. None of them studies all of the characteristics and, for this reason, methodological triangulation is necessary. We may also inquire into the lack of interest in representational stability and dynamics, which are, however, key theoretical points in the study of representational structure. We begin the text with Bourdieu’s (1992Bourdieu, P. (1992). Um progrès de la réflexivité. In W. Doise, A. Clemence, F. Lorenzi-Cioldi (Orgs.). Représentations sociales et analyses des données (pp. 7-8). Grenoble: PUG.) statement on the relation between method and theory, and, following this logic, we currently have no theoretical argument to assert that one characteristic is more important than another to study the structure, so we do not have theoretical elements to indicate one technique over another. But it is important to keep in mind what each technique studies for a better understanding of the research results and for a better interpretation of the social fact at stake.

References

  • Abric, J.-C. (1987). Coopération, competition et representations sociales. Cousset: Del Val.
  • Abric, J.-C. (1994). Représentations sociales: aspects théoriques. In J.-C. Abric (Org.). Pratiques sociales et representations (pp. 11-36). Paris: PUF.
  • Abric, J.-C. (2003). La recherche du noyau central et la zone muette des representations sociales. In J. C. Abric (Org.) Méthodes d’études des representations sociales (pp. 60-80). Ramonville Saint-Agne: Érès.
  • Abric, J.-C., Faucheux, C., Moscovici, S., & Plon, M. (1967). Rôle de l’image du partenaire sur la coopération en situation de jeu. Psychologie Française, 12, 267-275.
  • Abric, J.C. & Guimelli, C. (1998). Représentations sociales et effets de contexte. Connexions. N° spécial: Logiques sociales de la connaissance, 72 (2), 23-38.
  • Allport, G. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley
  • Bonomo, M., Faria, M. G. & Souza, L. d., Brasil, J. A. (2012). Temor e fascínio: dimensão afetiva e representações sociais de cigano entre população não cigana. Psicologia e Saber Social, 1 (2), 245-264.
  • Bourdieu, P. (1992). Um progrès de la réflexivité. In W. Doise, A. Clemence, F. Lorenzi-Cioldi (Orgs.). Représentations sociales et analyses des données (pp. 7-8). Grenoble: PUG.
  • Bourriche, B. (2003). L’analyse de similitude. In J.-C. Abric (Org.). Méthodes d’étude des représentations sociales (pp. 221-252). Ramonville Saint-Agne: Érès.
  • Camargo, B. V., Barbará, A., & Bertoldo, R. B. (2007). Concepção pragmática e científica dos adolescentes sobre a aids. Psicologia em Estudo, 12 (2), 277-284. Recuperado de: http://www.scielo.br/pdf/pe/v12n2/v12n2a08
    » http://www.scielo.br/pdf/pe/v12n2/v12n2a08
  • Campos, P., & Rouquette, M. (2003). Abordagem estrutural e componente afetivo das representações sociais. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 16 (3), 435-445.
  • Clémence, A., Doise, W. & Lorenzi-Cioldi, F. (1994). Prises de position et principes organisateurs des représentations sociales, In: C. Guimelli (Ed.), Structures et transformations des représentations sociales (pp. 119-152). Neuchâtel: Delachaux et Niestlé.
  • Codol, J.-P. (1969). Note terminologique sur l’emploi de quelques expressions concernant lês activités et processus cognitifs em psychologie social. Bulletin de Psychologie, 23, 63-71.
  • Durkheim, E. (1898). Représentations individuelles et représentations collectives. Revue de métaphysique et de morale, VI, 273-302. Recuperado de: http://classiques.uqac.ca/classiques/Durkheim_emile/Socio_et_philo/ch_1_representations/representations.html
    » http://classiques.uqac.ca/classiques/Durkheim_emile/Socio_et_philo/ch_1_representations/representations.html
  • Eagly, A. H. & Chaiken. S. (1998). Attitude, Structure and Function. In. D.T. Gilbert, Susan T. Fisk, & G. Lindsey (Orgs.). Handbook of Social Psychology (pp.269-322). New York: McGowan-Hill.
  • Flament, C. (1981). L’analyse de similitude: une technique pour les recherché sur les representations sociales. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 4, 357-396.
  • Flament, C. (1987). Pratiques et representations sociales. In J.-L. Beauvois, R. V. Joule et J. M. Monteil (Orgs.). Perspectives cognitive et conduits sociales. 1. Théorie simplicites et conflits cognitifs (pp.143-150). Cousset: Del Val.
  • Flament, C. (1994). Structure, dynamique et transformation des representations sociales. In J. C. Abric (Org.): Pratiques sociales et representations (pp. 37-57). Paris: PUF.
  • Flament, C. & Rouquette, M.-L. (2003). Anatomie des idées ordinaires. Comment étudier les représentations sociales. Paris: Armand Colin.
  • Guimelli, C. (1994). Transformations et représentations sociales, pratiques nouvelles et schèmes cognitifs de base. In: C. Guimelli (Org.). Structures et transformations des représentations sociales (pp. 171- 198). Lausanne: Delachaux et Niestlé.
  • Guimelli, C. (2003). Le modèle des schèmes cognitifs de base: méthodes et applications. In J.-C. Abric (Org.). Méthodes d’étude des représentations sociales (pp. 119-143). Ramonville Saint-Agne: Érès.
  • Guimelli, C. & Rouquette, M.-L. (1992). Contributions du modèle associatif des schemes cognitifs de base à l’analyse structurale des representations sociales. Bulletin de psychologie, XLV, 405, 196-202.
  • Guimelli, C. & Deschamps, J.C. (2000). Effets de contexte sur la production d’associations verbales. Le cas des représentations sociales des Gitans. Cahiers Internationaux de Psychologie Sociale, 47 (4), 44-54.
  • Jodelet, D. (1989). Représentations sociales: un domaine en expansion. In D. Jodelet (Org.) (pp, 31-61). Les representations sociales. Paris: P.U.F.
  • Lévi-Strauss, C. (1955). The Structural Study of Myth. Journal of American Folklore, 78 (270), 428-444.
  • Lheureux, F., Rateau, P., & Guimelli, C. (2008). Hiérarchie structurale, condionnalité et normativité des représentations sociales. Cahiers Internationaux de Psychologie Sociale, 77, 41-55. doi: 10.3917/cips.077.0041
    » https://doi.org/10.3917/cips.077.0041
  • Lo Monaco, G., Lheureux, F. & Halimi-Falkowicz, S. (2008). Test d’Indépendance au Contexte (TIC) et structure des représentations sociales. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 67(2), 119-123.
  • Moliner, P. (1989). Validation expérimentale de l’hypothèse du noyau central des représentations sociales. Bulletin de Psychologie, 41 (387), 759-762.
  • Moliner, P. (1993). Cinq questions à propos des représentations sociales. Cahiers Internationaux de Psychologie Sociale, 20, 5-52.
  • Moliner, P. (1994). Les méthodes de répérage et d’identification du noyau des representations sociales. In C. Guimelli (Org.). Structures et transformations des representations sociales (pp. 199-232). Lausanne: Delachaux et Niestlé.
  • Moscovici, S. (1976). La psychanalyse, son image et son public. Paris: PUF.
  • Piaget, J. (1968). Le structuralisme. Paris: PUF.
  • Osgood, C. E. (1953). Method and theory in experimental psychology. New-York: Oxford University Press.
  • Pecora, A. R. & Sá, C. P. (2008). Memórias e representações sociais da cidade de Cuiabá, ao longo de três gerações. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 21(2), 319-325.
  • Rateau, P (1995). Le noyau central des representations socials comme système hiérarchisé. Une etudes sur la représentation du groupe. Les Cahiers Internationaux de Psychologie Sociale, 26(2), 29-52.
  • Rouquette, M-L. (1994). Sur La connaissance des masses. Grenoble: Presses Universitaires de Grenoble.
  • Rouquette, M. L. (2000). Representações e práticas sociais: alguns elementos teóricos. In: A.S.P. Moreira e D.C. Oliveira (Orgs.). Estudos interdisciplinares de representação social (pp. 39-46). Goiânia: AB.
  • Sá, C. P. (1996). Núcleo central das representações sociais. Petrópolis: Vozes.
  • Sá, C. P. (1998). A construção do objeto de pesquisa em representações sociais. Rio de Janeiro: EdUERJ.
  • Sá. C. P., Castro, R. V., Möller, R. C., Perez, J. A., & Bezerra, F. C. C. (2008). A memória histórica do Regime Militar em três gerações: conteúdos factuais e juízos críticos. Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 10(1), 36-51. Recuperado de: http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1516-36872008000100004
    » http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1516-36872008000100004
  • Tura, L. F. R. (1998). Aids e estudantes: A estrutura das representações sociais. In D. Jodelet & M. Madeira (Orgs.), Aids e representações sociais: A busca de sentidos (pp. 121-154). Natal: Ed. UFRN.
  • Vergès, P. (1992). L’évocation de l’argent: une méthode pour la définition du noyau central d’une représentation. Bulletin de psychologie, XLV (405), 203-209.
  • Wachelke, J. (2013). Beyond social representations: A review of the structural approach on social thinking. Revista Interamericana de Psicologia/Interamerican Journal of Psychology, 47(1).
  • Wachelke, J., & Wolter, R. (2011). Critérios de construção e relato da análise prototípica para representações sociais. Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa, 27(4),521-526. Doi: 1590/S0102-37722011000400017
    » https://doi.org/1590/S0102-37722011000400017
  • Wolter, R. P. (2008). Pensée sociale et situations de crise: le rôle des nexus dans l’implication personnelle et les modes de raisonnement. Tese de Doutorado, Université Paris Descartes, Paris.
  • Wolter, R. P., & Sá. C. P. (2013). As relações entre representações e práticas: o caminho esquecido. Revista Internacional de Ciencias Sociales y Humanidades, 33 (2), 87-105. Recuperado de: https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Rafael_Wolter/publication/275889356_AS_RELAES_ENTRE_REPRESENTAES_E_PRTICAS_O_CAMINHO_ESQUECIDO/links/5549308a0cf25a87816ab2aa.pdf
    » https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Rafael_Wolter/publication/275889356_AS_RELAES_ENTRE_REPRESENTAES_E_PRTICAS_O_CAMINHO_ESQUECIDO/links/5549308a0cf25a87816ab2aa.pdf
  • Wolter, R. P. & Wachelke, J. (2013). Índices complementares para o estudo de uma representação social a partir de evocações livres: raridade, diversidade e comunidade. Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 15(2), 119-129.
  • Wolter, R. P., Wachelke, J., Sá, C., Passeri, C. & Naiff, D. (2015). Temporalidade e representações sociais: estabilidade e dinâmica dos elementos ativados pelo Regime Militar brasileiro. Psychologica (Coimbra), 28, (1), 107-125. doi: 10.14195/1647-8606_58-1_6.
    » https://doi.org/10.14195/1647-8606_58-1_6
  • Wolter, R. P., Wachelke, J., & Naiff, D. (2016). A abordagem estrutural das representações sociais e o modelo dos esquemas cognitivos de base: perspectivas teóricas e utilização empírica. Temas em Psicologia, 24(3), 1139-1152. doi: 10.9788/TP2016.3-18
    » https://doi.org/10.9788/TP2016.3-18

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Oct-Dec 2018
  • Date of issue
    Dec 2018

History

  • Received
    04 July 2016
  • Reviewed
    06 June 2017
  • Accepted
    03 May 2018
Universidade de São Francisco, Programa de Pós-Graduação Stricto Sensu em Psicologia R. Waldemar César da Silveira, 105, Vl. Cura D'Ars (SWIFT), Campinas - São Paulo, CEP 13045-510, Telefone: (19)3779-3771 - Campinas - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistapsico@usf.edu.br