Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Family life influence on the communicative performance of children with Down syndrome

Abstracts

PURPOSE:

to investigate the aspects of family life that influence communicative performance of children with Down syndrome.

METHODS:

application of the "Features of the Family Environment" questionnaire and pragmatic analysis of children with DS communication. 30 children of both genders, ranging from five to 10 years old, have partaken this research. Spearman's rank order correlation was applied. The analyses were performed using the SPSS' 10.0 version, and in all of them, a significance level of 5% was adopted.

RESULTS:

correlation between the items of the questionnaire and pragmatic analysis results regarding communicative means and communicative function was found to exist.

CONCLUSION:

it is necessary to take family life aspects that influence the communicative performance of children with DS into consideration.

Child Language; Down syndrome; Environment; Family Relationships; Child Development


OBJETIVO:

investigar os aspectos da vida familiar que influenciam o desempenho linguístico de crianças com síndrome de Down.

MÉTODOS:

aplicação do questionário Recursos do Ambiente Familiar e análise pragmática da comunicação de crianças com síndrome de Down. Participaram do estudo 30 crianças com SD, de cinco a 10 anos de idade e de ambos os sexos. Aplicou-se o teste de correlação de postos de Spearman. As análises foram feitas pelo programa SPSS versão 10.0 e, em todas elas, adotou-se um nível de significância de 5%.

RESULTADOS:

verificou-se correlação entre itens do questionário e os resultados referentes ao meio comunicativo e função comunicativa da análise pragmática utilizadas pelas crianças.

CONCLUSÃO:

é preciso considerar os aspectos do meio familiar que apresentam influência com o desempenho comunicativo de crianças com Sindrome de Down.

Linguagem Infantil; Síndrome de Down; Meio Ambiente; Relações Familiares; Desenvolvimento Infantil


Introduction

The extra genetic material characteristic of Down syndrome (DS) causes loss of cell function equilibrium, singularizing individuals in their acquisitions throughout life. In the bibliography studied, information about the correlation between cognitive performance of children who have DS was found to exist, along with evidence about intellectual imparity and its association with of the genetic condition manifestation 11. Voivodic MAMA. Inclusão Escolar de Crianças com Síndrome de Down. In. Voivodic MAMA. Síndrome de Down. Petrópolis: Ed. Vozes; 2005. P.39-66.

2. Silva MFMC, Kleinhans ACS. Processos cognitivos e plasticidade cerebral na Síndrome de Down. RevBrasEduc Espec. 2006;12(1):123-38.
- 33. McCann J, Wood SE, Hardcastle WJ, Wishart JG, Timmins C. Relationship between speech, oromotor, language and abilities in children with Down´s syndrome. Int J Lang Commun Dis. 2010;45(1):83-95..

Both language acquisition and development in DS cases are compromised. One can infer that the neurological conditions and intellectual disabilities result in a slower than average pace for language acquisition and development which, however, follows a pattern. In more severe cases there might be full impediment of certain linguistic abilities44. Guerra GR. Síndrome De Down: aspectos de sua comunicação (apresentação de dois casos). Temas desenvolv. 1997;6(33):12-7. , 55. Mancini MC, Silva PC, Gonçalves SC, Martins SM. Comparação do desempenho funcional de crianças portadoras de síndrome de Down e crianças com desenvolvimento normal aos 2 e 5 anos de idade. ArqNeuro-Psiquiatr. 2003;61(2B):409-15..

The diagnosis of the syndrome alone does not determine physical features or the level of intellectual efficiency. The individual's capacity to respond, according to epigenetic contributions, is influenced by the opportunities the surroundings provide. In order to acquire and master linguistic abilities, the individual's experience influences in the construction and improvement of linguistic abilities22. Silva MFMC, Kleinhans ACS. Processos cognitivos e plasticidade cerebral na Síndrome de Down. RevBrasEduc Espec. 2006;12(1):123-38. , 44. Guerra GR. Síndrome De Down: aspectos de sua comunicação (apresentação de dois casos). Temas desenvolv. 1997;6(33):12-7. , 66. Ansermet F, Magistretti PA. Chacun son ceveau: Plasticité neuronale et inconscient. Paris: Ed. Odile Jacob ; 2004..

The contrasting development of children with DS might be significantly influenced by education, as well as the environment where they are brought up since the first years of their lives. Their surroundings play a mediator role in child acquisition and learning processes, aiming at an ideal level of development11. Voivodic MAMA. Inclusão Escolar de Crianças com Síndrome de Down. In. Voivodic MAMA. Síndrome de Down. Petrópolis: Ed. Vozes; 2005. P.39-66. , 44. Guerra GR. Síndrome De Down: aspectos de sua comunicação (apresentação de dois casos). Temas desenvolv. 1997;6(33):12-7. , 77. Santos TR, Oliveira FN. As interações sociais e o brincar da criança com síndrome de Down. VIII Congresso Nacional de Educação, EDUCERE, III Congresso Ibero-americano sobre violências nas escolas CIAVE; out 6-9; Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Paraná. Curitiba: Índices de Comunicações: EDUCERE, Teorias, metodologias e práticas; 2008..

Children learn and acquire language through experience. The mastering of language is developed through social interaction, demands from the surroundings and the organization of experience. Therefore, the individual is enabled to build new knowledge and gradually improve mental activity content elaboration 88. Tomasello M. First steps toward a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cogn Linguist. 2000;11(1-2):61-82.

9. Zorzi JL. A evolução da brincadeira simbólica. In: Zorzi JL. Aquisição da linguagem infantil - desenvolvimento alterações - terapia. São Paulo: EdPancast; 1993. P. 5-23.
- 1010. Silva MPV, Salomão NMR. Interações verbais e não-verbais entre mães-crianças portadoras de Síndrome de Down e entre mães-crianças com desenvolvimento normal. Estud Psicol. 2002;7(2):311-23..

The act of playing has considerable social and emotional implications in an individual's life. Moreover, it presents aspects of cognitive performance. Games are the source children have to explore, manipulate, understand and modify their surroundings. There have been authors who have posited that children perfect their development through sensorial-motor exploration and symbolic behavior enabled by playing. This allows children to express the impressions lived during their interaction with the others in both family and school environments, while also broadening knowledge and abilities 77. Santos TR, Oliveira FN. As interações sociais e o brincar da criança com síndrome de Down. VIII Congresso Nacional de Educação, EDUCERE, III Congresso Ibero-americano sobre violências nas escolas CIAVE; out 6-9; Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Paraná. Curitiba: Índices de Comunicações: EDUCERE, Teorias, metodologias e práticas; 2008. , 1111. Venuti P, de Falco S, Esposito G, Bornstein MH. Mother-Child Play: children with DownSyndrome and typical development. Intellect devdisabil. 2009;114(4):284-8..

Due to the considerable relevance of a individual's surroundings in their development, whenever any type of intervention for a child with DS is considered, it is critical to consider family as a therapeutic strategy. Therefore, in family environment, the child be provided with the first acquisitions and receive important influences that will determine prevalent individual characteristics throughout life. 44. Guerra GR. Síndrome De Down: aspectos de sua comunicação (apresentação de dois casos). Temas desenvolv. 1997;6(33):12-7. , 1212. Casarin S. O ciclo vital da família do portador da Síndrome De Down: dificuldades específicas. Temas Desenvolv. 1997;6(33):18-28. , 1313. Cunha EP, Limongi SCO. Modo comunicativo utilizado por crianças com síndrome de Down. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2008;20(4):243-8..

The present study aims to investigate which resources from family life may influence the communicative performance of children with Down syndrome.

Methods

A cross sectional analytical study was performed, in order to carry out the present study, where family life resources that may influence the communicative performance of children with DS were analyzed. This project was approved by the committee of ethics in research (Comitê de Ética em Pesquisa - COEP) of the university where it was initiated, under the technical opinion number 602/09. A family resource questionnaire was then given to the mothers of DS children in order to investigate the mother-child relationship and ultimately carry out this study. All families that have taken part in the process have signed the Written Informed Consent (TCLE).

Participants

A sample of 30 families that have children of both genders with Down syndrome has partaken this research. They were randomly selected amongst the ones who are supported by APAE (Association of Parents and Friends of Exceptional Children), in the city of Belo Horizonte (MG).

In order to form the group the following inclusion criteria was utilized: families that had children from five to 10 years and 11 months old, who were accompanied by their mothers in their appointments and accepted the invitation to take part in the research and families of children who did not present a comorbidity clinical history.

The average age and level of school education the mothers had, as well as the socioeconomic situation of the families were not taken into consideration. Nevertheless, it is known that families assisted by APAE are usually lower class and learn about the institution through SUS (Public Healthcare System).

Material

The materials utilized were a Sony Cyber Shot DSC-W510 camcorder, the Communicative Profile (Perfil Comunicativo) protocol elaborated by Fernandes 20041414. Fernandes FDM. Pragmática. In: Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes D, Wertzber HF. ABFW: teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. 2ed Rev ampl e atual. Barueri: Ed. Pró Fono, 2004. P. 77-89. for the transcription of pragmatic analysis and the questionnaire of Family Environment Resource (RAF) 1515. Marturano EM. O inventário de Recursos do Ambiente Familiar. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2006;19(3):498-506..

In order to create a spontaneous situation where mother and child would play together, specific toys were used: miniatures of farm and jungle animals, miniatures of means of transportation, a doll, some hair accessories and bracelets, a shape sorting game, miniatures of food and kitchen utensils, colored pencils, crayons and a blank sheet of paper.

Procedures

A film of the interaction between mother and child was recorded for 30 non-stop minutes, in the same place where the children had their appointments, playing with the toys that were previously selected. The first 15 minutes of interaction of mother and child were analyzed. After the footage, the mothers were given the Family Resource Environment (RAF)

The pragmatic analysis1414. Fernandes FDM. Pragmática. In: Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes D, Wertzber HF. ABFW: teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. 2ed Rev ampl e atual. Barueri: Ed. Pró Fono, 2004. P. 77-89. utilized aims at investigating the functional aspects of communication related to the children's abilities to use language with communicative functions.

The questionnaire about Resources of Family Environment 1515. Marturano EM. O inventário de Recursos do Ambiente Familiar. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2006;19(3):498-506. consists of 10 topics that pertain to the following fields:

  • what the child does when away from school;

  • the hangouts of the last 12 months;

  • activities programmed to be done regularly;

  • activities done with their parents at home;

  • variety of toys available;

  • existence of newspapers and magazines at home;

  • variety of books available;

  • person responsible for assisting the child in the homework;

  • daily routines with pre-set time;

  • time of the day and the week the family gets together.

The combination of those topics resulted in a range of family environment resources that promote development of functional aspects of communication. The questionnaire was applied as a semi-structured interview.

Result analysis

After filmed, all data was transcribed to the specific protocols and analyzed with the aid of the procedures described in the protocol of communicative profile about pragmatic analysis 1414. Fernandes FDM. Pragmática. In: Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes D, Wertzber HF. ABFW: teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. 2ed Rev ampl e atual. Barueri: Ed. Pró Fono, 2004. P. 77-89. and the RAF1515. Marturano EM. O inventário de Recursos do Ambiente Familiar. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2006;19(3):498-506. questionnaire.

The correlation method was chosen, since the aim was to verify the probable association between the information obtained with the protocols utilized. The Spearman's rank order correlation enabled to measure the level of intensity of data association. There analyses were made with 10.0 version of the program SPSS and in all of them the significance level adopted was 5%.

Results

Figure 1 illustrates all items of RAF questionnaire and the items of pragmatic analysis, from which the correlation between variables was obtained. The p-values (statistic significance) obtained in the chart are related to the correlation coefficient test of Spearman applied to the data.

Figure 1:
Main Correlations

Table 1 features the result of the test carried out with only items where correlation was found. The numbers in bold indicate whether two variables are correlated to the statistic point of view or not.

Table 1:
Correlation between the results of the ABFW and the Family Environment Questionnaire

The analysis of data has proven the existence of a correlation between the item that relates the diversity of free time activities done with the child and the prevalence of the "performative" communication function.

It has also been verified that there is a correlation between the amount of scheduled activities performed by children with family incentive and a more frequent usage of the "verbal" communicative means. Additionally, it has been observed that as the amount of scheduled activities performed by the child increases, the usage of the functions "request for action" and "narrative" increases.

From the data obtained, it was possible to notice that a broader variety of newspapers and magazines in the family environment is inversely proportional to the use of the communicative function "game" and directly proportional to the function "narrative".

As for the item that discusses the existence of a more frequent supervision of an adult in the school tasks the child performs, more prevalent uses of the "verbal" and "gestural" means by children were found, as well as the use of the "acknowledgement of the other" communicative function.

It has been noticed that a more well-organized family environment, with daily routines and a pre-set schedule, increases the use of the "verbal" communicative means and the "comment" and "request for action" communicative functions.

Discussion

The pragmatic analysis is based on functional aspects of communication. In order to enable such verification, communicative acts were investigated, along with the reason why they had been carried out, "gestural", "vocal" or "verbal", and the communicative function of the act during a spontaneous moment of interaction between mother and child. As regards the RAF questionnaire, it aims to list the aspects of family environment that favor child development.

In order to perform data collection, a 30-minute footage of spontaneous interaction between child and mother was made. It was noticed that the mothers were tired of playing with their children after 30 minutes had passed since the beginning of the interaction. Many of them complained to the researcher about the excessive time of observation. A possible hypothesis, resulted from observation and clinical experience, is that this type of attitude is probably replicated in the family environment. Nowadays, due to the more active role women play in their families, the need to go to work and also take care of family issues reduces the amount of free time dedicated to their children, especially when it comes to situations that according to social culture are not exactly relevant, e.g., "playing".

It is important to point out that 15 minutes of interaction between mother and child in a spontaneous situation of playing were analyzed. In other articles where the same pragmatic analysis methodology was used with children with down syndrome no reference to significant difference in regards to how long the footage the samples under analysis was has been found. Bearing that in mind, a decision was made to watch the first 15 minutes of the footage, not losing track of both the quality and reliability of the answers provided1616. Porto E, Limongi SCO, Santos IG, Fernandes FDM. Amostra de filmagem e análise da pragmática na síndrome de Down. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2007;19(2):159-66..

One should remember that neither age, nor school level of the mothers was taken into consideration. Moreover, the socioeconomic conditions of the families were not assessed either. Bearing in mind the importance of environment in language development, one may infer that children from underprivileged families may present inferior linguistic performance parameters in comparison to other children of the same age group. However, due to the fact that this is a scientific study, the group has been kept as equal as possible.

It has been verified that the amount free time activities done with the child and use of the "performative" function of communication increase side-by-side. The "performative" function teaches about symbolic behavior applied to objects and events. Symbolism, aside from its ludic nature, anticipates the cognitive development of the individual, and is regarded as a primordial aspect of language development. The symbolic behavior that takes place when playing demonstrates some individual characteristics of the child, as well as his or her needs and interests. Some authors consider the symbolic function as the capacity to represent, which favors the surging of linguistic abilities, including verbal. The more diversified the experiences children are put in contact with, the more linguistic possibilities they are provided with. Therefore, they may present better and more thorough linguistic performance. Children's actions are based on the world around them and context they are surrounded by must provide them with opportunities to behave in this environment1717. Launay CL, Desenvolvimento normal da linguagem. In: Launay CL, Maisonny-Borel S. Distúrbios da linguagem da fala e da voz na infância. São Paulo: Ed. Roca; 1989. P.17-34..

An existing correlation between the habitual practice of activities, including therapeutic intervention, sports or even music initiation, is related to a more frequent "verbal" communicative means and the use of the communicative functions "request for action" and "narrative". The "narrative" function proves itself to be constructive playing, involving the mastery of symbolic, semantic and pragmatic abilities. Those children's "request for action" function shows that not only did they respond, but they also proposed activities and recreational tasks. All children who have partaken this study have had therapeutic intervention for an average of 6 years of assistance. One may infer, therefore, that the more frequent use of the "verbal" communicative means is coherent, since incentivizing oral production is one of the goals in children with DS's speech therapy. It may also be argued that additionally to school, it is important for children to do other activities, such as: occupational therapy, psychological therapy, swimming, group work and even music lessons, which affect language acquisition positively. Such tasks have been some of the examples mentioned by the mothers who have partaken this experiment. Speech therapy has shown itself to be an enabling factor for oral language. It is important to highlight that the genetic condition has already been established by the syndrome, hence therapists working on the child's surroundings in order to assure that the intervention favors development44. Guerra GR. Síndrome De Down: aspectos de sua comunicação (apresentação de dois casos). Temas desenvolv. 1997;6(33):12-7. , 1111. Venuti P, de Falco S, Esposito G, Bornstein MH. Mother-Child Play: children with DownSyndrome and typical development. Intellect devdisabil. 2009;114(4):284-8. , 1515. Marturano EM. O inventário de Recursos do Ambiente Familiar. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2006;19(3):498-506..

The family environment where magazines and newspapers are found encourages children to use the "narrative" communicative function, which, as mentioned prior, is oriented to the mastery of linguistic abilities that are important for better linguistic performance. It is very likely that reading and being in touch with that sort of graphic material provides children with exposure to broad vocabulary and prevalence of the correct models for oral speech structuring, with adequate morphological and syntactic aspects. Such a resource encourages children verbalize more often when describing real or imaginary facts during interaction moments. Authors have posited that the linguistic manifestations are related to prior experience, which constitute and create knowledge of the world. The understanding and appropriation of language are intertwined with global knowledge, and with the way specific aspects are selected according to the demands of a given situation. 88. Tomasello M. First steps toward a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cogn Linguist. 2000;11(1-2):61-82. - 99. Zorzi JL. A evolução da brincadeira simbólica. In: Zorzi JL. Aquisição da linguagem infantil - desenvolvimento alterações - terapia. São Paulo: EdPancast; 1993. P. 5-23. , 1717. Launay CL, Desenvolvimento normal da linguagem. In: Launay CL, Maisonny-Borel S. Distúrbios da linguagem da fala e da voz na infância. São Paulo: Ed. Roca; 1989. P.17-34..

A reduction of the communicative function "game" has been noticed when newspapers, magazines and books are found in the household. The availability of graphic and reading resources in the family environment allows children to handle books, magazines and newspapers, counting with the company of others during exploration process in certain occasions. The existence of those resources favors social interaction and reduces the use of communicative functions that do not include others in their process, such as "games" 11. Voivodic MAMA. Inclusão Escolar de Crianças com Síndrome de Down. In. Voivodic MAMA. Síndrome de Down. Petrópolis: Ed. Vozes; 2005. P.39-66. , 22. Silva MFMC, Kleinhans ACS. Processos cognitivos e plasticidade cerebral na Síndrome de Down. RevBrasEduc Espec. 2006;12(1):123-38..

The increase in the communicative function "acknowledgement of the other" appears when supervision of schoolwork by adults is verified to happen more frequently. Children who count on constant assistance of their parents in tasks and activities tend to more often request and acknowledge the others in the environment where they are. As a consequence of that, the others are more frequently included in the moments of playing and interaction1212. Casarin S. O ciclo vital da família do portador da Síndrome De Down: dificuldades específicas. Temas Desenvolv. 1997;6(33):18-28. , 1818. Silva NLP, Dessen MA. Crianças com síndrome de Down e suas interações familiares. Rev Psicol: Reflex Crit. 2003;16(3):503-14..

The use of "verbal" and "gestural" means of communication have also been noticed to take place more frequently used when the child's surroundings facilitate the execution of tasks. Due to the fact that "verbal" is the most common communicative means used by adults when explaining and assisting in activities, children who are more exposed to this tend to use it more frequently, since they listen to and observe such models constantly. Children with DS use the communicative means "gestural" more frequently than children with typical development. Because they are not fully able to express themselves verbally in a satisfactory way, they use isolated gestures or combine them with emissions in order to learn and express concepts and ideas. The bibliography used in this paper reinforces that gestures also facilitate oral language, since they too evoke the productions of adults in reference to the object or the child's attention focus, offering them the model of spoken words and how to express your desires through "verbal" means" 1313. Cunha EP, Limongi SCO. Modo comunicativo utilizado por crianças com síndrome de Down. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2008;20(4):243-8. , 1616. Porto E, Limongi SCO, Santos IG, Fernandes FDM. Amostra de filmagem e análise da pragmática na síndrome de Down. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2007;19(2):159-66. , 1919. Chapman RS, Streim NW, Crais ER, Salmon EA, Strand EA, Negri NA. Fala infantil: Suposições de um Modelo Processual Desenvolvimental para a Aprendizagem Inicial da Linguagem. In: Chapman RS. Processos e Distúrbios na Aquisição da linguagem. Porte Alegre: Ed. Artes Médicas; 1996. P.15-30.

20. Almeida FCF, Limongi SC. O papel dos gestos no desenvolvimento da linguagem oral de crianças com desenvolvimento típico e crianças com síndrome de Down. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2010;15(3):458-64.
- 2121. Andrade RV, Limongi SCO. A emergência da comunicação expressiva na criança com síndrome de Down. Pró Fono R Atual Cient. 2007;19(4):387-92..

It has been noticed that the surroundings that provide a daily routine with a pre-set schedule have caused children to use the communicative functions "comment" and "request for action" less frequently. Establishing a routine with a preset schedule is important for the organization of family environment and sometimes may reflect in the process of doing homework. However, this has proven itself to have reduced the interaction between children and family members. The reduction of both interactive and dialogue-oriented functions has shown itself to have taken place. This might be a consequence of a possible imposition of daily tasks in an authoritarian manner, with parents forcing children to behave in a certain way, which has kept the children who have partaken this research to solicit the adults. An increase in the "verbal" means has been observed whatsoever, when there is a preset schedule in the family environment. So, they provide models of oral language to children, therefore allowing them to replicate what is offered to them1212. Casarin S. O ciclo vital da família do portador da Síndrome De Down: dificuldades específicas. Temas Desenvolv. 1997;6(33):18-28. , 1313. Cunha EP, Limongi SCO. Modo comunicativo utilizado por crianças com síndrome de Down. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2008;20(4):243-8. , 1616. Porto E, Limongi SCO, Santos IG, Fernandes FDM. Amostra de filmagem e análise da pragmática na síndrome de Down. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2007;19(2):159-66..

The interactions established between family systems are the ones that enable the most meaningful considerations for child development. The way information is transmitted to children with Down syndrome influences in their development of verbal communication abilities. Such findings reinforce more and more that the importance of active intervention of family in the process of learning and cognizance of children with Down syndrome, focusing in the use of oral language2222. Santos LC, Marturano EM. Crianças com dificuldade de aprendizagem: Estudo de seguimento. Psicol Reflex Crit. 1999;12(2):377-94.

23. Soares EMF, Pereira MMB, Sampaio TMM. Habilidade pragmática e síndrome de Down. Rev CEFAC. 2009;11(4):579-86.
- 2424. Limongi SCO, Mendes AE, Carvalho AMA, Do Val DC, Andrade RV. A relação comunicação não verbal-verbal na síndrome de Down. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2006;11(3):135-41..

Conclusion

The tools used in the present study have enabled data collection in a natural spontaneous way, guaranteeing reliable results. The pragmatic analysis carried out enabled a communicative channel that favored a more accurate evaluation of the communicative and interactional performance of the children and their mothers. The RAF questionnaire is practical and easy to be applied. Its results enable a broader analysis of family environment characteristics.

The data presented in this research point to the comprehensive variety of events and resources that contribute to favor the acquisition and development of linguistic abilities in children with Down syndrome. It has also been verified that there certain types of behavior in family environment inhibit the manifestation of certain linguistic functions on behalf of the children.

Once the relationship between environmental resources that influence linguistic development in children with Down syndrome was confirmed, the importance of the role played by the surroundings of children with Down syndrome in linguistic acquisition and development in such population. Since Down syndrome determines a pre-established genetic condition, family and health professionals that deal Down syndrome participate in the evolutionary process of children as a tool that interferes, modifies, perfects and contributes to improve the child's performance. It is important to bear in mind the importance of family surroundings in the improvement of quality of life for children with Down syndrome.

  • 1
    Voivodic MAMA. Inclusão Escolar de Crianças com Síndrome de Down. In. Voivodic MAMA. Síndrome de Down. Petrópolis: Ed. Vozes; 2005. P.39-66.
  • 2
    Silva MFMC, Kleinhans ACS. Processos cognitivos e plasticidade cerebral na Síndrome de Down. RevBrasEduc Espec. 2006;12(1):123-38.
  • 3
    McCann J, Wood SE, Hardcastle WJ, Wishart JG, Timmins C. Relationship between speech, oromotor, language and abilities in children with Down´s syndrome. Int J Lang Commun Dis. 2010;45(1):83-95.
  • 4
    Guerra GR. Síndrome De Down: aspectos de sua comunicação (apresentação de dois casos). Temas desenvolv. 1997;6(33):12-7.
  • 5
    Mancini MC, Silva PC, Gonçalves SC, Martins SM. Comparação do desempenho funcional de crianças portadoras de síndrome de Down e crianças com desenvolvimento normal aos 2 e 5 anos de idade. ArqNeuro-Psiquiatr. 2003;61(2B):409-15.
  • 6
    Ansermet F, Magistretti PA. Chacun son ceveau: Plasticité neuronale et inconscient. Paris: Ed. Odile Jacob ; 2004.
  • 7
    Santos TR, Oliveira FN. As interações sociais e o brincar da criança com síndrome de Down. VIII Congresso Nacional de Educação, EDUCERE, III Congresso Ibero-americano sobre violências nas escolas CIAVE; out 6-9; Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Paraná. Curitiba: Índices de Comunicações: EDUCERE, Teorias, metodologias e práticas; 2008.
  • 8
    Tomasello M. First steps toward a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cogn Linguist. 2000;11(1-2):61-82.
  • 9
    Zorzi JL. A evolução da brincadeira simbólica. In: Zorzi JL. Aquisição da linguagem infantil - desenvolvimento alterações - terapia. São Paulo: EdPancast; 1993. P. 5-23.
  • 10
    Silva MPV, Salomão NMR. Interações verbais e não-verbais entre mães-crianças portadoras de Síndrome de Down e entre mães-crianças com desenvolvimento normal. Estud Psicol. 2002;7(2):311-23.
  • 11
    Venuti P, de Falco S, Esposito G, Bornstein MH. Mother-Child Play: children with DownSyndrome and typical development. Intellect devdisabil. 2009;114(4):284-8.
  • 12
    Casarin S. O ciclo vital da família do portador da Síndrome De Down: dificuldades específicas. Temas Desenvolv. 1997;6(33):18-28.
  • 13
    Cunha EP, Limongi SCO. Modo comunicativo utilizado por crianças com síndrome de Down. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2008;20(4):243-8.
  • 14
    Fernandes FDM. Pragmática. In: Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes D, Wertzber HF. ABFW: teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. 2ed Rev ampl e atual. Barueri: Ed. Pró Fono, 2004. P. 77-89.
  • 15
    Marturano EM. O inventário de Recursos do Ambiente Familiar. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2006;19(3):498-506.
  • 16
    Porto E, Limongi SCO, Santos IG, Fernandes FDM. Amostra de filmagem e análise da pragmática na síndrome de Down. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2007;19(2):159-66.
  • 17
    Launay CL, Desenvolvimento normal da linguagem. In: Launay CL, Maisonny-Borel S. Distúrbios da linguagem da fala e da voz na infância. São Paulo: Ed. Roca; 1989. P.17-34.
  • 18
    Silva NLP, Dessen MA. Crianças com síndrome de Down e suas interações familiares. Rev Psicol: Reflex Crit. 2003;16(3):503-14.
  • 19
    Chapman RS, Streim NW, Crais ER, Salmon EA, Strand EA, Negri NA. Fala infantil: Suposições de um Modelo Processual Desenvolvimental para a Aprendizagem Inicial da Linguagem. In: Chapman RS. Processos e Distúrbios na Aquisição da linguagem. Porte Alegre: Ed. Artes Médicas; 1996. P.15-30.
  • 20
    Almeida FCF, Limongi SC. O papel dos gestos no desenvolvimento da linguagem oral de crianças com desenvolvimento típico e crianças com síndrome de Down. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2010;15(3):458-64.
  • 21
    Andrade RV, Limongi SCO. A emergência da comunicação expressiva na criança com síndrome de Down. Pró Fono R Atual Cient. 2007;19(4):387-92.
  • 22
    Santos LC, Marturano EM. Crianças com dificuldade de aprendizagem: Estudo de seguimento. Psicol Reflex Crit. 1999;12(2):377-94.
  • 23
    Soares EMF, Pereira MMB, Sampaio TMM. Habilidade pragmática e síndrome de Down. Rev CEFAC. 2009;11(4):579-86.
  • 24
    Limongi SCO, Mendes AE, Carvalho AMA, Do Val DC, Andrade RV. A relação comunicação não verbal-verbal na síndrome de Down. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2006;11(3):135-41.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Feb 2015

History

  • Received
    17 Oct 2013
  • Accepted
    27 May 2014
ABRAMO Associação Brasileira de Motricidade Orofacial Rua Uruguaiana, 516, Cep 13026-001 Campinas SP Brasil, Tel.: +55 19 3254-0342 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistacefac@cefac.br