Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Sleep and executive functions in older adults: A systematic review

Sono e funções executivas em idosos: revisão sistemática

ABSTRACT

Introduction:

A recent increase in studies suggests a role of age-related sleep changes in executive functions (EF). However, this relationship remains unclear and mixed results have emerged.

Objective:

To investigate how age-related sleep changes may play an important role in the extent to which healthy older adults exhibit decline in EF.

Methods:

A systematic strategy was employed to identify the available literature on age-related sleep changes and EF. Results: Of the 465 studies identified, 26 were included.

Results

suggest that multiple sleep parameters differ in the way they benefit or impair EF. Parameters such as greater wake after sleep onset and lower sleep efficiency, in addition to circadian fragmentation of sleep, showed more consistent results and are potentially correlated with worsening in EF measures. However, other results seem inconclusive.

Conclusion:

These findings were discussed based on the prefrontal circuitry vulnerability model, in which sleep has been identified as a beneficial factor for prefrontal cortex functioning and hence for EF, which relies mostly on this brain area and its related networks.

Key words:
executive functions; sleep; older adults; prefrontal cortex

RESUMO

Introdução:

Um aumento recente de estudos sugere um papel das mudanças do sono relacionadas ao envelhecimento nas funções executivas (FE). Entretanto, essa relação ainda não é clara e resultados mistos têm emergido.

Objetivo:

Investigar como as mudanças do sono relacionadas à idade podem desempenhar um papel importante na medida em que idosos saudáveis apresentam um declínio nas FE.

Métodos:

Uma estratégia de revisão sistemática foi empregada para identificar a literatura disponível sobre mudanças no sono relacionadas ao envelhecimento e FE.

Resultados:

Dos 465 estudos identificados, 26 foram incluídos. Os resultados sugerem que múltiplos parâmetros do sono diferem na forma como beneficiam ou comprometem as FE. Parâmetros como maiores despertares após o início do sono e baixa eficiência do sono, além da fragmentação circadiana do sono, mostraram resultados mais consistentes e potencialmente se correlacionaram com a piora nas medidas de FE. Contudo, outros resultados parecem inconclusivos.

Conclusão:

Esses achados foram discutidos baseadamente no modelo de vulnerabilidade pré-frontal, no qual o sono tem sido apontado como um fator benéfico para o funcionamento do córtex pré-frontal e, consequentemente, para as FE que têm como substrato principal essa área cerebral e regiões relacionadas.

Palavras-chave:
funções executivas; sono; idoso; córtex pré-frontal

INTRODUCTION

The process of normal aging affects the sleep-wake regulatory system in ultradian, circadian and homeostatic levels. Changes in ultradian rhythm of sleep and sleep parameters include decreases in slow wave sleep (SWS, stage N3), which appear to begin as early as midlife, as well as decrease in total sleep time (TST) and increased wake after sleep onset (WASO), the latter resulting in poor sleep efficiency.11. Ancoli-Israel S, Ayalon L, Salzman C. Sleep in the elderly: normal variations and common sleep disorders. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2008; 16:279-286.

2. Buysse DJ, Monk TH, Carrier J, Begley A. Circadian patterns of sleep, sleepiness, and performance in older and younger adults. Sleep. 2005;28:1365-1376.
-33. O'Donnell D, Silva EJ, Munch M, Ronda JM, Wang W, Duffy JF. Comparison of subjective and objective assessments of sleep in healthy older subjects without sleep complaints. J Sleep Res. 2009;18:254-263. The percentage of SWS linearly decreases at a rate of approximately 2 per cent per decade up to 60 years and then stabilizes through the mid-90s.44. Cajochen C, Münch M, Knoblauch V, Blatter K, Wirz-Justice A. Age-related changes in the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep. Chronobiol Int. 2006;23:461-474.,55. Ohayon MM, Carskadon MA, Guilleminault C, Vitiello MV. Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan. Sleep. 2004;27:1255-1273.

Older adults spend an increasing percentage of sleep in stages N1 and N2, resulting in less restorative sleep.66. Roepke SK, Ancoli-Israel S. Sleep disorders in the elderly. Indian J Med Res. 2010;131:302-310. Crowley, Trinder, Kim, Carrington & Colrain found that sleep spindle number, density and duration, as well as K-complex number and density, are lower in younger than in older adults.77. Crowley K, Trinder J, Kim Y, Carrington M, Colrain IM. The effects of normal aging on sleep spindle and K-complex production. Clin Neurophysiol. 2002;113:1615-1622. A slight decrease in percentage of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep may occur, decaying by less than 1% per decade or may remain relatively unchanged.88. Floyd JA, Janisse JJ, Jenuwine ES, Ager JW. Changes in REM-sleep percentage over the adult lifespan. Sleep 2006;30:829-836.

Regarding circadian organization of sleep in elderly, there is commonly a phase advance in the circadian sleep cycle: a propensity toward an earlier sleep onset, together with an earlier morning wake pattern.99. Duffy JF, Zitting KM, Chinoy ED. Aging and Circadian Rhythms. Sleep Med Clin. 2015;10:423-434. In other words, older people become sleepy in the evening and tend to sleep quite early, hence waking up earlier in the morning.1010. Wolkove N, Elkholy O, Baltzan, M, Palayew M. Sleep and aging: 1. Sleep disorders commonly found in older people. Can Med Assoc J. 2007;176:1299-1304. This advance in sleep phase is complicated by social pressure to stay up later in the evening, despite their altered internal rhythm.

There are also changes in homeostatic organization of sleep: it has been hypothesized that in the elderly, homeostatic sleep pressure undergoes a decrease and may be a cause of reduced TST, SWS and sleep efficiency.1111. Duffy JF, Willson HJ, Wang W, Czeisler CA. Healthy older adults better tolerate sleep deprivation than young adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2009;57:1245-1251.,1212. Wigren HK, Rytkonen KM, Porkka-Heiskanen T. Basal forebrain lactase release and promotion of cortical arousal during prolonged waking is attenuated in aging. J Neurosci. 2009;29:11698-11707. This theory is evidenced by an increased amount of nocturnal awakenings and diminished daytime sleepiness in elderly compared to young adults.1313. Dijk DJ, Groeger JA, Stanley N, Deacon S. Age-related reduction in daytime sleep propensity and nocturnal slow wave sleep. Sleep. 2010;33:211-223. Despite this reduced daytime sleepiness, the coexistence of a high frequency of daytime napping seems paradoxical. Up to 60% of older adults takes a nap,1414. Dautovich ND, McCrae CS, Rowe M. Subjective and objective napping and sleep in older adults: are evening naps "bad" for nighttime sleep? J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008; 56:1681-1686. most likely resulting from lifestyle factors, such as opportunities to nap due to schedule flexibility as a result of retirement, maladaptive habits or even medication side effects. Although considered non-pathological, the ultradian, circadian and homeostatic age-related changes mentioned above may lead to complaints in the elderly and, when combined with lifestyle factors, lead to poor sleep quality or sleep deprivation.

Healthy aging also involves changes in cognitive functioning. Thereby a certain amount of cognitive decline is a normal part of aging, varying considerably across individuals and cognitive domains, with some cognitive functions appearing more susceptible than others to the effects of aging.1515. Spreng RN, Wojtowicz M, Grady CL. Reliable differences in brain activity between young and old adults: a quantitative meta-analysis across multiple cognitive domains. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;34:1178-1794. Declines in cognition tend to be most marked in executive functions (EF).1616. Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Scalf P. The Implications of Cortical Recruitment and Brain Morphology for Individual Differences in Inhibitory Function in Aging Humans. Psychol Aging 2005;20:363-375.,1717. Turner GR, Spreng RN. Executive functions and neurocognitive aging: dissociable patterns of brain activity. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33:826.e1-826.e13. EF is an umbrella term denoting a branch of top-down processes involved in the coordination and control of goal-directed behavior.1818. Cartwright KB. Insights From Cognitive Neuroscience: The Importance of Executive Function for Early Reading Development and Education. Early Educ Dev. 2012;23:24-36.

19. Chan RCK, Shumb D, Toulopoulou T, Chen EYH. Assessment of executive functions: Review of instruments and identification of critical issues. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2008;23:201-216.
-2020. Titz C, Karbach J. Working memory and executive functions: effects of training on academic achievement. Psychol Res. 2014;78:852-868. Diamond2121. Diamond A. Executive Functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135-168. describes three core executive functions: (1) inhibitory control (the ability to restrain one's habitual responses to override strong internal predispositions or external draw); (2) working memory (the ability to hold information in mind to support the completion of tasks); and (3) cognitive flexibility (the ability to ponder multiple sources and forms of information at one time and adaptively switch between them when engaging in a task). From these core functions, other EF functions are derived, such as planning, reasoning, problem-solving. However, other EF are also considered in other models, such as the ability to sustain attention, resistance to interference, utilization of feedback, multitasking, and verbal fluency.1919. Chan RCK, Shumb D, Toulopoulou T, Chen EYH. Assessment of executive functions: Review of instruments and identification of critical issues. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2008;23:201-216. Adequate executive functioning is necessary for selecting and monitoring actions that facilitate the fulfillment of chosen goals and supports older adults to perform complex activities in everyday life.2222. Johnson JK, Lui LY, Yaffe K. Executive function, more than global cognition, predicts functional decline and mortality in elderly women. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2007;62:1134-1141.

Changes in executive functioning related to healthy aging can be explained at least partially by structural and functional changes in the central nervous system. Although the loss of neurons in the elderly is moderate,2323. Morterá P, Herculano-Houzel S. Age-related neuronal loss in the rat brain starts at the end of adolescence. Front Neuroanat. 2012;6:1-9. studies have shown that loss of neuronal and dendritic architecture, rather than loss of neurons, underlies neocortical volume loss with increasing healthy age.2424. Freeman SH, Kandel R, Cruz L, et al. Preservation of neuronal number despite age-related cortical brain atrophy in elderly subjects without Alzheimer disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2008;67:1205-1212. Changes in frontal-striatal circuits are the most likely significant cause of reduced EF in non-mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and nondemented older adults.2525. Buckner RL. Memory and Executive Function Review in Aging and AD: Multiple Factors that Cause Decline and Reserve Factors that Compensate. Neuron. 2004;44:195-208. Frontal-striatal systems are preferentially vulnerable to white matter change, atrophy, and certain forms of neurotransmitter depletion.2525. Buckner RL. Memory and Executive Function Review in Aging and AD: Multiple Factors that Cause Decline and Reserve Factors that Compensate. Neuron. 2004;44:195-208.

26. Bloss EB, Janssen WG, Ohm DT, et al. Evidence for Reduced Experience-Dependent Dendritic Spine Plasticity in the Aging Prefrontal Cortex. J Neurosci. 2011;31:7831-7839.
-2727. Magnusson KR, Brim BL, Das SR. Selective vulnerabilities of N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors during brain aging. Front Aging Neurosci. 2010;2:1-15. Frontal-striatal and frontoparietal circuitry play an important role in executive functioning.2828. Alvarez JA, Emory E. Executive Function and the Frontal Lobes: A Meta Analytic Review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2006;16:17-42.,2929. Jacob SN, Nieder A. Complementary roles for primate frontal and parietal cortex in guarding working memory from distractor stimuli. Neuron. 2014;83:226-237.

Given that both sleep components and EF undergo alterations in healthy aging, associations between these factors have been considered, as well as the impact of sleep changes (i.e. ultradian, circadian and homeostatic) in prefrontal circuits that underlie EF. Horne & Harrison first hypothesized that the waking function of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the frontal predominance of EEG delta activity in sleep may be linked.3030. Horne JA. Human sleep, sleep loss and behaviour. Implications for the prefrontal cortex and psychiatric disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 1993; 162:413-419.

31. Harrison Y, Horne JA. Sleep loss impairs short and novel language tasks having a prefrontal focus. J Sleep Res. 1998;7:95-100.
-3232. Harrison Y, Horne JA, Rothwell A. Prefrontal neuropsychological effects of sleep deprivation in young adults--a model for healthy aging? Sleep. 2000;23:1067-1073. According to this model and more recent findings, sleep loss selectively affects the efficiency of PFC circuitry, producing changes in brain metabolism and affecting executive functioning.3333. Harrison Y, Jones, K, Waterhouse J. The in?uence of time awake and circadian rhythm upon performance on a frontal lobe task. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:1966-1972.

34. Killgore WD, Kahn-Greene ET, Lipizzi EL, Newman RA, Kamimori GH, Balkin TJ. Sleep deprivation reduces perceived emotional intelligence and constructive thinking skills. Sleep Med. 2008;9:517-526.
-3535. Schmidt C, Peigneux P, Cajochen C. Age-related changes in sleep and circadian rhythms: impact on cognitive performance and underlying neuroanatomical networks. Front Neurol. 2012;3:5-15. Since then, some studies have argued that cognitive functioning related to the PFC is particularly vulnerable to sleep loss or sleep deprivation.3636. Beebe DW, Gozal D. Obstructive sleep apnea and the prefrontal cortex: Towards a comprehensive model linking nocturnal upper airway obstruction to daytime cognitive and behavioral deficits. J Sleep Res. 2002;11:1-16.,3737. Almondes KM, Holanda Júnior FWN, Alves NT. Sleep deprivation and implications for recognition and perception of facial emotions. Sleep Biol Rhythms. 2015;14:13-22.

Assuming the prefrontal circuit vulnerability to sleep loss hypothesis and the parallel decreases in sleep and EF in older adults, the present article provides a systematic review of evidence of interaction between these two functions in healthy aging, that is, a growing body of literature suggesting that changes in sleep may play an important role in the extent to which healthy older adults exhibit decreases in executive functioning.

METHOD

Search strategy. A systematic strategy was employed to identify the available literature on age-related sleep changes and EF. Search terms included: (executive funct*) AND (sleep) AND (aging OR older adults OR elderly). The following databases were searched using these terms: Medline, PsycInfo and Scopus. Search terms were systematically applied across these three databases. The numbers of relevant hits from these databases are summarized in Figure 1. The retrieved articles were referenced in EndNote (Version 17.0) and duplicates removed. This search strategy was augmented with hand searches of reference lists of included studies. Searches were conducted on 10th May 2016.

Figure 1
Flow-chart describing process of study selection.

Study selection. Inclusion and exclusion criteria were outlined prior to the search. Inclusion criteria included: 1) articles published in the last 25 years (1990-2016); 2) sample comprising healthy older adults (60 years or older, without dementia, mild cognitive impairment, sleep disorders or loss of functionality); 3) empirical research; and 4) focus on sleep variables (e.g. ultradian, circadian and homeostatic), healthy aging and cognitive functioning, addressing EF or at least one EF domain evaluated. The exclusion criteria established were: 1) case studies, letters to the editor, and conference abstracts; 2) non-human experimental model studies; 3) non-healthy/clinical samples of older adults, such as diagnosed with sleep disorders, dementia and cognitive impairment; 4) EF not included in cognitive evaluation and variables; 5) non-English language; and 6) review articles.

The 465 articles initially retrieved from the preceding detailed searches were screened by their titles and abstracts, and articles not meeting the inclusion and/or exclusion criteria described earlier were removed. This gave a total of 26 articles eligible for review.

RESULTS

We included 26 articles in the review. A summary and analysis of these articles are given in Table 1. In general, the studies varied widely in their research design, participants and sleep and EF variables. A large portion of the studies were conducted by researchers in the United States (n=16). This was followed by the United Kingdom (n=2), France (n=2), the Netherlands (n=2), Canada (n=1), Ireland (n=1), Japan (n=1), and Switzerland (n=1). In relation to study populations, all the articles in this review were based on non-clinical elderly samples, most of them including home/community-dwelling subjects. The main sleep variables included in articles were WASO, sleep latency, sleep efficiency, TST, sleep quality and other less frequent variables such as sleep deprivation and sleep stages. Sleep circadian and homeostatic influences were also included in some studies.

Table 1
Overview of studies assessing the impact and the relation of sleep and executive functioning.

A total of 10 studies (n=9094) evaluated sleep using objective measures only. Seven studies (n=6383) were based on objective and subjective measures. A total of 9 studies (n=2261) evaluated sleep using subjective measures only. Actigraphy was the most used objective measure by studies (n=10), followed by polysomnography (PSG) (n=4) and other sleep detection devices (n=3). The most common subjective measure was the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI). Others included the Epworth sleepiness scale (ESS) (n=4), sleep diary (n=4) and other sleep questionnaires (n=3). A large variety of tests and instruments were used to assess EF. The Trail Making Test (TMT) was used in more than half of the studies (n=14). Other more frequent measures were Verbal Fluency Tasks (n=11), the Stroop test (n=7), Digit Span Test (n=7), n-back Task (n=6), and Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) (n=6). Other less frequent tests used are given in Table 1.

Wake after sleep onset (WASO). WASO means the total amount of time awake after falling sleep and it is considered a better reflection of sleep fragmentation. There is evidence that WASO could be related to executive functioning and global cognition. Using actigraphy, Blackwell et al.3838. Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Poor sleep is associated with impaired cognitive function in older women: The study of osteoporotic fractures. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2006;6:405-410. found that higher WASO was associated with higher risk of cognitive impairment, using the TMT as a measure of executive functioning and the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) for global cognition. In a longitudinal community-based study by Yaffe et al.3939. Yaffe K, Blackwell T, Barnes DE, Ancoli-Israel S, Stone KL. Preclinical Cognitive Decline and Subsequent Sleep Disturbance in Older Women. Neurology. 2007;69:237-422. elderly women who showed decline on the TMT (part B) experienced worse WASO. Using the same measures, another study by Blackwell et al.4040. Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Association of sleep characteristics and cognition in older community-dwelling men: The MrOS sleep study. Sleep. 2011;34:1347-1356. found a modest association. Recently, in two studies Wilckens et al.4141. Wilckens K A, Woo SG, Erickson KI, Wheeler ME. Sleep continuity and total sleep time are associated with task-switching and preparation in young and older adults. J Sleep Res. 2014;23:508-516.,4242. Wilckens KA, Woo SG, Kirk AR, Erickson KI, Wheeler ME. The Role of Sleep Continuity and Total Sleep Time in Executive Function Across the Adult Lifespan. Psychol Aging. 2014;29:658-665. found that in older adults, higher sleep continuity (i.e. lower WASO) was associated with better inhibitory control and that individuals with less WASO were more likely to engage preparatory strategies to reduce switch costs and boost task-switching performance. These findings suggest that a higher WASO negatively impacts executive functioning. Inversely, older adults may benefit from lower WASO in performing complex tasks.

In another study, however, although participants with WASO longer than 30 min tended to have lower accuracy on the n-back test (attention and working memory measure) than those with WASO <5 min or 5-30 min, these differences were not significant.4343. Miyata S, Noda A, Iwaoto K, Kawano N, Okuda M, Ozaki N. Poor sleep quality impairs cognitive performance in older adults. J Sleep Res. 2013; 22:535-541. McCrae et al.4444. McCrae CS, Vatthauer KE, Dzierzewski JM, Marsiske M. Habitual sleep, reasoning, and processing speed in older adults with sleep complaints. Cogn Ther Res. 2012;36:156-164. found that WASO did not predict executive functioning (measured by a reasoning test) but significantly predicted processing speed. This seems to indicate that in some cases, sleep fragmentation primarily affects basic processes, such as processing speed and alertness, rather than more complex ones.

Total sleep time (TST). In some studies, TST was examined as a continuous variable while in other studies it was dichotomized into short and long sleep duration. The results of its impact on EF and other cognitive domains are controversial. Many studies found no association between objectively measured total sleep duration and executive functioning.3838. Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Poor sleep is associated with impaired cognitive function in older women: The study of osteoporotic fractures. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2006;6:405-410.

39. Yaffe K, Blackwell T, Barnes DE, Ancoli-Israel S, Stone KL. Preclinical Cognitive Decline and Subsequent Sleep Disturbance in Older Women. Neurology. 2007;69:237-422.
-4040. Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Association of sleep characteristics and cognition in older community-dwelling men: The MrOS sleep study. Sleep. 2011;34:1347-1356.,4242. Wilckens KA, Woo SG, Kirk AR, Erickson KI, Wheeler ME. The Role of Sleep Continuity and Total Sleep Time in Executive Function Across the Adult Lifespan. Psychol Aging. 2014;29:658-665.,4646. Seelye A, Mattek N, Howieson D, Riley T, Wild K, Kaye J. The Impact of Sleep on Neuropsychological Performance in Cognitively Intact Older Adults Using a Novel In-Home Sensor-Based Sleep Assessment Approach. Clin Neuropsychol. 2015;29:53-66.,4747. Luik AI, Zuurbier LA, Hofman A, Van Someren EJ, Ikram MA, Tiemeier H. Associations of the 24-hour activity rhythm and sleep with cognition: A population-based study of middle-aged and elderly persons. Sleep Med. 2015;16:850-855. This lack of relationship was also detected in subjectively measured total sleep duration.4444. McCrae CS, Vatthauer KE, Dzierzewski JM, Marsiske M. Habitual sleep, reasoning, and processing speed in older adults with sleep complaints. Cogn Ther Res. 2012;36:156-164.,4545. Saint Martin M, Sforza E, Barthélémy JC, Thomas-Anterion C, Roche E. Does subjective sleep affect cognitive function in healthy elderly subjects? The Proof cohort. Sleep Med. 2012;13:1146-1152.,4848. Nebes RD, Buysse DJ, Halligan EM, Houck PR, Monk TH. Self-reported sleep quality predicts poor cognitive performance in healthy older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2009;64:180-187. Miyata et al.4343. Miyata S, Noda A, Iwaoto K, Kawano N, Okuda M, Ozaki N. Poor sleep quality impairs cognitive performance in older adults. J Sleep Res. 2013; 22:535-541. found that TST was correlated with the 0-back test from the n-back test, a simple measure of attention and short-term memory, but not for the 1-back test, which reflects working memory capacity.

Nevertheless, in the study by Wilckens et al.4141. Wilckens K A, Woo SG, Erickson KI, Wheeler ME. Sleep continuity and total sleep time are associated with task-switching and preparation in young and older adults. J Sleep Res. 2014;23:508-516. a better performance on task-switching, a model paradigm of executive functioning that involves cognitive flexibility and the ability to shift attention between one task and another, was associated with longer TST. There have been cases in which subjective and objective measures diverged in associations: Lambiase et al.4949. Lambiase MJ, Gabriel KP, Kuller LH, Matthews KA. Sleep and Executive Function in Older Women: The Moderating Effect of Physical Activity. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69:1170-1176. reported that total sleep duration measured using a sleep diary, a subjective instrument, was associated with cognitive flexibility; on the other hand, in the same study, actigraphy measures did not show this association. Taken together, these studies of sleep duration and executive functioning in older adults have produced mixed results, although most of the evidence mentioned above indicates no relationship between them and may suggest that it is interruption of sleep, such as higher WASO, rather than quantity, that most affects EF.

Sleep latency. In relation to sleep latency, defined as the length of time that it takes to accomplish the transition from wakefulness to sleep, the reviewed articles reported mixed results. When examining objective reports, Blackwell et al.,3838. Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Poor sleep is associated with impaired cognitive function in older women: The study of osteoporotic fractures. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2006;6:405-410. Yaffe et al.3939. Yaffe K, Blackwell T, Barnes DE, Ancoli-Israel S, Stone KL. Preclinical Cognitive Decline and Subsequent Sleep Disturbance in Older Women. Neurology. 2007;69:237-422. and Blackwell et al.4040. Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Association of sleep characteristics and cognition in older community-dwelling men: The MrOS sleep study. Sleep. 2011;34:1347-1356. showed that higher sleep latency was associated with worse executive functioning based on performance on the TMT (part B). Schmutte et al.5050. Schmutte T, Harris S, Levin R, Zweig R, Katz M, Lipton R. The relation between cognitive functioning and self-reported sleep complaints in nondemented older adults: results from the Bronx aging study. Behav Sleep Med. 2007;5:39-56. reported that longer self-reported sleep latency was significantly and inversely related to verbal-based cognitive measures, abstract reasoning, and longer latencies associated with poorer cognitive functioning. Nebes et al.4848. Nebes RD, Buysse DJ, Halligan EM, Houck PR, Monk TH. Self-reported sleep quality predicts poor cognitive performance in healthy older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2009;64:180-187. also showed that a longer self-reported time to fall asleep was associated with poorer abstract reasoning. Others reported lack of association between sleep latency and EF through subjective4545. Saint Martin M, Sforza E, Barthélémy JC, Thomas-Anterion C, Roche E. Does subjective sleep affect cognitive function in healthy elderly subjects? The Proof cohort. Sleep Med. 2012;13:1146-1152. and objective4343. Miyata S, Noda A, Iwaoto K, Kawano N, Okuda M, Ozaki N. Poor sleep quality impairs cognitive performance in older adults. J Sleep Res. 2013; 22:535-541. measures. It may be that longer sleep latency disturbs sleep quality and quantity and thus exacerbates the negative impacts on EF.

Sleep efficiency. Sleep efficiency, defined as the percentage of total time in bed actually spent in sleep, seems to impact EF. Blackwell et al.3838. Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Poor sleep is associated with impaired cognitive function in older women: The study of osteoporotic fractures. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2006;6:405-410. found that compared with women that had a sleep efficiency >70%, those with <70% had a higher risk of cognitive impairment measured by the TMT (part B) for executive functioning and by the MMSE for global cognition. In addition, older women who showed decline on the TMT (part B) had worse sleep efficiency.3939. Yaffe K, Blackwell T, Barnes DE, Ancoli-Israel S, Stone KL. Preclinical Cognitive Decline and Subsequent Sleep Disturbance in Older Women. Neurology. 2007;69:237-422. Participants with sleep efficiency <85% showed a significant decrease in 0- and 1-back test accuracy compared to individuals with sleep efficiency ≥85%, i.e. as objective sleep efficiency decreased attention and working memory worsened.4343. Miyata S, Noda A, Iwaoto K, Kawano N, Okuda M, Ozaki N. Poor sleep quality impairs cognitive performance in older adults. J Sleep Res. 2013; 22:535-541. Lambiase et al.4949. Lambiase MJ, Gabriel KP, Kuller LH, Matthews KA. Sleep and Executive Function in Older Women: The Moderating Effect of Physical Activity. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69:1170-1176. also reported that lower actigraphy-assessed sleep efficiency was associated with poorer performance on executive function tasks of attention, set-shifting and cognitive flexibility. On subjective measures, sleep efficiency was correlated with measures of abstract reasoning and working memory.4848. Nebes RD, Buysse DJ, Halligan EM, Houck PR, Monk TH. Self-reported sleep quality predicts poor cognitive performance in healthy older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2009;64:180-187. Nevertheless, using the same measures as Blackwell et al.3838. Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Poor sleep is associated with impaired cognitive function in older women: The study of osteoporotic fractures. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2006;6:405-410. and Yaffe et al.,3939. Yaffe K, Blackwell T, Barnes DE, Ancoli-Israel S, Stone KL. Preclinical Cognitive Decline and Subsequent Sleep Disturbance in Older Women. Neurology. 2007;69:237-422. Blackwell et al.4040. Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Association of sleep characteristics and cognition in older community-dwelling men: The MrOS sleep study. Sleep. 2011;34:1347-1356. failed to find these associations. According to the authors, this less consistent finding observed for sleep efficiency may relate to greater measurement error of this variable. Overall, the studies indicated that older adults with lower percentage sleep efficiency exhibited worse executive functioning.

Sleep quality. Sleep quality does not refer only to the number of hours in bed. It is the quality of these hours and how other factors affect sleep that are important, such as fragmentation, sleep deprivation, perception of restorative sleep, daytime sleepiness, having trouble falling sleep and waking up in the morning or staying alert during the daytime. Most of the studies used the PSQI as a general measure of sleep quality. The results in the literature regarding the effect of this parameter on EF tend to be mixed. In older women, good (PSQI < 6) and poor (PSQI ≥ 6) sleepers differed significantly on tests of working memory, attention, set shifting, and abstract problem solving,4848. Nebes RD, Buysse DJ, Halligan EM, Houck PR, Monk TH. Self-reported sleep quality predicts poor cognitive performance in healthy older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2009;64:180-187. suggesting an important role of sleep quality in executive functioning. Sutter et al.5151. Sutter C, Zöllig J, Allemand M, Martin M. Sleep Quality and Cognitive Function in Healthy Old Age: The Moderating Role of Subclinical Depression. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:768-775. sought to clarify the relationship between sleep quality and cognitive performance in healthy older adults, and to evaluate the moderating role of subclinical depression in this relationship. The study found that self-reported sleep quality in healthy older adults seemed to be selectively related to higher order executive functions in those participants with high versus low levels of subclinical depression.

In other lines of evidence, these effects were not found. Gamaldo et al.5252. Gamaldo AA, Allaire JC, Whitfield KE. Exploring the within-person coupling of sleep and cognition in older African Americans. Psychol Aging. 2010;25:851-857. used inhibition, inductive reasoning, verbal fluency and global executive functioning together with other cognitive domains, to create a global composite score and found that sleep quality measured by PSQI was not a significant predictor of performance across cognitive domains. Although Blackwell et al.4040. Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Association of sleep characteristics and cognition in older community-dwelling men: The MrOS sleep study. Sleep. 2011;34:1347-1356. reported that almost half of the men (44%) had self-reported poor sleep quality, defined as PSQI > 5, no significant association between this subjective measure and executive functioning was confirmed. Also, excessive daytime sleepiness did not correlate with cognitive outcomes. Martin et al.4545. Saint Martin M, Sforza E, Barthélémy JC, Thomas-Anterion C, Roche E. Does subjective sleep affect cognitive function in healthy elderly subjects? The Proof cohort. Sleep Med. 2012;13:1146-1152. observed that good and poor sleepers did not differ on any of the cognitive function measures, including those evaluating EF domains, except on the TMT (part A), where this latter result may reflect attention impairment. Luik et al.4747. Luik AI, Zuurbier LA, Hofman A, Van Someren EJ, Ikram MA, Tiemeier H. Associations of the 24-hour activity rhythm and sleep with cognition: A population-based study of middle-aged and elderly persons. Sleep Med. 2015;16:850-855. also found no relationship between lower reported sleep quality, as measured by a sleep diary and global cognitive functioning or performance on specific cognitive tasks, such as inhibition. Taken together, these contrary findings do not seem to confirm the relationship between sleep quality and EF.

Some articles reviewed also used other variables related to sleep quality. Gamaldo et al.5353. Gamaldo AA, Allaire JC, Whitfield KE. The Relationship Between Reported Problems Falling Asleep and Cognition Among African American Elderly. Res Aging. 2008;30:752-767. examined the relationship between elders' cognitive performance and self-reported trouble falling asleep and found that this complaint significantly predicted performance on the digit span and alpha span task (measures of working memory and attention). This demonstrates that a self-report of sleep difficulty may be a predictor of cognitive performance. Zimmermann et al.5454. Zimmerman ME, Bigal ME, Katz MJ, Brickman AM, Lipton RB. Sleep Onset/Maintenance Difficulties and Cognitive Function in Nondemented Older Adults: the Role of Cognitive Reserve. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;18:461-470. reported that sleep difficulties (i.e. sleep initiation and sleep maintenance) appear to selectively impact the verbal fluency process in older adults with lower education measured by category fluency and letter fluency tests, suggesting that those with higher education are better able to engage active cognitive compensation against sleep difficulties than individuals with lower education.

Circadian, homeostatic and ultradian factors. Another study found an association between time to bed and cognitive functioning, independent of sleep duration. Older adults who went to bed much later showed a poor performance on the TMT (A and B) and on the drawing test (prefrontally-mediated tasks) compared to older adults who went to bed within the robust norm window.5555. McHugh JE, Walsh L, Lawlor BA. Time to bed is associated with cognitive outcome: an analysis of sleep-times and wake-times in community-dwelling older adults. Biol Rhythm Res. 2014;45:103-114. Thus, it appears that severe deviations in time to bed represent a marker of circadian misalignment and a possible marker of cognitive impairment. The study by Oosterman et al.5656. Oosterman JM, Van Someren EJW, Vogels RLC, Van Harten B, Scherder EJA. Fragmentation of the rest-activity rhythm correlates with age-related cognitive deficits. J Sleep Res. 2009;18:129-135. reported that fragmentation of the sleep-wake rhythm predicted neuropsychological functioning such as mental speed, verbal memory and EF (cognitive flexibility, interference and working memory) in home-dwelling elderly people. Partial correlations showed that the association of rhythm fragmentation with cognitive decline is partly independent of the main effects of age. Therefore, part of age-related cognitive decline could independently be associated with sleep and its circadian organization. Another study found that greater fragmentation of rest and activity measured by actigraphy was associated with lower levels of cognitive performance, including working memory.5757. Lim AS, Yu L, Costa MD, et al. Increased fragmentation of rest-activity patterns is associated with a characteristic pattern of cognitive impairment in older individuals. Sleep. 2012;35:633-640.

A prospective observational study in a large cohort of older adult women without dementia showed that weaker circadian activity rhythm patterns (i.e. amplitude, mesor, rhythm robustness, and acrophase) were associated with worse cognitive function, especially executive functioning measured by the TMT (part B).5858. Walsh CM, Blackwell T, Tranah GJ, et al. Weaker circadian activity rhythms are associated with poorer executive function in older women. Sleep 2014;37:2009-2016. Disrupted circadian activity rhythms could be an early indicator of future executive function decline. In another study, actigraphy recordings were used to quantify 24-h rhythms by calculating the stability and fragmentation of the rhythm over a period of days. Both aspects of the 24-h activity rhythm and sleep parameters were related to global cognitive functioning, but specifically, disturbances in the 24-h activity rhythm were mostly related to tasks that draw on perceptual speed and executive functioning (mental flexibility and inhibition).4747. Luik AI, Zuurbier LA, Hofman A, Van Someren EJ, Ikram MA, Tiemeier H. Associations of the 24-hour activity rhythm and sleep with cognition: A population-based study of middle-aged and elderly persons. Sleep Med. 2015;16:850-855. Perhaps his association could reflect a direct effect of disturbed rhythms on perceptual speed and executive functioning.

Sagaspe et al.5959. Sagaspe P, Taillard J, Amiéva H, et al. Influence of Age, Circadian and Homeostatic Processes on Inhibitory Motor Control: A Go/Nogo Task Study. Plos One. 2012;7(6): e39410. evaluated inhibitory motor control and sustained attention under controlled high or low sleep pressure conditions in young and older males. Under the sleep deprivation condition, inhibitory motor control was equally impaired by extended wakefulness in both age groups. Although sustained attention also decreased under sleep deprivation conditions in both groups, this effect was more pronounced for young participants. This might indicate that aging is a protective factor against the effects of extended wakefulness on simple tasks (i.e. sustained attention) due to an attenuation of sleep pressure with duration of time awake. In other words, homeostatic sleep pressure would be lower in the older people, allowing them to be less vulnerable to sustained attentional failure after a night of sleep deprivation.

Lastly, some of the studies reviewed focused on sleep stages and sleep EGG measures. Anderson & Horne6060. Anderson C, Horne JA. Prefrontal cortex: links between low frequency delta EEG in sleep and neuropsychological performance in healthy, older people. Psychophysiology. 2003;40:349-357. examined associations between neuropsychological performance, such as planning, flexibility, verbal fluency, and fluid intelligence, and sleep EEG characteristics within the prefrontal cortex in 24 healthy 61-75-year-olds. They found significant associations between 0.5-1.0 Hz power from the left frontal EEG channel, in the first non-REM period, and performance on tasks more specific to the left PFC (e.g., nonverbal planning and verbal fluency). These results pointed to a sleep EEG correlate of neuropsychological performance centering on the PFC. Lafortune et al.6161. Lafortune M, Gagnon JF, Martin N, et al. Sleep spindles and rapid eye movement sleep as predictors of next morning cognitive performance in healthy middle-aged and older participants. J Sleep Res. 2014; 23:159-167. found that spindle density in healthy middle-aged and older participants predicted verbal learning, visual attention and verbal fluency performance. Slow wave density and slow wave slope predicted verbal fluency performance only. These results suggest that spindle density is a marker of cognitive functioning in older adults and may reflect neuroanatomic integrity.

Groeger et al.6262. Groeger JA, Stanley N, Deacon S, Dijk DJ. Dissociating effects of global SWS disruption and healthy aging on waking performance and daytime sleepiness. Sleep. 2014;37:1127-1142. assessed the effects of reducing SWS on daytime functioning and whether these effects differ across groups of healthy young, middle-aged, and older individuals. Evaluating domains such as sustained and divided attention processing speed, decision, sequence and motor control, working memory, and verbal fluency, the study showed that SWS disruption resulted in slower or impaired information processing and sustained attention, less precise motor control, and erroneous implementation, rather than inhibition, of well-practiced actions. Younger participants performed better at baseline than older participants across many cognitive domains, with largest effects on executive function, response time, sustained attention, and motor control. SWS can possibly be considered a potential mediator of age-related decline in performances. Song et al.6363. Song Y, Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, Redline S, Stone KL; Osteoporotic Fractures in Men (MrOS) Study Group. Relationships between sleep stages and changes in cognitive function in older men: the MrOS Sleep Study. Sleep. 2015;38:411-421. investigated the relationship between sleep stage distributions and subsequent decline in cognitive function in older men over time, using the TMT (part B) as a measure of executive functioning and the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination (3MS) for global measurement of cognitive function. Increased time in stage N1sleep (considered a "light" sleep and marker of poorer sleep quality) and less time in stage REM sleep were associated with worsening general cognitive and executive performance in older men, suggesting a beneficial role of REM sleep in cognition.

DISCUSSION

The aim of the present review was to systematically understand the evidence of interaction between sleep and executive functions in healthy aging, considering a growing body of literature suggesting that changes in sleep parameters and in its ultradian, circadian and homeostatic organization may play an important role in the executive functioning. In the elderly population, however, the evidence for this association reviewed tends to be mixed and involves various inconsistencies. Multiple sleep parameters appear to differ, with some benefiting and others impairing executive functioning.

Most of the studies found evidence for the association between WASO and EF and between sleep efficiency and EF. Both sleep parameters change with normal aging. WASO is a measure of time spent awake during the night. WASO time is a better reflection of sleep fragmentation and lower values indicate better sleep continuity during the night. Thus, elevated WASO directly affects sleep efficiency (proportion of total time in bed spent asleep). It is possible that higher WASO prevents older adults from progressing normally through sleep stages and attenuates the length of time spent in SWS.4141. Wilckens K A, Woo SG, Erickson KI, Wheeler ME. Sleep continuity and total sleep time are associated with task-switching and preparation in young and older adults. J Sleep Res. 2014;23:508-516.,4242. Wilckens KA, Woo SG, Kirk AR, Erickson KI, Wheeler ME. The Role of Sleep Continuity and Total Sleep Time in Executive Function Across the Adult Lifespan. Psychol Aging. 2014;29:658-665. SWS and slow wave activity (SWA) have been identified as a beneficial factor for prefrontal cortex functioning and hence for executive functioning, which relies mostly on this brain area and its connections.6464. Wilckens KA, Erickson KI, Wheeler ME. Age-related decline in controlled retrieval: the role of the PFC and sleep. Neural Plast. 2012;2012:1-14.,6565. Wilckens KA, Aizenstein HJ, Nofzinger EA, et al. The role of non-rapid eye movement slow wave activity in prefrontal metabolism across young and middle-aged adults. J Sleep Res. 2016; 25:296-306. Supporting lines of evidences include: (1) slow waves show frontal predominance both under baseline conditions and in response to sleep loss;6666. Cajochen C, Foy R, Dijk DJ. Frontal predominance of a relative increase in sleep delta and theta EEG activity after sleep loss in humans. Sleep Res Online. 1999;2:65-69.,6767. Blatter K, Cajochen C. Circadian rhythms in cognitive performance: methodological constraints, protocols, theoretical underpinnings. Physio Behav. 2007;90:196-208. (2) delta activity that is high during SWS is associated with cognitive performance;6868. Scullin MK. Sleep, Memory, and Aging: The Link Between Slow-Wave Sleep and Episodic Memory Changes From Younger to Older Adults. Pychol Aging. 2012;28:105-114. (3) selective SWS disruption is associated with poor cognitive performance, and even impaired executive functioning;6262. Groeger JA, Stanley N, Deacon S, Dijk DJ. Dissociating effects of global SWS disruption and healthy aging on waking performance and daytime sleepiness. Sleep. 2014;37:1127-1142. (4) age-related medial prefrontal cortex gray matter atrophy has recently been shown to be associated with reduced SWA in older adults. Lower SWS was associated with reduced functional connectivity within PFC-hippocampal networks, suggesting that neural synchrony during sleep strengthens connections between the PFC and functionally related brain regions;6969. Mander BA, Rao V, Lu B, et al. Prefrontal atrophy, disrupted NREM slow waves and impaired hippocampal-dependent memory in aging. Nat Neurosci.2013;16:357-364 (5) greater relative slow wave activity was associated with higher dorsolateral prefrontal metabolism.6565. Wilckens KA, Aizenstein HJ, Nofzinger EA, et al. The role of non-rapid eye movement slow wave activity in prefrontal metabolism across young and middle-aged adults. J Sleep Res. 2016; 25:296-306. In addition, there is also migration of cortical activity patterns from the posterior (decreased at occipital and temporal channels) to anterior (increased at central and frontal channels) area after sleep deprivation.7070. Boonstra T, Stins J, Daffertshofer A, Beek P. Effects of sleep deprivation on neuronal functioning: An integrative review. Cell Mol Life Sci. 2007;64:934-946.

Thus, this growing body of evidence supports a potential role of certain aspects of sleep cerebral activity in benefiting prefrontal areas and its connections. This highlights the PFC vulnerability model which holds that sleep deprivation and poor sleep affect PFC circuitry, producing a mild, sub-clinical level of impairment.3232. Harrison Y, Horne JA, Rothwell A. Prefrontal neuropsychological effects of sleep deprivation in young adults--a model for healthy aging? Sleep. 2000;23:1067-1073.,3333. Harrison Y, Jones, K, Waterhouse J. The in?uence of time awake and circadian rhythm upon performance on a frontal lobe task. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:1966-1972.,7171. Harrison Y, Horne JA. The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: A review. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2000;6:236-249.,7272. Muzur A, Pace-Schott E, Hobson A. The prefrontal cortex in sleep. Trends Cogn Sci. 2002;6:475-481. The cognitive processes supported by PFC-associated networks (i.e. frontal-striatal and frontoparietal), especially EF, seem to be the most sensitive to individual differences in sleep.3434. Killgore WD, Kahn-Greene ET, Lipizzi EL, Newman RA, Kamimori GH, Balkin TJ. Sleep deprivation reduces perceived emotional intelligence and constructive thinking skills. Sleep Med. 2008;9:517-526.,7272. Muzur A, Pace-Schott E, Hobson A. The prefrontal cortex in sleep. Trends Cogn Sci. 2002;6:475-481. This relationship may be strengthened considering that in normal aging both sleep and executive functioning change, which may produce a synergistic effect on the performance of executive tasks.

High levels of sleep fragmentation (recurrent awakenings and/or stage shifts) may result in complaints of non-restorative sleep even when an apparently normal total sleep time is present. This may be a factor explaining why most studies reviewed here found inconsistent or mixed associations between TST and EF: interruption of sleep, such as higher sleep fragmentation and lower sleep efficiency appears to affect EF more than sleep quantity. It seems that continuous and consolidated sleep, which allows adequate progression through sleep stages and NREM-REM cycling, is important for executive functioning.

Contrary to the PFC vulnerability model, other lines of evidence suggest that impaired sleep continuity and deprivation may cause sleepiness, which in turn may result in reduced vigilance and attentional failures. This reduced vigilance may negatively impact executive functioning. Therefore sleep could affect EF mediated by basic process impairment, such as vigilance, alertness and attention processes.7373. Lim J, Dinges DF. A meta-analysis of the impact of short-term sleep deprivation on cognitive variables. Psychol Bull. 2010;136:375-389. In this case, we can only cautiously infer that older adults with poor sleep quality and sleep deprivation may be less alert during the day, which directly worsens their performance on executive functioning tasks. On the other hand, another potential factor is that it can be assumed that impairment in the executive performance of elderly caused by sleep loss only becomes apparent when the PFC areas of the brain are activated under excessively high demand.5151. Sutter C, Zöllig J, Allemand M, Martin M. Sleep Quality and Cognitive Function in Healthy Old Age: The Moderating Role of Subclinical Depression. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:768-775.

Age-related changes in sleep parameters and ultradian sleep factors do not act solely on executive functioning. The review also showed that the changes in circadian organization of wake-sleep cycle (i.e. time to bed as marker of sleep phase, rhythm fragmentation, and weaker circadian activity) and the homeostatic factors also appear to play an important role. The aging process appears to generate vulnerability to the impact of sleep and circadian rhythm disturbances on executive performance.4747. Luik AI, Zuurbier LA, Hofman A, Van Someren EJ, Ikram MA, Tiemeier H. Associations of the 24-hour activity rhythm and sleep with cognition: A population-based study of middle-aged and elderly persons. Sleep Med. 2015;16:850-855. By affecting both cognition and the rest-activity rhythm, age-related changes in brain structures may account for an association between these variables. Regarding homeostatic pressure, some results have indicated that an increase in errors on an executive task under extended wakefulness can be attributed mainly to the effect of sleep pressure with duration of time awake.5959. Sagaspe P, Taillard J, Amiéva H, et al. Influence of Age, Circadian and Homeostatic Processes on Inhibitory Motor Control: A Go/Nogo Task Study. Plos One. 2012;7(6): e39410.

Taken together, all of the relationships discussed above have important implications for clinicians and other health professionals, where psychoeducational and cognitive-behavioral interventions focusing on sleep hygiene and sleep parameters could improve global sleep and consequently may benefit cognitive functioning.7474. Pace-Schott EF, Spencer RMC. Age-related changes in the cognitive function of sleep. Prog Brain Res. 2011;191:75-88. Sleep quality in the elderly population is often poor not because of specific pathologies, but due to lifestyle factors, such as daytime napping, alcohol consumption, medication side effects, that detract from adequate nocturnal sleep. In these cases, psychoeducational and cognitive-behavioral interventions can help by reducing the effects of insufficient sleep on waking performance. In addition, higher physical activity levels can protect against the negative effects of poor sleep on executive functioning, and also promote other well-documented benefits of a physically active lifestyle.4949. Lambiase MJ, Gabriel KP, Kuller LH, Matthews KA. Sleep and Executive Function in Older Women: The Moderating Effect of Physical Activity. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69:1170-1176.

While the majority of studies address many cognitive domains, here we focused on EF only. Consequently, we did not explore the relationship between sleep and domains such as episodic memory and visuospatial abilities. Few studies focus solely on the relationship between sleep and executive functioning. Concerning limitations of the literature reviewed, some studies did not exclude participants based on measures of sleep disorders, for example examining the possible presence of sleep disordered breathing, a condition that impairs cognition in older adults. In addition, many studies relied on subjective rather than objective assessments of sleep. For example, self-reported items about trouble falling asleep or time of sleep duration do not account for other possible sleep problems and are highly dependent on participant's perception and beliefs about sleep, which may lead to differential misclassification and selective drop-out. For example, polysomnography is considered the "gold standard" for sleep measurement and may shed light on brain activity during sleep and on its role in cognitive functioning. Covariables such as depression, anxiety, and medical illness should also be accounted for when both sleep and EF are investigated. Another important consideration is that most of the studies used relatively few EF tests and tasks. Further studies should employ a more comprehensive range of EF tasks and components to establish a better association between sleep and executive functioning.

In conclusion, healthy aging is marked by changes in both sleep and executive functioning. Although evidence of the role of sleep and its ultradian, circadian and homeostatic organization in EF has grown, the association between them is partially inconclusive and studies using valid and reliable measures for sleep and EF variables are clearly needed. However, sleep parameters such as wake after sleep onset and sleep efficiency, in addition to circadian fragmentation of sleep, showed less mixed results and are potentially correlated with EF measures. This relationship has important implications for clinicians and other health professionals in that psychoeducational and cognitive-behavioral interventions focusing on sleep hygiene and sleep parameters can improve global sleep and consequently may benefit cognitive functioning.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Ancoli-Israel S, Ayalon L, Salzman C. Sleep in the elderly: normal variations and common sleep disorders. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2008; 16:279-286.
  • 2
    Buysse DJ, Monk TH, Carrier J, Begley A. Circadian patterns of sleep, sleepiness, and performance in older and younger adults. Sleep. 2005;28:1365-1376.
  • 3
    O'Donnell D, Silva EJ, Munch M, Ronda JM, Wang W, Duffy JF. Comparison of subjective and objective assessments of sleep in healthy older subjects without sleep complaints. J Sleep Res. 2009;18:254-263.
  • 4
    Cajochen C, Münch M, Knoblauch V, Blatter K, Wirz-Justice A. Age-related changes in the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep. Chronobiol Int. 2006;23:461-474.
  • 5
    Ohayon MM, Carskadon MA, Guilleminault C, Vitiello MV. Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan. Sleep. 2004;27:1255-1273.
  • 6
    Roepke SK, Ancoli-Israel S. Sleep disorders in the elderly. Indian J Med Res. 2010;131:302-310.
  • 7
    Crowley K, Trinder J, Kim Y, Carrington M, Colrain IM. The effects of normal aging on sleep spindle and K-complex production. Clin Neurophysiol. 2002;113:1615-1622.
  • 8
    Floyd JA, Janisse JJ, Jenuwine ES, Ager JW. Changes in REM-sleep percentage over the adult lifespan. Sleep 2006;30:829-836.
  • 9
    Duffy JF, Zitting KM, Chinoy ED. Aging and Circadian Rhythms. Sleep Med Clin. 2015;10:423-434.
  • 10
    Wolkove N, Elkholy O, Baltzan, M, Palayew M. Sleep and aging: 1. Sleep disorders commonly found in older people. Can Med Assoc J. 2007;176:1299-1304.
  • 11
    Duffy JF, Willson HJ, Wang W, Czeisler CA. Healthy older adults better tolerate sleep deprivation than young adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2009;57:1245-1251.
  • 12
    Wigren HK, Rytkonen KM, Porkka-Heiskanen T. Basal forebrain lactase release and promotion of cortical arousal during prolonged waking is attenuated in aging. J Neurosci. 2009;29:11698-11707.
  • 13
    Dijk DJ, Groeger JA, Stanley N, Deacon S. Age-related reduction in daytime sleep propensity and nocturnal slow wave sleep. Sleep. 2010;33:211-223.
  • 14
    Dautovich ND, McCrae CS, Rowe M. Subjective and objective napping and sleep in older adults: are evening naps "bad" for nighttime sleep? J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008; 56:1681-1686.
  • 15
    Spreng RN, Wojtowicz M, Grady CL. Reliable differences in brain activity between young and old adults: a quantitative meta-analysis across multiple cognitive domains. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;34:1178-1794.
  • 16
    Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Scalf P. The Implications of Cortical Recruitment and Brain Morphology for Individual Differences in Inhibitory Function in Aging Humans. Psychol Aging 2005;20:363-375.
  • 17
    Turner GR, Spreng RN. Executive functions and neurocognitive aging: dissociable patterns of brain activity. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33:826.e1-826.e13.
  • 18
    Cartwright KB. Insights From Cognitive Neuroscience: The Importance of Executive Function for Early Reading Development and Education. Early Educ Dev. 2012;23:24-36.
  • 19
    Chan RCK, Shumb D, Toulopoulou T, Chen EYH. Assessment of executive functions: Review of instruments and identification of critical issues. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2008;23:201-216.
  • 20
    Titz C, Karbach J. Working memory and executive functions: effects of training on academic achievement. Psychol Res. 2014;78:852-868.
  • 21
    Diamond A. Executive Functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135-168.
  • 22
    Johnson JK, Lui LY, Yaffe K. Executive function, more than global cognition, predicts functional decline and mortality in elderly women. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2007;62:1134-1141.
  • 23
    Morterá P, Herculano-Houzel S. Age-related neuronal loss in the rat brain starts at the end of adolescence. Front Neuroanat. 2012;6:1-9.
  • 24
    Freeman SH, Kandel R, Cruz L, et al. Preservation of neuronal number despite age-related cortical brain atrophy in elderly subjects without Alzheimer disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2008;67:1205-1212.
  • 25
    Buckner RL. Memory and Executive Function Review in Aging and AD: Multiple Factors that Cause Decline and Reserve Factors that Compensate. Neuron. 2004;44:195-208.
  • 26
    Bloss EB, Janssen WG, Ohm DT, et al. Evidence for Reduced Experience-Dependent Dendritic Spine Plasticity in the Aging Prefrontal Cortex. J Neurosci. 2011;31:7831-7839.
  • 27
    Magnusson KR, Brim BL, Das SR. Selective vulnerabilities of N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors during brain aging. Front Aging Neurosci. 2010;2:1-15.
  • 28
    Alvarez JA, Emory E. Executive Function and the Frontal Lobes: A Meta Analytic Review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2006;16:17-42.
  • 29
    Jacob SN, Nieder A. Complementary roles for primate frontal and parietal cortex in guarding working memory from distractor stimuli. Neuron. 2014;83:226-237.
  • 30
    Horne JA. Human sleep, sleep loss and behaviour. Implications for the prefrontal cortex and psychiatric disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 1993; 162:413-419.
  • 31
    Harrison Y, Horne JA. Sleep loss impairs short and novel language tasks having a prefrontal focus. J Sleep Res. 1998;7:95-100.
  • 32
    Harrison Y, Horne JA, Rothwell A. Prefrontal neuropsychological effects of sleep deprivation in young adults--a model for healthy aging? Sleep. 2000;23:1067-1073.
  • 33
    Harrison Y, Jones, K, Waterhouse J. The in?uence of time awake and circadian rhythm upon performance on a frontal lobe task. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:1966-1972.
  • 34
    Killgore WD, Kahn-Greene ET, Lipizzi EL, Newman RA, Kamimori GH, Balkin TJ. Sleep deprivation reduces perceived emotional intelligence and constructive thinking skills. Sleep Med. 2008;9:517-526.
  • 35
    Schmidt C, Peigneux P, Cajochen C. Age-related changes in sleep and circadian rhythms: impact on cognitive performance and underlying neuroanatomical networks. Front Neurol. 2012;3:5-15.
  • 36
    Beebe DW, Gozal D. Obstructive sleep apnea and the prefrontal cortex: Towards a comprehensive model linking nocturnal upper airway obstruction to daytime cognitive and behavioral deficits. J Sleep Res. 2002;11:1-16.
  • 37
    Almondes KM, Holanda Júnior FWN, Alves NT. Sleep deprivation and implications for recognition and perception of facial emotions. Sleep Biol Rhythms. 2015;14:13-22.
  • 38
    Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Poor sleep is associated with impaired cognitive function in older women: The study of osteoporotic fractures. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2006;6:405-410.
  • 39
    Yaffe K, Blackwell T, Barnes DE, Ancoli-Israel S, Stone KL. Preclinical Cognitive Decline and Subsequent Sleep Disturbance in Older Women. Neurology. 2007;69:237-422.
  • 40
    Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Association of sleep characteristics and cognition in older community-dwelling men: The MrOS sleep study. Sleep. 2011;34:1347-1356.
  • 41
    Wilckens K A, Woo SG, Erickson KI, Wheeler ME. Sleep continuity and total sleep time are associated with task-switching and preparation in young and older adults. J Sleep Res. 2014;23:508-516.
  • 42
    Wilckens KA, Woo SG, Kirk AR, Erickson KI, Wheeler ME. The Role of Sleep Continuity and Total Sleep Time in Executive Function Across the Adult Lifespan. Psychol Aging. 2014;29:658-665.
  • 43
    Miyata S, Noda A, Iwaoto K, Kawano N, Okuda M, Ozaki N. Poor sleep quality impairs cognitive performance in older adults. J Sleep Res. 2013; 22:535-541.
  • 44
    McCrae CS, Vatthauer KE, Dzierzewski JM, Marsiske M. Habitual sleep, reasoning, and processing speed in older adults with sleep complaints. Cogn Ther Res. 2012;36:156-164.
  • 45
    Saint Martin M, Sforza E, Barthélémy JC, Thomas-Anterion C, Roche E. Does subjective sleep affect cognitive function in healthy elderly subjects? The Proof cohort. Sleep Med. 2012;13:1146-1152.
  • 46
    Seelye A, Mattek N, Howieson D, Riley T, Wild K, Kaye J. The Impact of Sleep on Neuropsychological Performance in Cognitively Intact Older Adults Using a Novel In-Home Sensor-Based Sleep Assessment Approach. Clin Neuropsychol. 2015;29:53-66.
  • 47
    Luik AI, Zuurbier LA, Hofman A, Van Someren EJ, Ikram MA, Tiemeier H. Associations of the 24-hour activity rhythm and sleep with cognition: A population-based study of middle-aged and elderly persons. Sleep Med. 2015;16:850-855.
  • 48
    Nebes RD, Buysse DJ, Halligan EM, Houck PR, Monk TH. Self-reported sleep quality predicts poor cognitive performance in healthy older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2009;64:180-187.
  • 49
    Lambiase MJ, Gabriel KP, Kuller LH, Matthews KA. Sleep and Executive Function in Older Women: The Moderating Effect of Physical Activity. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69:1170-1176.
  • 50
    Schmutte T, Harris S, Levin R, Zweig R, Katz M, Lipton R. The relation between cognitive functioning and self-reported sleep complaints in nondemented older adults: results from the Bronx aging study. Behav Sleep Med. 2007;5:39-56.
  • 51
    Sutter C, Zöllig J, Allemand M, Martin M. Sleep Quality and Cognitive Function in Healthy Old Age: The Moderating Role of Subclinical Depression. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:768-775.
  • 52
    Gamaldo AA, Allaire JC, Whitfield KE. Exploring the within-person coupling of sleep and cognition in older African Americans. Psychol Aging. 2010;25:851-857.
  • 53
    Gamaldo AA, Allaire JC, Whitfield KE. The Relationship Between Reported Problems Falling Asleep and Cognition Among African American Elderly. Res Aging. 2008;30:752-767.
  • 54
    Zimmerman ME, Bigal ME, Katz MJ, Brickman AM, Lipton RB. Sleep Onset/Maintenance Difficulties and Cognitive Function in Nondemented Older Adults: the Role of Cognitive Reserve. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;18:461-470.
  • 55
    McHugh JE, Walsh L, Lawlor BA. Time to bed is associated with cognitive outcome: an analysis of sleep-times and wake-times in community-dwelling older adults. Biol Rhythm Res. 2014;45:103-114.
  • 56
    Oosterman JM, Van Someren EJW, Vogels RLC, Van Harten B, Scherder EJA. Fragmentation of the rest-activity rhythm correlates with age-related cognitive deficits. J Sleep Res. 2009;18:129-135.
  • 57
    Lim AS, Yu L, Costa MD, et al. Increased fragmentation of rest-activity patterns is associated with a characteristic pattern of cognitive impairment in older individuals. Sleep. 2012;35:633-640.
  • 58
    Walsh CM, Blackwell T, Tranah GJ, et al. Weaker circadian activity rhythms are associated with poorer executive function in older women. Sleep 2014;37:2009-2016.
  • 59
    Sagaspe P, Taillard J, Amiéva H, et al. Influence of Age, Circadian and Homeostatic Processes on Inhibitory Motor Control: A Go/Nogo Task Study. Plos One. 2012;7(6): e39410.
  • 60
    Anderson C, Horne JA. Prefrontal cortex: links between low frequency delta EEG in sleep and neuropsychological performance in healthy, older people. Psychophysiology. 2003;40:349-357.
  • 61
    Lafortune M, Gagnon JF, Martin N, et al. Sleep spindles and rapid eye movement sleep as predictors of next morning cognitive performance in healthy middle-aged and older participants. J Sleep Res. 2014; 23:159-167.
  • 62
    Groeger JA, Stanley N, Deacon S, Dijk DJ. Dissociating effects of global SWS disruption and healthy aging on waking performance and daytime sleepiness. Sleep. 2014;37:1127-1142.
  • 63
    Song Y, Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, Redline S, Stone KL; Osteoporotic Fractures in Men (MrOS) Study Group. Relationships between sleep stages and changes in cognitive function in older men: the MrOS Sleep Study. Sleep. 2015;38:411-421.
  • 64
    Wilckens KA, Erickson KI, Wheeler ME. Age-related decline in controlled retrieval: the role of the PFC and sleep. Neural Plast. 2012;2012:1-14.
  • 65
    Wilckens KA, Aizenstein HJ, Nofzinger EA, et al. The role of non-rapid eye movement slow wave activity in prefrontal metabolism across young and middle-aged adults. J Sleep Res. 2016; 25:296-306.
  • 66
    Cajochen C, Foy R, Dijk DJ. Frontal predominance of a relative increase in sleep delta and theta EEG activity after sleep loss in humans. Sleep Res Online. 1999;2:65-69.
  • 67
    Blatter K, Cajochen C. Circadian rhythms in cognitive performance: methodological constraints, protocols, theoretical underpinnings. Physio Behav. 2007;90:196-208.
  • 68
    Scullin MK. Sleep, Memory, and Aging: The Link Between Slow-Wave Sleep and Episodic Memory Changes From Younger to Older Adults. Pychol Aging. 2012;28:105-114.
  • 69
    Mander BA, Rao V, Lu B, et al. Prefrontal atrophy, disrupted NREM slow waves and impaired hippocampal-dependent memory in aging. Nat Neurosci.2013;16:357-364
  • 70
    Boonstra T, Stins J, Daffertshofer A, Beek P. Effects of sleep deprivation on neuronal functioning: An integrative review. Cell Mol Life Sci. 2007;64:934-946.
  • 71
    Harrison Y, Horne JA. The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: A review. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2000;6:236-249.
  • 72
    Muzur A, Pace-Schott E, Hobson A. The prefrontal cortex in sleep. Trends Cogn Sci. 2002;6:475-481.
  • 73
    Lim J, Dinges DF. A meta-analysis of the impact of short-term sleep deprivation on cognitive variables. Psychol Bull. 2010;136:375-389.
  • 74
    Pace-Schott EF, Spencer RMC. Age-related changes in the cognitive function of sleep. Prog Brain Res. 2011;191:75-88.
  • 3
    This study was conducted at the Postgraduate Program in Psychology, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, Natal RN, Brazil.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Jul-Sep 2016

History

  • Received
    11 July 2016
  • Accepted
    16 Aug 2016
Academia Brasileira de Neurologia, Departamento de Neurologia Cognitiva e Envelhecimento R. Vergueiro, 1353 sl.1404 - Ed. Top Towers Offices, Torre Norte, São Paulo, SP, Brazil, CEP 04101-000, Tel.: +55 11 5084-9463 | +55 11 5083-3876 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistadementia@abneuro.org.br | demneuropsy@uol.com.br