Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Vibrational communication in insects

A comunicação entre insetos através de vibrações

Abstracts

Communication through substrate-borne vibrations has for long been recognized but in comparison with air-borne sound it has received very little attention. However, in recent years it has become increasingly clear that vibrational signals play a crucial role in communication in many insect groups and we provide a short overview. Vibrational signals are related to sexual behavior, alarm and defensive behavior and are often used to mediate coordinated group actions and complex social interactions. For small insects they are probably the least costly and most far-reaching signals for intraspecific communication and also not easily perceived by a potential predator or parasitoid. Substrate-borne signals are produced by diversed methods and detected by sensitive receptors in all six legs. The courtship behavior of the southern green stink bug Nezara viridula (L.) (Hemiptera: Pentatomidae) is taken as a representative model in illustrating some principal mechanisms of vibrational communication in insects. Species and sex specific vibrational signals produced during the courtship are well suited for propagation through plants and to transmit the relevant information about the species and sex of the sender as well as provide the directional cue for locating the mate. The role of substrate-borne signals as a part of the specific mate recognition systems which are unique for each species makes studies of vibrational signals a very useful tool for resolving taxonomic problems.

Vibrational communication; substrate vibration; behavior; Nezara viridula


A comunicação através de vibrações originárias do substrato tem sido reconhecida há muito tempo, mas tem recebido muito menos atenção que a comunicação através do som propagado pelo ar. Recentemente, entretanto, tem se tornado cada vez mais claro o papel crucial que sinais vibratórios desempenham na comunicação em muitos grupos de insetos, o que motivou essa revisão suscinta. Sinais vibracionais estão relacionados ao comportamento sexual, de alarme e de defesa, e são em geral usados para mediar ações coordenadas em grupo e interações sociais complexas. Para insetos pequenos esses sinais representam um custo energético mínimo, com alcance máximo, e que não são percebidos facilmente por predadores e por parasitóides em potencial. Sinais originários dos substrato são produzidos por diversos métodos e detectados por receptores presentes nas seis pernas. O comportamento de corte do percevejo verde, Nezara viridula (L.) (Hemiptera: Pentatomidae) é analisado como um modelo representativo na ilustração de alguns mecanismos importantes na comunicação vibracional em insetos. Sinais vibratórios são específicos das espécies e de sexos, os quais são produzidos durante a corte e que são adequados para serem transmitidos pelas plantas, contendo informações relevantes sobre a espécie e o sexo do organismo emissor, bem como informações de onde localizar o parceiro. O papel de sinais originados do substrato, os quais são únicos para cada espécie, faz com que os estudos em sinais vibracionais sejam uma importante ferramenta para resolver problemas taxonômicos.

Comunicação vibracional; vibração do substrato; comportamento; Nezara viridula


FORUM

Vibrational communication in insects

A comunicação entre insetos através de vibrações

Meta Virant-Doberlet; Andrej Cokl

Dept. Entomology, National Institute of Biology, Veèna pot 111, SI-1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia

ABSTRACT

Communication through substrate-borne vibrations has for long been recognized but in comparison with air-borne sound it has received very little attention. However, in recent years it has become increasingly clear that vibrational signals play a crucial role in communication in many insect groups and we provide a short overview. Vibrational signals are related to sexual behavior, alarm and defensive behavior and are often used to mediate coordinated group actions and complex social interactions. For small insects they are probably the least costly and most far-reaching signals for intraspecific communication and also not easily perceived by a potential predator or parasitoid. Substrate-borne signals are produced by diversed methods and detected by sensitive receptors in all six legs. The courtship behavior of the southern green stink bug Nezara viridula (L.) (Hemiptera: Pentatomidae) is taken as a representative model in illustrating some principal mechanisms of vibrational communication in insects. Species and sex specific vibrational signals produced during the courtship are well suited for propagation through plants and to transmit the relevant information about the species and sex of the sender as well as provide the directional cue for locating the mate. The role of substrate-borne signals as a part of the specific mate recognition systems which are unique for each species makes studies of vibrational signals a very useful tool for resolving taxonomic problems.

Key words: Vibrational communication, substrate vibration, behavior, Nezara viridula

RESUMO

A comunicação através de vibrações originárias do substrato tem sido reconhecida há muito tempo, mas tem recebido muito menos atenção que a comunicação através do som propagado pelo ar. Recentemente, entretanto, tem se tornado cada vez mais claro o papel crucial que sinais vibratórios desempenham na comunicação em muitos grupos de insetos, o que motivou essa revisão suscinta. Sinais vibracionais estão relacionados ao comportamento sexual, de alarme e de defesa, e são em geral usados para mediar ações coordenadas em grupo e interações sociais complexas. Para insetos pequenos esses sinais representam um custo energético mínimo, com alcance máximo, e que não são percebidos facilmente por predadores e por parasitóides em potencial. Sinais originários dos substrato são produzidos por diversos métodos e detectados por receptores presentes nas seis pernas. O comportamento de corte do percevejo verde, Nezara viridula (L.) (Hemiptera: Pentatomidae) é analisado como um modelo representativo na ilustração de alguns mecanismos importantes na comunicação vibracional em insetos. Sinais vibratórios são específicos das espécies e de sexos, os quais são produzidos durante a corte e que são adequados para serem transmitidos pelas plantas, contendo informações relevantes sobre a espécie e o sexo do organismo emissor, bem como informações de onde localizar o parceiro. O papel de sinais originados do substrato, os quais são únicos para cada espécie, faz com que os estudos em sinais vibracionais sejam uma importante ferramenta para resolver problemas taxonômicos.

Palavras - chave: Comunicação vibracional, vibração do substrato, comportamento, Nezara viridula

Studies of insect communication have revealed a great variety of mechanisms, tactics and systems, which are either chemical, visual or acoustical. Many insects like crickets, katydids, grasshoppers and cicadas produce air-borne sounds which can be heard by humans and consequently have been extensively investigated for many years. On the other hand, we can detect and measure substrate-borne signals only with sensitive equipment and since substrate vibrations generally do not play any important role in our interactions, we can hardly imagine what it is like to live in a 'vibratory world'. Therefore it is not a suprise that although vibrational communication has been recognized for centuries it was thought to be of minor importance. However, in recent years it has become clear that the use of substrate-borne vibrations in insect communication is more widespread and more important than previously thought.

The objective of this paper is to provide a brief overview of the substrate-borne vibrational communication in insects. However, special emphasis is put on the vibrational communication in the southern green stink bug Nezara viridula (L.) (Heteroptera: Pentatomidae). N. viridula has been a model species for the whole family and studies of vibrational communication in this species extended our knowledge on substrate-borne communication in small plant dwelling insects. We have tried to keep the amount of cited literature to a minimum and therefore we mostly cite the latest or most comprehensive publications.

Vibrational Communication in Insects - Overview

Table 1 provides a summary of major insect groups in which vibrational communication has been described. Substrate-borne communication is involved in a variety of interactions and serves several functions. Most of the investigated vibrational signals are related to sexual behavior, to finding and attracting a partner. Vibrational communication as a part of the courtship has been found in Orthoptera (Loher & Dambach 1989, Field & Bailey 1997), Plecoptera (Stewart 1997), Sternorrhyncha (Kanmiya & Sonobe 2002), Auchenorrhyncha (Claridge 1985), Heteroptera (Wilcox 1995, Cokl & Virant-Doberlet 2003), Neuropteroidea (Henry 1994, Devetak 1998), Coleoptera (Birch & Keenlyside 1991, Hirschberger 2001), Mecoptera (Rupprecht 1974), Diptera (Hoy et al. 1988, Kanmiya 1990) and Trichoptera (Ivanov 1993).

Even insects using air-borne sounds during their courtship, can use vibrational components of these signals as an additional channel in intraspecific communication. In Tettigoniidae, substrate vibrations improve the localization of the stridulating male (Kalmring et al. 1997) and it seems that in Gryllotalpidae vibrational component of the male call plays a role in male-male spacing (Hill & Shadley 2001). However, even in the above-mentioned insect groups vibrational signals are not important only in intrasexual communication. Stridulatory signals are often related to defensive behavior, especially in Coleoptera (Schmitt & Traue 1990, Wilson et al. 1993, Serrano et al. 2003) and Heteroptera (Gogala 1985, Schilman et al. 2001).

In group-living treehoppers (Membracidae) vibratory signals are also associated with defense and parental care (Cocroft 1999), locating the new feeding site and possibly also in ant-mutualism (Cocroft 2001). In beetle larvae (Chrysomelidae) vibrational signals have been observed as a part of defensive behavior and group movement (Cocroft 2001, Greenfield 2002). It seems that vibrational signaling during group movement is also present in tingid bug nymphs (Cocroft 2001).

In Isoptera, Lepidoptera and Hymenoptera vibratory signals are not associated with mating behavior. Caterpillars and pupae of ant-associated butterfly species produce substrate-borne signals that attract ants and ensure high attendance level thus increasing their protection against predators (DeVries 1990, Travassos & Pierce 2000). Caterpillars of the family Drepanidae use vibratory signals in territorial displays while defending silk nests from conspecifics (Yack et al. 2001).

Many termite species produce substrate-borne vibrations as an alarm signal in response to a disturbance of the nest by a predator (Kirchner 1997). A different vibratory alarm signal is apparently used to convey the information about the presence of pathogenic fungal spores (Rosengaus et al. 1999).

Young honeybee queens communicate through vibrational signals called queen piping (Kirchner 1997). In honeybees most other vibratory signals are generally regarded as modulatory communication that elicits a general increase in activity that may help to mediate complex social interactions (Nieh 1998, Lewis et al. 2002). They are associated with queen competition (Schneider et al. 2001), house hunting (Dohanue et al. 2003) and activating or deactivating foraging (Kirchner 1997, Nieh 1998). Vibratory signals are also transmitted during the waggle dance (Nieh & Tautz 2000), however, their role is not clear. In social wasps vibrational signals have been implicated in adult-larval communication (Savoyard et al. 1998, Cummings et al. 1999) and also associated with worker-queen communication and intensification of activities within the nest (Ishay et al. 1974). A very special way of using vibrational signals is vibrational sounding. Some parasitoid wasps locate their hosts by vibrations produced by tapping the substrate with their antennae and detecting the echoes with subgenual organs in the legs (Broad & Quicke 2000, Vilhelmsen et al. 2001).

Sawfly larvae living gregariously use vibratory signals to keep the colony together and in locating fresh food sources (Cocroft 2001).

Although ant workers produce acoustic signals that are audible to humans, they are insensitive to air-borne sound, but instead they are highly responsive to the substrate-borne components of these signals (Hölldobler 1999, Roces & Tautz 2001). Vibrational communication in ants occurs in a variety of contexts, often as a part of multimodal signal (as a modulatory signal) and the effects can depend on the social context where the signals are perceived. It plays a role in underground alarm communication, recruitment to food sources and communication between castes in the context of defense against parasites (Kirchner 1997, Hölldobler 1999).

Signal Production. Every movement of the insect body or its parts induces vibrations in the substrate and even such inadvertent signals could be used in intraspecific interactions, as shown for some beetles (Hanrahan & Kirchner 1994). However, most of the signals used for communication are species-specific and their production involves specific movements of the body and/or specialized structures. The most simple and widespread production mechanisms of vibrational signals are percussion on the substrate and tremulation (Table 1). Percussion is often called drumming and involves striking of different body parts directly against the substratum. Tremulation is associated with oscillating, rocking and jerking movements of the body and signals are produced without striking the substrate and transmitted to the substrate via the legs. Substrate-borne signals are also produced by tymbal or tymbal-like mechanisms and stridulation (Gogala 1984, Claridge 1985, Gogala 1985, Ewing 1989).

Sensory Organs. The best known receptors for substrate vibration0s are campaniform sensilla and leg scolopidial organs. Campaniform sensilla are often located near the intersegmental joints on the legs. They are most sensitive for substrate vibrations below 100 Hz, however their sensitivity is low. The most sensitive organ is a complex scolopidial organ named the subgenual organ located in the tibia of all six legs, distal of the femur-tibia joint (Debaisieux 1938, Menzel & Tautz 1994, Devetak 1998, Vilhelmsen et al. 2001). The subgenual organ detects displacements of the substrate below 1 nm and acceleration values below 0.1 ms-2 in the optimal frequency range between 700 and 1500 Hz (Shaw 1994, Devetak 1998). The structure of subgenual organ differs between insect groups. It can consist from only one sensory cell like in Panorpa (Mecoptera) (Debaisieux 1938) or two as in Auchenorrhyncha (Cercopidae) (Debaisieux 1938) and Heteroptera (Michel et al. 1983) to up to 400 in orussid wasps (Vilhelmsen et al. 2001). Up to now, subgenual organ has been found in all Pterygote insects, except Coleoptera and Diptera. Substrate-borne vibrations could be detected also by Johnston's organ in antennae (Jeram & Cokl 1996).

Why Use Substrate-Borne Signals? Signals used for communication should convey the context (the meaning of the message) as well as provide the information about the identity and location of the sender. It is essential for successful communication that signals should travel over some distance without losing their specific characteristics. For effective communication with air-borne acoustic signals insects have to be relatively large in respect to the wavelength of the emitted sound (Markl 1983, Bennet-Clark 1998). Smaller insects should produce higher frequency signals; however high frequency sound propagates through environment with greater attenuation and degradation of signal structure (Markl 1983, Bennet-Clark 1998). As a result, many small insects use substrate-borne signals, since vibrations are probably the least costly and most far-reaching signals for communication. This could be also true for insects living on water surface (Markl 1983). Although vibrational signals allow communication range of several meters, vibrational communication is considered a short-range communication channel. However, in comparison with the size of the insects, communication range can be up to 1000 times the length of the body (Stewart 1997, Bennet-Clark 1998). Propagation of vibrational signals is also less diffuse and the signal is confined within the substrate and is therefore on one hand easier to locate, but on the other hand is also less likely to attract the enemies (Bennet-Clark 1998, Barth 2002). For as long as continuity of the substrate is maintained, the transmission of vibrational signal is not greatly affected by obstacles in its path, which is especially important in the complex environment of the plant.

Constraints of Vibrational Communication. As well as the above mentioned advantages, substrate-borne communication also has some disadvantages. The main one is that vibratory signals are distorted during the transmission through the substrate in time and frequency domain and become increasingly more degraded with increasing distance from the source (Michelsen et al. 1982, Markl 1983, Bennet-Clark 1998). An additional complication arises from the fact that on plants the intensity of the signal often does not decrease monotonically with the distance from the emitter (Michelsen et al. 1982, Barth 2002). Regardless of these complications, if vibratory signals are used for communication, insects should be able to extract from them the relevant information about the context, species, sex, and position of the sender.

Vibrational communication can be regarded as an interaction between the physical properties of the environment and insect's anatomy and physiology and resulting behavior. How insects overcome the constraints imposed by the physical properties of their natural environment is exemplified with the studies of vibrational communication in the southern green stink bug N. viridula.

Vibrational Communication in N. viridula

The southern green stink bug N. viridula (Fig.1) is a member of the family Pentatomidae which with over 4000 described species is one of the largest families within Heteroptera (Panizzi 1997). N. viridula is a cosmopolitan species and highly polyphagous and among several pentatomid pests of legume crops, is ecologically and economically, perhaps one of the most important ones.


As a part of the courtship males and females produce several stereotyped species and sex-specific vibratory signals called songs that are often exchanged in a duet (Cokl et al. 2000) (Fig 2). These songs are low frequency signals with the dominant frequency range between 80 Hz and 120 Hz. Signals are produced by vibrations of the body. Production of these signals is accompanied by movement of the abdominal tergal plate and dorso-ventral vibrations of the whole abdomen (Gogala 1984, 1990, Cokl & Virant-Doberlet 2003).


Suitability of the Signals. Are such vibratory signals suitable to function as an effective communication channel? Low frequency signals with dominant frequency around 100 Hz propagate through plants with very little attenuation (Barth 2002) and, therefore, enable longer communication range. Furthermore, as it is shown in Fig. 3 some plants might be better suited for transmission of Nezara's songs to greater distances. At distances above 50 cm from the source there is up to 20 dB (10 times) difference in the attenuation of the signals in the relevant frequency range between the two groups of plants. Bean, for example, showed an outstanding efficiency of transmission of signals below 200 Hz. At distances of 3 m, it filtered out all signals above 120 Hz and 90 Hz signal was not attenuated to any appreciable extent. Bean is one important host plant of N. viridula, however, it is not known whether bugs primarily gather on plants on which vibrations relevant to their behavior propagate particularly well.


There is also an additional consideration. Insects use for their communication vibratory signals that travel over the plant in the form of bending waves (Michelsen et al. 1982, Barth 2002). Their propagation velocity is relatively low and decreases with decreasing frequency. If vibrational signals are used for orientation, lower propagation velocity results in increased time difference in the arrival of the signal to various legs and thus provides the possible cue for orientation. N. viridula can detect substrate vibrations with campaniform sensilla, four scolopidial organs in each leg including subgenual organ and also with Johnston's organ in the antennae (Cokl & Virant-Doberlet 2003). Comparing the frequency spectra of Nezara's vibrational signals with the frequency range and sensitivity of it's vibroreceptors in the legs shows that these receptors are well suited for detection of conspecific signals (Cokl 1983).

Specificity of the Signals. One of the most important roles of species and sex-specific acoustic signals used in insect communication is to enable mate recognition and reproductive isolation. How species-specific are the vibrational signals of N. viridula? Are bugs able to discriminate among different signals? Most of so far studied pentatominae species show similar acoustic behavior, vibratory repertoire and syntax, although some songs might be missing from the repertoire (Cokl et al. 2000, 2001, McBrien et al. 2002, McBrien & Millar 2003). A different repertoire has been found in Holcostethus strictus (Fabricius) (Pavlovcic & Cokl 2001). All stink bugs so far studied produce vibratory signals that have low frequency characteristics with dominant frequencies around 100 Hz. Tuning of pentatomid vibrational signals to mechanical properties of plants most probably limits frequency characteristics of vibratory songs to play a major role in song and species identification, for as long as the dominant frequency remains in the appropriate frequency range. Species specificity of vibrational signals has been found mainly in temporal characteristics such as duration of the signals and their repetition time.

In the behavioral context of courtship and rivalry males of N. viridula are able to distinguish between conspecific male and female signals. They also distinguish heterospecific signals, which differ in temporal parameters from conspecific female signals (Miklas et al. 2003a, Hrabar et al. in press). However, they did not discriminate between the conspecific female calling song and the heterospecific signal, which has very similar pulse duration and repetition time values (Hrabar et al. in press).

Recently it became evident that vibratory songs of existing geographically isolated populations of N. viridula differ in their temporal characteristics, such as duration and repetition time of the signals (Ryan et al. 1996; Cokl et al. 2000, 2001; Miklas et al. 2003a). Fig. 4 shows differences in duration and repetition time for the male and female calling songs. It seems that these differences are genetically determined (Virant-Doberlet et al. 2000). Although males recognize female calling signals from another population as a conspecific female call it seems that males still prefer vibratory signals of females from their own population (Miklas et al. 2001, 2003a).


Orientation to a Source of Vibration. For a long time it was thought that small insects might be unable to extract information about the position of the sender from vibrational signals, the main reason being that amplitude and time differences of arrival of the signal to spatially separated vibroreceptors in various legs are too small to be evaluated in the central nervous system. Transmission of vibrational signals through plants in the form of bending waves imposes some additional constraints on localization. On plants there is often no predictable relation between the amplitude of the signal and distance from the sender (Michelsen et al. 1982, Barth 2002). Furthermore, propagation velocity of the signals can substantially differ even between different parts of the same plant. Behavioral experiments however, showed that males of N. viridula can accurately localize a vibration source, i.e., singing female on a plant (Cokl et al. 1999). Although the exact mechanism underlying vibrational directionality in small insects is still not known, studies in N. viridula provide some insight into possible solutions.

In the complex environment of a plant, at each branching point between the main stem and the side branches, a searching male has to decide which one to take to reach the female. Searching behavior includes characteristic stops at branching points, waiting for the next female calling signal and testing possible paths with the legs.

Larger arthropods like scorpions and spiders use time-of-arrival and amplitude differences in stimulation of various legs as directional cues (Brownel & van Hemmen 2001, Barth 2002). Distances between legs of N. viridula do not exceed 1 cm and calculated time delays are on the borderline of the lowest behavioral threshold time delay ever found which is as short as 0.2 ms in scorpions (Brownel & van Hemmen 2001). However, at branching points male stretches the legs between branches and thus increases the distance between legs and the resulting time delay.

On bean plants the measured amplitude differences of vibrational signals at adjacent points around the branching points were large enough that they could release differential neuronal responses at legs positioned at stem and petiolus; however, the amplitude of the signal is not necessarily higher at the branch closer to the source of vibration (Stritih et al. 2000).

N. viridula could extract directional information also from phase-shifts in the signals arriving to different legs and from evaluation of differences between components of different frequencies (Michelsen et al. 1982, Barth 2002). Directional information could also be available in mechanical response of the body to substrate vibrations as was recently proposed for treehoppers (Cocroft et al. 2000) and such mechanism could amplify the effects of very small time differences between inputs from the legs.

Modulation of Pheromone Emission by Vibrational Signals. Mating behavior of N. viridula has been divided into two phases: long-range mate attraction and short-range courtship (Borges et al. 1987). Sex pheromone produced by males of N. viridula is regarded as long-range attractant that brings bugs to the same plant whereas vibrational signals are used on a closer range once bugs are already on the same plant. Recently it was shown that males release larger amount of pheromone when they are stimulated with the female calling song (Miklas et al. 2003b). This appears to be a specific reaction since pheromone production did not increase when males were stimulated with male rival song or artificial signals. The female calling song of another population was less efficient in stimulating pheromone emission than the female song from male's own population. Such feedback modulatory mechanism may improve synchronization of sexual activity and provide sensory cues for sexual selection as well as reduce energy costs and the risk of parasitism.

Concluding Remarks and Future Perspectives

Current data indicate that substrate-borne vibrational communication is most widespread in Plecoptera, Hemiptera and Neuroptera but the reason for this might only be that most of the research on insect vibrational communication has been concentrated on these taxonomic groups. More detailed investigations in other groups will probably reveal even wider use and importance of vibratory signals. It is important to bear in mind that the signal modality most obvious (and easily detectable and recordable) to us is not necessarily biologically the most relevant one to the insects. We can not dismiss vibrational signals as a relevant communication channel, because from our current knowledge and perspective they impose too many constraints on communication. Insects have shown us many times that in the field they perform much better than our predictions and theoretical calculations imply. Solution does not necessarily have to be perfect or flawless, just good enough to be effective in 'real life'. Solutions to very complex situations often involve behavioral adaptations and such strategies might not be immediately obvious to the observer.

Vibrational signals are not regarded only as a communication but also as cues (stimuli whose perception by other animals is not beneficial to the emitter). While moving either prey or predator can not prevent inducing vibrations in the substrate. Mechanoreceptors able to detect vibrations of the substrate are so common in insects that vibrational cues are certainly very important even for insects in which vibrational signals are not used for intraspecific communication.

But what use can vibrational signals be to us? Vibrational signals have often been associated with cryptic species as seen in Auchenorrhyncha (Claridge et al. 1997) and Neuroptera (Henry 1994) and they were sometimes the first clue of hidden diversity within the taxon. Their role as a part of the specific mate recognition system, which is unique for each species and results in reproductive isolation, makes the recording of substrate-borne signals a very useful tool for solving taxonomic problems that defy traditional morphology based methods. Ultimately they can tell us more about the species itself than molecular markers since not only they provide the information that differences exist, but also whether we are actually dealing with biological species.

Many insects that use vibrational signals during their courtship are recognized as important pests. It is necessary to stress the possibility that for species in which substrate-borne signals are an essential part of localization, pheromone traps might not be efficient.

Although it is obvious that vibrational signals and cues play a very important role in insect life, vibrations are probably still the least understood channel of communication.

Taking into account the great number and variability of insects, it is not suprising that in many cases even the mechanism of signal production is not known and that for some insect groups a more detailed knowledge about vibroreceptors is lacking. There are only a few studies about processing of vibrational information in the central nervous system, very little is actually known about mechanisms of vibrational directionality in small insects and almost nothing about determining the distance of the source of vibrations. Recently published comprehensive reviews exist for air-borne acoustic communication in insects (Gerhardt & Huber 2002) or vibrational communication in spiders (Barth 2002) but are not available for insect vibrational communication. To close these gaps in our understanding of fundamental mechanisms of vibrational communication some effort should be made to study the vibratory environment of insects using vibrational communication living in different ecological conditions. Some future research should be on the one hand extended to greater number of insect species in different taxonomic groups and on the other hand to more detailed neurophysiological investigations of some model species. Understanding communication system such as insect vibrational communication needs a multi-level approach linking physical properties of the environment to behavior, behavior to processing in the central nervous system and eventually also to evolution. The sheer complexity of such approach and the work needed is of course overwhelming. But this should not prevent us from trying. We sincerely hope that in the future many biologist, entomologists, neurobiologists and evolutionary biologists will find insect vibrational communication as important and interesting as we do.

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank all our colleagues, past and present. Without their contribution there would be even greater gaps in Nezara's story. Our work has been supported by Slovenian Ministry of Education, Science and Sport. We also thank Drs. Mike Wilson and Antônio R. Panizzi for comments on the manuscript.

Literature Cited

Barth, F.G. 2002. A spider's world. Springer Verlag, Berlin, Heidelberg, 394p.

Bell, P.D. 1980. Multimodal communication by the black-horned tree cricket, Oecanthus nigricornis (Walker) (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). Can. J. Zool. 58: 1861-1868.

Bennet-Clark, H.C. 1998. Size and scale effects as constraints in insect sound communication. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. London B 353: 407-419.

Birch, M.C. & J.J. Keenlyside. 1991. Tapping behavior is a rhythmic communication in the death-watch beetle, Xestobium rufovillosum (Coleoptera: Anobiidae). J. Insect Behav. 4: 257-263.

Borges, M., P.C. Jepson & P.E. Howse. 1987. Long-range mate location and close-range courtship behavior of the green stink bug, Nezara viridula and its mediation by sex-pheromones. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 44: 205-212.

Breidbach, O. 1986. Studies on the stridulation of Hylotrupes bajulus (L.) (Cerambycidae: Coleoptera) - communication through support vibration - morphology and mechanics of the signal. Behav. Process. 12: 169-186.

Broad, G.R. & D.L.J. Quicke. 2000. The adaptive significance of host location by vibrational sounding in parasitoid wasps. Proc. Roy. Soc. London Ser. B 267: 2403-2409.

Brownell, P.H. & J.L. van Hemmen. 2001. Vibration sensitivity and a computational theory for prey- localizing behavior in sand scorpions. Amer. Zool. 41: 1229-1240.

Claridge, M.F. 1985. Acoustic signals in the Homoptera - Behavior, taxonomy, and evolution. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 30: 297-317.

Claridge, M.F., H.A. Dawah & M.R. Wilson. 1997. Species in insect herbivores and parasitoids - sibling species, host races and biotypes, p. 247-272. In M.F.Claridge, H.A.Dawah & M.R.Wilson (eds.). Species: The units of biodiversity. Chapman & Hall, 439p.

Claridge, M.F., J.C. Morgan & M.S. Moulds. 1999. Substrate-transmitted acoustic signals of the primitive cicada, Tettigarcta crinita Distant (Hemiptera: Cicadoidea: Tettigarctidae). J. Natur. Hist. 33: 1831-1834.

Cocroft, R.B. 1999. Parent-offspring communication in response to predators in a subsocial treehopper (Hemiptera: Membracidae: Umbonia crassicornis). Ethology 105: 553-568.

Cocroft, R.B. 2001. Vibrational communication and the ecology of group-living, herbivorous insects. Amer. Zool. 41: 1215-1221. Cocroft, R.B., T.D. Tieu, R.R. Hoy & R.N. Miles. 2000. Directionality in the mechanical response to substrate vibration in a treehopper (Hemiptera: Membracidae: Umbonia crassicornis). J. Comp. Physiol. A1 186: 695-705.

Cummings, D.L.D., G.J. Gamboa & B.J. Harding. 1999. Lateral vibrations by social wasps signal larvae to withhold salivary secretions (Polistes fuscatus, Hymenoptera: Vespidae). J. Insect Behav. 12: 465-473.

Cokl, A. 1983. Functional properties of vibroreceptors in the legs of Nezara viridula (L.) (Heteroptera: Pentatomidae). J. Comp. Physiol. 150: 261-269.

Cokl, A., H.L. McBrien & J.G. Millar. 2001. Comparison of substrate-borne vibrational signals of two stink bug species, Acrosternum hilare and Nezara viridula (Heteroptera: Pentatomidae). Ann. Entomol. Soc. Amer. 94: 471-479.

Cokl, A. & M. Virant-Doberlet. 2003. Communication with substrate-borne signals in small plant - dwelling insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 48: 29-50.

Cokl, A., M. Virant-Doberlet & A. McDowell. 1999. Vibrational directionality in the southern green stink bug, Nezara viridula (L.), is mediated by female song. Anim. Behav. 58: 1277-1283.

Cokl, A., M. Virant-Doberlet & N. Stritih. 2000. The structure and function of songs emitted by southern green stink bugs from Brazil, Florida, Italy and Slovenia. Physiol. Entomol. 25: 196-205.

De Luca, P.A. & G.K. Morris. 1998. Courtship communication in meadow katydids: Female preference for large male vibrations. Behaviour 135: 777-794.

Debaisieux, P. 1938. Organes scolopidiaux des pattes d'insectes II. Cellule 47: 77-202.

Devetak, D. 1998. Detection of substrate vibration in Neuropteroidea: A review. Acta. Zool. Fenn. 209: 87-94.

DeVries, P.J. 1990. Enhancement of symbioses between butterfly caterpillars and ants by vibrational communication. Science 248: 1104-1106.

Donahoe, K., L.A. Lewis & S.S. Schneider. 2003. The role of the vibration signal in the house-hunting process of honey bee (Apis mellifera) swarms. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 54: 593-600.

Dumortier, B. 1963. Morphology of sound emission apparatus in Arthropoda, p. 277-345. In R.-G.Busnel (ed.). Acoustic behaviour of animals. Elsevier, Amsterdam, London, New York, 933p.

Ewing, A.W. 1989. Arthropod bioacoustics. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh, 260p.

Field, L.H. & W.J. Bailey. 1997. Sound production in primitive Orthoptera from Western Australia: Sounds used in defence and social communication in Ametrus sp. and Hadrogryllacris sp. (Gryllacrididae: Orthoptera). J. Natur. Hist. 31: 1127-1141.

Gerhardt, C.H. & F. Huber. 2002. Acoustic communication in insects and anurans. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, London, 531p.

Gogala, M. 1984. Vibration producing structures and songs of terrestrial Heteroptera as systematic character. Biol. Vestn. 32: 19-36.

Gogala, M. 1985. Vibrational songs of land bugs and their production, p. 143-150. In K.Kalmring & N.Elsner (eds.). Acoustic and vibrational communication in insects. Paul Parey Verlag, Berlin, 230p.

Gogala, M. 1990. Distribution of low frequency vibrational songs in local Heteroptera. Scopolia Suppl 1: 125-132.

Greenfield, M.D. 2002. Signalers and receivers. Oxford University Press, New York, 414p.

Groot, A.T., E. van der Wal, A. Schuurman, J.H. Visser, L.H.M. Blommers & T.A. van Beek. 1998. Copulation behaviour of Lygocoris pabulinus under laboratory conditions. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 88: 219-228.

Hanrahan, S.A. & W.H. Kirchner. 1994. Acoustic orientation and communication in desert tenebrionid beetles in sand dunes. Ethology 97: 26-32.

Henry, C.S. 1994. Singing and cryptic speciation in insects. Trend. Ecol. Evolut. 9: 388-392

Hill, P.S.M. & J.R. Shadley. 2001. Talking back: Sending soil vibration signals to lekking prairie mole cricket males. Amer. Zool. 41: 1200-1214.

Hirschberger, P. 2001. Stridulation in Aphodius dung beetles: Behavioral context and intraspecific variability of song patterns in Aphodius ater (Scarabaeidae). J. Insect Behav. 14: 69-88.

Hölldobler, B. 1999. Multimodal signals in ant communication. J. Comp. Physiol. A 184: 129-141.

Hoy, R.R., A. Hoikkala & K. Kaneshiro. 1988. Hawaiian courtship songs - Evolutionary innovation in communication signals of Drosophila. Science 240: 217-219.

Hrabar, N., A. Cokl & M. Virant-Doberlet. Species specificity of male southern green stink bug reactions to the female calling song. Acta Zoologica Sinica (in press).

Hunt, R.E. 1994. Vibrational signals associated with mating behavior in the treehopper, Enchenopa binotata Say (Hemiptera: Homoptera: Membracidae). J. N. Y. Entomol. Soc. 102: 266-270.

Ishay, J., A. Motro, S. Gitter & M.B. Brown. 1974. Rhythms in acoustical communication by oriental hornet, Vespa orientalis. Anim. Behav. 22: 741-744.

Ivanov, V.D. 1993. Principles of sexual communication in caddisflies (Insecta, Trichoptera), p. 609-626. In K. Wiese, F.G. Gribakin, A.V. Popov & G. Renneinger (eds.). Sensory systems of arthropods. Birkenhäuser Verlag, Basel, 682p.

Jeram, S. & A. Cokl. 1996. Mechanoreceptors in insects: Johnston's organ in Nezara viridula (L.) (Pentatomidae: Heteroptera). Pflügers Arch.-Eur. J. Physiol. 431: R281-R282.

Kalmring, K., M. Jatho, W. Rossler & T. Sickmann. 1997. Acousto-vibratory communication in bushcrickets (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae). Entomol. Gen. 21: 265-291.

Kanmiya, K. 1990. Acoustic properties and geographic-variation in the vibratory courtship signals of the European chloropid fly, Lipara lucens Meigen (Diptera: Chloropidae). J. Ethol. 8: 105-119.

Kanmiya, K. & R. Sonobe. 2002. Records of two citrus pest whiteflies in Japan with special reference to their mating sounds (Homoptera : Aleyrodidae). Appl. Entomol. Zool. 37: 487-495.

Kingsolver, J.M., J. Romero & C.D. Johnson. 1993. Files and scrapers - circumstantial evidence for stridulation in 3 species of Amblycerus, one new (Coleoptera: Bruchidae). Pan-Pac. Entomol. 69: 122-132.

Kirchner, W.H. 1997. Acoustical communication in social insects, p. 273-300. In M.Lehrer (ed.). Orientation and communication in arthropods. Birkenhäuser Verlag, Basel, 395p.

Kubota, S. 1985. Rubbing behaviours in some aphids. Kontyû 53: 595-603.

Lewis, L.A., S.S. Schneider & G. Degrandi-Hoffman. 2002. Factors influencing the selection of recipients by workers performing vibration signals in colonies of the honeybee, Apis mellifera. Anim. Behav. 63: 361-367.

Loher, W. & M. Dambach. 1989. Reproductive behaviour, p. 43-82. In F.Huber, J.E.Moore & W.Loher (eds.). Cricket behaviour and neurobiology. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, London, 565p.

Markl, H. 1983. Vibrational communication, p. 332-353. In F.Huber & H.Markl (eds.). Neuroethology and behavioral physiology. Springer Verlag, Berlin, Heidelberg, 412p.

McBrien, H.L., A. Cokl & J.G. Millar. 2002. Comparison of substrate-borne vibrational signals of two congeneric stink bug species, Thyanta pallidovirens and T. custator accerra (Heteroptera : Pentatomidae). J. Insect Behav. 15: 715-738.

McBrien, H.L. & J.G. Millar. 2003. Substrate-borne vibrational signals of the consperse stink bug (Hemiptera: Pentatomidae). Can. Entomol. 135: 555-567.

McVean, A. & L.H. Field. 1996. Communication by substratum vibration in the New Zealand tree weta, Hemideina femorata (Stenopelmatidae: Orthoptera). J. Zool. 239: 101-122.

Menzel, J.G. & J. Tautz. 1994. Functional morphology of the subgenual organ of the carpenter ant. Tissue Cell 26: 735-746.

Michel, K., T. Amon & A. Cokl. 1983. The morphology of the leg scolopidial organs in Nezara viridula (L.) (Heteroptera: Pentatomidae). Rev. Can. Biol. Exp. 42: 139-150.

Michelsen, A., F. Fink, M. Gogala & D. Traue. 1982. Plants as transmission channels for insect vibrational songs. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 11: 269-281.

Miklas, N., A. Cokl, M. Renou & M. Virant-Doberlet. 2003a. Variability of vibratory signals and mate choice selectivity in the southern green stink bug. Behav. Process. 61: 131-142.

Miklas, N., T. Lasnier & M. Renou. 2003b. Male bugs modulate pheromone emission in response to vibratory signals from conspecifics. J. Chem. Ecol. 29: 561-574.

Miklas, N., N. Stritih, A. Cokl, M. Virant-Doberlet & M. Renou. 2001. The influence of substrate on male responsiveness to the female calling song in Nezara viridula. J. Insect Behav. 14: 313-332.

Mook, J.H. & C.G. Bruggemann. 1968. Acoustical communication by Lipara lucens (Diptera: Chloropidae). Entomol. Exp. Appl. 11: 397-402.

Nieh, J.C. 1998. The honey bee shaking signal: function and design of a modulatory communication signal. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 42: 23-36.

Nieh, J.C. & J. Tautz. 2000. Behaviour-locked signal analysis reveals weak 200-300 Hz comb vibrations during the honeybee waggle dance. J. Exp. Biol. 203: 1573-1579.

Numata, H., M. Kon, H. Fujii & T. Hidaka. 1989. Sound production in the bean bug, Riptortus clavatus Thunberg (Heteroptera, Alydidae). Appl. Entomol. Zool. 24: 169-173.

Ohya, E. 1996. External structure of the putative stridulatory apparatus of the fungus beetles, Dacne japonica and D. picta (Coleoptera: Erotylidae). Appl. Entomol. Zool. 31: 321-325.

Ohya, E. & H. Kinuura. 2001. Close range sound communications of the oak platypodid beetle Platypus quercivorus (Murayama) (Coleoptera: Platypodidae). Appl. Entomol. Zool. 36: 317-321.

Ossiannilsson, F. 1949. Insect drummers. A study on the morphology and function of the sound-producing organ of Swedish Homoptera Auchenorrhyncha with notes on their sound production. Opuscula Entomologica Supplementum 10: 1-145.

Panizzi, A.R. 1997. Wild hosts of pentatomids: Ecological significance and role in their pest status on crops. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 42: 99-122.

Pavlovcic, P. & A. Cokl. 2001. Songs of Holcostethus strictus (Fabricius): a different repertoire among landbugs (Heteroptera: Pentatomidae). Behav. Process. 53: 65-73.

Pearson, G.A. & D.M. Allen. 1996. Vibrational communication in Eusattus convexus LeConte (Coleoptera: Tenebrionidae). Coleopts. Bull. 50: 391-394.

Percy, D.M. 2002. Songs, sex and psyllid systematics.www.ento.csiro.au/psyllidssound.htm

Roces, F. & J. Tautz. 2001. Ants are deaf. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer. 109: 3080-3082.

Röhrig, A., W.H. Kirchner & R.H. Leuthold. 1999. Vibrational alarm communication in the African fungus-growing termite genus Macrotermes (Isoptera: Termitidae). Insectes Soc. 46: 71-77.

Rosengaus, R.B., C. Jordan, M.L. Lefebvre & J.F.A. Traniello. 1999. Pathogen alarm behavior in a termite: A new form of communication in social insects. Naturwissenschaften 86: 544-548.

Rupprecht, R. 1974. Vibration signals during copulation of Panorpa (Mecoptera Insecta). Experientia 30: 340-341.

Rupprecht, R. 1975. Communication of Sialis (Megaloptera) by means of vibration signals. J. Insect Physiol. 21: 305-320.

Ryan, M.A., A. Cokl & G.H. Walter. 1996. Differences in vibratory sound communication between a Slovenian and an Australian population of Nezara viridula (L) (Heteroptera: Pentatomidae). Behav. Process. 36: 183-193.

Savoyard, J.L., G.J. Gamboa, D.L.D. Cummings & R.L. Foster. 1998. The communicative meaning of body oscillations in the social wasp, Polistes fuscatus (Hymenoptera: Vespidae). Insectes Soc. 45: 215-230.

Schilman, P.E., C.R. Lazzari & G. Manrique. 2001. Comparison of disturbance stridulations in five species of triatominae bugs. Acta Tropica 79: 171-178.

Schmitt, M. & D. Traue. 1990. Morphological and bioacoustic aspects of stridulation in Criocerinae (Coleoptera: Chrysomelidae). Zool. Anz. 225: 225-240.

Schneider, S.S., S. Painter-Kurt & G. Degrandi-Hoffman. 2001. The role of the vibration signal during queen competition in colonies of the honeybee, Apis mellifera. Anim. Behav. 61: 1173-1180.

Serrano, A.R., A.C. Diogo, E. Vicoso & P.J. Fonseca. 2003. New stridulatory structures in a tiger beetle (Coleoptera: Carabidae: Cicindelinae): Morphology and sound characterization. Coleopts. Bull. 57: 161-166.

Shaw, S.R. 1994. Re-evaluation of the absolute threshold and response mode of the most sensitive known vibration detector, the cockroach's subgenual organ - A cochlea-like displacement threshold and a direct response to sound. J. Neurobiol. 25: 1167-1185.

Sismondo, E. 1980. Physical characteristics of the drumming of Meconema thalassinum. J. Insect Physiol. 26: 209-212.

Slobodchikoff, C.N. & H.G. Spangler. 1979. Two types of sound production in Eusophulus castaneus (Colepotera: Tenebrionidae). Coleopts. Bull. 33: 239-243.

Stewart, K.W. 1997. Vibrational communication in insects. Amer. Entomol. 43: 81-91.

Stölting, H., T.E. Moore & R. Lakes-Harlan. 2002. Substrate vibrations during acoustic signalling in the cicada Okanagana rimosa. J. Insect Sci. 2:2: 1-7.

Stritih, N., M. Virant-Doberlet & A. Cokl. 2000. Green stink bug Nezara viridula detects differences in amplitude between courtship song vibrations at stem and petiolus. Pflügers Arch.-Eur. J. Physiol. 439: R190-R192.

Tishechkin, D.Y. 1997. Calling signals in males of Cixiidae (Homoptera, Cicadinea) compared with acoustic signals in some other flugoroidea (Homoptera, Cicadinea, Fulgoroidea). Zool. Zh. 76: 1016-1024.

Tishechkin, D.Y. 1998. Acoustic signals of Issidae (Homoptera, Cicadinea, Fulgoroidea) compared with signals of some other Fulgoroidea with notes on taxonomic status of the subfamily Caliscelinae. Zool. Zh. 77: 1257-1265.

Travassos, M.A. & N.E. Pierce. 2000. Acoustics, context and function of vibrational signalling in a lycaenid butterfly-ant mutualism. Anim. Behav. 60: 13-26.

Vilhelmsen, L., N. Isidoro, R. Romani, H.H. Basibuyuk & D.L.J. Quicke. 2001. Host location and oviposition in a basal group of parasitic wasps: the subgenual organ, ovipositor apparatus and associated structures in the Orussidae (Hymenoptera, Insecta). Zoomorphology 121: 63-84.

Virant-Doberlet, M., A. Cokl & N. Stritih. 2000. Vibratory songs of hybrids from Brazilian and Slovenian populations of the green stink bug Nezara viridula. Pflügers Arch.-Eur. J. Physiol. 439: R196-R198.

Wilcox, R.S. 1995. Ripple communication in aquatic and semiaquatic insects. Ecoscience 2: 109-115.

Wilson, L.M., C.S. Henry, J.B. Johnson & J.P. Mccaffrey. 1993. Sound production in Phrydiuchus tau (Coleoptera, Curculionidae). Ann. Entomol. Soc. Amer. 86: 621-630.

Yack, J.E., M.L. Smith & P.J. Weatherhead. 2001. Caterpillar talk: Acoustically mediated territoriality in larval Lepidoptera. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA 98: 11371-11375.

  • Barth, F.G. 2002. A spider's world. Springer Verlag, Berlin, Heidelberg, 394p.
  • Bell, P.D. 1980. Multimodal communication by the black-horned tree cricket, Oecanthus nigricornis (Walker) (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). Can. J. Zool. 58: 1861-1868.
  • Bennet-Clark, H.C. 1998. Size and scale effects as constraints in insect sound communication. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. London B 353: 407-419.
  • Birch, M.C. & J.J. Keenlyside. 1991. Tapping behavior is a rhythmic communication in the death-watch beetle, Xestobium rufovillosum (Coleoptera: Anobiidae). J. Insect Behav. 4: 257-263.
  • Borges, M., P.C. Jepson & P.E. Howse. 1987. Long-range mate location and close-range courtship behavior of the green stink bug, Nezara viridula and its mediation by sex-pheromones. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 44: 205-212.
  • Breidbach, O. 1986. Studies on the stridulation of Hylotrupes bajulus (L.) (Cerambycidae: Coleoptera) - communication through support vibration - morphology and mechanics of the signal. Behav. Process. 12: 169-186.
  • Broad, G.R. & D.L.J. Quicke. 2000. The adaptive significance of host location by vibrational sounding in parasitoid wasps. Proc. Roy. Soc. London Ser. B 267: 2403-2409.
  • Brownell, P.H. & J.L. van Hemmen. 2001. Vibration sensitivity and a computational theory for prey- localizing behavior in sand scorpions. Amer. Zool. 41: 1229-1240.
  • Claridge, M.F. 1985. Acoustic signals in the Homoptera - Behavior, taxonomy, and evolution. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 30: 297-317.
  • Claridge, M.F., H.A. Dawah & M.R. Wilson. 1997. Species in insect herbivores and parasitoids - sibling species, host races and biotypes, p. 247-272. In M.F.Claridge, H.A.Dawah & M.R.Wilson (eds.). Species: The units of biodiversity. Chapman & Hall, 439p.
  • Claridge, M.F., J.C. Morgan & M.S. Moulds. 1999. Substrate-transmitted acoustic signals of the primitive cicada, Tettigarcta crinita Distant (Hemiptera: Cicadoidea: Tettigarctidae). J. Natur. Hist. 33: 1831-1834.
  • Cocroft, R.B. 1999. Parent-offspring communication in response to predators in a subsocial treehopper (Hemiptera: Membracidae: Umbonia crassicornis). Ethology 105: 553-568.
  • Cocroft, R.B. 2001. Vibrational communication and the ecology of group-living, herbivorous insects. Amer. Zool. 41: 1215-1221.
  • Cocroft, R.B., T.D. Tieu, R.R. Hoy & R.N. Miles. 2000. Directionality in the mechanical response to substrate vibration in a treehopper (Hemiptera: Membracidae: Umbonia crassicornis). J. Comp. Physiol. A1 186: 695-705.
  • Cummings, D.L.D., G.J. Gamboa & B.J. Harding. 1999. Lateral vibrations by social wasps signal larvae to withhold salivary secretions (Polistes fuscatus, Hymenoptera: Vespidae). J. Insect Behav. 12: 465-473.
  • Cokl, A. 1983. Functional properties of vibroreceptors in the legs of Nezara viridula (L.) (Heteroptera: Pentatomidae). J. Comp. Physiol. 150: 261-269.
  • Cokl, A., H.L. McBrien & J.G. Millar. 2001. Comparison of substrate-borne vibrational signals of two stink bug species, Acrosternum hilare and Nezara viridula (Heteroptera: Pentatomidae). Ann. Entomol. Soc. Amer. 94: 471-479.
  • Cokl, A. & M. Virant-Doberlet. 2003. Communication with substrate-borne signals in small plant - dwelling insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 48: 29-50.
  • Cokl, A., M. Virant-Doberlet & A. McDowell. 1999 Vibrational directionality in the southern green stink bug, Nezara viridula (L.), is mediated by female song. Anim. Behav. 58: 1277-1283.
  • Cokl, A., M. Virant-Doberlet & N. Stritih. 2000. The structure and function of songs emitted by southern green stink bugs from Brazil, Florida, Italy and Slovenia. Physiol. Entomol. 25: 196-205.
  • De Luca, P.A. & G.K. Morris. 1998. Courtship communication in meadow katydids: Female preference for large male vibrations. Behaviour 135: 777-794.
  • Debaisieux, P. 1938. Organes scolopidiaux des pattes d'insectes II. Cellule 47: 77-202.
  • Devetak, D. 1998. Detection of substrate vibration in Neuropteroidea: A review. Acta. Zool. Fenn. 209: 87-94.
  • DeVries, P.J. 1990. Enhancement of symbioses between butterfly caterpillars and ants by vibrational communication. Science 248: 1104-1106.
  • Donahoe, K., L.A. Lewis & S.S. Schneider. 2003. The role of the vibration signal in the house-hunting process of honey bee (Apis mellifera) swarms. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 54: 593-600.
  • Dumortier, B. 1963. Morphology of sound emission apparatus in Arthropoda, p. 277-345. In R.-G.Busnel (ed.). Acoustic behaviour of animals. Elsevier, Amsterdam, London, New York, 933p.
  • Ewing, A.W. 1989. Arthropod bioacoustics. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh, 260p.
  • Field, L.H. & W.J. Bailey. 1997. Sound production in primitive Orthoptera from Western Australia: Sounds used in defence and social communication in Ametrus sp. and Hadrogryllacris sp. (Gryllacrididae: Orthoptera). J. Natur. Hist. 31: 1127-1141.
  • Gerhardt, C.H. & F. Huber. 2002. Acoustic communication in insects and anurans. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, London, 531p.
  • Gogala, M. 1984. Vibration producing structures and songs of terrestrial Heteroptera as systematic character. Biol. Vestn. 32: 19-36.
  • Gogala, M. 1985. Vibrational songs of land bugs and their production, p. 143-150. In K.Kalmring & N.Elsner (eds.). Acoustic and vibrational communication in insects. Paul Parey Verlag, Berlin, 230p.
  • Gogala, M. 1990. Distribution of low frequency vibrational songs in local Heteroptera. Scopolia Suppl 1: 125-132.
  • Greenfield, M.D. 2002. Signalers and receivers. Oxford University Press, New York, 414p.
  • Groot, A.T., E. van der Wal, A. Schuurman, J.H. Visser, L.H.M. Blommers & T.A. van Beek. 1998. Copulation behaviour of Lygocoris pabulinus under laboratory conditions. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 88: 219-228.
  • Hanrahan, S.A. & W.H. Kirchner. 1994. Acoustic orientation and communication in desert tenebrionid beetles in sand dunes. Ethology 97: 26-32.
  • Henry, C.S. 1994. Singing and cryptic speciation in insects. Trend. Ecol. Evolut. 9: 388-392
  • Hill, P.S.M. & J.R. Shadley. 2001. Talking back: Sending soil vibration signals to lekking prairie mole cricket males. Amer. Zool. 41: 1200-1214.
  • Hirschberger, P. 2001. Stridulation in Aphodius dung beetles: Behavioral context and intraspecific variability of song patterns in Aphodius ater (Scarabaeidae). J. Insect Behav. 14: 69-88.
  • Hölldobler, B. 1999. Multimodal signals in ant communication. J. Comp. Physiol. A 184: 129-141.
  • Hoy, R.R., A. Hoikkala & K. Kaneshiro. 1988. Hawaiian courtship songs - Evolutionary innovation in communication signals of Drosophila. Science 240: 217-219.
  • Hrabar, N., A. Cokl & M. Virant-Doberlet. Species specificity of male southern green stink bug reactions to the female calling song. Acta Zoologica Sinica (in press).
  • Hunt, R.E. 1994. Vibrational signals associated with mating behavior in the treehopper, Enchenopa binotata Say (Hemiptera: Homoptera: Membracidae). J. N. Y. Entomol. Soc. 102: 266-270.
  • Ishay, J., A. Motro, S. Gitter & M.B. Brown. 1974. Rhythms in acoustical communication by oriental hornet, Vespa orientalis. Anim. Behav. 22: 741-744.
  • Ivanov, V.D. 1993. Principles of sexual communication in caddisflies (Insecta, Trichoptera), p. 609-626. In K. Wiese, F.G. Gribakin, A.V. Popov & G. Renneinger (eds.). Sensory systems of arthropods. Birkenhäuser Verlag, Basel, 682p.
  • Jeram, S. & A. Cokl. 1996. Mechanoreceptors in insects: Johnston's organ in Nezara viridula (L.) (Pentatomidae: Heteroptera). Pflügers Arch.-Eur. J. Physiol. 431: R281-R282.
  • Kalmring, K., M. Jatho, W. Rossler & T. Sickmann. 1997. Acousto-vibratory communication in bushcrickets (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae). Entomol. Gen. 21: 265-291.
  • Kanmiya, K. 1990. Acoustic properties and geographic-variation in the vibratory courtship signals of the European chloropid fly, Lipara lucens Meigen (Diptera: Chloropidae). J. Ethol. 8: 105-119.
  • Kanmiya, K. & R. Sonobe. 2002. Records of two citrus pest whiteflies in Japan with special reference to their mating sounds (Homoptera : Aleyrodidae). Appl. Entomol. Zool. 37: 487-495.
  • Kingsolver, J.M., J. Romero & C.D. Johnson. 1993. Files and scrapers - circumstantial evidence for stridulation in 3 species of Amblycerus, one new (Coleoptera: Bruchidae). Pan-Pac. Entomol. 69: 122-132.
  • Kirchner, W.H. 1997. Acoustical communication in social insects, p. 273-300. In M.Lehrer (ed.). Orientation and communication in arthropods. Birkenhäuser Verlag, Basel, 395p.
  • Kubota, S. 1985. Rubbing behaviours in some aphids. Kontyû 53: 595-603.
  • Lewis, L.A., S.S. Schneider & G. Degrandi-Hoffman. 2002. Factors influencing the selection of recipients by workers performing vibration signals in colonies of the honeybee, Apis mellifera. Anim. Behav. 63: 361-367.
  • Loher, W. & M. Dambach. 1989. Reproductive behaviour, p. 43-82. In F.Huber, J.E.Moore & W.Loher (eds.). Cricket behaviour and neurobiology. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, London, 565p.
  • Markl, H. 1983. Vibrational communication, p. 332-353. In F.Huber & H.Markl (eds.). Neuroethology and behavioral physiology. Springer Verlag, Berlin, Heidelberg, 412p.
  • McBrien, H.L., A. Cokl & J.G. Millar. 2002. Comparison of substrate-borne vibrational signals of two congeneric stink bug species, Thyanta pallidovirens and T. custator accerra (Heteroptera : Pentatomidae). J. Insect Behav. 15: 715-738.
  • McBrien, H.L. & J.G. Millar. 2003. Substrate-borne vibrational signals of the consperse stink bug (Hemiptera: Pentatomidae). Can. Entomol. 135: 555-567.
  • McVean, A. & L.H. Field. 1996. Communication by substratum vibration in the New Zealand tree weta, Hemideina femorata (Stenopelmatidae: Orthoptera). J. Zool. 239: 101-122.
  • Menzel, J.G. & J. Tautz. 1994. Functional morphology of the subgenual organ of the carpenter ant. Tissue Cell 26: 735-746.
  • Michel, K., T. Amon & A. Cokl. 1983. The morphology of the leg scolopidial organs in Nezara viridula (L.) (Heteroptera: Pentatomidae). Rev. Can. Biol. Exp. 42: 139-150.
  • Michelsen, A., F. Fink, M. Gogala & D. Traue. 1982. Plants as transmission channels for insect vibrational songs. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 11: 269-281.
  • Miklas, N., A. Cokl, M. Renou & M. Virant-Doberlet. 2003a. Variability of vibratory signals and mate choice selectivity in the southern green stink bug. Behav. Process. 61: 131-142.
  • Miklas, N., T. Lasnier & M. Renou. 2003b Male bugs modulate pheromone emission in response to vibratory signals from conspecifics. J. Chem. Ecol. 29: 561-574.
  • Miklas, N., N. Stritih, A. Cokl, M. Virant-Doberlet & M. Renou. 2001. The influence of substrate on male responsiveness to the female calling song in Nezara viridula J. Insect Behav. 14: 313-332.
  • Mook, J.H. & C.G. Bruggemann. 1968. Acoustical communication by Lipara lucens (Diptera: Chloropidae). Entomol. Exp. Appl. 11: 397-402.
  • Nieh, J.C. 1998. The honey bee shaking signal: function and design of a modulatory communication signal. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 42: 23-36.
  • Nieh, J.C. & J. Tautz. 2000. Behaviour-locked signal analysis reveals weak 200-300 Hz comb vibrations during the honeybee waggle dance. J. Exp. Biol. 203: 1573-1579.
  • Numata, H., M. Kon, H. Fujii & T. Hidaka. 1989. Sound production in the bean bug, Riptortus clavatus Thunberg (Heteroptera, Alydidae). Appl. Entomol. Zool. 24: 169-173.
  • Ohya, E. 1996. External structure of the putative stridulatory apparatus of the fungus beetles, Dacne japonica and D. picta (Coleoptera: Erotylidae). Appl. Entomol. Zool. 31: 321-325.
  • Ohya, E. & H. Kinuura. 2001. Close range sound communications of the oak platypodid beetle Platypus quercivorus (Murayama) (Coleoptera: Platypodidae). Appl. Entomol. Zool. 36: 317-321.
  • Ossiannilsson, F. 1949. Insect drummers. A study on the morphology and function of the sound-producing organ of Swedish Homoptera Auchenorrhyncha with notes on their sound production. Opuscula Entomologica Supplementum 10: 1-145.
  • Panizzi, A.R. 1997. Wild hosts of pentatomids: Ecological significance and role in their pest status on crops. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 42: 99-122.
  • Pavlovcic, P. & A. Cokl. 2001. Songs of Holcostethus strictus (Fabricius): a different repertoire among landbugs (Heteroptera: Pentatomidae). Behav. Process. 53: 65-73.
  • Pearson, G.A. & D.M. Allen. 1996. Vibrational communication in Eusattus convexus LeConte (Coleoptera: Tenebrionidae). Coleopts. Bull. 50: 391-394.
  • Roces, F. & J. Tautz. 2001 Ants are deaf. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer. 109: 3080-3082.
  • Röhrig, A., W.H. Kirchner & R.H. Leuthold. 1999. Vibrational alarm communication in the African fungus-growing termite genus Macrotermes (Isoptera: Termitidae). Insectes Soc. 46: 71-77.
  • Rosengaus, R.B., C. Jordan, M.L. Lefebvre & J.F.A. Traniello. 1999. Pathogen alarm behavior in a termite: A new form of communication in social insects. Naturwissenschaften 86: 544-548.
  • Rupprecht, R. 1974. Vibration signals during copulation of Panorpa (Mecoptera Insecta). Experientia 30: 340-341.
  • Rupprecht, R. 1975. Communication of Sialis (Megaloptera) by means of vibration signals. J. Insect Physiol. 21: 305-320.
  • Ryan, M.A., A. Cokl & G.H. Walter. 1996. Differences in vibratory sound communication between a Slovenian and an Australian population of Nezara viridula (L) (Heteroptera: Pentatomidae). Behav. Process. 36: 183-193.
  • Savoyard, J.L., G.J. Gamboa, D.L.D. Cummings & R.L. Foster. 1998. The communicative meaning of body oscillations in the social wasp, Polistes fuscatus (Hymenoptera: Vespidae). Insectes Soc. 45: 215-230.
  • Schilman, P.E., C.R. Lazzari & G. Manrique. 2001. Comparison of disturbance stridulations in five species of triatominae bugs. Acta Tropica 79: 171-178.
  • Schmitt, M. & D. Traue. 1990. Morphological and bioacoustic aspects of stridulation in Criocerinae (Coleoptera: Chrysomelidae). Zool. Anz. 225: 225-240.
  • Schneider, S.S., S. Painter-Kurt & G. Degrandi-Hoffman. 2001. The role of the vibration signal during queen competition in colonies of the honeybee, Apis mellifera. Anim. Behav. 61: 1173-1180.
  • Serrano, A.R., A.C. Diogo, E. Vicoso & P.J. Fonseca. 2003. New stridulatory structures in a tiger beetle (Coleoptera: Carabidae: Cicindelinae): Morphology and sound characterization. Coleopts. Bull. 57: 161-166.
  • Shaw, S.R. 1994. Re-evaluation of the absolute threshold and response mode of the most sensitive known vibration detector, the cockroach's subgenual organ - A cochlea-like displacement threshold and a direct response to sound. J. Neurobiol. 25: 1167-1185.
  • Sismondo, E. 1980. Physical characteristics of the drumming of Meconema thalassinum. J. Insect Physiol. 26: 209-212.
  • Slobodchikoff, C.N. & H.G. Spangler. 1979. Two types of sound production in Eusophulus castaneus (Colepotera: Tenebrionidae). Coleopts. Bull. 33: 239-243.
  • Stewart, K.W. 1997. Vibrational communication in insects. Amer. Entomol. 43: 81-91.
  • Stölting, H., T.E. Moore & R. Lakes-Harlan. 2002. Substrate vibrations during acoustic signalling in the cicada Okanagana rimosa. J. Insect Sci. 2:2: 1-7.
  • Stritih, N., M. Virant-Doberlet & A. Cokl. 2000. Green stink bug Nezara viridula detects differences in amplitude between courtship song vibrations at stem and petiolus. Pflügers Arch.-Eur. J. Physiol. 439: R190-R192.
  • Tishechkin, D.Y. 1997. Calling signals in males of Cixiidae (Homoptera, Cicadinea) compared with acoustic signals in some other flugoroidea (Homoptera, Cicadinea, Fulgoroidea). Zool. Zh. 76: 1016-1024.
  • Tishechkin, D.Y. 1998. Acoustic signals of Issidae (Homoptera, Cicadinea, Fulgoroidea) compared with signals of some other Fulgoroidea with notes on taxonomic status of the subfamily Caliscelinae. Zool. Zh. 77: 1257-1265.
  • Travassos, M.A. & N.E. Pierce. 2000. Acoustics, context and function of vibrational signalling in a lycaenid butterfly-ant mutualism. Anim. Behav. 60: 13-26.
  • Vilhelmsen, L., N. Isidoro, R. Romani, H.H. Basibuyuk & D.L.J. Quicke. 2001. Host location and oviposition in a basal group of parasitic wasps: the subgenual organ, ovipositor apparatus and associated structures in the Orussidae (Hymenoptera, Insecta). Zoomorphology 121: 63-84.
  • Virant-Doberlet, M., A. Cokl & N. Stritih. 2000. Vibratory songs of hybrids from Brazilian and Slovenian populations of the green stink bug Nezara viridula. Pflügers Arch.-Eur. J. Physiol. 439: R196-R198.
  • Wilcox, R.S. 1995. Ripple communication in aquatic and semiaquatic insects. Ecoscience 2: 109-115.
  • Wilson, L.M., C.S. Henry, J.B. Johnson & J.P. Mccaffrey. 1993. Sound production in Phrydiuchus tau (Coleoptera, Curculionidae). Ann. Entomol. Soc. Amer. 86: 621-630.
  • Yack, J.E., M.L. Smith & P.J. Weatherhead. 2001. Caterpillar talk: Acoustically mediated territoriality in larval Lepidoptera. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA 98: 11371-11375.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    14 July 2004
  • Date of issue
    Apr 2004
Sociedade Entomológica do Brasil Sociedade Entomológica do Brasil, R. Harry Prochet, 55, 86047-040 Londrina PR Brasil, Tel.: (55 43) 3342 3987 - Londrina - PR - Brazil
E-mail: editor@seb.org.br