Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Psychometric properties of empathy questionnaire for Spanish adolescents

Abstract

The aim of this study was to adapt and test the empathy questionnaire in Spanish adolescents (N = 701, age = 13.47 years). The study involved two different strands: (1) the questionnaire was translated into Spanish, and its internal consistency, factorial structure and convergent validity were assessed; (2) the questionnaire was subject to confirmatory factor analysis.

The results of the confirmatory factor analysis show that the questionnaire's factors present an aggregate variance of 58.588%, which suggests that the questionnaire is a valid tool to represent affective empathy, cognitive empathy and empathic concern. On the other hand, the confirmatory factor analysis confirmed the sustainability of the model, which comprises three identified factors and twelve items. The empathy questionnaire is easy to understand and can be completed in a short time, so it is considered a useful tool to assess empathy in Spanish adolescents. The results are discussed in the context of theoretical accounts of empathy.

Keywords:
Empathy; Assessment; Adolescents

Background

Empathy is the ability to understand other people's minds, to feel emotions other than our own and to respond with concern, goodwill and care for other people's hardship. Generally, it is understood as the comprehension of the emotional state of others (Cohen & Strayer, 1996Cohen, D., & Strayer, J. (1996). Empathy in conduct-disordered and comparison youth. Developmental Psychology, 32, 988-998.). Empathy (Vossen, Piotrowski, & Valkenburg, 2015Vossen, H. G. M., Piotrowski, J. T., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2015). Development of the adolescent measure of empathy and sympathy. Personality and individual Differences, 74, 66-71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.040.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.0...
) comprises two interrelated components: affective empathy (the ability to experience other people's feelings) and cognitive empathy (the ability to understand other people's feelings). In childhood, low empathy is related to poor relationships with peers, hostility and intimidation. In adolescents, low empathy results in aggression and antisocial behaviour. In adulthood, low empathy leads to child abuse, violence and psychopathy. Conversely, high empathy is related to good social skills, the ability to solve problems and pro-social behaviour; among both children and adults, highly empathetic people have greater chances to share resources, help those in need and care for others (Cuff, Brown, Taylor, & Howat, 2014Cuff, B. M. P., Brown, S. J., Taylor, L., & Howat, D. J. (2014). Empathy: a review of the concept. Emotion Review, 8(2), 144-153. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073914558466.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073914558466...
; Mitsopoulou & Giovazolias, 2015Mitsopoulou, E., & Giovazolias, T. (2015). Personality traits, empathy and bullying behavior: a meta-analytic approach. Agression and Violent Behavior, 21, 61-72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2015.01.007.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2015.01.00...
; Williams, O'Driscoll, & Moore, 2014Williams, A., O’Driscoll, K., & Moore, C. (2014). The influence of empathic concern on prosocial behavior in children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 425. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00425.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00425...
).

Affective empathy, cognitive empathy and empathic concern

Empathy plays a key role in the development of social behaviour. Initially, empathy was conceptualised from an emotional perspective and was understood as a vicarious emotional response to the emotions perceived in others. Later, it was understood that this one-dimensional approach to empathy erroneously disregarded the role of cognition. Currently, empathy is considered a multidimensional concept with both emotional and cognitive components (Vossen et al., 2015Vossen, H. G. M., Piotrowski, J. T., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2015). Development of the adolescent measure of empathy and sympathy. Personality and individual Differences, 74, 66-71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.040.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.0...
).

Based on this, it is important to distinguish between the different components of empathy: (1) Cognitive empathy refers to the ability to adopt the point of view of others and to understand their emotions and feelings. It involves “putting” oneself in the place of others, without judging them from our perspective. For instance, a student understands that one of his classmates likes having a different haircut and dressing in different clothes. (2) Emotional empathy involves feeling what other people feel. For instance, if a student expresses his suffering, his classmates feel it so deeply that they end up crying in response to his ordeal. (3) Empathic or sympathetic concern refers to the ability to understand when someone else needs our help, and this help is offered spontaneously and unconditionally. A student offers his support to a socially isolated classmate to help him overcome this situation.

Research to date has shown that people with high empathy are more prone to develop pro-social and altruistic behaviour (Cuff, Brown, Taylor, & Howat, 2014Cuff, B. M. P., Brown, S. J., Taylor, L., & Howat, D. J. (2014). Empathy: a review of the concept. Emotion Review, 8(2), 144-153. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073914558466.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073914558466...
; Telle & Pfister, 2014Telle, N. T., & Pfister, H. R. (2014). Positive empathy and prosocial behavior: a neglected link. Emotion Review, 8(2), 154-163. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073915586817.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073915586817...
), whereas individuals with low empathy tend to be aggressive (Garaigordobil, Martínez, & Aliri, 2013Garaigordobil, M., Martínez, V., & Aliri, J. (2013). Autoestima, empatía y conducta agresiva en adolescentes víctimas de bullying presencial. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education, 3(1), 29-40. https://doi.org/10.1989/ejihpe.v3i1.21.
https://doi.org/10.1989/ejihpe.v3i1.21...
; Gutiérrez, Escartí, & Pascual, 2011Gutiérrez, M., Escartí, A., & Pascual, C. (2011). Relaciones entre empatía, conducta prosocial, agresividad, autoeficacia y responsabilidad personal y social. Psicothema, 23(1), 13-19.; Winter, Spengler, Bermpohl, Singer, & Kanske, 2017Winter, K., Spengler, S., Bermpohl, F., Singer, T., & Kanske, P. (2017). Social cognition in aggressive offenders: Impaired empathy, but intact theory of mind. Scientific Reports, 7, 670. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-00745-0.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-00745...
).

The aim of this study was to assess the use of the empathy questionnaire (Vossen et al., 2015Vossen, H. G. M., Piotrowski, J. T., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2015). Development of the adolescent measure of empathy and sympathy. Personality and individual Differences, 74, 66-71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.040.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.0...
) in Spanish adolescents, providing Spanish investigators with a hitherto non-existing tool to measure empathy in members of this age group.

Study 1 involved the translation of the questionnaire into Spanish, and the assessment of the questionnaire's internal consistency, factorial structure and convergent validity. In study 2, the confirmatory factor analysis of the scale was carried out. Following the questionnaire's theoretical background, the working hypothesis was that the questionnaire follows a three-factor structure, which allows for the assessment of affective empathy, cognitive empathy and empathic concern. The core hypothesis, therefore, was that the questionnaire is a valid tool to measure empathy in Spanish adolescents.

Currently, there are several scales available to measure empathy: the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI; Davis, 1980Davis, M.H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85, 1-17.); the Empathy Index for Children and Adolescents (ACEI; Bryant, 1982Bryant, B.K. (1982). An index of empathy for children and adolescents Child Development, 413-425.); and the Children's Behavior Questionnaire (CBQ; Rothbart, Ahadi, & Hershey, 1994Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Hershey, K. L. (1994). Temperament and social behavior in childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40(1), 21-39.) with its Empathy Subscale and the Basic Empathy Scale (Jolliffe & Farrington, 2006Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D. P. (2006). Development and validation of the basic empathy scale. Journal of Adolescence, 29(4), 589-611. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.010.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.20...
). Some of these scales do not distinguish between the affective and cognitive component of empathy, considering empathy as a unique construction. In addition, the wording of the elements is sometimes ambiguous. The empathy scale of Vossen et al. (2015)Vossen, H. G. M., Piotrowski, J. T., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2015). Development of the adolescent measure of empathy and sympathy. Personality and individual Differences, 74, 66-71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.040.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.0...
has focused its efforts on guaranteeing to solve these difficulties, ensuring that the elements are clear and unambiguous.

The objective of this study is to develop a validated measure of empathy for Spanish adolescents and to address the aforementioned limitations of the existing scales.

Method

Study 1

For the translation of the questionnaire into Spanish, we followed the back-translation method (Muñiz, Elosua, & Hambleton, 2013Muñiz, J., Elosua, P., & Hambleton, R. K. (2013). International Test Commission. Guidelines for test translation and adaptation, (2nd ed.,). https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema/2013.24.
https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema/2013....
), which involves the following steps:

  1. The original English questionnaire was translated into Spanish by two bilingual persons with experience in the field of psychology. These translations were discussed by the research team, and a first draft was produced.

  2. A psychologist with work experience in English-speaking countries assessed the conceptual equivalence, clarity and intuitiveness of the expressions and answer contained in the first draft, suggesting corrections that led to the second draft.

  3. This second draft was presented to experts in the field of empathy.

  4. The second Spanish draft was back-translated into English by a bilingual native-English speaker professional translator to analyse and verify the correspondence of both.

  5. A pilot test with 50 respondents was carried out to assess comprehensibility, response time, clarity of questions and suitability of answers. No changes in the content of the questionnaire were necessary.

  6. Following this test, the research team developed a third Spanish version of the questionnaire which was to be presented to a sample of Spanish adolescents.

Study 2

Using the AMOS v.24 statistical program, a structural equation model was developed to validate and quantify the causal relationships between the questionnaire's items and questions. This technique combines factor analysis with linear regression to test the degree of fit of observed data to a hypothesised model and expressed by means of a path diagram. As a result, it provides the values belonging to each relationship and, more importantly, a statistic that expresses the degree to which the data fit the proposed model, confirming its validity. Following Batista and Coenders (2000)Batista, J. M., & Coenders, G. (2000). Modelos de ecuaciones estructurales. Madrid: La Muralla. and Rhemtulla, Brosseau-Liard, and Savalei (2012)Rhemtulla, M., Brosseau-Liard, P. É., & Savalei, V. (2012). When can categorical variables be treated as continuous? A comparison of robust continuous and categorical SEM estimation methods under suboptimal conditions. Psychological Methods, 17(3), 354. based on the number of variables, we used the maximum likelihood estimation method, rather than the weighted least squares method.

Participants

The population of samples included 701 subjects: 343 men (48.93%) and 358 women (51.07%); the average age of participants was 13.47, ranging from 12 to 18, with a standard deviation of 1.796. The first study included 337 respondents (166 men and 171 women) and the second 364 respondents (177 men and 187 women). Both samples were homogeneous. All respondents were volunteers, and parents and children signed an informed consent form; all the guidelines established by the Declaration of Helsinki and ethical criteria for research with humans were followed. The answers were treated anonymously. Representativeness estimates yielded a 95% confidence level and a 5% sampling error, and it was concluded that the final results are representative of the province of Zaragoza. The study was designed as an instrumental study (Ato, López, & Benavente, 2013Ato, M., López, J. J., & Benavente, A. (2013). Un sistema de clasificación de los diseños de investigación en psicología. Anales de Psicología, 29(3), 1038-1059. https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.293.178511.
https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.293.178...
).

Instruments

Empathy questionnaire (Vossen et al., 2015Vossen, H. G. M., Piotrowski, J. T., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2015). Development of the adolescent measure of empathy and sympathy. Personality and individual Differences, 74, 66-71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.040.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.0...
)

It is a 12-item scale that consists of three dimensions with four items each: cognitive empathy, affective empathy and empathic concern. The respondents must read a statement and answer on a 5-point Likert scale divided as follows: (1) strongly disagree, (2) disagree, (3) neutral, (4) agree and (5) strongly agree. Four items were generated to measure affective empathy (e.g. “When a friend is scared, I feel afraid”), 6 were generated to measure cognitive empathy (e.g. “I can often understand how people are feeling even before they tell me”) and 4 were generated to measure sympathy (e.g. “I feel sorry for someone who is treated unfairly”). Permission was sought from the authors of questionnaire to use it for research purposes. The questionnaire was translated into Spanish following the back-translation method (Muñiz et al., 2013Muñiz, J., Elosua, P., & Hambleton, R. K. (2013). International Test Commission. Guidelines for test translation and adaptation, (2nd ed.,). https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema/2013.24.
https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema/2013....
).

Interpersonal Reactivity Index (Davis, 1980Davis, M.H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85, 1-17.)

For the study, the Spanish version of IRI was used (Pérez-Albéniz, de Paúl, Etxeberría, Montes, & Torres, 2003Pérez-Albéniz, A., de Paúl, J., Etxeberría, J., Montes, M. P., & Torres, E. (2003). Adaptación de Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) al español. Psicothema, 15(2), 267-272.).

IRI is a self-report questionnaire used to measure empathy, comprising 28 statements and 5-point Likert scale responses ranging from 0 (it does not describe me well) to 4 (it describes me very well). The questionnaire is divided into four independent dimensions, with seven items each: (1) fantasy (tendency of subjects to identify with fictional characters, such as book and movie characters), (2) perspective (tendency or ability of subjects to adopt the perspective or point of view of others), (3) empathic concern (feelings of sympathy, compassion and concern for others) and (4) personal anguish (feelings of anxiety and discomfort caused by the negative experiences suffered by others). For this study, the perspective and empathic concern dimensions were selected, which are often used to measure the affective and cognitive components of empathy, respectively.

Procedure

In order to recruit the participants, schools were contacted by phone, and after they agreed to participate, a list of centres was drafted. When the questionnaires were handed out, the participants were informed about the aim of the study, and the importance of all items being answered was stressed, in addition to the minimum task instructions, and was highlighted, as this could influence their responses to the questionnaires. The respondents, who read the questionnaires silently, were given 45 min to complete the questionnaires and sign the informed consent form. Every time, it was emphasised that the information provided was anonymous and confidential. All participants, in addition to parents in the case of minors (in Spain, minors are up to 18 years old), signed the informed consent form. The data was collected between October and November 2018.

Analysis of the data

The statistical analysis of the data was carried out with the SPSS v.26.0 program. The data was initially subject to normality and variance tests, and the use of parametric techniques was selected for further analysis. In all cases, we worked at the lowest possible level of significance. Bilateral tests were performed. For two-group hypothesis testing, we used Student's t distribution. Cronbach's alpha was used to test the reliability of the scale. The structure of the scale was tested by means of an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) to explore the factorial structure of the data. Since the data were normally distributed and the factors were expected to be correlated, a principal component analysis was used with a varimax rotation method.

Finally, confirmatory factor analysis was carried out with a model of structural equations, using the AMOS v.24 statistical program.

Results

The first study included 337 respondents (166 men and 171 women). Empathy scores (Table 1) were higher in women in all three factors under analysis (cognitive empathy, affective empathy and empathic concern), with near-average scores (Cohen's d) in all variables.

Table 1
Empathy questionnaire descriptive items

The results were organised according to the different stages in our research programme. The results of study 1 included construct validity, internal consistency analysis, convergence analysis and exploratory factor analysis (EFA), and those of study 2 included confirmatory factor analysis (CFA).

Study 1

Construct validity

The aim of this step is to validate the empathy questionnaire (Vossen et al., 2015Vossen, H. G. M., Piotrowski, J. T., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2015). Development of the adolescent measure of empathy and sympathy. Personality and individual Differences, 74, 66-71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.040.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.0...
), following its translation.

After testing the viability of factor analysis, we used a principal component analysis with a varimax rotation: the correlation matrix showed a high number of correlations (87.9%) with a value over .30 (coefficient determination of .002), while Bartlett's test of sphericity showed that the variables were not independent (Bartlett test = 2870.823, p < .001). The Kaiser-Meyer Adequacy (KMO) test yielded a result of .870, which suggests that pairwise variable correlations are nearly entirely explained by the other variables. All the values yielded by the Measures of Sampling Adequacy (MSA) test were above .80. These results show the suitability of the correlation matrix for factor analysis. As shown in Table 2, after assigning an item to all factors with a load factor of .40 or more, three values with an eigenvalue of 1 or above were obtained, explaining 58.588% of total variance. In this section, we would highlight the good factor load obtained. The greatest saturation was associated to affective empathy (26.352%), followed by factor of cognitive empathy (22.066%), and empathic concern (10.171%).

Table 2
Standardised factors of the empathy questionnaire

Table 2 shows the matrix of components resulting from the principal component analysis. The table shows the factors and their associated questionnaire items, as well as the saturation values. For comparison purposes, the data model was considered adequate if the chi-square and degrees of freedom values were 3 or below (Hu & Bentler, 1999Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 1-55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118.
https://doi.org/10.1080/1070551990954011...
). Our scales yielded values below 3, showing that they are well adjusted and are internally consistent.

Convergence analysis

The correlations yielded by the empathy questionnaire were compared with those obtained with the IRI's perspective taking and empathic concern values (Table 3). The dimensions of the empathy questionnaire showed statistically significant correlations with the other variables. The correlations with personality factors were negative, whereas concerning the two IRI scales, affective empathy yielded lower results than cognitive empathy and empathic concern.

Table 3
Correlations with dispositional empathy (IRI)

Reliability

In order to test the reliability of the questionnaire, Cronbach's coefficient was used to calculate internal consistency, because this test has not been translated into Spanish, and we wanted to test our version. The results to all of the questionnaire's items yielded α results (Table 4) of .80 or above, and it can thus be assumed that the items measure the same construct and are highly correlated. In terms of reliability, the questionnaire yielded a high score with .872. Cronbach's alpha coefficients of the total scale (total = .872; cognitive empathy = .786; affective empathy = .744; empathic concern = .750) were moderately high, which shows that the different items in the questionnaire are highly consistent.

Table 4
Internal consistency of the factors in the empathy questionnaire

Study 2

Confirmatory factor analysis

Confirmatory factor analysis was used to test the internal structure of the data; this is the most appropriate statistical framework to evaluate the validity and reliability of each item, rather than the overall data, allowing the researcher to design and adapt the questionnaire (Batista & Coenders, 2000Batista, J. M., & Coenders, G. (2000). Modelos de ecuaciones estructurales. Madrid: La Muralla.).

Figure 1 shows the result of the CFA of the model developed in the exploratory study. This was carried out with structural equations following a maximum likelihood model. The results confirmed the validity of the model, since the results presented a sustainable model constituted by the three identified factors and a total of 12 items.

Fig. 1
Standardised solution of the CFA of the empathy scale

Concerning the adjustment of the model, the adjustment indexes were adequate, which confirms that the model proposed for the factor structure is sustainable: χ2(51) = 85.659; p < .001; χ2/gl = 1.679; CFI = .986; NFI = .969; TLI = .981; RMSEA = .032, 95% CI (.021-.044). The confirmatory factor analysis used a three-factor model. This is the a priori structure, which confirms that the results of the model are fully confirmatory. The results suggest that the model is reasonably well adjusted.

These results (Fig. 1) indicate that the model is optimally adjusted to the data and that the questionnaire is thus a valid tool.

Subsequently, the different factor components were analysed in order to ascertain the existence of significant correlations between the different dimensions: cognitive empathy, affective empathy and empathic concern. The correlations were as follows: r = .54 between cognitive empathy and affective empathy, r = .70 between cognitive empathy and empathic concern and r = .75 between affective empathy and empathic concern (p < .001; see Fig. 1).

Figure 1 shows the hierarchical model of the three factors.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to test the use of the empathy questionnaire to assess Spanish teenagers. The results confirm the validity of the questionnaire, which was subject to a strict translation process and to the analysis of its psychometric features. This process aimed to guarantee that the translated questionnaire was culturally and linguistically compatible with the original English version, as well as to ensure its internal consistency, validity and factorial structure.

The main results of the factor analysis indicate a three-factor structure and a high saturation value for all items, reflecting the internal consistency of the questionnaire (these results are similar to those yielded by the original questionnaire and previous translations) (Vossen et al., 2015Vossen, H. G. M., Piotrowski, J. T., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2015). Development of the adolescent measure of empathy and sympathy. Personality and individual Differences, 74, 66-71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.040.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.0...
; Wang, Wen, Fu, & Zeng, 2017Wang, Y., Wen, Z. L., Fu, Y. S., & Zeng, L. L. (2017). Psychometric properties of a Chinese version of the Measure of Empathy and Sympathy. Personality and individual Differences, 119, 168-174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.07.019.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.07.0...
; Zengin, Yalnizoglu-Çaka, & Çinar, 2018Zengin, H., Yalnizoglu-Çaka, S., & Çinar, N. (2018). Adaptation of the Adolescent Measure of Empathy and Sympathy (AMES) in Turkish: a validity and reliability study. Anatolian Journal of Psychiatry, 19(2), 184-191. https://doi.org/10.5455/apd.262004.
https://doi.org/10.5455/apd.262004...
).

These results show that the factors in the empathy scale present an aggregate variance of 58.588%. Results provide evidence for validity also in connection with other variables, showing that the different elements of the scale are highly consistent, which emphasises the model's reliability. Also, the exploration of the underlying factorial structure has shown that the reliability indexes apply to all three subscales, with results that mirror those obtained with the original scale. It can, therefore, be argued that the translation of the scale into Spanish does not undermine its value as a tool. The structure has been validated on three levels, which provides solid empirical evidence for the validity of the model. In addition, the factors of the scale were highly correlated with dispositional empathy factors, especially empathic concern.

This constitutes preliminary evidence for links between empathy questionnaire and IRI (personal reactivity index), which also emphasises the validity of the questionnaire, a tool which is both easy to apply and suitable for the purposes for which it was designed. One of the distinguishing features of this scale rests with its theoretical foundations, according to which empathy plays a crucial role in the way people deal with their emotions and the way they react according to capacity, motivation and context.

The model proposed for the factorial structure is sustainable, with three identified factors and twelve items overall. We think that these results show that the structure is solid, and reveal that the Spanish translation faithfully replicates the original theoretical structure.

The data yielded by the study with Spanish adolescents is satisfactory, fitting the underlying theoretical model and showing high internal consistency and validity. The study has also provided useful data concerning affective empathy, cognitive empathy and empathic concern in adolescents. These are key features for keeping good interpersonal relationships, and for efficiently appraising emotions and adjusting social and emotional conducts accordingly. Therefore, consideration should be given to applying instruments that assess interpersonal relationships and emotional regulation so that it is possible to observe the correlations between these and empathy. Furthermore, the relationship between empathy and affective communication capacity, respect for others, prosocial attitudes, emotions, etc. should be studied (Batchelder, Brosnan, & Ashwin, 2017Batchelder, L., Brosnan, M., & Ashwin, C. (2017). The development and validation of the Empathy Components Questionnaire (ECQ). PLoS ONE, 12(1), e0169185. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169185.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.016...
; Belacchi & Farina, 2012Belacchi, C., & Farina, E. (2012). Feeling and thinking of others: affective and cognitive empathy and emotion comprehension in prosocial/hostile preschoolers. Aggressive Behavior, 38, 150-165. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21415.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21415...
; Bisquerra, 2009Bisquerra, R. (2009). Psicopedagogía de las emociones. Madrid: Síntesis.; Cook et al., 2008Cook, C. R., Gresham, F. M., Kern, L., Barreras, R. B., Thornton, S., & Crews, S. D. (2008). Social skills training for secondary students with emotional and/or behavioral disorders: a review and analysis of the meta-analytic literature. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 16, 131-144. https://doi.org/10.1177/1063426608314541.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1063426608314541...
; Gini et al, 2007Gini, G., Albiero, P., Benelli, B., & Altoé, G. (2007). Does empathy predict adolescents’ bullying and defending behavior? Aggressive Behavior, 33, 467-476.; Kinnaman & Bellack, 2012Kinnaman, J. E. S., & Bellack, A. S. (2012). Social skills. In W. O’Donohue, & J. E. Fisher (Eds.), Cognitive Behavior Therapy: Core principles for practice. Nevada: Wiley.; Kotsou, Leys, & Fossion, 2018Kotsou, I., Leys, C., & Fossion, P. (2018). Acceptance alone is a better predictor of psychopathology and well-being than emotional competence, emotion regulation and mindfulness. Journal of Affective Disorders, 226, 142-145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.09.047.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.09.04...
; Lockwood et al, 2014Lockwood, P. L., Seararcardoso, A., & Viding, E. (2014). Emotion regulation moderates the association between empathy and prosocial behavior. PloS One, 9, 1051-1064.; Salavera, Usán and Jarie, 2017Salavera, C., Usán, P., & Jarie, L. (2017). Emotional intelligence and social skills on self-efficacy in secondary education students. Are there gender differences? Journal of Adolescence, 60, 39-46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2017.07.009.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.20...
; Salavera et al, 2020Salavera, C., Usán, P., Teruel, P., Antoñanzas, J. L. (2020). Eudaimonic Well-Being in Adolescents: The Role of Trait Emotional Intelligence and Personality. Sustainability, 12, 2742.; Thompson, 2000Thompson, E. (2000). Empathy and consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8, 1-32., Wai & Tiliopoulos, 2012Wai, M., & Tiliopoulos, N. (2012). The affective and cognitive empathic nature of the dark triad of personality. Personality & individual Differences, 52, 794-799.), what will benefit from the future use of the empathy scale in relation to these variables, as well as from deeper analyses and even a revaluation of the variables.

These results must be interpreted taking the limitations of the study into account. Although the sample is statistically relevant, the questionnaire should be applied to other population groups, in which the correlation of empathy and other variables may be greater. Lateral studies are also advisable to evaluate the evolution of empathy over time. In addition, empathy often appears in relation with other skills, for instance cognitive skills, and this relationship is worth exploring. Similar studies could also target special groups, such as adolescents with social anxiety or autism or with different empathy characteristics.

Conclusions

The main conclusion of the study is that the empathy questionnaire is a valid tool to assess empathy in adolescents. The questionnaire has shown to be a valuable psychometric tool, with high values of factorial validity and internal consistency; in addition, it is quick and easy to use. Although more research is needed, our results show that the empathy questionnaire is a valid tool to measure empathy in adolescents.

  • Funding
    This study was performed by Research Group OPIICS (S46_20R), University of Zaragoza (Zaragoza, Spain), and was supported by research funds provided by the Department of Science and Technology of the Government of Aragón (Spain) and the European Social Fund.
  • Availability of data and materials
    The authors do not wish to make the data available as it contains information that could identify specific individuals.
  • Ethics approval and consent to participate
    All users were informed of the aim of the study and gave their written consent to participate. The study was conducted in compliance with the Helsinki Declaration.
  • Consent for publication
    Not applicable
  • Publisher's Note
    Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Acknowledgements

Not applicable

    Abbreviations
  • AMES  Adolescent Measure of Empathy and Sympathy
  • CFA  Confirmatory factor analysis
  • EFA  Exploratory factor analysis
  • IRI  Interpersonal Reactivity Index
  • KMO  Kaiser-Meyer Adequacy
  • MSA  Measures of Sampling Adequacy

References

  • Ato, M., López, J. J., & Benavente, A. (2013). Un sistema de clasificación de los diseños de investigación en psicología. Anales de Psicología, 29(3), 1038-1059. https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.293.178511
    » https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.293.178511
  • Batchelder, L., Brosnan, M., & Ashwin, C. (2017). The development and validation of the Empathy Components Questionnaire (ECQ). PLoS ONE, 12(1), e0169185. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169185
    » https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169185
  • Batista, J. M., & Coenders, G. (2000). Modelos de ecuaciones estructurales Madrid: La Muralla.
  • Belacchi, C., & Farina, E. (2012). Feeling and thinking of others: affective and cognitive empathy and emotion comprehension in prosocial/hostile preschoolers. Aggressive Behavior, 38, 150-165. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21415
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21415
  • Bisquerra, R. (2009). Psicopedagogía de las emociones Madrid: Síntesis.
  • Bryant, B.K. (1982). An index of empathy for children and adolescents Child Development, 413-425.
  • Cohen, D., & Strayer, J. (1996). Empathy in conduct-disordered and comparison youth. Developmental Psychology, 32, 988-998.
  • Cook, C. R., Gresham, F. M., Kern, L., Barreras, R. B., Thornton, S., & Crews, S. D. (2008). Social skills training for secondary students with emotional and/or behavioral disorders: a review and analysis of the meta-analytic literature. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 16, 131-144. https://doi.org/10.1177/1063426608314541
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/1063426608314541
  • Cuff, B. M. P., Brown, S. J., Taylor, L., & Howat, D. J. (2014). Empathy: a review of the concept. Emotion Review, 8(2), 144-153. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073914558466
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073914558466
  • Davis, M.H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85, 1-17.
  • Garaigordobil, M., Martínez, V., & Aliri, J. (2013). Autoestima, empatía y conducta agresiva en adolescentes víctimas de bullying presencial. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education, 3(1), 29-40. https://doi.org/10.1989/ejihpe.v3i1.21
    » https://doi.org/10.1989/ejihpe.v3i1.21
  • Gini, G., Albiero, P., Benelli, B., & Altoé, G. (2007). Does empathy predict adolescents’ bullying and defending behavior? Aggressive Behavior, 33, 467-476.
  • Gutiérrez, M., Escartí, A., & Pascual, C. (2011). Relaciones entre empatía, conducta prosocial, agresividad, autoeficacia y responsabilidad personal y social. Psicothema, 23(1), 13-19.
  • Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 1-55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
  • Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D. P. (2006). Development and validation of the basic empathy scale. Journal of Adolescence, 29(4), 589-611. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.010
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.010
  • Kinnaman, J. E. S., & Bellack, A. S. (2012). Social skills. In W. O’Donohue, & J. E. Fisher (Eds.), Cognitive Behavior Therapy: Core principles for practice Nevada: Wiley.
  • Kotsou, I., Leys, C., & Fossion, P. (2018). Acceptance alone is a better predictor of psychopathology and well-being than emotional competence, emotion regulation and mindfulness. Journal of Affective Disorders, 226, 142-145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.09.047
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.09.047
  • Lockwood, P. L., Seararcardoso, A., & Viding, E. (2014). Emotion regulation moderates the association between empathy and prosocial behavior. PloS One, 9, 1051-1064.
  • Mitsopoulou, E., & Giovazolias, T. (2015). Personality traits, empathy and bullying behavior: a meta-analytic approach. Agression and Violent Behavior, 21, 61-72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2015.01.007
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2015.01.007
  • Muñiz, J., Elosua, P., & Hambleton, R. K. (2013). International Test Commission. Guidelines for test translation and adaptation, (2nd ed.,). https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema/2013.24
    » https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema/2013.24
  • Pérez-Albéniz, A., de Paúl, J., Etxeberría, J., Montes, M. P., & Torres, E. (2003). Adaptación de Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) al español. Psicothema, 15(2), 267-272.
  • Rhemtulla, M., Brosseau-Liard, P. É., & Savalei, V. (2012). When can categorical variables be treated as continuous? A comparison of robust continuous and categorical SEM estimation methods under suboptimal conditions. Psychological Methods, 17(3), 354.
  • Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Hershey, K. L. (1994). Temperament and social behavior in childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40(1), 21-39.
  • Salavera, C., Usán, P., & Jarie, L. (2017). Emotional intelligence and social skills on self-efficacy in secondary education students. Are there gender differences? Journal of Adolescence, 60, 39-46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2017.07.009
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2017.07.009
  • Salavera, C., Usán, P., Teruel, P., Antoñanzas, J. L. (2020). Eudaimonic Well-Being in Adolescents: The Role of Trait Emotional Intelligence and Personality. Sustainability, 12, 2742.
  • Telle, N. T., & Pfister, H. R. (2014). Positive empathy and prosocial behavior: a neglected link. Emotion Review, 8(2), 154-163. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073915586817
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073915586817
  • Thompson, E. (2000). Empathy and consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8, 1-32.
  • Vossen, H. G. M., Piotrowski, J. T., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2015). Development of the adolescent measure of empathy and sympathy. Personality and individual Differences, 74, 66-71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.040
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.040
  • Wai, M., & Tiliopoulos, N. (2012). The affective and cognitive empathic nature of the dark triad of personality. Personality & individual Differences, 52, 794-799.
  • Wang, Y., Wen, Z. L., Fu, Y. S., & Zeng, L. L. (2017). Psychometric properties of a Chinese version of the Measure of Empathy and Sympathy. Personality and individual Differences, 119, 168-174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.07.019
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.07.019
  • Williams, A., O’Driscoll, K., & Moore, C. (2014). The influence of empathic concern on prosocial behavior in children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 425. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00425
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00425
  • Winter, K., Spengler, S., Bermpohl, F., Singer, T., & Kanske, P. (2017). Social cognition in aggressive offenders: Impaired empathy, but intact theory of mind. Scientific Reports, 7, 670. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-00745-0
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-00745-0
  • Zengin, H., Yalnizoglu-Çaka, S., & Çinar, N. (2018). Adaptation of the Adolescent Measure of Empathy and Sympathy (AMES) in Turkish: a validity and reliability study. Anatolian Journal of Psychiatry, 19(2), 184-191. https://doi.org/10.5455/apd.262004
    » https://doi.org/10.5455/apd.262004

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    04 Dec 2020
  • Date of issue
    2020

History

  • Received
    24 Mar 2020
  • Accepted
    09 Sept 2020
  • Published
    31 Oct 2020
Curso de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia da Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul Rua Ramiro Barcelos, 2600 - sala 110, 90035-003 Porto Alegre RS - Brazil, Tel.: +55 51 3308-5691 - Porto Alegre - RS - Brazil
E-mail: prc@springeropen.com