Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Parents Dealing with the Expression of Sadness by their Children

Manejo dos Pais frente à Expressão de Tristeza dos Filhos

Proceder de los Padres frente a la Expresión de Tristeza de los Hijos

Abstract

Objectives:

Identifying sadness inductors on children according to parents' perception; checking strategies of emotion regulation used by parents before children's sadness; comparing the ways mothers and fathers deal with children's sadness.

Participants:

26 heterosexual couples (Average age = 37 years old), at least two children from six up to twelve years old, one boy and one girl.

Delineation:

collective case study.

Instruments:

General data questionnaire; Econômica Brasil qualification criteria; The questions were: "What usually upsets your child?"; "Tell me a situation which made your son sad."; "What did you do in this situation?"; stimulus-story.

Outcome:

fathers tend to punish sons and daughters when they are sad and replace something lost; mothers minimize it, i. e., reduce the importance of child's reaction, or the problem that caused it. Reaction centered in the problem was the most used by father and mother before children's sadness.

Keywords:
sadness; emotional regulation; children; parent practices

Resumo

Objetivos:

identificar eliciadores de tristeza nos filhos na percepção dos pais; verificar estratégias de regulação emocional usadas pelos pais frente à tristeza dos filhos; comparar como pai e mãe lidam com a tristeza dos filhos.

Participantes:

26 casais heterossexuais (M = 37 anos), pelo menos dois filhos de seis a doze anos, um de cada sexo.

Delineamento:

estudos de casos coletivos.

Instrumentos:

Questionário de Dados Gerais; Critério de Classificação Econômica Brasil; perguntas: "O que costuma deixar seu filho triste?"; "Conte uma situação em que seu filho ficou triste."; "O que você fez nessa situação?"; história-estímulo.

Resultados:

o pai tende a punir os filhos e filhas diante da tristeza e substituir algo perdido; as mães minimizam, ou seja, diminuem a importância da reação emocional da criança, ou do problema que a provocou. A reação centrada no problema foi a estratégia mais utilizada por pai e mãe frente à tristeza dos filhos.

Palavras-chave:
tristeza; regulação emocional; filhos

Resumen

Objetivos:

Identificar los inductores de tristeza en los hijos según la percepción de sus padres; verificar estrategias de regulación emocional usadas por los padres y también comparar como ellos se comportan en relación a la tristeza de sus hijos.

Participantes:

26 parejas heterosexuales, edad media de 37 años, casadas o en unión estable con dos o más hijos, con edad entre 6 y 12 años, uno de cada sexo.

Delineamiento:

estudios de casos colectivos.

Instrumentos:

Cuestionario de Datos Generales; Criterio de Clasificación Económica Brasil; guion de entrevista conteniendo tres preguntas: "¿Qué suele dejar a su hijo triste?" "Relate una situación en que su hijo quedó triste." "¿Qué hizo usted en esa situación?; y una historia de estímulo.

Resultados:

el padre tiende a castigar a los hijos frente a la reacción de tristeza y substituir algo perdido; ya las madres minimizan, o sea disminuyen la importancia de reacción emocional del niño, o del problema que la provocó. La reacción centrada en el problema fue la estrategia más usada por los padres frente a la tristeza de sus hijos.

Palabras clave:
tristeza; regulación emocional; hijos; prácticas parentales

Introduction

Emotion is fundamental to human development and functioning, and may be classified as positive or negative. Among negative emotions, there is sadness, which is a state of inhibition and decreased cognitive activity. Through this emotion it is shown that some desire was not carried out and/or it signals the existence of problems involving the individual (Melo, 2005Melo, A. I. M. T. d. (2005). Emoções no período escolar: Estratégias parentais face à expressão emocional e sintomas de internalização e externalização da criança (Dissertação de mestrado). Curso de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia Clínica, Universidade do Minho, Portugal. Retrieved from http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitstream/1822/4926/1/TESE%20MESTRADO%20ANA%20MELO.pdf
http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitst...
).

Sadness is usually elicited by adverse events, such as being rejected, losing something, or simply not getting what you want; evaluating an event as negative and themselves as powerless to reverse the outcome or change the situation. In childhood, for example, the parents' decision to divorce is characterized as a large inductor of that feeling. The experience of sadness may result in giving up on a goal or in developing a new plan; and even in avoiding contact with other people (Reis, Habigzang, & Sperb, 2015Reis, A. H., Habigzang, L. F., & Sperb, T. M. (2015). Emoções na infância e influência parental na regulação emocional infantil em uma perspectiva cognitivo-comportamental. In C. B. Neufeld, E. M. O. Falcone & B. Rangé (Ed.). PROCOGNITIVA Programa de atualização em Terapia Cognitivo-comportamental: ciclo 1, v. 4. Porto Alegre: Artmed Panamericana.). Sadness is a basic emotion, present in all individuals and also linked to the development of empathy, seek for social support, evaluation of possible inductors of the problem and aid in narrowing the interpersonal relationship (Melo, 2005Melo, A. I. M. T. d. (2005). Emoções no período escolar: Estratégias parentais face à expressão emocional e sintomas de internalização e externalização da criança (Dissertação de mestrado). Curso de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia Clínica, Universidade do Minho, Portugal. Retrieved from http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitstream/1822/4926/1/TESE%20MESTRADO%20ANA%20MELO.pdf
http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitst...
).

The parental response to the child's emotion is related to the development of children's emotional, social and interpersonal competence (Almeida, 2006Almeida, M. B. M. D. (2006). Socialização parental das emoções e a competência social da criança (Dissertação de mestrado). Instituto Superior de Psicologia Aplicada. Retrieved from http://repositorio.ispa.pt/bitstream/10400.12/291/1/DM%20ALME-MB1.pdf
http://repositorio.ispa.pt/bitstream/104...
; Denham, Basset, & Wyatt, 2010Denham, S. A., Bassett, H. H., & Wyatt, T. M. (2010). Gender differences in the socialization of preschooler's emotional competence. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development(128), 29-49. doi: 10.1002/cd.267
https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.267...
). One aspect of emotional competence is the regulation of emotions (RE), which consists of an extrinsic and intrinsic process responsible for monitoring, evaluation and alteration of emotional reactions (Thompson, 1991Thompson, R. (1991). Emotional regulation and emotional development. Educational Psychology Review3(4), 269-307. doi: 10.1007/BF01319934
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01319934...
). Several characteristics of the parents influence the socialization of child RE, among them the attachment style and past family experiences, stress levels, social support, mental health and parental beliefs about emotions. Child-related factors such as temperament, sex, stage of development and the child's behaviors in emotionally challenging situations also moderate the parental response to the child, making this, therefore, a two-way process (Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers, & Robinson, 2007Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development16(2), 361-388. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007...
). For example, children with high levels of frustration, impulsiveness, irritability, low self-control are more vulnerable to negative parental behaviors that, in turn, are predictors of the increase in these children's characteristics (Kiff, Lengua, & Zalewski, 2011Kiff, C., Lengua, L., & Zalewski, M. (2011). Nature and nurturing: Parenting in the context of child temperament. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review14(3), 251-301. doi: 10.1007/s10567-011-0093-4
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0093-...
).

According to (Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson, 2007Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development16(2), 361-388. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007...
), the influence of parents in the development of RE in children occurs from three complementary processes: observation for modeling, parental actions in response to the child's emotional and family emotional environment. The observation by modeling suggests that the child learns to regulate emotions themselves with family life, watching emotional displays of parents that elucidate what emotion is acceptable and expected, taking them as social reference on how they should react in similar situations.

The reaction of parents to the child's emotion and the strategies they teach the child to deal with emotions, for example, distracting the child from a negative event, or promoting a reassessment of the situation also influence the child's RE. Therefore, parents may consider the emotional expression of the child as an opportunity to teach them how to handle such experiences. In this case, they pay attention and are aware of the child's emotion, encourage them to identify and name the emotion, have empathy, validate the child's emotional state and help the child to solve problems related to inductors of emotion. On the other hand, parents may feel uncomfortable with the child's emotional response and disapprove of it (Morris et al., 2007Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development16(2), 361-388. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007...
).

Children that come from families with positive emotional expression and who accept the children's emotions tend to feel safe and free to express emotion, for they understand that their need will be met. Responsiveness to the child's emotions is positively associated with good self- esteem index, good school results and cognitive development, and lower incidence of behavior problems (Sanders & Morawska, 2011Sanders, M., & Morawska, A. (2011). Mudanças nos conhecimentos, em expectativas e atribuições disfuncionais, e na regulação emocional dos pais podem melhorar os resultados da criança? In R. E. Tremblay, M. Boivin & R. D. V. Peters (Eds.), Enciclopédia sobre o desenvolvimento na primeira infância (pp. 1-13). Montreal: Centre. Retrieved from http://www.enciclopedia-crianca.com/documents/Sanders-MorawskaPRTxp1.pdf
http://www.enciclopedia-crianca.com/docu...
). In turn, punitive parents can teach the child to avoid negative emotional demonstration, leading to inadequate expression of emotion (Gottman, Katz, & Hooven, 1996Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1996) Parental meta-emotion philosophy and the emotional life of families: Theoretical models and preliminary data. Journal of Family Psychology10(3), 243-268. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.10.3.243
https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.10.3.2...
; Katz, Maliken, & Stettler, 2012Katz, L. F., Maliken, A. C., & Stettler, N. M. (2012). Parental meta-emotion philosophy: A review of research and theoretical framework. Child Development Perspectives6(4), 417-422. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00244.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012...
; Morris et al., 2007Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development16(2), 361-388. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007...
). Ignoring the child's emotion is associated with poorer developmental outcome of emotion (Melo, 2005Melo, A. I. M. T. d. (2005). Emoções no período escolar: Estratégias parentais face à expressão emocional e sintomas de internalização e externalização da criança (Dissertação de mestrado). Curso de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia Clínica, Universidade do Minho, Portugal. Retrieved from http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitstream/1822/4926/1/TESE%20MESTRADO%20ANA%20MELO.pdf
http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitst...
). Lack of parental support for the child's emotion leads to lower coping with sorrow (Sanders, Zeman, Poon, & Miller, 2013Sanders, W., Zeman, J., Poon, J., & Miller, R. (2013). Child regulation of negative emotions and depressive symptoms: The moderating role of parental emotion socialization. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 1-14. doi: 10.1007/s10826-013-9850-y
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-013-9850-...
).

The emotional environment is constituted by the quality of the interpersonal relationship (involving attachment, relationship between parents and parenting style) and the amount of positive and negative emotion arising from family. When the emotional atmosphere is negative, the child can become reactive and emotionally insecure (Morris et al., 2007Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development16(2), 361-388. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007...
). Nevertheless, parental training for handling the emotions of the children has shown positive results (Wilson, Havighurst, & Harley, 2012Wilson, K. R., Havighurst, S. S., & Harley, A. E. (2012). Tuning in to kids: An effectiveness trial of a parenting program targeting emotion socialization of preschoolers. Journal of Family Psychology26(1), 56-65. doi: 10.1037/a0026480
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026480...
), indicating that the influence of parents on the child's RE can be adjusted.

The punitive or minimizing parental reaction can impair a child's ability to process emotional information (Eisenberg, Fabes, & Murphy, 1996Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Murphy, B. C. (1996). Parent's reactions to children's negative emotions: Relations to children's social competence and comforting behavior. Child Development67(5), 2227-2247. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01854.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996...
), and the low sensitivity of the mother has been correlated with a greater emotional deregulation of the child (Friedlmeier & Trommsdorff, 1999Friedlmeier, W., & Trommsdorff, G. (1999). Emotion regulation in early childhood: A cross-cultural comparison between german and japanese toddlers. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology30(6), 684-711. doi: 10.1177/0022022199030006002
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022199030006...
). Parents of aggressive children tend to be highly punitive and critics (Sanders & Morawska, 2011Sanders, M., & Morawska, A. (2011). Mudanças nos conhecimentos, em expectativas e atribuições disfuncionais, e na regulação emocional dos pais podem melhorar os resultados da criança? In R. E. Tremblay, M. Boivin & R. D. V. Peters (Eds.), Enciclopédia sobre o desenvolvimento na primeira infância (pp. 1-13). Montreal: Centre. Retrieved from http://www.enciclopedia-crianca.com/documents/Sanders-MorawskaPRTxp1.pdf
http://www.enciclopedia-crianca.com/docu...
); anguished parents who use harsh coping strategies such as aggressive behavior in response to negative emotion, have children who express the emotion intensely and who find it hard to behave in a socially responsible way (Fabes, Leonard, Kupanoff, & Martin, 2001Fabes, R. A., Leonard, S. A., Kupanoff, K., & Martin, C. L. (2001). Parental coping with children's negative emotions: Relations with children's emotional and social responding. Child Development72(3), 907-920. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00323
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00323...
).

Regarding the differences between father and mother to deal with their son's emotion, (Melo, 2005Melo, A. I. M. T. d. (2005). Emoções no período escolar: Estratégias parentais face à expressão emocional e sintomas de internalização e externalização da criança (Dissertação de mestrado). Curso de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia Clínica, Universidade do Minho, Portugal. Retrieved from http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitstream/1822/4926/1/TESE%20MESTRADO%20ANA%20MELO.pdf
http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitst...
), using the Coping with Children's Negative Emotions Scale (CCNES), which assesses parental response to negative emotions of the child, found that father and mother tended to react to the child's emotion more often with constructive strategy, but when considering the whole sample, fathers had more constructive reactions than mothers. This showed that the positive reaction of the father and mother is more frequent with internalizing emotions such as sadness, humiliation/shame or anxiety/fear, than with externalizing emotions such as anger. Another study conducted in China found that the mother had less aggressive strategies on the emotions of son and daughter, while their father acted more often with aggressive and punitive reaction. Such reactions were positively correlated with the child's aggressive behavior (Chang, Schwartz, Dodge, & McBride-Chang, 2003Chang, L., Schwartz, D., Dodge, K. A., & McBride-Chang, C. (2003). Harsh parenting in relation to child emotion regulation and aggression. Journal of Family Psychology17(4), 598-606. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.17.4.598
https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.17.4.5...
).

Also through CCNES scale, (Eisenberg, Fabes and Murphy, 1996Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Murphy, B. C. (1996). Parent's reactions to children's negative emotions: Relations to children's social competence and comforting behavior. Child Development67(5), 2227-2247. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01854.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996...
) found that, compared to the father, the mother showed more strategies to encourage the expression of negative feelings and validate this state; focusing on emotion, he made use of a strategy that helped the child feel better; and with emphasis on the problem, helped him cope with the inductor of sadness. This study concluded that the lack of child's social competence is linked to the reaction of minimization and punishment of parents. Accordingly, (Cassano, Perry-Parrish and Zeman, 2007Cassano, M., Perry-Parrish, C., & Zeman, J. (2007). Influence of gender on parental socialization of children's sadness regulation. Social Development16(2), 210-231. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00381.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007...
) found that both father and mother tend to respond to the child's sadness with behaviors that minimize emotion. However, minimization tends to decrease when the child becomes older.

The parental RE can also be distinguished taking into account the child's gender. In a study of children 3-4 years old, it was discovered that the mother manifests more about the child's emotional experience than their father. When discussing sad events, the father uses more emotional expressions with his daughter than with his son (Fivush, Brotman, Buckner, & Goodman, 2000Fivush, R., Brotman, M., Buckner, J., & Goodman, S. (2000). Gender differences in parent-child emotion narratives. Sex Roles42(3-4), 233-253. doi: 10.1023/A:1007091207068
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007091207068...
). Another survey revealed that both parents tended to have more negative reactions to the son than to the daughter, when facing the expression of these emotions (Melo, 2005Melo, A. I. M. T. d. (2005). Emoções no período escolar: Estratégias parentais face à expressão emocional e sintomas de internalização e externalização da criança (Dissertação de mestrado). Curso de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia Clínica, Universidade do Minho, Portugal. Retrieved from http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitstream/1822/4926/1/TESE%20MESTRADO%20ANA%20MELO.pdf
http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitst...
). Corroborating this finding, (Cassano, Perry-Parrish and Zeman, 2007Cassano, M., Perry-Parrish, C., & Zeman, J. (2007). Influence of gender on parental socialization of children's sadness regulation. Social Development16(2), 210-231. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00381.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007...
) noted that facing the child's sadness, expressive encouragement was more common for the girl than for the boy, and the mother was more likely to use this strategy than the father, especially with the older child. Still, the mother reported distress when facing her daughter's sadness and tended to encourage emotional expression, using a strategy directed to solving the problem.

(Cassano, Zeman and Sanders, 2014Cassano, M. C., Zeman, J. L., & Sanders, W. M. (2014). Responses to children's sadness: Mothers' and fathers' unique contributions and perceptions. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly60(1), 1-23. doi: 10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.60.1.0001
https://doi.org/10.13110/merrpalmquar198...
) evaluated the response of father and mother to the expression of sadness taking into account the sex of the children. From a measure of self-report, mother and father react to the sadness of the children in the same way, regardless of the sex of the child. However, when a spouse assessed the reaction of the other, the husband reported that his wife provided more support for the sons and his wife reported that her husband gave more support to the daughters. According to (Almeida, 2006Almeida, M. B. M. D. (2006). Socialização parental das emoções e a competência social da criança (Dissertação de mestrado). Instituto Superior de Psicologia Aplicada. Retrieved from http://repositorio.ispa.pt/bitstream/10400.12/291/1/DM%20ALME-MB1.pdf
http://repositorio.ispa.pt/bitstream/104...
), the father tends to minimize the negative emotion of the daughter and react more negatively against their emotion of anxiety/fear than when compared to the son.

Whereas parental response to children's emotional expression influences the child's socialization RE (Morris et al., 2007Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development16(2), 361-388. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007...
), it is important to conduct a survey about parenting and management of children's' emotion, that is, the second process proposed by (Morris et al., 2007Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development16(2), 361-388. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007...
). Moreover, as international studies are inconclusive as to differences between the father and mother responses to the emotions of the male and female children, it is relevant to see whether there are differences in Brazilian culture in these relations. We start with the hypothesis that perception and parental response to the child's expression of sadness varies depending on the gender of the parents (Eisenberg et al., 1996Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Murphy, B. C. (1996). Parent's reactions to children's negative emotions: Relations to children's social competence and comforting behavior. Child Development67(5), 2227-2247. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01854.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996...
) and the child (Cassano et al., 2007Cassano, M., Perry-Parrish, C., & Zeman, J. (2007). Influence of gender on parental socialization of children's sadness regulation. Social Development16(2), 210-231. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00381.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007...
; Melo, 2005Melo, A. I. M. T. d. (2005). Emoções no período escolar: Estratégias parentais face à expressão emocional e sintomas de internalização e externalização da criança (Dissertação de mestrado). Curso de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia Clínica, Universidade do Minho, Portugal. Retrieved from http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitstream/1822/4926/1/TESE%20MESTRADO%20ANA%20MELO.pdf
http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitst...
).

The research objective is to identify the inductors of sadness in children, in the perception of the parents; ascertain the RE strategy that parents use with the male and female child's expression of sadness and compare the way in which both parents deal individually with the child's expression of sadness.

Method

Participants

The participants were 26 heterosexual couples, married or in a civil union. The age of the mothers ranged from 27 to 45 years (M = 35.92, SD = 5.32) and fathers from 29 to 49 (M = 39.62, SD = 5.05). The average schooling of the total sample was 13.55 years, mothers 13.88 and fathers 13.21 years of study. The duration of relationship for the couples was 9-19 years (M = 13.92, SD = 3.23). Family income varied from R$ 1,100.00 to R$ 50,000.00 (M = 3,848.90, SD = 6,976.27), socioeconomic classes ranging from A1 to C2, according to the Brazilian Economic Classification Criterion (CCEB) (Brazilian Association in Research Studies [ABEP], 2014Associação Brasileira de Empresas de Pesquisa. (2014). Critério de classificação econômica do Brasil São Paulo: ABEP.).

The inclusion criterion sought couples with at least two children, these aged six to 12 years, one of each sex, both biological children of the couple; married or in a stable relationship and residing in the same home with the children. Participants were selected according to the chain-referral technique (snowball) (Goodman, 1961Goodman, L. A. (1961). Snowball sampling. Annals of Mathematical Statistics32(1), 148-170. Retrieved from http://projecteuclid.org/download/pdf_1/euclid.aoms/1177705148
http://projecteuclid.org/download/pdf_1/...
).

Instruments

To collect demographic data we used a General Data Questionnaire, which addressed education, age and profession of parents, marriage time, age of children and family composition, and also the CCEB, economic segmentation tool that identifies household characteristics to economically classify the population (ABEP, 2014Associação Brasileira de Empresas de Pesquisa. (2014). Critério de classificação econômica do Brasil São Paulo: ABEP.).

In individual interviews the father and the mother had three open questions both for the son and the daughter: "What usually makes your child sad?" "Tell a situation where your child was sad."; "What did you do in this situation?". Also a stimulus based in a literature story was used (Fabes, Eisenberg, & Bernzweig, 1990Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N.; Bernzweig, J. (1990The coping with children's negative emotions scale: Procedures and scoring. Available from authors. Arizona State University. Retrieved from http://www.public.asu.edu/~rafabes/cnesall.pdf
http://www.public.asu.edu/~rafabes/cnesa...
; Shields, Ryan, & Cicchetti, 2001Shields, A., Ryan, R. &Cicchetti, D. (2001). Narrative representations of caregivers and emotion dysregulation as predictors of maltreated children's rejection by peers. Developmental Psychology37(3), 321-337. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11370909
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11370...
), "You go out to walk with your son/daughter and (s)he takes her/his favorite toy. Arriving home after the walk, (s)he asks you where the toy is. You say you do not know, and (s)he lost their favorite toy. Your child is very sad and starts to cry a lot. What happens next?".

Outlining and Procedures

The research has qualitative, quantitative and transversal character, with outlining of collective case studies and narrative interview method (Bauer & Jovchelovitch, 1999Bauer, M., & Jovchelovitch, S. (1999) A questão da metodologia em pesquisa: Texto, imagem e materiais sonorosUnpublished. Curso de Extensão Pós-Graduação em Psicologia da Pontifícia Universidade Católica, RS.).

From the researchers' contact network, the first couples were selected by convenience, which were then asked to indicate other couples. The explanation and scheduling of the interview happened by telephone. In all, contact was established with 43 couples, but only 26 agreed to participate. The interviews took place at the residence of the couples. Each spouse was interviewed separately, and one investigator interviewed the mother and one the father at the same time, making it that one could not participate or interfere in the interview of the other. To start, each participant signed the consent form for recording and participation in the research, answered the CCEB, followed by General Data Questionnaire and interview guide containing the three questions, followed by the story-stimulation and its questioning. Father and mother responded to the interview questions twice, once about the son and the other regarding the daughter. The interviews lasted about 30 minutes, having been recorded and transcribed. The study was approved by the Ethics in Human Research Committee (information removed to preserve the anonymity of the author), according to Resolution 466/12.

Data Analysis

The data were analyzed from a quantitative content perspective, in which, in predetermined categories, the frequency of responses was scored. The chi-square test was also used, in order to verify differences in fathers' responses in comparison to the mothers' responses.

After transcribing the interviews, two researchers individually evaluated parental responses as: (1) the sadness inductors for the children; (2) the parental reaction in the situation where the son/daughter was sad; and (3) regarding stimulus-story. The inductors of sadness were classified considering the categories of Cruvinel's study (2009Cruvinel, M. (2009). Correlatos cognitivos e psicossociais de crianças com e sem sintomas de depressivos (Doctorates thesis). Faculdade de Educação, Universidade Estadual de Campinas, São Paulo. Retrieved from http://www.bibliotecadigital.unicamp.br/document/?code=000446385
http://www.bibliotecadigital.unicamp.br/...
). In this study, the author listed eight categories such as inductors of sadness: physical and verbal aggression, bad events, lack of emotional-social support, illness or death, prohibition/frustration, lack of recreational activities, school environment, and "do not know". In the present study all categories cited by (Cruvinel, 2009Cruvinel, M. (2009). Correlatos cognitivos e psicossociais de crianças com e sem sintomas de depressivos (Doctorates thesis). Faculdade de Educação, Universidade Estadual de Campinas, São Paulo. Retrieved from http://www.bibliotecadigital.unicamp.br/document/?code=000446385
http://www.bibliotecadigital.unicamp.br/...
) were found and a new category emerged from the data and was added to the above (conflict between peers/siblings). Table 1 shows the definition of categories inducting of sadness and examples from our sample that composed each category, followed by a few lines from the participants. The correlation was calculated between the ratings for the inductors of sadness, which was 80.76%. In regard the disagreements, there was a consensus among researchers in a total of 175 occurrences (Table 2).

Table 1
Elicitors of Sadness (based on Cruvinel, 2009Cruvinel, M. (2009). Correlatos cognitivos e psicossociais de crianças com e sem sintomas de depressivos (Doctorates thesis). Faculdade de Educação, Universidade Estadual de Campinas, São Paulo. Retrieved from http://www.bibliotecadigital.unicamp.br/document/?code=000446385
http://www.bibliotecadigital.unicamp.br/...
) and Examples of the Categories, out of the Researched Sample
Table 2
Percentage and Absolute Numbers as to Categories of Elicitors of Sadness, According to the Answers of Father, Mother and Gender of the Child

The responses of parents face the question: "Tell a situation where your child was sad." and "What did you do in that situation?" and stimulus-story were classified according to reaction categories to sadness: distraction, focused on the problem, validation of emotions, punitive, minimizing (Eisenberg et al., 1996Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Murphy, B. C. (1996). Parent's reactions to children's negative emotions: Relations to children's social competence and comforting behavior. Child Development67(5), 2227-2247. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01854.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996...
; Cassano et al., 2007Cassano, M., Perry-Parrish, C., & Zeman, J. (2007). Influence of gender on parental socialization of children's sadness regulation. Social Development16(2), 210-231. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00381.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007...
; Shields et al., 2001Shields, A., Ryan, R. &Cicchetti, D. (2001). Narrative representations of caregivers and emotion dysregulation as predictors of maltreated children's rejection by peers. Developmental Psychology37(3), 321-337. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11370909
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11370...
) and disturbed (Melo, 2005Melo, A. I. M. T. d. (2005). Emoções no período escolar: Estratégias parentais face à expressão emocional e sintomas de internalização e externalização da criança (Dissertação de mestrado). Curso de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia Clínica, Universidade do Minho, Portugal. Retrieved from http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitstream/1822/4926/1/TESE%20MESTRADO%20ANA%20MELO.pdf
http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitst...
). The categories "substitution" and "do nothing" emerged from this research. Table 3 presents the categories and definition on the parental reaction to sadness, and examples from our sample that composed each category, followed by speeches of participants for both the questions and the stimulus-story. The correlation for the answers on the parental reaction to sadness, both for the questions and stimulus-story, was calculated jointly for being of the same categories. We obtained matches of 83.22% and after consensus was reached, we had a total of 284 instances (Tables 4 and 5).

Table 3
Parental Reactions to Sadness and Examples of the Categories out of the Researched Sample
Table 4
Percentage and Absolute Numbers Regarding the Categories of Parental Reaction Facing Sadness, According to Answers from Fathers, Mothers and Gender of the Child
Tabela 5
Percentage and Absolute Numbers Regarding the Categories of Parental Reaction Facing Sadness, According to the Stimulus-Story and Answers from Fathers, Mothers and Gender of the Child

Results

1. Elicitors of Sadness

In order to investigate the factors that, in the perception of parents, cause sorrow in the child, two questions were analyzed together: "What usually makes your child sad" and "Tell a situation where (s)he was upset." Parents could list as many reasons for sadness as they wanted. Therefore, the amount of responses to the category "elicitors of sadness" is greater than the number of participants. In some cases, they reported as a reason, for example, staying away from the parents, but in situations eliciting sadness cited another cause, for example, canceling a scheduled trip.

In Table 2 we find that both the father and the mother identified as main sadness elicitors situations involving child frustration/ban, coupled with the lack of emotional/social support. It is noteworthy that, although uncommon, some parents were unable to report episodes of sadness in children of both sexes, though the sorrow is a basic emotion present in the daily lives of all people. There was a statistically significant difference only for the "bad events" category (X2 = 6.05, p = 0.014), where mothers considered more often than the fathers that such events elicit sadness in children.

2. Parental Eeactions Facing Sorrow

To investigate how parents handle sadness of both daughter and son in everyday situations, immediately after the description of a sad situation, a question was posed: "What did you do in this situation?". Importantly, parents often described more than one reaction in the same answer, such as: "I talked to her, it was difficult for us to go back there because the project had already closed... in another possibility we would buy it, or not, I mean she has so many toys to play, it didn't need to be exactly that one." In this response the mother fit in three response categories simultaneously: reaction focused on the problem, replacement and minimization. As a result, the amount of responses for the "parental" reactions was greater than the amount of participants. Table 3 shows the description for each category within parental sadness reactions.

Parents facing the expression of sadness regarding both children from the reported cases, made more use of reactions centered on the problem, followed by minimization and distraction (Table 4). None of them made emotional validation for male children and the percentage of this reaction was also small for the girls. Still, though with little occurrence, it is noted that some parents do nothing against the sadness of the children. There was a statistically significant difference in the use of punitive reaction between father and mother (X2 = 8.23, p = 0.004), in which the father reported using more of this strategy with both children, when compared to the mother.

3. Stimulus-Story

We also verified the parental reaction to a stimulus-story, and the parents could list as many strategies as they wanted in their answer. As a consequence, the number of responses was greater than the amount of participants (Table 5).

It was found that the categories most used by parents for both children facing the expression of sadness related to the loss of a toy, were, respectively, for father and mother, Reaction centered in Problem, Punitive Reaction and Replacement (Table 5). The father tends to use more Replacement than the mother (X2 = 4.7, p = 0.03). The mother minimizes most often the sadness feeling of the children, when compared to the father (X2 = 4.98, p = 0.026). There was no significant difference in the strategies used by father and mother in relation to son and daughter.

Discussion

The most cited category as an elicitor of sadness according to parents was frustration/prohibition both for the sons and daughters. Not getting something that was desired (Melo, 2005Melo, A. I. M. T. d. (2005). Emoções no período escolar: Estratégias parentais face à expressão emocional e sintomas de internalização e externalização da criança (Dissertação de mestrado). Curso de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia Clínica, Universidade do Minho, Portugal. Retrieved from http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitstream/1822/4926/1/TESE%20MESTRADO%20ANA%20MELO.pdf
http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitst...
), and feeling helpless as to change a result (Reis et al., 2015Reis, A. H., Habigzang, L. F., & Sperb, T. M. (2015). Emoções na infância e influência parental na regulação emocional infantil em uma perspectiva cognitivo-comportamental. In C. B. Neufeld, E. M. O. Falcone & B. Rangé (Ed.). PROCOGNITIVA Programa de atualização em Terapia Cognitivo-comportamental: ciclo 1, v. 4. Porto Alegre: Artmed Panamericana.) are also elicitors of sadness. It is possible that parents notice the sadness in the children more often in situations of prohibition or frustration because they, themselves, are the agents in such events. Thus, upon being forbidden of something or having a claim denied by a parent, the child automatically expresses the according emotion, in case, the sadness, according to the perception of parents. Similarly, the second most cited category, Lack of social emotional support, is also directly related to the interaction with parents and, probably for this reason, most salient to them. That's because the main examples given by parents involved situations in which children reported sadness due to absence of parents and jealous brothers.

The mother more often identifies the category of bad events, or fights between father and mother, or their separation, as elicitors of sadness. According to (Cruvinel, 2009Cruvinel, M. (2009). Correlatos cognitivos e psicossociais de crianças com e sem sintomas de depressivos (Doctorates thesis). Faculdade de Educação, Universidade Estadual de Campinas, São Paulo. Retrieved from http://www.bibliotecadigital.unicamp.br/document/?code=000446385
http://www.bibliotecadigital.unicamp.br/...
), family discord, parental illness, instability in family or school environment, are among the situations that facilitate stress and negative emotions such as sadness. Such situations do not involve a direct interaction between the parents and the child. Thus, to realize that the child is sad because of a bad event, parents should note that this situation affected the child in some way, pay attention to how the child was affected and identify the emotion arising as sadness. (Crespo, 2013Crespo, L. M. (2013). Child emotion talk in relation to parental socialization and emotion regulation: The role of parent and child gender. Bachelors Article. Retrieved from https://digitalarchive.wm.edu/bitstream/handle/10288/18428/CrespoLaura2013.pdf
https://digitalarchive.wm.edu/bitstream/...
) found that fathers tend to employ more emotional words related to anger and discussions of this issue, and that mothers use more words associated with sadness in talks addressing such emotion. Thus, it is plausible to assume that the mother is more attentive than the father to events that are elicitors of sadness in the children, while the father may be more aware of situations that cause anger, illustrating, according to the author, that parents feel more comfortable to discuss emotions according to the gender stereotype.

The Parental reaction strategy most used by father and mother, considering the general situations and stimulus-story, was a reaction focused on the issue. This corroborates findings in the literature showing the father and mother in fact tend to have positive reactions to internalizing emotions such as sadness, humiliation/shame or anxiety/fear (Melo, 2005Melo, A. I. M. T. d. (2005). Emoções no período escolar: Estratégias parentais face à expressão emocional e sintomas de internalização e externalização da criança (Dissertação de mestrado). Curso de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia Clínica, Universidade do Minho, Portugal. Retrieved from http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitstream/1822/4926/1/TESE%20MESTRADO%20ANA%20MELO.pdf
http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitst...
). In addition, the school also measured the parental reaction to the emotional expression of children. Mothers with higher levels of education tend to have more problem-centered responses (Alves & Cruz, 2011Alves, D., & Cruz, O. (2011). Reações parentais às emoções negativas do filhos (Rpen): Um questionário de avaliação da meta-emoção parental. Artigo em conferência internacional. Retrieved from http://repositorio-aberto.up.pt/handle/10216/57244
http://repositorio-aberto.up.pt/handle/1...
) and the average education of this sample was 13.55 years. The family that expresses themselves emotionally and positively and accepts the emotional expression of their child can assist them in emotional regulation. (Morris et al., 2007Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development16(2), 361-388. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007...
).

As for parental reaction, a difference found in our sample indicated that the fathers reported using more punishment against the sadness of the children, regardless of gender. It is assumed that the father has difficulty accepting the expression of sadness of the children also because it is not easy for him to deal with sadness. (Chaplin, Cole and Zahn-Waxler, 2005Chaplin, T. M., Cole, P. M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2005). Parental socialization of emotion expression: Gender differences and relations to child adjustment. Emotion5(1), 80-88. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.80
https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.80...
) found that fathers of children in preschool respond to daughter's anxiety and sadness more often than they do for the son, in which event the father tends to attend to anger more often. The authors suggest that the attention given by parents to a particular emotion can lead to its increased expression. So it can be assumed that from childhood men learn to express anger, and to have lower receptivity to expressions of sadness, rarely manifest it and, later as parents, repeat the pattern learned in childhood. (Chang, Schwartz, Dodge and McBride-Chang, 2003Chang, L., Schwartz, D., Dodge, K. A., & McBride-Chang, C. (2003). Harsh parenting in relation to child emotion regulation and aggression. Journal of Family Psychology17(4), 598-606. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.17.4.598
https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.17.4.5...
) also found that the fathers acted more often with aggressiveness and punitive reaction to the emotional expression of the sons.

However, in regard to stimulus-story, both punished equally. It is possible that fathers and mothers who interpreted the loss of the toy as the child's error have suggested punitive reaction. According to (Deffenbacher, 1999Deffenbacher, J. L. (1999). Cognitive-behavioral conceptualization and treatment of anger. Journal of Clinical Psychology55(3), 295-309. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpra.2009.12.004
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.2009.12....
), when an event is perceived as avoidable, that is, if something could have been done to prevent the incident, it is common that the individual feels anger. In this case, given the assessment that the child was careless about the care for the toy, although they have been saddened by the loss, it is possible for parents to have a punitive response, focusing on the fact (loss toy) and not emotion of the child (sadness).

Please note that a simple punishment does not teach the child to handle the emotional reaction due to the significant loss of something nor does it model problem-solving strategies in the child. Parents who behave in a more detached way and tend to ignore or ridicule the child's emotional responses, punishing them when they experience strong emotional reaction, undermine the emotional regulation of the child (Gottman et al., 1996Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1996) Parental meta-emotion philosophy and the emotional life of families: Theoretical models and preliminary data. Journal of Family Psychology10(3), 243-268. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.10.3.243
https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.10.3.2...
). When parents are punitive they can teach the child to avoid their negative emotional expression, like sadness, which in turn makes possible the occurrence of inadequate expressions of emotion (Morris et al., 2007Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development16(2), 361-388. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007...
). In addition, an authoritarian parenting style, which devalues communication and prioritizes punishment, affects the development of the child's empathy (Justo, Carvalho, & Kristensen, 2014Justo, A. R., Carvalho, J. C. N., & Kristensen, C. H. (2014). Desenvolvimento da empatia em crianças: A influência dos estilos parentais. Psicologia, Saúde & Doenças15(2), 510-523. Retrieved from http://www.scielo.gpeari.mctes.pt/pdf/psd/v15n2/v15n2a14.pdf
http://www.scielo.gpeari.mctes.pt/pdf/ps...
).

When considering the stimulus-story, we highlight the minimization category, taking into account that the mother showed greater use of this technique to deal with the emotion of sadness of both children. It is assumed that, upon realizing that the child is sad, the mother is sensitive and uses minimization to try to lessen the importance of the event, reminding the child of the fact that the lost toy was not the only one, and argue that now another child can use it to play. In contrast, in the study of (Cassano et al., 2007Cassano, M., Perry-Parrish, C., & Zeman, J. (2007). Influence of gender on parental socialization of children's sadness regulation. Social Development16(2), 210-231. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00381.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007...
) both the father and the mother responded to the sorrow of their son with behaviors that minimize emotion. It is noteworthy that minimization is a negative RE strategy, while maternal minimizing of the child's emotion was associated with lower level of social competence and a child's high level of avoidance against the emotional situation (Eisenberg et al., 1996Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Murphy, B. C. (1996). Parent's reactions to children's negative emotions: Relations to children's social competence and comforting behavior. Child Development67(5), 2227-2247. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01854.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996...
).

Another difference found in the stimulus-story was the father more often made use of a replacement strategy on toy loss. It is assumed that, in contexts in which the loss of a toy is generating sadness and there is a possibility of repair, the father tends to employ replacement strategy as a way to mitigate, compensate or even stop the negative emotion. Please note that replacing something that was lost, hinders the development of tolerance to frustration on the part of the child. This is because the loss cannot always be reversed, and it is important to teach them to deal with such situations in a gradual way. In doing so their father or mother will transmit the notions of responsibility, and assist in the management of sadness caused by something that could have been avoided, suggesting that, at a later situation they will pay more attention to their own belongings. That's a possible strategy due to the socioeconomic level of the studied sample, however, one may question whether the parental reactions would be so in similar situations in samples with few economic resources.

The emotion validation strategy has been little used by the fathers and the mothers when facing the expression of sadness of both children. It is believed that parental beliefs about emotions and cultural aspects can mediate the strategies employed by the parents. This is because the emotional validation involves the identification of the emotion expressed by the child, talking about the emotional experience and welcoming the affective response. Therefore, it is necessary for parents to pay attention to the emotional experience of the child, which is more difficult when it comes to sadness, when compared to rage, for example. In addition, they should empathize with the child, seeking to understand them, to hear their perception about the reasons that led them to grieve in the manifested intensity. However, according to Palm et al. (2011), having lived in a hostile environment during childhood enables the development of dysfunctional beliefs about acceptance and invalidation of emotional experiences, contributing to the adoption, in adulthood, of negative reactions such as rejection, devaluation, criticism and punishment before the children's expressions of negative emotions. Therefore, it would be important to check, in addition to the strategies used against the sadness of the children, the emotional climate of the family of origin and their beliefs regarding emotions.

Finally, we see no significant difference from the strategies used by both parents for the son and daughter. This result differs from other studies as (Fivush, Brotman, Buckner and Goodman, 2000Fivush, R., Brotman, M., Buckner, J., & Goodman, S. (2000). Gender differences in parent-child emotion narratives. Sex Roles42(3-4), 233-253. doi: 10.1023/A:1007091207068
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007091207068...
) which states that when discussing sad events, the father uses more emotional expressions with their daughter and (Melo, 2005Melo, A. I. M. T. d. (2005). Emoções no período escolar: Estratégias parentais face à expressão emocional e sintomas de internalização e externalização da criança (Dissertação de mestrado). Curso de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia Clínica, Universidade do Minho, Portugal. Retrieved from http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitstream/1822/4926/1/TESE%20MESTRADO%20ANA%20MELO.pdf
http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitst...
), which asserted that parents tended to have more negative reactions to their sons than theirs daughters when facing their emotional expressions. It is believed that the use of a methodology to assess the interaction between parents and children involving conversation between dyads, according to the study by (Fivush et al., 2000Fivush, R., Brotman, M., Buckner, J., & Goodman, S. (2000). Gender differences in parent-child emotion narratives. Sex Roles42(3-4), 233-253. doi: 10.1023/A:1007091207068
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007091207068...
), may favor the identification of differences between the father and mother reactions as the child's sex.

Final Considerations

In the perception of father and mother, the main sadness elicitors involve frustration / prohibition, followed by lack of emotional-social support. The mother rather than the father, point bad events as frequent sadness stimuli in their children. Both repeatedly use the reaction strategy, focused on the problem, which is considered adaptive. However, it was also common the use of strategies that you can not adapt, as punishment, substitution and minimization, with the father punishing more than the mother, and also tending to replace the loss of a toy when the child is sad while the mother makes more use of minimization. The parents had difficulty validating the emotion of sadness of the children of both sexes and managing this emotion positively, failing to use everyday situations to promote emotional education, which leads to the assumption that children can present difficulties in RE, arising from inadaptable strategies reported by parents.

Limitations of this study involve the lack of a standardized instrument for assessment of RE in children, being it all based on parental reports. In addition, it is possible that both parents have responded according to what they believed to be socially acceptable. Not having assessed the knowledge and parental beliefs about their own emotions limits the understanding of their reactions to the children's sorrow.

Future studies should consider a methodology that involves observation of the interaction between parents and children by checking the parental emotional and linguistic expressions when facing the emotions of the children, addressing other emotions. It is also necessary to check the reciprocity between child temperament and parental responses to the child's emotions. This is because children with high levels of impulsivity and frustration and low self-control are more vulnerable to the adverse effects of negative parenting (Kiff et al., 2011Kiff, C., Lengua, L., & Zalewski, M. (2011). Nature and nurturing: Parenting in the context of child temperament. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review14(3), 251-301. doi: 10.1007/s10567-011-0093-4
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0093-...
). We must consider the role of other people in the child's RE, such as peers and teachers. The most appropriate parental behavior must be defined, according to age range of the child, and also seeking differences in the RE strategies adopted by parents concerning their children in emotionally challenging situations as well as using as a criterion the parents' education. Finally, determine whether training parents to deal with children's emotions improves the children's RE.

References

  • Almeida, M. B. M. D. (2006). Socialização parental das emoções e a competência social da criança (Dissertação de mestrado). Instituto Superior de Psicologia Aplicada. Retrieved from http://repositorio.ispa.pt/bitstream/10400.12/291/1/DM%20ALME-MB1.pdf
    » http://repositorio.ispa.pt/bitstream/10400.12/291/1/DM%20ALME-MB1.pdf
  • Alves, D., & Cruz, O. (2011). Reações parentais às emoções negativas do filhos (Rpen): Um questionário de avaliação da meta-emoção parental Artigo em conferência internacional. Retrieved from http://repositorio-aberto.up.pt/handle/10216/57244
    » http://repositorio-aberto.up.pt/handle/10216/57244
  • Associação Brasileira de Empresas de Pesquisa. (2014). Critério de classificação econômica do Brasil São Paulo: ABEP.
  • Bauer, M., & Jovchelovitch, S. (1999) A questão da metodologia em pesquisa: Texto, imagem e materiais sonorosUnpublished. Curso de Extensão Pós-Graduação em Psicologia da Pontifícia Universidade Católica, RS.
  • Cassano, M., Perry-Parrish, C., & Zeman, J. (2007). Influence of gender on parental socialization of children's sadness regulation. Social Development16(2), 210-231. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00381.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00381.x
  • Cassano, M. C., Zeman, J. L., & Sanders, W. M. (2014). Responses to children's sadness: Mothers' and fathers' unique contributions and perceptions. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly60(1), 1-23. doi: 10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.60.1.0001
    » https://doi.org/10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.60.1.0001
  • Chang, L., Schwartz, D., Dodge, K. A., & McBride-Chang, C. (2003). Harsh parenting in relation to child emotion regulation and aggression. Journal of Family Psychology17(4), 598-606. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.17.4.598
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.17.4.598
  • Chaplin, T. M., Cole, P. M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2005). Parental socialization of emotion expression: Gender differences and relations to child adjustment. Emotion5(1), 80-88. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.80
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.80
  • Crespo, L. M. (2013). Child emotion talk in relation to parental socialization and emotion regulation: The role of parent and child gender. Bachelors Article. Retrieved from https://digitalarchive.wm.edu/bitstream/handle/10288/18428/CrespoLaura2013.pdf
    » https://digitalarchive.wm.edu/bitstream/handle/10288/18428/CrespoLaura2013.pdf
  • Cruvinel, M. (2009). Correlatos cognitivos e psicossociais de crianças com e sem sintomas de depressivos (Doctorates thesis). Faculdade de Educação, Universidade Estadual de Campinas, São Paulo. Retrieved from http://www.bibliotecadigital.unicamp.br/document/?code=000446385
    » http://www.bibliotecadigital.unicamp.br/document/?code=000446385
  • Deffenbacher, J. L. (1999). Cognitive-behavioral conceptualization and treatment of anger. Journal of Clinical Psychology55(3), 295-309. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpra.2009.12.004
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.2009.12.004
  • Denham, S. A., Bassett, H. H., & Wyatt, T. M. (2010). Gender differences in the socialization of preschooler's emotional competence. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development(128), 29-49. doi: 10.1002/cd.267
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.267
  • Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Murphy, B. C. (1996). Parent's reactions to children's negative emotions: Relations to children's social competence and comforting behavior. Child Development67(5), 2227-2247. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01854.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01854.x
  • Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N.; Bernzweig, J. (1990The coping with children's negative emotions scale: Procedures and scoring. Available from authors. Arizona State University. Retrieved from http://www.public.asu.edu/~rafabes/cnesall.pdf
    » http://www.public.asu.edu/~rafabes/cnesall.pdf
  • Fabes, R. A., Leonard, S. A., Kupanoff, K., & Martin, C. L. (2001). Parental coping with children's negative emotions: Relations with children's emotional and social responding. Child Development72(3), 907-920. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00323
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00323
  • Fivush, R., Brotman, M., Buckner, J., & Goodman, S. (2000). Gender differences in parent-child emotion narratives. Sex Roles42(3-4), 233-253. doi: 10.1023/A:1007091207068
    » https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007091207068
  • Friedlmeier, W., & Trommsdorff, G. (1999). Emotion regulation in early childhood: A cross-cultural comparison between german and japanese toddlers. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology30(6), 684-711. doi: 10.1177/0022022199030006002
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022199030006002
  • Goodman, L. A. (1961). Snowball sampling. Annals of Mathematical Statistics32(1), 148-170. Retrieved from http://projecteuclid.org/download/pdf_1/euclid.aoms/1177705148
    » http://projecteuclid.org/download/pdf_1/euclid.aoms/1177705148
  • Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1996) Parental meta-emotion philosophy and the emotional life of families: Theoretical models and preliminary data. Journal of Family Psychology10(3), 243-268. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.10.3.243
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.10.3.243
  • Justo, A. R., Carvalho, J. C. N., & Kristensen, C. H. (2014). Desenvolvimento da empatia em crianças: A influência dos estilos parentais. Psicologia, Saúde & Doenças15(2), 510-523. Retrieved from http://www.scielo.gpeari.mctes.pt/pdf/psd/v15n2/v15n2a14.pdf
    » http://www.scielo.gpeari.mctes.pt/pdf/psd/v15n2/v15n2a14.pdf
  • Katz, L. F., Maliken, A. C., & Stettler, N. M. (2012). Parental meta-emotion philosophy: A review of research and theoretical framework. Child Development Perspectives6(4), 417-422. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00244.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00244.x
  • Kiff, C., Lengua, L., & Zalewski, M. (2011). Nature and nurturing: Parenting in the context of child temperament. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review14(3), 251-301. doi: 10.1007/s10567-011-0093-4
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0093-4
  • Melo, A. I. M. T. d. (2005). Emoções no período escolar: Estratégias parentais face à expressão emocional e sintomas de internalização e externalização da criança (Dissertação de mestrado). Curso de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia Clínica, Universidade do Minho, Portugal. Retrieved from http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitstream/1822/4926/1/TESE%20MESTRADO%20ANA%20MELO.pdf
    » http://repositorium.sdum.uminho.pt/bitstream/1822/4926/1/TESE%20MESTRADO%20ANA%20MELO.pdf
  • Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development16(2), 361-388. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x
  • Reis, A. H., Habigzang, L. F., & Sperb, T. M. (2015). Emoções na infância e influência parental na regulação emocional infantil em uma perspectiva cognitivo-comportamental. In C. B. Neufeld, E. M. O. Falcone & B. Rangé (Ed.). PROCOGNITIVA Programa de atualização em Terapia Cognitivo-comportamental: ciclo 1, v. 4. Porto Alegre: Artmed Panamericana.
  • Sanders, M., & Morawska, A. (2011). Mudanças nos conhecimentos, em expectativas e atribuições disfuncionais, e na regulação emocional dos pais podem melhorar os resultados da criança? In R. E. Tremblay, M. Boivin & R. D. V. Peters (Eds.), Enciclopédia sobre o desenvolvimento na primeira infância (pp. 1-13). Montreal: Centre. Retrieved from http://www.enciclopedia-crianca.com/documents/Sanders-MorawskaPRTxp1.pdf
    » http://www.enciclopedia-crianca.com/documents/Sanders-MorawskaPRTxp1.pdf
  • Sanders, W., Zeman, J., Poon, J., & Miller, R. (2013). Child regulation of negative emotions and depressive symptoms: The moderating role of parental emotion socialization. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 1-14. doi: 10.1007/s10826-013-9850-y
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-013-9850-y
  • Shields, A., Ryan, R. &Cicchetti, D. (2001). Narrative representations of caregivers and emotion dysregulation as predictors of maltreated children's rejection by peers. Developmental Psychology37(3), 321-337. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11370909
    » http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11370909
  • Thompson, R. (1991). Emotional regulation and emotional development. Educational Psychology Review3(4), 269-307. doi: 10.1007/BF01319934
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01319934
  • Wilson, K. R., Havighurst, S. S., & Harley, A. E. (2012). Tuning in to kids: An effectiveness trial of a parenting program targeting emotion socialization of preschoolers. Journal of Family Psychology26(1), 56-65. doi: 10.1037/a0026480
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026480

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Jan-Apr 2016

History

  • Received
    20 Apr 2015
  • Reviewed
    03 July 2015
  • Accepted
    13 July 2015
Universidade de São Francisco, Programa de Pós-Graduação Stricto Sensu em Psicologia R. Waldemar César da Silveira, 105, Vl. Cura D'Ars (SWIFT), Campinas - São Paulo, CEP 13045-510, Telefone: (19)3779-3771 - Campinas - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistapsico@usf.edu.br