Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Orality, literacy and reading: dealing with differences and complexities in the public school

Abstracts

The text reports on a research in the field of the teaching of reading and writing carried out with two first year classes of fundamental education at a public school in the city of São Paulo. Starting from diagnostics based on the oral culture of the pupils that displayed learning difficulties in reading even after three or four years of schooling, we sought to create, administer, and monitor a teaching program based on the transition between oral culture and written culture. It is a research of qualitative nature, whose concern is to deal with the heterogeneity of the classroom in its process and in its complexity. The theoretical framework is multidisciplinary, including areas of study under the purview of linguistics, psychoanalysis, and education and, also, of the work of researchers of the field conventionally denominated "oral-literate equation" (Havelock, 1995). The results bring data, reports of interventions, and reflections that can help to structure teaching programs for the age group studied here, whilst questioning the excessive focus that constructivist methodologies place upon the act of writing or on writing itself. It puts forward a new perspective of the intertwining between pupils' oral culture and school literacy, assuming an ambience of orality, literacy, and reading that includes electronic media and a more collective organization of the school work in the first series, particularly in the articulation ("hinge") between early childhood education and the first grade.

Orality; Literacy; Reading; New supports; Early childhood


Apresenta-se o relato de uma pesquisa, na área de ensino da leitura e da escrita, realizada em duas séries de primeiros anos do ensino fundamental, em uma escola pública da cidade de São Paulo. A partir de diagnósticos baseados na cultura oral de alunos que apresentavam dificuldades de aprendizagem de leitura mesmo após três ou quatro anos de escolarização, procurou-se criar, ministrar e monitorar um programa de ensino baseado na transição entre cultura oral e cultura escrita. Trata-se de uma pesquisa de natureza qualitativa, cuja preocupação é lidar com a heterogeneidade da sala de aula em seu processo e em sua complexidade. O viés teórico é multidisciplinar, incluindo áreas de pesquisas sujeitas às influências da linguística, da psicanálise e da educação e, ainda, dos estudos de pesquisadores da área que se convencionou chamar "equação oralidade-escrita" (Havelock,1995). Os resultados trazem dados, relatos de intervenções e reflexões que podem subsidiar programas de ensino para a faixa etária estudada e questiona ainda o foco excessivo que as metodologias construtivistas põem sobre o ato de escrever ou sobre a própria escrita. Propõe uma nova perspectiva de enlaçamento entre a cultura oral dos alunos e letramento escolar, assumindo uma ambiência de oralidade, alfabetização e leitura que inclui o suporte eletrônico e uma organização mais coletiva do trabalho escolar nas séries iniciais, sobretudo na articulação ("dobradiça") entre o ensino infantil e o fundamental I.

Oralidade; Alfabetização; Leitura; Novos suportes; Infantil


ARTICLES

Orality, literacy and reading: differences and complexities facing the public school

Claudemir Belintane

Contact

ABSTRACT

The article presents findings of a research in teaching reading and writing undertaken in two classes during the early years of primary education in a public school in São Paulo. Departing from diagnostics based on oral culture of pupils experiencing learning difficulties in reading even after three or four years of schooling, we tried to design, implement, and monitor a program of instruction based on the transition between oral and written culture. This is a qualitative research, whose aim is to deal with the heterogeneity of the classroom in its process and complexity. The theoretical perspective adopted is a multidisciplinary one, including research areas influenced by linguistics, psychoanalysis, education and but by studies conducted by researchers in a field that came to be known "orality-literacy equation" (Havelock, 1995). The results provide data, reports of interventions, and reflections that may support education programs for the age groups studied; moreover, they also question the excessive focus constructivist methodologies place on the act of writing or on writing itself. The paper proposes a new perspective for relating the oral culture of the pupils and school literacy, presupposing an environment of orality, literacy and reading which includes electronic support (e. g., digital media) and a more collective organization of school work in the early grades, especially in the transition ("hinge") between early childhood education and primary level.

Keywords: Orality - Literacy - Reading - New media - Child.

This paper reports and discusses data, events and strategies of a research conducted in a public school in western São Paulo, from April 2007 to December 2009. The research project was funded by FAPESP

The research was conducted in two phases: first, we tried to diagnose 43 pupils selected by their teachers and the coordinator of the school as being "pre-syllabic" or "syllabic", i. e., pupils with three or four years of schooling not yet fulfilling the basic conditions for proficiently in reading and writing. Pupils in this reading/writing level make for approximately 20% of those fourth-grade pupils, who score below the minimum rating scales, i. e., those whose scores range between 120 and 150 points in the SAEB evaluation (Brazil, 2007).

In the second phase, the research accompanied two first grade classes in elementary school (6-7 year-olds); the author implemented, along with their two teachers, a program of orality, reading, and writing based on the results obtained in the first phase. In this article we discuss broader insights of the research and report the more general findings and conclusions reached in the two phases. More punctual reflections that open up prospects for further research will be discussed elsewhere and in partnership with other project participants.

The explicit recognition of this involvement and its effects and possible misunderstandings allowed the researcher and his team to maintain some precision when considering the involvement of participants, especially the filtering and the relativization of their written or oral reports, and still preserve certain readiness to document events with no apparent importance (nonsense, repetitions, unusual utterances), which could contain revealing signifiers and clues (Ginzburg, 1989) that originated in both individual assistance as more collective interactions.

It is recognized here that locating the research and the analysis of data among the perspectives of different disciplines (psychoanalysis, linguistics, discourse analysis) is a risky and complex methodology. On the other hand, working from a very narrow perspective, or even from too close an affiliation to any particular theoretical perspective is not an attractive solution in the face of the complexity of contemporary education. We looked, then, for a paradigm more open, more prone to adjusting or even recognizing the limits of its concepts in face of the data that emerged. In search of singularities, the researchers were faced with the need to borrow and reconceptualize procedures and techniques from several fields, including certain adjustments between the design of a qualitative research – perhaps closer to the "case study" – and a "psychoanalytic listening." To better understand this need we have to consider that the research unfolded along two axes: (1) more objective

The research: starting from interviews, interventions and findings

In the first phase of the project, the aim of the research group was to diagnose 43 children identified by school as pupils who, after three or four years of schooling (aged 9 to 13 years), not yet fully mastered the alphabetic principle (in the terminology used by the school and the network to which the school belongs, they were 'pre-syllabic' or 'syllabic-alphabetic' pupils).

Since we wanted a diagnosis that took into account the singularities of each child interviewed, and at the same time, recognized the risk that the data would be lost in a plurality of information, we chose to conduct semi-structured interviews during which the researcher could paraphrase or explain and illustrate the questions to the pupils according to their needs. The function of the interview guide was to standardize the interviews to some degree and to avoid drifting and extrapolations.

The data were re-examined one by one in a supervision session held by the coordinator of the project – in short, the technique was to juxtapose two kinds of listening: the interviewer listening to the pupil during the interview, the coordinator listening to the interviewer, and then comparing the two records (the oral communication during the meeting and the written report) while still referring to other documents, video and audio recordings. The most obvious data contained in a written report received quantitative treatment and helped choosing the group of pupils who would undergo individual counseling, since the team could not follow in detail all 43 pupils.

Table 1 presents a quantitative picture of the data obtained and the following paragraphs provide then the justifications for the choice of items diagnosed and present some more qualitative comments from the listening sessions.

Items 1 to 4 diagnosed key elements of the child's own identification. According to the data in Table 1, we noted that these basic elements of citizenship are still a considerable problem for these children. The date of birth, for example, is still a problem for more than 70% – typically, the interviewer used other expressions more typical of childhood and of spoken language to translate the term date of birth: "When is your birthday?", "When do you have a birthday?", and yet the answers were incomplete and provided, in general, only a day or a month. The full name of the family also came to be a discovery for some pupils, as they were surprised to learn that their own name coincided with that of their father or grandfather. Departing from these data, we can already conclude that the request for this information has two origins: one originated from everyday family speech ("When is your birthday?", "What day were you born?", "Who are your parents?", "Where do you live?," etc..), another based on bureaucratic formality of writing (date of birth, affiliation, address, etc., as it could appear, for example, in a application form for employment or even in the school record) – already at this point are evident signs of a confrontation between writing and speaking skills that might hinder the access of pupils to the minimum requirements of citizenship. This evidence was confirmed by another piece of research by the team conducted with older pupils from a slum in the western zone of São Paulo,

Items 5, 6 and 7 had as objective to study the oral memory of the pupils and their narrative and intertextual skills. In these activities, we found strong evidence confirming the initial hypothesis of the project: about 70% of pupils who not yet mastered the alphabet presented difficulties in these activities.

Item 8 consisted of only naming objects from images; with this item we sought to check whether there were cases of severe aphasia. It was observed whether the pupil named the objects directly or if the image was first put in the context of use, for example, using "for cutting" and meaning "scissors" or "knife," information that might point to a possible "similarity disorder" (Jakobson, 1995). With the data from this item we found that nearly all pupils did well in naming, only two pupils showed partial problems in this skill, but not enough to clearly characterize a type of aphasia.

Items 9 to 13 were to assess the abilities in the handling of significant units of speech, especially syllabic severability and reversibility. Here, pupils showed more difficulties than in previous sections. The oral syllabic reversibility, item 11 (syllabic inversion of dissyllables), for example, for boys, was an almost insurmountable difficulty and reached 100% of respondents; although later during the individual sessions we noted that many of them learned the strategy of inversion with relative ease. Also in item 13, reading rebus, almost all pupils showed many difficulties, however, as in relation to item 11, with a little instruction, the majority quickly mastered the process.

Comparing these data, we can already show a correlation among items 5, 6, and 7 (intertextuality and handling of full texts) and items 9 to 13 (syllabic units), i. e., this comparison reinforces the hypothesis of the research: children who know little about full texts of oral tradition and their handling do not master very well syllabic units, and, as discussed in the second phase of the research, the more complicated the case becomes the more consistent this correlation.

Situating this diagnosis in the general horizon of the educational and linguistic concepts, one can say that its items include oral skills that cover a scope that goes from the textual level, with its intertextual relations – a position rigorously advocated by constructivist and socio-interactionist methodologies, and also by official documents of education systems – to the lower levels (phoneme, syllable, word) – a position advocated by traditional methods and by advocates of the phonic method of alphabetization (Brasil, 2003). However, the theoretical basis of our diagnosis is not merely a compromise with these theoretical-methodological poles, the emphasis, for example, on the elements on the lower levels finds its theoretical justification in the history of writing, as is the case, for example, in the adoption of the rebus as an element of diagnosis, since it figured as a kind of hinge between the ideographic and alphabetic writing (Gelb, 1952) and in studies that juxtapose diachronically orality and writing, which Havelock (1995) termed "orality-literacy equation"(p. 17). The hypothesis that it is necessary to resort to lower levels is also based on previous studies (Belintane, 2008), which found that pupils subjected almost exclusively to everyday speech (the use of the imperative, the hasty dialogues composed of short sentences and fragments in which the contextual factors had an excessive weight) and little exposure to narrative and linguistic playfulness tend to take words and sentences as indivisible blocks ([tir'sudaí] – for "tire isso daí" ("take it from there"), or [pegalá] for "pegue lá" ("take it there") and be surprised with segmentations, cuts and losses required by alphabetic writing.

When discussing the topic of the oral language of poor children, there is almost immediate allusion to the controversy about the 'inadequate mother' and theories of linguistic deficit, and the controversies summarized by Soares (1989) and discussed in the collection edited by Patto (1997) on language and poverty. In the case of this project, what is at stake is not the detection of a restricted code that would be visible the speech of children from the periphery as opposed to the elaborated code of the more privileged classes (Bernstein, 1997). What we tried to find during the study was the greater or lesser presence of texts of oral origin (narratives, rhymes, songs and others) both in the memory of the children and in the school program. In the case of the school program, the team sought to ascertain whether in the classroom the oral text was utilized in its bodily and linguistic dynamics, which, according Belintane (2008), are strategies essential in preparing the foundations of literacy and reading. In other words, we wondered how orality, defined as a set of genres of oral origin for fiction and entertainment, was implicated in the general conditions of literacy of the children and in the systematization of the regular teaching of reading and writing.

What did we find out after the interventions?

After these diagnoses, the group sought to teach two weekly classes of reinforcement for part of the pupils (six girls and 10 boys). The sessions were individual or in small groups, always with reference to the initial diagnosis. Consider the relationship between initial diagnosis and final performance after one semester of intervention (August-November 2007) in Table 2:

As can be seen in Table 3 below, there is no case of a pupil who – having showed an interesting oral repertoire (+ sign) in the initial diagnosis – has not experienced some improvement in reading in the final situation. The two more complex cases which remained stagnant even after the sessions, showed an initial situation jeopardized (- sign) in terms of mastering the items of the diagnosis and were exactly those two pupils who were confused when simply naming images. These two pupils were a girl (IV, line 1), whose attendance could not be continued because of her resistance, and a boy (RD, line 10) who is being treated for the past two years, and whose progress was achieved only after a year attendance. RD was one of the most complex cases of this public school, but his stay in the program has allowed considerable advances: when we concluded the second phase of the research, this pupil read with some fluency.

We would reemphasize, then, the following hypothesis: pupils lacking capacity for intertextual handling and language and narrative skills tend to experience serious problems while learning how to read and write.

The project started another phase, during which we sought support two grades of elementary school, maintaining a teaching program based on diagnoses, i. e., for each item in Table 2 a curricular topic was set up. In the course of the year, we also sought to observe the "dynamics of renitency" of pupils who tended to complete the year without complete mastery of the alphabet and reading. "Dynamics of renitency" is defined here as the way how a pupil resisted to school strategies, both those that aimed at teaching reading and writing and those that aimed to involve them in performative activities in the field of orality.

Search for a singular model of treatment

The listening of the pupils in the school situation as it was exercised in this study differs substantially from a psychoanalytic listening. While listening in psychoanalysis, as Freud pointed out (1988),

While in the clinic the patient's interaction is entirely open, motivated by situations experienced, imagined or even dreamed of and still scored by the scansion of the analyst, in the sessions in our research we confronted the pupil with a textual set full of ambiguities and possibilities for subjective understandings. Most times, the trigger of a significant chain was a ludic-literary text or an element of a narrative, as can be seen in these two cases below, one from the first phase and another from the second:

  • VG (11 year-old child with four years of schooling, came to the sessions with many difficulties, without yet mastering the alphabetic principle and with an oversensitive renitency). S/he got involved in the sessions after the valorization of a significant clues obtained when – while s/he was paraphrasing the rhyme "Hoje é domingo," ["Today is Sunday,"] – s/he took on a tone of jest reading the words "a gente é fraco/cai no buraco/o buraco é fundo/acabou-se o mundo" ["we are weak / fall into the pit / the hole is deep / the world ends" and added: "I once fell into a hole when I was little." As s/he noticed that the researcher appreciated her/his speech, s/he continued: "My mom says I don't learn to read because I fell into the hole and a stove fell on my head." The researcher seized on the association between "not read" and "fall into the hole" and together they remade the rhymes citing comically the situation of VG falling into the hole. This delicate listening to his situation led him to enjoy more of the texts used in class and playing with words. Shortly thereafter, VG committed her/himself very much in the sessions, and little by little, escaping the awkward position in which the speech of his mother put him; in the end, s/he stated that s/he would show her/his mother that s/he could read, and that the story of "falling in the hole" was not true.

  • MT (7 year-old former street child). Experienced improvement with the help of two figures: "Little Thumb" of Perrault's tales, and "Saci Perêre", a character of Brazilian folklore,

From these and other cases experienced in the project, it began to be possible to devise a model of instruction which, although based on psychoanalytic listening, was also structured in the linguistic-literary handling of texts of children's oral culture – for example, in case I, a rhyme and acceptance of a nonsense allowed the pupil to come closer to teaching proposals; in case II, it was a narrative with its mythical fictional universe that allows for this openning. From this fictional ambience full of ambiguities, it becomes easier the emergence of a subjectivity predisposed to a confrontation between an excessive corporeality (run around and play without too many rules) and a posture of self-restraint that writing brings into play (be seated, concentrate). It is at this point that the situation comes closer to a transferential dynamics from which values can be rebalanced. Just as in psychoanalysis, in which the patient invariably introduces a theory about their symptoms and eventually includes the analyst as part of this theory and the very symptom (Nasio, 1993), in this study the researcher was faced with explanations similar to those reported above.

The same happens with the concept that the pupils present about their way to read or deal with letters and texts. For example, there were many utterances as these: "one must read slowly not to make mistakes," "I can only write, not read," "this is no class because there's nothing to copy", "I'll write my way," or "I will not read anything." From the conjunction of these two types of explanation, it is possible to detect some subjective positions regarding writing: (1) "of conformism with familiar justifications," (2) "of excessive caution because of the possibility of making mistakes" (3) "of carelessness" and (4) "of compensation" (says s/he knows something else, like how to write, copy), (5) "of renitency", (6) "of infantilization". This list may remain opened, and the number of such positions need not be fixed, it is important to include this readiness when listening, the idea that this explanation (the theory about the symptom / failure) will eventually appear and needs to be re-signified.

It is interesting to emphasize that the concept of subject that guides this research is not the same as that sustained by constructivists or sociointeractionists, i. e., the active subject that changes phases, who builds the writing from their trials and errors, but rather an eccentric subject, who is positioned in relation to the discourse of the Other (Lacan, 1998) and who emerges in the language in such an evanescent manner, between words, between sentences, between texts. For example, MT, when he started the sessions, had a position of reluctance, but became enchanted with the stories of Little Thumb and Saci and with his instructor (who was the source of these stories); he gained some fluency and agreed to reposition himself in the face of the letters and reading and writing activities. However, the staff knew his position would oscilate, i. e., it departed from the idea that this predisposition that looks like an active subject would eclipse and might require new strategies of the person who instructed him. Another interesting fact that seems to support the hypothesis of this subjective evanescence is the fact that MT and other pupils studied did not transfer directly to the classroom the knowledge they gained in the consultations; to make that happen, even partially, specific strategies were needed as for example, transfer to the classroom content that the pupil mastered in the sessions, or also that the researcher participated in some classes with the teacher and explicitly recognize the changed position of the pupil.

The model of listening sought here is based on language and has as its tools orality and literature. Unlike other forms of listening, its goal is quite explicit: to change the pupil's subjective positioning in relation to writing and orality skills. Listening, thus, is an activity whose ethics and technique need to be redefined specifically for this field, because it became very clear in the research, from the initial series, that we need to enhance subtle verbalizations and attitudes of pupils, in order to reposition them face a more oral language, more prone to the poetic function, that is, rich enough to give consistency to the scansions of the alphabet and to the textual logic of reading and writing.

Phase II of the project: some possible conclusions

The first important observation refers to the entry of the child in this cycle. There is, at least at the school studied, an explicit concern in learning more about the children and their conditions of orality and literacy (what they bring from their environment and early childhood education). The team studied the records of pupils coming from elementary schools, but as there is still no practice of systematically organizing this transition, children's materials were delivered by the parents not before one month after the school start – the ideal would have been to start the year with an assessment of this material.

After the analysis of portfolios received, it was possible to detect that a good part of activities to which the children were exposed aimed directly at mastering numbers and letters. In only one of the preschools we detected a more systematic concern with texts of oral origin, but even then, from which we could analyze, the texts were exploited for purposes related to the field of writing. The involvement of the team with the pupils in the course of the year confirmed this initial suspicion: the children had a small and undiversified repertoire of oral genres of childhood, that is, during their early childhood education they have not had enough opportunities to deal sufficiently with texts of oral origin for these to actually constitute a foundation for textual memory.

Based on these findings, the research team along with the school teachers tried to provide a program that would link a network of oral texts; that is, the concern was to ensure that a number of texts were stabilized in the memory, as narratives matrices or sources of linguistic skills with which and among which a subjectivity more adequate with the requirements of reading and writing could emerge. As already stated, if in the memory of a pupil there is a net of texts, which are associated either by common themes, or by their formal elements, the effect of such a network should also extend to the field of writing. If the pupils know how to listen, tell and relate stories (thematic intertextuality), if they haves ludic-poetic texts in their memory and learned to identify the aesthetic elements of each genre oral (parallelism, alliteration, assonance, rhymes and other iterations), most likely their reading and their handling of writing will become more dynamic and meaningful.

The team sought to mobilize the teachers of both classes to understand well the principles that guided the work perspective and to build into their programs items proposed by the project. However, these insertions could not be made without regard to the curriculum for the project "Ler e Escrever." From the hypotheses of the project, some strategies have guided these insertions:

1. Collective Oral Diagnosis – we began the process with a collective diagnosis, or through conversations in the classroom, trying to map the thematic and formal universe of the oral texts that children brought in their memory.

2. Performative Activities – in collective performative activities, a researcher observed the reaction of pupils signaling evidence of little repertoire or even a little practice in collective oral activities. In each classroom, it was noticed from the outset, that eight or nine pupils did not favor such activities: there was for instance complete avoidance, or exaggerated and indisciplined participation. These pupils were interviewed and treated separately, and, where possible, we sought to catch up on the activity or talk about it. In subsequent classes, special attention was given to engaging these pupils.

3. Route to Oral Activities – the activities pursued the following route: (1) performance, (2) talk about the activity, (3) ask for games or similar story in the memory of pupils, (4) search at home and/or in books with the aim of finding similarities, (5) organization of collections. Some examples may illustrate the process:

  • Riddles: according to the classroom teachers it was a moment of euphoria when the pupils received school supplies from the government; hence the team decided to present some riddles whose answers were exactly the materials they had in hand (e. g., "it is born large and dies small"? – answer: the pencil). The work was successful and ended with a collection of riddles. When school was resumed at the second half of the year, the project coordinator noted that pupils easily recalled the name of all materials, their riddles and others have already addressed during the first semester.

  • European and Brazilian Tales: as children brought in their memories only a few European tales, the most widely covered by the media, the team aimed at rapidly expanding this repertoire and, preferably, entangling intertextuality as well as introducing several Brazilian tales. The following tales were narrated and intertwined from the outset: "Jack and the Beanstalk," "Hansel and Gretel" and "Little Thumb", also offering a repertoire of Brazilian tales: "A árvore de Tamorumu" ["The tree of Tamorumu"], "O Macaco Simão" ["The Monkey Simon"], "O macaco e o pé de milho" ["The monkey and the corn stalk,"], "A história da coca" "The story of coca"], "A árvore da montanha" ["The tree of the mountain"], "A velha a fiar" ["The old woman spinning"] and others. After the performances and conversations, the pupils knew the stories in books or, where appropriate, in movies. Only after mastery of these matrices they started working with passages, words, phrases and images as dynamic motivators for the use of writing.

Memory between texts, images, and media

In the diagnosis of phase I, the rebus appeared as a difficult element for pupils who did not master writing even after three or four years of schooling. In the program, it was used as a special strategy, because, besides being taken as a hinge between images and writing, it also functioned as a strong element of diagnosis. Using rebus is a qualitative step that allows diagnostics to go beyond the stages or levels described by Ferreiro y Teberosky (1989), since the passage from the image to the syllable requires "cuts" and "losses" whose meanings go beyond the domain of conscious writing. The following is an example of the implications that rebus reading proposes:

First, the subject has to "detach" herself/himself from the image of the rooster (leave the "imaginary pregnance") and read the figure only as GA, cutting the LO. After this, s/he has to do the same with the second image. The subject will have to accept the adjustments between the two fragments to find there the word GATO [CAT]. In building the word, there is also a breach of the "pregnance of sound," because the subject has to decenter herself/himself from syllabification and assume the meaning. Since the project activities always sought an entanglement of levels (syllable, word, text), the cat rediscovered as well as other words, allowed to find a hidden rhyme (the words cat, forest, fire, water, beef, wheat, chicken, egg, priest and mass formed the rhyme "Cadê o toucinho daqui?" ["Where's the bacon from here?"]). The game was called "Discover the rhymes."

The rebus was also the basis of several games developed by the staff as electronic support, in which the pupil rediscovered, in addition to rhymes, other oral games and narratives they already had in memory. In addition to this reading of the rebus, we also practiced reading illustrations of books; for example, they read episodes of "Little Thumb" in the illustrations of Gustave Doré, which served as a mnemonic aid, especially for those children who did not yet have practice in the reconstruction of narratives. The use of the rebus is opposed by constructivists and sociointeractionists as it is understood as a pedagogy of the smaller parts of speech, syllabication, letter, etc. However, in our research, it proved effective and necessary, especially for pupils who had difficulty understanding the syllable as an algorithm.

Films were also used: the two animations of Michel Ocelot, "Kiriku and the Witch" and "Kiriku and the wild animals" were showed more than once. Many scenes were isolated so that the narratives were reconstructed in a fairly complete manner. Film narratives were also reused in computer games produced specifically for the project.

Naming

Another equally important point was to expand the theme of "naming", whose goal was not only expand the vocabulary, but also confront the child with two phenomena: (1) the relationship between more general terms (hypernymy: trees, animals, birds) and more specific popular names (ipê tree, fox, tié-sangue [Ramphocelus bresilius]), calling attention to the mode of naming ("Why is this bird called "Quero-quero" [Southern Lapwing]?," and this one, why is it called "coruja buraqueira" [athene cunilaria]?), in short, the purpose was of creating meaning (onomatopoeia, metaphoric and metonymic effects), (2) the significant relationship, showing odd names, curious sound effects, for example," murucututu "(a type of owl), "jaratataca" (a kind of skunk), "Xexéu" (a bird) – such words allowed some children to realize the repetitions of vowels and consonants, which allowed adding to the process the dimension of the poetic, of a significant effect that goes beyond the direct relationship between signifier and signified (bilabial consonants [m], [t] and the alveolar tap/flap of [r], associated with the repetition of [u], which is also a closed vowel; this introduces new meanings to this coruja [owl], whose singing and mystery seem to resonate in its own name, so does the "jaratataca" that opens itself in its cracking [noises] and jumps). We found that the onomatopoeic and semiotic re-motivation may have a crucial role in literacy.

As can be seen, the research project emphasized the building of a textual basis in memory, always taking as its starting point the performative orality, the body as support, but always exploring other parallel linguistic forms and media (writing, pictures, posters, books, computer and cinema). It became clear to the team that very positive effects on the classroom dynamics can be obtained from this entanglement: the constant rediscovery of the texts, words, images, resulted in a rich, diverse and interactive educational environment.

Alternating: tell, read; read and tell

Another important strategy used by the group was switching between narrative modes. The storytelling always came to the fore; it was the main responsible for the foundation of a narrative memory. The reading came next and, finally, commenting and exchanging opinions, for example: the Saci as a folklore character, was presented through conversation and storytelling, then through the book "The Saci" by Monteiro Lobato, whose narrative was paraphrased and at the same time, read aloud. The apex of this activity was reached when the teacher, during a more cathartic moment, interrupted the reading and left the final passage for the pupils who made an effort and rushed to read in pairs or small groups.

An important observation regarding the training of teachers is due at this point: considering the present research and the training courses offered by the project team to teachers in the public sector, one can conclude that many teachers do not feel responsible for not knowing how to tell stories or do other performative activities with the children, for example, alternating "reading aloud" and "storytelling" or even stimulating activities that add body movement, rhythm and voice. The arguments generally are as follows: "I cannot memorize stories and texts, "pupils are too undisciplined and everything becomes a mess," "the program is too long, we cannot waste time with games" and others. These skills should be understood as essential to the profession and be required in official examination for public posts and in the work itself.

Paralell Monitorings

The research makes it clear in its results, that the classrooms are already heterogeneous from the beginning, i. e., there will be mismatches in the levels of learning already from the initial two months. If one desires, thus, to assume a true policy of literacy and reading, it will be necessary to have team work that exceeds the current model (one teacher for every room with thirty or more pupils). Since, among Brazilian constructivists, the idea that the child has two years to become literate is widely disseminated, all remnants of 1st grade go to second grade without the slightest embarrassment.

What is not realized is that from the initial two months, the pupil who does not assimilate the rhythm of the activities of mastery of the code already positions herself/himself as a renitent subjectivity; as the one who cannot learn to read and who, for this reason, also isolates herself/himself from oral games and other group activities.

The project experimented with diagnosing and monitoring these pupils in their singularities, trying to detect their movements of approaching and distancing the world of reading and writing. The team followed those pupils who, from the beginning, deviated from the class. The synthesis of these results is presented in Table 4.

These sessions took part weekly with supervision of the project coordinator. The intention was to seek a model of team work that could cope with all kinds of difficulties the pupils had from the first day of class. The monitoring of the sessions allows for some conclusions:

1) Attendance is critical: Much of the lag in performance/achievement is caused by the pupils' low frequency, despite the offer of parallel monitoring. During the sessions, it was common to experience a pupil advance in the expansion of both their oral memory and in the field of the alphabet, but undergo the same setbacks after a prolonged absence. One of the researchers directed their research to the families, trying to understand this apparent neglect in relation to school. Although the work is not yet completed, the researcher found a complex set of reasons, ranging from family difficulties (e. g., parental conflict) to other work-related issues (leaving home early, the child is alone and decides for itself) or even housing and transportation.

2) Teamwork ("the hinge"): Responsibility for the early grades should be assigned to a team of educators from kindergarten and the first two grades – one could use the image of a hinge as a metaphor for the articulation of kindergarten and early phase of elementary school (ensino fundamental I) – the curriculum must be articulated, the continuity of the program has to be guaranteed, grade 1 should not be understood as the absolute starting point. Each class of 1st grade could be under the responsibility of a pair of teachers, one of them being more specialized in linguistic skills and more likely to continuing training in issues that promote the understanding of singularities, i. e., s/he would be able to diagnose and monitor pupils in groups or in individual sessions, according to the needs. This handling would start from both the oral diagnostic of the pupil and his involvement with writing as well as with the educational relationship proper (subjective position). As observed in the two phases of the research the renitency of the pupils regarding writing is progressive, it becomes increasingly complex over the years; so one should not wait until the third year of schooling, as does the PIC program municipal education (São Paulo, 2006) to start with recovery classes. What is advocated here is different from what the Secretaria Municipal de São Paulo (Municipal Secretariat of Sao Paulo) aims with TOF program (Toda Força ao Prhymeiro Ano [All Efforts in the First Year]), whose model, also adopted by the state network of São Paulo (Rede Estadual de São Paulo), assigns "an assistant – research student, student of Pedagogy and Literature (Pedagogia e Letras) to each first grade teacher" (São Paulo, 2006,. p. 5) and provides for a training program that strives to define itself as constructivist, and whose focus is placed on writing to the detriment of orality (Belintane, 2008), of reading (Bajard, 1992) and other dimensions of language (Borges, 2006; Bosco, 2002). As noted in the research, the excessive submission to constructivism in the school routine results in two serious problems: (1) a single approach to diagnosis focusing on writing, through which pupils are classified, (2) a strong ideological refusal against the use of syllabic families and other techniques that use the smaller elements of language and writing.

3) Shifting the focus from writing to orality: The project started from a form of orality that dynamizes memory, body language abilities and linguistic skills, which favors an approach to writing that, at the same time, aims at the full mastery of difficulties of the code and the expansion of matrices that will support a significant fluency in reading. The pilot scheme implemented in the research project was successful in the collective activities, in the individual sessions and even in the more complex cases.

General Conclusion

The implementation of the program in the two classrooms followed an interesting dynamic, though it could not enjoy exclusivity, since the teachers also had to meet the requirements of the program "Ler e Escrever" ["Read and Write"], which required the researchers to establish some convergence between the two programs. As what concerns the support, we also sought to maintain convergent paths, from the support of the body (memory, performance), to electronic media, through to the graphical, but always opening up possibilities for other languages. It created an environment of immersion and submersion of subjectivities, that is, we have provided the basic linguistic conditions for subjective movements among the different genres, means, suports, and languages. The introduction of electronic support, as it was done, with richly contextualized activities using orality and books allowed us a glimpse into the possibilities of introducing the computer to pupils in early grades, but without imaginary ruptures in the world of orality and of the book. All pupils in the two classrooms, during their early years, they understood the computer merely as an opportunity to "play games", but when faced with the figures and situations in stories, in books and movies, they realized they were learning to read and, at the same time, that they were rediscovering themes and characters already in their memory.

Using a program

Although the planned program was not implemented with exclusivity in the two classrooms, the results were interesting and promising. The graph below represents an assessment of the six classes of first grade school, held on July 13, 2009, by the school itself at the request of the Secretariat of Education. Note that the two series followed by our research (B and E) showed the best performance, even when taking into account only the constructivist classification:

In the evaluative dialogues with pupils, conducted during the last week of the classes, which recapitulated stories, games, plays, readings and characters, one could clearly see two dynamic rooms, animated, and predisposed to continue the work – in the language of the research , "two ambiances conducive to the formation of readers." However, despite all the difficulties to assess reading skills, the team decided to administer, in the first week of December, a final evaluation which aimed to detect more precisely the reading level of the two rooms. The instrument featured the following activities:

  • Correlation between word and image (the image of a banana tree [bananeira], for example, should be associated with the correct written form, placed randomly among four other segments with almost homographs:

    barateira, banheira, pananeira, baladeira, bananeira);

  • Naming of characters known from the rebus, the computer or from graphical support;

  • Reading and understanding familiar texts – choose the correct alternative, reading it with autonomy;

  • Identification of excerpts from stories (paraphrased) from its illustrations – for example, the image of Little Thumb taking off the Ogre's boots or of Macaco Simão [Monkey Simon] eating his bananas should be pasted so as if they were baseball cards into the spaces containing their respective captions.

The overall result of the evaluation was as follows:

Would the monitoring of the groups have been more complete, with a good transition between kindergarten and elementary school and an exclusive implementation of the program recommended, the results would have possibly been even better. What becomes clear is that the focus of research and teaching activities should be placed more on the personal and cultural singularity of the Brazilian child. The transition from orality to writing entails more accurate and contextualized didactic arrangements, with constant monitoring of level gaps since the beginning of the first years of elementary school.

The articulation of academic research and public education can improve significantly if there is a detachment of researchers and educators in relation to the methodologies or theories that make rigid their ways of dealing with the complex Brazilian reality.

References

  • BELINTANE, C. Vozes da escrita: entre crianças e menestréis. Estilos da clínica, São Paulo, v. 1, n. 1, p. 36-51, 2008.
  • ______. Leitura e alfabetização no Brasil: uma busca para além da polarização. Educação e Pesquisa, São Paulo, v. 32, n. 2, p. 261-277, maio/ago. 2006.
  • BAJARD, E. Afinal, onde está a leitura? Cadernos de Pesquisa, São Paulo, n. 83, p. 29-41, 1992.
  • BERNSTEIN, B. Estrutura social, linguagem e aprendizagem. In: PATTO, M. H. S. Introdução à psicologia escolar São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo, 1997. p. 145-169.
  • BORGES, S. O quebra-cabeça da escrita: a alfabetização depois de Lacan. Goiânia: UCG, 2006.
  • BOSCO, Z. R. No jogo dos significantes: a infância da letra. Campinas: Pontes, 2002.
  • BRASIL. Ministério da Educação. Plano de Desenvolvimento da Educação. Prova Brasil: Ensino Fundamental: matrizes de referência, tópicos e descritores. Brasília: MEC, SEB, INEP, 2008/2009.
  • BRASIL. Sistema Nacional de Avaliação da Educação Básica. Instituto Nacional de Estudos e Pesquisas Educacionais Anísio Teixeira (Inep/MEC). Notícias da Prova Brasil e Saeb 2007 Disponível em: <http://www.inep.gov.br/imprensa/noticias/saeb/arquivo07.htm>. Acesso em: ago. 2010.
  • BRASIL. Relatório Final do Grupo de Trabalho Alfabetização Infantil: os novos caminhos. Seminário "O Poder Legislativo e a Alfabetização Infantil: novos caminhos". Comissão de Cultura e Educação da Câmara dos Deputados, 2003. Disponível em: <http://www.mec.gov.br>. Acesso em: 15 set. 2003.
  • FERREIRA, E.; TEBEROSKY, A. Los sistemas de escritura en el desarrollo del niño 11. ed. México: Siglo Ventiuno, 1989.
  • FREUD, S. Obras Completas de Sigmund Freud Rio de Janeiro: Imago, 1988.
  • GELB, J. I. Historia de la escritura Versión española de Alberto Adell. Madrid-Espanha: Alianza, 1952.
  • GINZBURG, C. Sinais: raízes de um paradigma indiciário. In: GINSBURG, C. Mitos, emblemas e sinais: morfologia e história. Tradução Federico Carotti. São Paulo: Companhia das Letras, 1989. p. 143-179.
  • HAVELOCK, E. A equação oralidade-cultura escrita: uma fórmula para a mente moderna. In: OLSON, D. R.; TORRANCY, N. Cultura escrita e oralidade Tradução Valter Lellis Siqueira. São Paulo: Ática, 1995. p. 17-34.
  • JAKOBSON, R. Dois aspectos da linguagem e dois tipos de afasia. In: ________. Lingüística e comunicação Tradução Izidora Blikstein e José Paulo Paes. São Paulo: Cultrix, 1995. p. 34-62.
  • LACAN, J. A instância da letra no inconsciente. In: _______. Escritos Tradução Vera Ribeiro. Rio de Janeiro: Jorge Zahar, 1998. p. 496-533
  • LEMOS, C. Sobre a aquisição da escrita: algumas questões. In: ROJO, R. (Org.). Alfabetização e letramento São Paulo: Mercado de Letras, 1998. p. 13-53.
  • LUDKE, M.; ANDRÉ, M. E. D. Pesquisa em educação: abordagens qualitativas. São Paulo: EPU, 1986.
  • NASIO, J. Cinco lições sobre a teoria de Jacques Lacan Tradução Vera Ribeiro. Rio de Janeiro: Jorge Zahar, 1993.
  • PATTO, M. H. S. (Org.). Introdução à psicologia escolar São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo, 1997.
  • SÃO PAULO. Secretaria Municipal da Educação. Projeto "Toda força ao 1º ano": guia para planejamento ao professor alfabetizador - orientações para o planejamento e avaliação do trabalho com o 1º ano do Ensino Fundamental. São Paulo: SME/DOT, 2006.
  • SÃO PAULO. Secretaria Municipal da Educação. Ler e escrever: guia de planejamento e orientações didáticas; professor alfabetizador 1ª série. São Paulo: FDE, 2008.
  • SOARES, M. Linguagem e escola: uma perspectiva social. São Paulo: Ática, 1989.
  • *
  • **
    University of São Paulo
  • 1
    and its main objective was to examine the possibilities of implementing a language teaching program in early grades. The program was designed taking account of more accurate diagnoses of the conditions of orality and literacy of the pupils and the general conditions and limitations of teaching they were actually exposed to (including the praxis of the school, teachers and the influences of existing government programs).
  • 2
  • 3
  • 4
    Before describing the research process of each phase, it is worth addressing some aspects and characteristics of the research. The researcher and his team (graduate students, internship students, and faculty fellows) had intense involvement with the subjects of the research and shared with them common expectations of its results – that means: the literacy of the pupils involved. The research is thus characterized by a complex nature which could be approached, on the one hand, by a qualitative methodology – for example, a "case study" (Ludke; André, 1986) – and on the other, by – especially when considering the subjective position of the team and their training – a kind of "engagement" and "listening" with origins in psychoanalysis, but also crossed by linguistics and other fields (Ginzburg, 1989; Lemos, 1998).
  • 5
    diagnoses, made with the help of a semi-structured interview guide; and (2) listening to each individual pupil who signaled differences and to each teacher involved as well as documenting impressions of the researchers reported in their field research diary. In the following section the two axes are presented.
  • 7
    which found that youth of 14, 15 and 16 years could not understand a simple form for job application; they did not understand items such as date of birth, parentage, ID, etc. – the surnames of the parents almost always constituted a major problem, some pupils even cited nicknames in the family: Zombie, Bigfoot, Baiano. This topic constituted a thematic focus entitled "naming", as discussed below.
  • 8
    focuses on the "free-floating attention," on the silent waiting for a lapse, a Freudian slip, or even on significant games in accounts of dreams, in this research we also valued as significant evidence that emerged in the relationship, but from a semi-structured teaching situation, mediated by literary activities such as storytelling, recitation, ludic games and talks about the heroes of the narratives, about the ways of how to play with words.
  • 9
    incorporated orally and through reading the book "O Saci" by Monteiro Lobato. According to one of the researchers, the boy questioned the very nature of fiction, seemed to demonstrate certain surprise with the stories, he almost always tried to question the storytelling introducing other possibilities of action rather than passively accepting the progression of the story as do children who accept the fiction – an example: when Little Thumb and his brothers are abandoned in the woods, MT, distressed, interrupted and pointed to another solution: "they can go after the parents back, and return, follow their track back home." Gradually, he assimilated the narrative, accepting its progression and identifying with this tiny character who, with his cunning, triumphed over the huge ogre and brought his brothers back home. As for the Saci, a little being, deprived of one leg, but full of artfulness and powers, it offered the boy good possibilities of commiting himself during the sessions. These movements of his subjectivity functioned as turning points in his reluctance: MT had difficulties to behave as a pupil, he lived on the streets, did not even have an idea of how to behave in a classroom or even actually how to handle school supplies. By identifying himself with Little Thumb and Saci, his relation with storytelling, with reading aloud and with the letters themselves have changed enough so that major breakthroughs happened. In just three months, MT mastered the alphabet, just having some difficulties with complex syllables (usually digraphs) and sustaining a subjective position as a reading learner, showing great interest in the history books, the work of Monteiro Lobato, in the library, in playing and in games based on the narratives created especially for the project in the classrooms.
  • 10
    What our research shows is that the isolation of the teacher with her/his class, associated with this diagnostic classification system centered on writing, leaves aside the most concrete burning issues of a class, which generally requires a more detailed listening to its heterogeneity and more contextualized and diversified strategies.
  • 11
    that allows for simple commands ("drag and drop", "connect points", "image-sound"), the team created games of orality and literacy
  • 12
    that allowed fun playing with the oral texts seen in class and, at the same time, propitiated the use of multimedia (sound, image, moving image, text, hypertext, and recursivity).
  • Publication Dates

    • Publication in this collection
      16 May 2011
    • Date of issue
      Dec 2010

    History

    • Received
      06 Mar 2010
    • Accepted
      22 June 2010
    Faculdade de Educação da Universidade de São Paulo Av. da Universidade, 308 - Biblioteca, 1º andar 05508-040 - São Paulo SP Brasil, Tel./Fax.: (55 11) 30913520 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
    E-mail: revedu@usp.br