Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

The Wishes and Expression of Gratitude of Youth

Os Desejos e a Expressão de Gratidão dos Jovens

Los Deseos y la Expresión de la Gratitud en los Jóvenes

Abstract

Little research has examined the development of gratitude in youth, and none has related their gratitude to their wishes. We therefore asked 358 7- to 14-year-old North American participants (56% female; 55% White) to write their greatest wish and what they would do for their benefactor. Using chi-square and linear curve estimation, we found that younger youth (aged 7 to 10) were significantly more likely to express hedonistic wishes (desire for immediate gain) and concrete gratitude (not taking the benefactor's wishes into account); older youth (aged 11 to 14) were significantly more likely to wish either for something involving future well-being for themselves or the well-being of others and connective gratitude (taking into account the benefactor's wishes). Within both age groups, there was a significant inverse relation between hedonistic wishes and connective gratitude. This research has implications for encouraging the feeling and expression of connective gratitude.

Keywords
gratitude; ;child development; adolescent development

Resumo

Poucos estudos examinaram o desenvolvimento da gratidão em jovens, e nenhum estabeleceu relações entre sua gratidão e seus desejos. Assim, solicitamos a 358 participantes norte-americanos entre 7 e 14 anos de idade (56% meninas; 55% brancos) que escrevessem qual seria o seu maior desejo e o que eles fariam a seu benfeitor. Utilizando qui quadrado e estimativa de curva linear, encontramos que os mais jovens (de 7 a 10 anos de idade) foram significativamente mais propensos a expressar desejos hedonistas (desejo por ganho imediato) e gratidão concreta (não considerando os desejos do benfeitor); os jovens mais velhos (de 11 a 14 anos de idade) foram significativamente mais propensos a desejar seja algo envolvendo bem-estar futuro para si mesmos seja o bem-estar de outros e gratidão conectiva (levando em conta os desejos do benfeitor).Em ambos os grupos etários, houve uma relação inversa significativa entre desejos hedonistas e gratidão conectiva.Esta pesquisa tem implicações para encorajar o sentimento e a expressão de gratidão conectiva.

Palavras-chave
gratidão; desenvolvimento infantil; desenvolvimento do adolescente

Resumen

Pocos estudios han examinado el desarrollo de gratitud en los jóvenes, y ninguno ha relacionado su gratitud a sus deseos. Por lo tanto, pedimos a 358 participantes de América del Norte de 7 a 14 años de edad (55% femenino, 55% blanco) a escribir su mayor deseo y lo que harían por su benefactor. Utilizando un distribución ji-cuadrado y estimación curva lineal, encontramos que los más jóvenes (de 7 a 10 años de edad) fueron significativamente más propensos a expresar los deseos hedonistas (deseo de ganancia inmediata) y la gratitud de concreto (no tomar los deseos del benefactor en cuenta); los jóvenes mayores (de 11 a 14 años de edad) fueron significativamente más propensos a desear sea para algo relacionado con el bienestar futuro para sí mismos o para el bienestar de los demás y gratitud conectivo (tomando en cuenta los deseos del benefactor). Dentro de ambos grupos de edad, existe una relación inversa significativa entre los deseos hedonistas y gratitud conectivo. Esta investigación tiene implicaciones para fomentar el sentimiento y la expresión de gratitud conectivo.

Palabras clave
gratitud; desarrollo infantil; desarrollo del adolescente

Gratitude is a socially desirable trait valued in many cultures for its role in strengthening social bonds. Parents encourage children to express gratitude, and ungrateful individuals are viewed negatively. Gratitude has been defined in at least two ways. Some include in the definition positive feelings about the good things that life offers - such as beautiful sunsets or good health, or being grateful to God ( Froh, Yurkewicz, & Kashdan, 2009Kashdan TB, Mishra A, Breen WE, Froh JJ. (2009). Gender differences in gratitude: Examining appraisals, narratives, the willingness to express emotions, and changes in psychological needs. Journal of Personality77(3), 691-730.doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00562.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009...
; Wood, Froh, & Geraghty, 2010Wood AM, Froh JJ, Geraghty AWA. (2010). Gratitude and well-being: A review and theoretical integration. Clinical Psychology Review30(7), 890-905.doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.005
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.00...
). Others, in both psychology and philosophy, have defined gratitude more specifically as positive feelings towards and desire to repay a benefactor who has intentionally and freely provided a gift or help, without requesting or requiring payback( Baumgarten-Tramer, 1938Baumgarten-Tramer F. (1938)."Gratefulness" in children and young people. Journal of Genetic Psychology53, 53-66. ; McConnell, 1993McConnell T. (1993).Gratitude. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press. ; 2013McConnell T. (20 13 ).Gratitude.In H LaFollette (Ed.), International encyclopedia of ethics(Vol., 4 pp. 2220-2229).Malden MA: Wiley-Blackwell. ).We use this latter definition, which is also the one accepted by psychologists who were among the first to conduct empirical research on gratitude ( Baumgarten-Tramer, 1938Baumgarten-Tramer F. (1938)."Gratefulness" in children and young people. Journal of Genetic Psychology53, 53-66. ) as well as by recent scholars ( Freitas, Pieta, & Tudge, 2011Freitas LBL, Pieta MAM, Tudge JRH. (2011). Beyond politeness: The expression of gratitude in children and adolescents. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica24(4), 757-764.doi:10.1590/S0102-79722011000400016
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7972201100...
; Froh, Bono, & Emmons, 2010Froh JJ, Bono G, Emmons R. (2010). Being grateful is beyond good manners: Gratitude and motivation to contribute to society among early adolescents. Motivation and, Emotion 34(2), 144-157.doi:10.1007/s11031-010-9163-z
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-010-9163-...
; McCullough, Kimeldorf, & Cohen, 2008McCullough ME, Kimeldorf MB, Cohen AD. (2008).An adaptation for altruism?The social causes, social effects, and social evolution of gratitude.Current Directions in Psychological Science17(4), 281-285.doi:10.1111/j.1 467-8721.2008.00590.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008...
; Rava & Freitas, 2013Rava PGS, Freitas LBL. (20 13 ). Gratidão e sentimento de obrigatoriedade na infânciaGratitudeandthe feeling ofobligation in childhood. Psico-USF18(3), 383-39 4 .doi:10.1590/S1 413 -827120 13 000300005
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-8271201300...
).

The sense of connection to a benefactor and desire to reciprocate are key components of gratitude in that they contribute to a positive cycle of giving or helping. In the course of repaying the benefactor, the original beneficiary becomes a benefactor as well, and the cycle repeats, thus helping to form ever-closer relations between the individuals concerned. This positive cycle has been recognized in humans ( Piaget, 1954/1981Piaget J. (1981). Intelligence and affectivity: Their relationship during child development. Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews. (Original work published 195 4 ). ) and may also occur in non-human primates ( Bonnie & de Waal, 2004Bonnie KE, de Waal FBM . (2004). Primate social reciprocity and the origin of gratitude. In RA Emmons, ME McCullough (Eds.), The psychology of gratitude (pp. 2 13 -229). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. ).

Gratitude is present in the life of youth who live in different contexts ( Paludo, 2014Paludo SS. (201 4 ).As emoções no universo moral dos adolescentes [ Emotion s in the moral universe of adolescents]. In LF Habigzang, E Diniz, SH Koller (Orgs.), Trabalhando com adolescentes: Teoria e intervençãopsicológica[Working with adolescents: Psychological theory and intervention](pp. 16 4 -179). Porto Alegre, RS: Artmed. ), features as a part of young adolescents' conceptions of what it takes to be considered a moral person ( Hardy, Walker, Olsen, Skalski, & Basinger, 2011Hardy SA, Walker LJ, Olsen JA, Skalski JE, Basinger JC. (2011). Adolescent naturalistic conceptions of moral maturity.Social Development20(3), 562-586.doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2010.00590.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2010...
), and develops during childhood and adolescence ( Baumgarten-Tramer, 1938Baumgarten-Tramer F. (1938)."Gratefulness" in children and young people. Journal of Genetic Psychology53, 53-66. ; Freitas et al., 2011Freitas LBL, Pieta MAM, Tudge JRH. (2011). Beyond politeness: The expression of gratitude in children and adolescents. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica24(4), 757-764.doi:10.1590/S0102-79722011000400016
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7972201100...
; Piaget, 1954/1981Piaget J. (1981). Intelligence and affectivity: Their relationship during child development. Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews. (Original work published 195 4 ). ) as children form a theory of mind, are able to think in non-egocentric ways, and can take into account others' intentionality( Kawashima & Martins, 2015Kawashima RA, Martins RA. (2015). The absence of generosity and obedience to authority: Judgments of teachers and students from kindergarten. Paidéia (Ribeirão Preto)25(60), 115-123. doi:10.1590/1982-43272560201514
https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-43272560201...
).We should thus expect outh of different ages to express gratitude in different ways. Young children can be taught to say "thank you", but a learned social convention is not the same as the more sophisticated type of gratitude fitting with the definition we have used. Older children may learn that one should reciprocate in some way beyond saying "thank you" for help or gifts received. However, reciprocation in a way designed to be helpful or pleasurable to the benefactor requires taking another's perspective; egocentric children or those who show little understanding of theory of mind are unlikely to express gratitude in its most sophisticated form ( Freitas, O'Brien, Nelson, & Marcovitch, 2012Freitas LBL, O'Brien M, Nelson JA, Marcovitch S. (2012).A compreensão da gratidão e teoria da mente em crianças de 5 anos [ The understanding of gratitude and theory of mind in 5-year-olds] .Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica25(2), 330-338.doi:10.1590/S0102-79722012000200015
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7972201200...
; Nelson et al., 2013Nelson JA, Freitas LBL, O'Brien M, Calkins S D, Leerkes EM, Marcovitch S. (20 13 ). Preschool-aged children's understanding of gratitude: Relations with emotion and mental state knowledge. British Journal of Developmental Psychology31(1), 4 2-56doi:10.1111/j.20 4 4-835X.2012.02077.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2012...
). Adolescents are more likely than younger children to take into account benefactors' intentions ( Weiner & Graham, 1988Weiner B, Graham S. (1988). Understanding the motivational role of affect: Life-span research from an attributional perspective.Cognition and, Emotion 3(4), 4 01- 4 19.doi:10.1080/02699938908 41271 4
https://doi.org/10.1080/0269993890841271...
) and thus should be more likely to express the more sophisticated type of gratitude than are younger children. One of the main aims of this study is to test this hypothesis.

To date, the development of gratitude has received little empirical focus, with research mainly on adults ( Freitas et al., 2011Freitas LBL, Pieta MAM, Tudge JRH. (2011). Beyond politeness: The expression of gratitude in children and adolescents. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica24(4), 757-764.doi:10.1590/S0102-79722011000400016
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7972201100...
).Among adults, gratitude is related to individuals' sense of well-being and quality of life ( Emmons & Mishra, 2012Emmons RA, Mishra A. (2012). Why gratitude enhances well-being: What we know, what we need to know. In KM Sheldon, TB Kashdan, MF Steger (Eds.),Designing the future of positive psychology: Taking stock and moving forward (pp. 248-264). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. ; Wood et al., 2010Wood AM, Froh JJ, Geraghty AWA. (2010). Gratitude and well-being: A review and theoretical integration. Clinical Psychology Review30(7), 890-905.doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.005
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.00...
): individuals who more often experience and express gratitude report being happier; enjoying their work more; being more optimistic, energetic, and helpful; and having lower levels of depression and stress than those who do not ( Kerr, O'Donovan, & Pepping, 2015Kerr SL, O'Donovan A, Pepping CA. (2015). Can gratitude and kindness interventions enhance well-being in a clinical sample? Journal of Happiness Studies16(1), 17-36.doi:10.1007/s10902-0 13 -9492-1
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-013-9492-...
; McCullough, Emmons,& Tsang, 2002McCullough ME, Emmons RA, Tsang J-A. (2002). The grateful disposition: A conceptual and empirical topography. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology82(1), 112-127.doi:10.1037//0022-351 4 .82.1.112
https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.82.1....
). In adolescents gratitude is related to life satisfaction, contentment, optimism, and positive affect ( Algoe, 2012Algoe SB. (2012). Find, remind, and bind: The functions of gratitude in everyday relationships. Social and Personality Psychology Compass6(6), 455-469. doi:10.1111/lj.1751-9004.2012.00439.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/lj.1751-9004.201...
; Algoe, Fredrickson, & Gable, 2013Algoe SB, Fredrickson BL, Gable SL. (2013). The social functions of the emotion of gratitude via expression. Emotion13(4), 605-609. doi:10.1037/a0032701
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032701...
; Froh et al., 2009Froh JJ, Yurkewicz C, Kashdan TB. (2009). Gratitude and subjective well-being in early adolescence: Examining gender differences. Journal of Adolescence32(3), 633-650.doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.06.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.20...
).Experimental studies have shown that youth who are encouraged to express gratitude subsequently feel more positive about their school, teachers, and friends ( Bono & Froh, 2009Bono G, Froh J. (2009). Gratitude in school: Benefits to students and schools. In R Gilman, ES Huebner, MJ Furlong (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology in schools (pp. 77-88) New York, NY: Routledge. ; Froh et al., 2014Froh JJ, Bono G, Fan J, Emmons RA, Henderson K, Harris C, Wood AM (2014). Nice thinking! An educational intervention that teaches children to think gratefully.School Psychology Review43(2), 13 2-152. ).

Much of this research, however, does not treat gratitude as the "positive feeling towards and desire to repay a benefactor", as described above. Except for studies in which gratitude is induced by having participants write a letter to someone who has helped them in some way ( Froh et al., 2014Froh JJ, Bono G, Fan J, Emmons RA, Henderson K, Harris C, Wood AM (2014). Nice thinking! An educational intervention that teaches children to think gratefully.School Psychology Review43(2), 13 2-152. ), the most widely used measures are the Gratitude Questionnaire ( McCullough et al., 2002McCullough ME, Emmons RA, Tsang J-A. (2002). The grateful disposition: A conceptual and empirical topography. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology82(1), 112-127.doi:10.1037//0022-351 4 .82.1.112
https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.82.1....
), most items of which are not related to a benefactor, or the Gratitude Adjective Checklist ( McCullough et al., 2002McCullough ME, Emmons RA, Tsang J-A. (2002). The grateful disposition: A conceptual and empirical topography. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology82(1), 112-127.doi:10.1037//0022-351 4 .82.1.112
https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.82.1....
), which asks participants the extent to which they feel grateful, thankful, and appreciative over the previous 24 hours, without any reference to a benefactor. A different type of measure is needed to assess gratitude towards a benefactor( Gulliford, Morgan, & Kristjánsson, 2013Gulliford L, Morgan B, Kristjánsson K. (20 13 ).Recent work on the concept of gratitude in philosophy and psychology.Journal of Value Inquiry47(3), 285-317.doi:10.1007/s10790-0 13 -9387-8
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10790-013-9387-...
). Moreover, none of these studies are designed to examine the development of gratitude.

In the first study designed specifically to examine the development of gratitude, Baumgarten-Tramer (1938Baumgarten-Tramer F. (1938)."Gratefulness" in children and young people. Journal of Genetic Psychology53, 53-66. ) asked 1,0597- to 15-year-old Swiss participants to state their greatest wish and what they would do for a benefactor who granted that wish. Responses to the second question were categorize das: verbal gratitude(e.g., "thank you"); concrete gratitude (providing the benefactor something valuable to the beneficiary, such as offering a doll in return for a game);and connective gratitude (providing something valuable to the benefactor, such as saying "I would do whatever I could to help her"). Baumgarten-Tramer found that verbal gratitude was common at all ages but most frequent among 15-year-olds, concrete gratitude was more common in her younger than older participants, and connective gratitude was more common in those who were older. Connective gratitude, as the name suggests, should help build or maintain relationships with others and is the most sophisticated type.

Recent research using Baumgarten-Tramer's(1938)Baumgarten-Tramer F. (1938)."Gratefulness" in children and young people. Journal of Genetic Psychology53, 53-66. method has supported her findings with data from 430 Brazilian children and young adolescents ( Freitas et al., 2011Freitas LBL, Pieta MAM, Tudge JRH. (2011). Beyond politeness: The expression of gratitude in children and adolescents. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica24(4), 757-764.doi:10.1590/S0102-79722011000400016
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7972201100...
). In this study verbal gratitude was expressed at all ages but most frequently by 12- and 14-year-olds. Children (7- to 10-year-olds) were more likely to express concrete gratitude whereas adolescents (11- to 14-year-olds) were more likely to express connective gratitude. Neither study provided data on the types of wishes the participants had. One might expect, however, some link between children's wishes and their gratitude. For example, youth who wish for something that is designed to benefit others may be more likely to express connective gratitude than those who wish for something of benefit to themselves. The content of youths' wishes is also likely to vary by age. It is possible that adolescents' more sophisticated expressions of gratitude are related to the nature of their wishes, which may focus less on material objects than those of younger children. Support for this idea comes from research by Chaplin and John (2007)Chaplin LN, John DR. (2007).Growing up in a material world: Age differences in materialism in children and adolescents.Journal of Consumer Research34(4), 480-493. who found 12- to 13-year-oldschose more material objects to place in a collage than did either 8- to 9-year-olds or 16- to 18-year-olds.They interpreted their findings to show age differences in materialism.

Gratitude researchers have generally not examined gender or ethnic differences. In one study girls were found to be more likely to express gratitude for interpersonal events such as being helped, whereas boys were more likely to express gratitude for material things ( Gordon, Musher-Eizenman, Holub, & Dalrymple, 2004Gordon AK, Musher-Eizenman DR, Holub SC, Dalrymple J. (2004). What are children thankful for? An archival analysis of gratitude before and after the attacks of September 11.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology25(5), 541-553.doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2004.08.004
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2004.08...
). Such a finding is supported by other research into American adolescents' values, showing that females are less materialistic than are males ( Goldberg, Gorn, Peracchio, & Bamossy, 2003Goldberg ME, Gorn GJ, Peracchio LA, Bamossy G. (2003).Understanding materialism among youth.Journal of Consumer Psychology13 (3), 278-288.doi:10.1207/S15327663JCP 13 03_09
https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327663JCP1303...
; Kasser, 2005)Kasser T. (2005).Frugality, generosity, and materialism in children and adolescents. In KA Moore, LH Lippman (Eds.), What do children need to flourish?Conceptualizing and measuring indicators of positive development (pp. 357-373). New York, NY: Springer. . Kashdan, Mishra, Breen, and Froh (2009)Kashdan TB, Mishra A, Breen WE, Froh JJ. (2009). Gender differences in gratitude: Examining appraisals, narratives, the willingness to express emotions, and changes in psychological needs. Journal of Personality77(3), 691-730.doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00562.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009...
found that adult women were more likely to express gratitude than were men, and women who expressed more gratitude were more likely to feel a sense of connectedness with others. No studies have examined cultural or ethnic variation in the expression of gratitude.

The Present Study

The main goal was to examine the expression of gratitude in children and adolescents and to identify age differences in the types of gratitude expressed, rather than variations in the extent to which individuals feel grateful, the typical focus in gratitude research with adolescents and adults. However, as our aim was to assess gratitude relating to the hypothetical receipt of something wished for, we first asked our participants to identify their "greatest wish". Initially, therefore, we examined the content of our participants' wishes. We hypothesized that those who were younger would be more likely to wish for immediate hedonistic material gain (money, toys, etc.) whereas adolescents' wishes would focus on longer-term benefits such as their own future well-being or the well-being of others.

Second, we asked our participants what they would do for the person who granted them their wish. We hypothesized that those who were younger would be more likely to express concrete gratitude whereas those who were older would be more likely to express connective gratitude. We expected no age differences in verbal gratitude, as found in earlier research.

Finally, we examined the expression of gratitude in relation to participants' wishes. We anticipated that those wishing for immediate benefits for themselves (hedonistic wishes) would be more likely to express concrete gratitude, whereas those whose wishes are focused on the well-being of others would be more likely to express connective gratitude. Youths wishing for their own future well-being were expected to fall in between. Based on earlier findings, we hypothesized that girls would be less likely than boys to express hedonistic wishes and more likely than boys both to express connective gratitude and to wish for social well-being. No gender difference was expected in wishes for self well-being.

Method

Participants

A total of 358 7- to 14-year-old youths from a city in the southeastern United States participated in this study. Females (mean age 10years 10 months, SD = 24.4 months) comprised 56% of the sample (197) and males 44% (155) (mean age 10 years 4 months, SD = 25.7 months) (six participants did not report gender, and were excluded from all analyses involving gender). For purposes of analysis, children were divided into younger (7-10 years) and older (11-14 years) age groups. Approximately 55% (197) self-identified as European American (White), 33.5% (120) as African American (Black) or Black/White bi-racial, 8.4% (30) as Hispanic or from Spanish-speaking homes, and 4% (11) from other ethnic groups or provided no information about ethnicity. Because of the low numbers of Hispanic and other groups, they were excluded from analysis of ethnic group differences.

The participants were recruited from six public schools (three elementary and three middle schools). Although we did not collect data on social class, the schools were selected to reflect the range of SES represented by the city's public schools. Students were asked to take home, and return, a parental consent letter; we described the study to those youths whose parents had provided consent and invited them to participate. Classrooms were provided $2 for each student who returned the parental consent letter, regardless of parental permission. The return rate was 56%; 86% of these gave consent and 84% of this number participated (the remainder were absent or chose not to participate).All youths whose parents gave permission and who themselves wished to participate were allowed to do so.

Instruments

The participants were asked to respond, in writing, to the Wishes and Gratitude Survey ( Freitas, Tudge, & McConnell, 2008Freitas LBL, Tudge JRH, McConnell T. (2008). The wishes and gratitude survey (Adapted from Baumgarten-Tramer, 1938). Greensboro, NC. Unpublished questionnaire. ), derived from Baumgarten-Tramer, 1938Baumgarten-Tramer F. (1938)."Gratefulness" in children and young people. Journal of Genetic Psychology53, 53-66. , consisting of four open-ended questions: "What is your greatest wish?", "What will you do for the person who granted you this wish?", "Is there anything else you should do for this person?" and "Who is this person?" For this study, only the first two were coded and analyzed. The second question includes the assumption that the wish is being freely and intentionally granted, thus fitting our definition of gratitude.

Procedure

Data collection. Data collection took place in groups in the participants' classrooms at school or in a central place, such as the school library or cafeteria. Research assistants were available to help any participants who had reading difficulties and to make sure that their responses were legible. Data collection took approximately 20 to 30 minutes.

Data analysis. Responses to the first question were coded into three categories: (a) hedonism, or a desire for something that brings immediate pleasure; (b) self well-being, or a desire for future-related personal benefits; and (c) social well-being, or a desire for current or future benefits for others. Fourteen youths (4%) were excluded from the analyses involving wishes as they did not provide a wish or the wish's meaning was unclear. Responses to the second question about what each respondent would do for the person who granted his or her wish were coded into the three main categories used by Baumgarten-Tramer (1938)Baumgarten-Tramer F. (1938)."Gratefulness" in children and young people. Journal of Genetic Psychology53, 53-66. : (a) verbal gratitude ("thank you" or "I would thank him 1,000 times"); (b) concrete gratitude (offering the benefactor something in return, but something of value to the child and not to the benefactor; e.g.,in return for being granted the wish"to become a pro basketball player," offering the benefactor an autographed basketball); and (c) connective gratitude(offering an object or help of value to the benefactor, such as: "I'll help her in any way that I can"). In addition, 32 (9%) of the participants were coded as "other" (including no response and "don't know") and were dropped from the analyses involving gratitude.

Two of the research team (the first author and a graduate student, trained in coding both wishes and gratitude) independently coded 50% of the responses to these two questions; kappas were .82 and above for wishes, and.90 and above for gratitude. All disagreements were discussed until agreement was reached. A large majority of the participants provided a response that could be coded into just one of the possible types; in the 76 cases (21%) in which participants provided two or more responses, the more advanced was included in the analyses.

The coded data were then analyzed using SPSS Version 18. Chi-square analysis was used to examine whether younger (7- to 10-year-olds) were more or less likely than those who were older (11- to 14-year-olds) to choose one or other type of wish and to respond with one or other type of gratitude. Chi-square analysis was also used to examine the relation between types of wishes and the types of gratitude expressed. We used curve estimation (one of the available options in SPSS for linear regression analysis) to assess the extent of age-related changes in both types of wishes and types of gratitude.

Ethical Considerations

This research project was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of North Carolina at Greensboro (IRB # 11-0315), and was carried out in full compliance with all ethical considerations. Parents first needed to provide their consent before students were asked to listen to and/or read a child-assent form that clearly explained the project and their role. Only students who signed that form were permitted to engage in the research.

Results

Types of Wishes

As hypothesized, 7- to 10-year-olds were more likely than 11- to 14-year-olds to express hedonistic wishes (56% vs. 24%), and less likely to express wishes relating to either self-well-being (36% vs. 55%) or social well-being (8% vs. 21%), X2(2) = 37.49, p < .0001 ( Figure 1 ). The two age groups differed significantly between hedonistic wishes and both self well-being (X 2[1] = 27.61, p < .0001) and social well-being (X 2[1] = 25.04, p < .0001), but not between self and social well-being (X 2[1] = 1.55, ns ). We also ran curve-estimation analyses to test whether the relation between age and types of wishes changed in a linear fashion. As only four 14-year olds were included in our sample, the curve estimations were conducted with 3547- to 13-year olds. The results indicated a significant linear decline in hedonistic wishes with age ( R2 = .74, p = .012), and significant linear increases in wishes for both selfwell-being ( R2 = .73, p = .015) and social well-being ( R2 = .64, p = .031).

Figure 1
Percentages of expression of wishes for hedonism, future-oriented self well-being, and social well-being by gender, ethnicity, and age group.

Although we had hypothesized that fewer girls than boys would express hedonistic wishes, no significant differences were found (X 2[2] = 3.18, ns ). We had not hypothesized ethnic variation in the participants' wishes, but we found African American youths to be more likely than their European American counterparts to wish for self well-being (52% vs. 41%) and less likely to wish for social well-being (5% vs. 20%), X 2(2) = 11.83, p < .005. No difference was found for hedonistic wishes (42% vs. 39%).

Types of Gratitude

As predicted, younger participants were more likely than those who were older to express concrete gratitude (47% vs. 27%) and less likely to express connective gratitude (32% vs. 49%), with little difference (21% vs. 24%) in expression of verbal gratitude (X 2[2] = 14.95, p < .01)( Figure 2 ). To test the specific hypothesis dealing solely with concrete and connective gratitude, we reanalyzed the data without including those participants who had expressed verbal gratitude; as predicted, the younger age group ( n = 119) was significantly more likely to express concrete gratitude and the older group ( n = 119)significantly more likely to express connective gratitude, X 2(1) = 15.01, p = .0001. The curve-estimation analyses revealed no significant linear change in terms of verbal gratitude ( R2 = .43, p = .109), but a significant linear decline, with age, in concrete gratitude ( R2 = .76, p = .011) and a significant linear increase in connective gratitude ( R2 = .91, p = .001).We found no gender or ethnic differences in expressions of gratitude.

Figure 2
Percentages of responses of verbal, concrete, and connective gratitude, by gender, ethnicity, and age group.

The Connection Between Wishes and Gratitude

Data on the relation between types of wishes and types of gratitude are in Table 1 . We predicted that participants whose wishes were hedonistic would be more likely to express concrete gratitude whereas those who wished for social well-being would be more likely to express connective gratitude. This prediction was supported, X 2(2) = 26.87, p < .0001. The finding also held across both age groups: younger group, X 2(2) = 10.82, p = .004; older group, X 2(2) = 10.01, p = .007.

Table 1
The Relation BetweenTypes of Wishes and Types of Gratitude

More 7- to 10-year-olds expressed hedonistic wishes (82, 53.6%) than wishes for self well-being (57, 37.3%) or social well-being (14, 9.2%)( Table 1 ). However, of those expressing hedonistic wishes only 19 (23%) expressed connective gratitude, whereas 8 of 14 (57%) who expressed wishes for social well-being did so. As for the 11- to 14-year-olds, the majority (85, 53.8%) expressed wishes for self well-being, followed by 40 (25.3%) who expressed hedonistic wishes and 33 (20.9%) who wished for social well-being. Of the 40 expressing hedonistic wishes, 13 (32%) expressed concrete gratitude and 16 (40%) expressed connective gratitude. By contrast, of the 33 in the older group who had wished for social well-being only 2 (6%) expressed concrete gratitude and 23 (70%) expressed connective gratitude. As expected, the participants at both ages who wished for self well-being were more evenly divided in their expression of concrete versus connective gratitude.

Discussion

The purpose of the present research was to examine age-related changes in the expression of three types of gratitude in youth, and the extent to which these types of gratitude were related to the participants' wishes. Verbal gratitude is the simplest form, and is found in children as young as 3 ( Becker & Smenner, 1986Becker JA, Smenner PC. (1986).The spontaneous use of thank you by preschoolers as a function of sex, socioeconomic status, and listener status.Language in Society15(4), 537-546.doi:10.1017/S0047404500012008
https://doi.org/10.1017/S004740450001200...
). It is difficult, however, to distinguish verbal gratitude from politeness. Concrete gratitude involves reciprocity in that children indicate that they should repay a favor, but, in egocentric fashion, they think that what they themselves value will also be valued by the benefactor. Connective gratitude is yet more sophisticated, and thus the most advanced, developmentally, because it requires that youth take into account the desires or needs of the benefactor. Only connective gratitude fully captures the moral sense described by Piaget (1954/1981) as the feeling that occurs when a person does not simply value the help or favor but the person who provided it.

As hypothesized, 7- to 10-year-oldswere most likely to express concrete gratitude whereas 11- to 14-year-oldswere most likely to express connective gratitude, with little age-group difference in the expression of verbal gratitude. There was, however, a good deal of individual variation. About one third of the younger group expressed connective gratitude, and a just over one quarter of the older group expressed concrete gratitude. Nonetheless, the findings provide evidence that with develop mentcomes a greater propensity to express gratitude in a way that involves forming or strengthening a bond with a benefactor.

We wanted to know the extent to which the participants' wishes varied by age. Our data support our hypothesis that younger children are more likely than those who are older to wish for things that benefit themselves fairly immediately and less likely to wish for things that are related to their own future well-being or the well-being of others. As with gratitude, there was a good deal of individual variability. Although a majority of the younger group's wishes were coded as hedonistic, more than a third wished for something that involved future-oriented self well-being and some wished for something that would benefit the wider community. There was a similar degree of variability in the older group, with more than half the participants wishing for something related to their own future well-being and the remainder being divided between those who expressed hedonistic wishes and those who wished for something to benefit others.

In examining the relation between wishes and expressions of gratitude, we found an inverse relation between hedonistic wishes and connective gratitude. Some scholars studying older adolescents and adults have reported an inverse relation between materialism and the expression of gratitude ( Froh, Emmons, Card, Bono, & Wilson, 2011Froh JJ, Emmons RA, Card NA, Bono G, Wilson JA. (2011).Gratitude and the reduced costs of materialism in adolescents.Journal of Happiness Studies12(2), 289-302.doi:10.1007/s10902-010-9195-9
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-010-9195-...
; Lambert, Fincham, Stilman, & Dean, 2009Lambert NM, Fincham FD, Stilman TF, Dean LR. (2009). More gratitude, less materialism: The mediating role of life satisfaction. The Journal of Positive Psychology4(1), 32- 4 2.doi:10.1080/17 439760802216311
https://doi.org/10.1080/1743976080221631...
). To the extent to which a desire for things that give immediate pleasure is one component of materialism, our findings suggest that the issue might be more complex, at least with children and younger adolescents. Those participants who wished for something that would immediately benefit themselves were just as likely as others to express gratitude, but it was concrete gratitude that did not involve taking another person's wishes, needs, or desires into account. By contrast, those who wished for something that would benefit the wider community were more likely to express the sort of gratitude that involves connections with others. Although the older group was more likely than those who were younger to show both connective gratitude and social well-being, this inverse relation between hedonistic wishes and connective gratitude was found equally in the younger and older age groups.

In the adult literature, gratitude is linked to many indices of well-being. It is thus surprising that so little attention has been paid to the development of this positive characteristic. From a developmental perspective it is important to consider the implications of the existence of different types of gratitude. Saying "thank you" may be politeness and concrete gratitude may be a mark of a child's egocentric thinking whereas connective gratitude fulfills the required aspects of gratitude as defined by philosophers and psychologists. Specifically, connective gratitude encompasses both pleasure in the gift or help received and a desire to repay with something that is of benefit or pleasure to the benefactor. As its name suggests, connective gratitude is most likely to build or strengthen connections between benefactor and beneficiary. The vast majority of research on gratitude conducted with older adolescents and adults has focused on how much individuals express gratitude, regardless of the type of gratitude expressed or the source of the feeling of gratitude. Further research is needed to examine variations in the types of gratitude expressed across a range of contexts and situations.

Future studies will also need to go further and assess the development of gratitude longitudinally as well as examining precursors to individual differences in the expression of gratitude. No studies have so far examined ways in which parents encourage their children to feel and express gratitude. Similarly, more work is needed to understand the broader social contexts of social class, ethnicity, and culture, and the ways in which these contexts influence the development of gratitude. We had not expected to find racial/ethnic variations in the types of wishes expressed, yet we found more African American youths to express wishes for self well-being and fewer to express wishes for social well-being as compared with European American youths. Although our results are similar to those reported from Brazil by Freitas et al. (2011)Freitas LBL, Pieta MAM, Tudge JRH. (2011). Beyond politeness: The expression of gratitude in children and adolescents. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica24(4), 757-764.doi:10.1590/S0102-79722011000400016
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7972201100...
, more work is needed to determine whether these findings hold in other samples both within and outside the United States and, if so, to understand the source of these differences. Clearly, with data only collected from one part of the United States we would not wish to generalize our findings to other regions of the country or to other parts of the world ( Tudge & Freitas, 2012Tudge JRH, Freitas LBL. (2012). Internationalization, globalization and culture. Psicologia&Sociedade24 (3), 5 47-556.doi:10.1590/S0102-71822012000300008
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-7182201200...
). We should also note that 44% of the permission letters sent home with the students did not come back to the school. This is not necessarily indicative of a high refusal rate; of those letters that came back (classrooms were promised $2 for each letter returned, whether parents granted permission or not), only16% indicated refusal. It is possible that the letters were simply lost en route . Unfortunately, we were unable to collect data on the socioeconomic backgrounds of the participants in our sample, but understanding the links with social class must be a priority for future work in this area. Moreover, research is needed to elucidate how gratitude develops in societies (and in cultural groups) in which parents are more closely connected with community and value relatedness as much as or more than autonomy ( Kağitçibaşi, 2012Kağitçibaşi C. (2012). Sociocultural change and integrative syntheses in human development: Autonomous-related self and social-cognitive competence. Child Development Perspectives6(1), 5-11.doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00173.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011...
).

The connection we found between wishes and gratitude is an interesting one, and something that needs further exploration. The data presented here are correlational, and given that the question regarding wishes preceded the question about gratitude, it might be that youths who have just expressed a wish about others' well-being are more ready to express connective gratitude than are those who think about their own immediate desires. Nonetheless, encouraging youth to feel and express connective gratitude might not only help strengthen ties with others and with the broader community but may also reduce the desire to satisfy hedonistic wishes. Regardless, it might be helpful for parents and teachers to encourage their children to express their gratitude not so much for the gif tor help they received but to the person providing it, thereby supporting the development of connective gratitude. Similarly, these findings should be helpful to professionals seeking ways to encourage youth well-being and connection with others in the community.

Acknowledgement

We would like to express our gratitude to the youth who participated in this study

  • Algoe SB. (2012). Find, remind, and bind: The functions of gratitude in everyday relationships. Social and Personality Psychology Compass6(6), 455-469. doi:10.1111/lj.1751-9004.2012.00439.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/lj.1751-9004.2012.00439.x
  • Algoe SB, Fredrickson BL, Gable SL. (2013). The social functions of the emotion of gratitude via expression. Emotion13(4), 605-609. doi:10.1037/a0032701
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032701
  • Baumgarten-Tramer F. (1938)."Gratefulness" in children and young people. Journal of Genetic Psychology53, 53-66.
  • Becker JA, Smenner PC. (1986).The spontaneous use of thank you by preschoolers as a function of sex, socioeconomic status, and listener status.Language in Society15(4), 537-546.doi:10.1017/S0047404500012008
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/S0047404500012008
  • Bonnie KE, de Waal FBM . (2004). Primate social reciprocity and the origin of gratitude. In RA Emmons, ME McCullough (Eds.), The psychology of gratitude (pp. 2 13 -229). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Bono G, Froh J. (2009). Gratitude in school: Benefits to students and schools. In R Gilman, ES Huebner, MJ Furlong (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology in schools (pp. 77-88) New York, NY: Routledge.
  • Chaplin LN, John DR. (2007).Growing up in a material world: Age differences in materialism in children and adolescents.Journal of Consumer Research34(4), 480-493.
  • Emmons RA, Mishra A. (2012). Why gratitude enhances well-being: What we know, what we need to know. In KM Sheldon, TB Kashdan, MF Steger (Eds.),Designing the future of positive psychology: Taking stock and moving forward (pp. 248-264). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Freitas LBL, O'Brien M, Nelson JA, Marcovitch S. (2012).A compreensão da gratidão e teoria da mente em crianças de 5 anos [ The understanding of gratitude and theory of mind in 5-year-olds] .Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica25(2), 330-338.doi:10.1590/S0102-79722012000200015
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722012000200015
  • Freitas LBL, Pieta MAM, Tudge JRH. (2011). Beyond politeness: The expression of gratitude in children and adolescents. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica24(4), 757-764.doi:10.1590/S0102-79722011000400016
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722011000400016
  • Freitas LBL, Tudge JRH, McConnell T. (2008). The wishes and gratitude survey (Adapted from Baumgarten-Tramer, 1938). Greensboro, NC. Unpublished questionnaire.
  • Froh JJ, Bono G, Emmons R. (2010). Being grateful is beyond good manners: Gratitude and motivation to contribute to society among early adolescents. Motivation and, Emotion 34(2), 144-157.doi:10.1007/s11031-010-9163-z
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-010-9163-z
  • Froh JJ, Bono G, Fan J, Emmons RA, Henderson K, Harris C, Wood AM (2014). Nice thinking! An educational intervention that teaches children to think gratefully.School Psychology Review43(2), 13 2-152.
  • Froh JJ, Emmons RA, Card NA, Bono G, Wilson JA. (2011).Gratitude and the reduced costs of materialism in adolescents.Journal of Happiness Studies12(2), 289-302.doi:10.1007/s10902-010-9195-9
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-010-9195-9
  • Froh JJ, Yurkewicz C, Kashdan TB. (2009). Gratitude and subjective well-being in early adolescence: Examining gender differences. Journal of Adolescence32(3), 633-650.doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.06.006
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.06.006
  • Goldberg ME, Gorn GJ, Peracchio LA, Bamossy G. (2003).Understanding materialism among youth.Journal of Consumer Psychology13 (3), 278-288.doi:10.1207/S15327663JCP 13 03_09
    » https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327663JCP1303_09
  • Gordon AK, Musher-Eizenman DR, Holub SC, Dalrymple J. (2004). What are children thankful for? An archival analysis of gratitude before and after the attacks of September 11.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology25(5), 541-553.doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2004.08.004
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2004.08.004
  • Gulliford L, Morgan B, Kristjánsson K. (20 13 ).Recent work on the concept of gratitude in philosophy and psychology.Journal of Value Inquiry47(3), 285-317.doi:10.1007/s10790-0 13 -9387-8
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10790-013-9387-8
  • Hardy SA, Walker LJ, Olsen JA, Skalski JE, Basinger JC. (2011). Adolescent naturalistic conceptions of moral maturity.Social Development20(3), 562-586.doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2010.00590.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2010.00590.x
  • Kağitçibaşi C. (2012). Sociocultural change and integrative syntheses in human development: Autonomous-related self and social-cognitive competence. Child Development Perspectives6(1), 5-11.doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00173.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00173.x
  • Kashdan TB, Mishra A, Breen WE, Froh JJ. (2009). Gender differences in gratitude: Examining appraisals, narratives, the willingness to express emotions, and changes in psychological needs. Journal of Personality77(3), 691-730.doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00562.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00562.x
  • Kasser T. (2005).Frugality, generosity, and materialism in children and adolescents. In KA Moore, LH Lippman (Eds.), What do children need to flourish?Conceptualizing and measuring indicators of positive development (pp. 357-373). New York, NY: Springer.
  • Kawashima RA, Martins RA. (2015). The absence of generosity and obedience to authority: Judgments of teachers and students from kindergarten. Paidéia (Ribeirão Preto)25(60), 115-123. doi:10.1590/1982-43272560201514
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-43272560201514
  • Kerr SL, O'Donovan A, Pepping CA. (2015). Can gratitude and kindness interventions enhance well-being in a clinical sample? Journal of Happiness Studies16(1), 17-36.doi:10.1007/s10902-0 13 -9492-1
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-013-9492-1
  • Lambert NM, Fincham FD, Stilman TF, Dean LR. (2009). More gratitude, less materialism: The mediating role of life satisfaction. The Journal of Positive Psychology4(1), 32- 4 2.doi:10.1080/17 439760802216311
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760802216311
  • McConnell T. (1993).Gratitude. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press.
  • McConnell T. (20 13 ).Gratitude.In H LaFollette (Ed.), International encyclopedia of ethics(Vol., 4 pp. 2220-2229).Malden MA: Wiley-Blackwell.
  • McCullough ME, Emmons RA, Tsang J-A. (2002). The grateful disposition: A conceptual and empirical topography. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology82(1), 112-127.doi:10.1037//0022-351 4 .82.1.112
    » https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.82.1.112
  • McCullough ME, Kimeldorf MB, Cohen AD. (2008).An adaptation for altruism?The social causes, social effects, and social evolution of gratitude.Current Directions in Psychological Science17(4), 281-285.doi:10.1111/j.1 467-8721.2008.00590.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00590.x
  • Nelson JA, Freitas LBL, O'Brien M, Calkins S D, Leerkes EM, Marcovitch S. (20 13 ). Preschool-aged children's understanding of gratitude: Relations with emotion and mental state knowledge. British Journal of Developmental Psychology31(1), 4 2-56doi:10.1111/j.20 4 4-835X.2012.02077.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2012.02077.x
  • Paludo SS. (201 4 ).As emoções no universo moral dos adolescentes [ Emotion s in the moral universe of adolescents]. In LF Habigzang, E Diniz, SH Koller (Orgs.), Trabalhando com adolescentes: Teoria e intervençãopsicológica[Working with adolescents: Psychological theory and intervention](pp. 16 4 -179). Porto Alegre, RS: Artmed.
  • Piaget J. (1981). Intelligence and affectivity: Their relationship during child development. Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews. (Original work published 195 4 ).
  • Rava PGS, Freitas LBL. (20 13 ). Gratidão e sentimento de obrigatoriedade na infânciaGratitudeandthe feeling ofobligation in childhood. Psico-USF18(3), 383-39 4 .doi:10.1590/S1 413 -827120 13 000300005
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-82712013000300005
  • Tudge JRH, Freitas LBL. (2012). Internationalization, globalization and culture. Psicologia&Sociedade24 (3), 5 47-556.doi:10.1590/S0102-71822012000300008
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-71822012000300008
  • Weiner B, Graham S. (1988). Understanding the motivational role of affect: Life-span research from an attributional perspective.Cognition and, Emotion 3(4), 4 01- 4 19.doi:10.1080/02699938908 41271 4
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/02699938908412714
  • Wood AM, Froh JJ, Geraghty AWA. (2010). Gratitude and well-being: A review and theoretical integration. Clinical Psychology Review30(7), 890-905.doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.005
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.005
  • The first author received support from the Coordination for the Improvement of Higher Education Personnel (CAPES - Grant no. BEX18344/12-7) and The John Templeton Foundation (Grant no. 43510) ; the second author was supported by the National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq - Grant no. 301714/2012-6).

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Sep-Dec 2015

History

  • Received
    17 Oct 2014
  • Reviewed
    22 Apr 2015
  • Accepted
    27 Apr 2015
Universidade de São Paulo, Faculdade de Filosofia Ciências e Letras de Ribeirão Preto, Programa de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia Av.Bandeirantes 3900 - Monte Alegre, 14040-901 Ribeirão Preto - São Paulo - Brasil, Tel.: (55 16) 3315-3829 - Ribeirão Preto - SP - Brazil
E-mail: paideia@usp.br