Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Structure and function of calls of the Masked Gnatcatcher Polioptila dumicola in Mid-eastern Argentina

Abstract

Acoustic communication plays a key role in the life of birds and it is useful in phylogenetic and evolutionary investigations. This study described the structure and function of Polioptila dumicola´s call repertoire in riparian environments from the Mid-eastern of Argentina. Male calls (N=644) were recorded in the field during the pairs nuptial interactions; nests exploration and previously capture of male. Then, specific function was assigned to different calls using standardized methods of acoustic classification and behavioral observations. Twelve different types of calls were classified. Alert and call to female were the most frequent calls and they represented 12.04% and 10.24% of recordings. Anguish call was also recorded in this species. These are the first results regarding to the calls and repertoire of P. dumicola. These data could be used for new studies on environmental selection pressures and for conservation of this species and its habitats.

Key words
Alarm; calls; contact; behavior; Polioptila dumicola

INTRODUCTION

Animals communicate through a diversity of signals and different sensory channels (Partan & Marler 2005PARTAN S & MARLER P. 2005. Issues in the classification of multimodal communication signals. Am Nat 166: 231-245.). Signals of acoustic communication are used for species recognition, mate selection, and territorial and anti-predator defense. These signals are likely shaped both by natural and sexual selection (Ryan & Brenowitz 1985RYAN MJ, BRENOWITZ EA. 1985. The role of body size, phylogeny, and ambient noise in the evolution of bird song. Am Nat 126: 87-100., Mathevon et al. 2008MATHEVON N, AUBIN T, VIELLIARD J, DA SILVA ML, SEBE F & BOSCOLO D. 2008 Singing in the rain forest: how a tropical bird song transfers information. PLoS ONE 3:e1580., Podos 2014PODOS J. 2014. Sexual selection and the evolution of vocal mating signals: lessons from neotropical songbirds. In: Macedo RH and Machado G (Eds) Sexual selection: perspectives and models from the neotropics. Elsevier Inc., Amsterdam, p. 341-363.). The emitter mechanisms likely evolve to increase efficiency and reliability of the information delivered whereas reception mechanisms evolve so that receivers can produce appropriate behavioral or physiological responses (Endler 1993ENDLER JA. 1993. Some general comments on the evolution and design of animal communication systems. Philos Trans Biol Sci 340: 215-225., Maynard-Smith & Harper 2003MAYNARD-SMITH J & HARPER D. 2003. Animal signals. Oxford University Press.). In this sense, animal communication is under some selective pressure (e.g. adapting the signal to reach great distances; adjusting signals to avoid localization by predators), which becomes highly relevant in environments acoustically contaminate such as those that occur in urban sites (Boncoraglio & Saino 2007BONCORAGLIO G & SAINO N. 2007. Habitat structure and the evolution of bird song: A meta-analysis of the evidence for the acoustic adaptation hypothesis. Funct Ecol 21: 134-142.). In this context, the study of vocal communication is an important component to different disciplines such as evolutionary ecology and conservation biology, especially in birds (Naguib & Riebel 2006NAGUIB M & RIEBEL K. 2006. Bird song: a key model in animal communication. In: Brown K (Ed). Encyclopaedia of Language and Linguistics, 2nd ed. Elsevier, Oxford, p. 40-53.).

Bird vocalizations, especially of passerines, are among the most complex sounds produced by animals (Briefer et al. 2010BRIEFER E, OSIEJUK TS, RYBAK F & AUBIN T. 2010. Are bird song complexity and song sharing shaped by habitat structure? An information theory and statistical approach. J Theor Biol 262(1): 151-164.). This complexity of sounds in passerine’s birds emit can be classified in two types of vocalizations: song and calls. The songs tend to be long, complex, and emitted by males in the breeding season. Generally, songs occur over long periods of time and with a characteristic daytime rhythm (Catchpole & Slater 2008CATCHPOLE CK & SLATER JB. 2008. Bird song: biological themes and variations. Ed: Cambridge University Press, USA, 335 p.). Unlike songs, calls are simpler, with fewer notes, shorter duration, more stereotyped within species, and genetically predetermined (Marler 2004MARLER P. 2004. Bird calls: their potential for behavioral neurobiology. Ann New York Acad Sci 1016: 31-44.). Thus, animals are able to produce calls without intensive learning. Also, calls occur in a variety of environmental situations throughout the day (Beckers & Gahr 2010).

Calls are essential within the bird population because they give information related to parental behavior, nepotism, altruism, and cooperation (Wheeler 2008WHEELER BC. 2008. Selfish or altruistic? An analysis of alarm call function in wild capuchin monkeys, Cebusa pellanigritus. Anim Behav 76: 1465-1475., Goedert et al. 2014GOEDERT D, DIAS RI & MACEDO RH. 2014. Nestling use of alternative acoustic antipredator responses is related to immune condition and social context. Anim Behav 91: 161-169.). They can be classified as alarm and contact calls (Marler 1955MARLER P. 1955. Characteristics of some animal calls. Nature 176: 6-8., 2004). Alarm calls occur during hostile conspecifics and heterospecifics interactions when, for example, individuals detect potential predators (Kondo & Watanabe 2009KONDO N & WATANABE S. 2009. Contact calls: Information and social function. Jpn Psychol Res 51: 197-208.). Alarm calls are loud and repetitive, audible near the caller, and difficult to locate (Marler 1955MARLER P. 1955. Characteristics of some animal calls. Nature 176: 6-8.). The type of alarm call can transmit information about the predator’s type (Suzuki 2011SUZUKI TN. 2011. Parental alarm calls warn nestlings about different predatory threats. Curr Biol 21(1): R15-R16.), size (Templeton et al. 2005TEMPLETON CN, GREENE E & DAVIS K. 2005. Allometry of alarm calls: black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science 308: 1934-1937.), and distance (Leavesley & Magrath 2005LEAVESLEY AJ & MAGRATH RD. 2005. Communicating about danger: Urgency alarm calling in a bird. Anim Behav 70(2): 365-373.). Due to their role in interactions such as predation or competition, alarm calls probably result from adaptive processes (Klump & Shalter 1984KLUMP GM & SHALTER MD. 1984. Acoustic behaviour of birds and mammals in the predator context. I. Factors affecting the structure of alarm signals. II. The functional significance and evolution of alarm signals. Zeitschrift fürTierpsychoogie 66: 189-226.). Several studies have classified alarm calls considering the sound (e.g. “whistle”, “rough sound”), the behavior of both the emitter and the receiver (e.g. “mobbing”, “persecution”) and the context in which they occur (e.g. “anguish”, “distress” (Caro 2005CARO T. 2005. Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, USA, p. 398-403., Wheeler 2008WHEELER BC. 2008. Selfish or altruistic? An analysis of alarm call function in wild capuchin monkeys, Cebusa pellanigritus. Anim Behav 76: 1465-1475.). Acoustically, alarm calls are similar in structure among species (Davis 1988DAVIS WE. 1988. Distress calls in birds: an avian enigma. Birds Obs 16(4): 205-211., Jurisevic & Sanderson 1998JURISEVIC MA & SANDERSON KJ. 1998. Acoustic discrimination of passerine anti-predator signals by Australian raptors. Aust J Zool 46(4): 369-379.), but there is considerable interspecific variation in the use of these types of calls. For instance, the proportion of individuals that emit alarm calls can vary among species (Rohwer et al. 1976ROHWER S, FRETWELL SD & TUCKFIELD RC. 1976. Distress Screams as a Measure of Kinship in Birds. Am Midl Nat 96(2)., Grieg-Smith 1984GRIEG-SMITH IW. 1984. Distress calling by woodland birds: seasonal patterns, individual consistency and the presence of conspecifics. Z Tierpsychol 66: 1-10.), an aspect that is important to characterize to assess selective pressures within populations and among species.

On the other hand, contact calls are emitted between conspecific individuals and they encode several types of information (recognition, signaling about food, maintaining social cohesion, synchronizing and coordinating of flight). These calls are soft and wideband, audible only at close range (Hamilton 1962HAMILTON WJ. 1962. Evidence concerning the Function of Nocturnal Call Notes of Migratory Birds. The Condor 64(5).). The contact signal is recognized as unique, and the receiver learns the cues and uses them to identify the emitter during future interactions (Tibbetts & Dale 2007TIBBETTS EA & DALE J. 2007. Individual recognition: it is good to be different. Trends Ecol Evol 22(10): 529-537.). Several studies in some species like Aratinga canicularis and Brotogeris jugularis have shown that individuals exchange contact calls with overflying groups while feeding, probably to recruit other individuals to the food site (Bradbury & Allen 2003BRADBURY RB & ALLEN DS. 2003. Evaluation of the impact of the pilot UK Arable Stewardship Scheme on breeding and wintering birds. Bird Study 50(2): 131-141.).

Studies focused on the calling behavior of birds are more limited than those studying the singing behavior (Neudorf & Sealy 2002NEUDORF DL & SEALY SG. 2002. Distress Calls of Birds in a Neotropical Cloud Forest1. Biotropica 34(1): 118-126., Nocera et al. 2008NOCERA JJ, TAYLOR PD & RATCLIFFE LM. 2008. Inspection of mob-calls as sources of predator information: Response of migrant and resident birds in the Neotropics. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62(11)., Martin et al. 2011MARTIN JP, DOUCET SM, KNOX RC & MENILL DJ. 2011. Body size correlates negatively with frequency of distress calls and songs of Neotropical birds. J Field Ornith 82(3): 259-268.), despite its importance for predation or competition events. Moreover, the vocal repertoire of Neotropical birds is much less known than that of birds from Northern temperate areas.

Polioptila dumicola is a small size bird species belonging to a family of small passerine birds (Polioptilidae) that do not exceed 20 species. According to BirdLife International and the Red List of Threatened Species (IUCN 2020IUCN. 2020. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. Version 2020-1. https://www.iucnredlist.org. Downloaded on 19 March 2020.
https://www.iucnredlist.org...
) their population trends are threatened and in decline due to human activities such as deforestation of forests, and wetland fires. Polioptila dumicola is distributed in tropical and subtropical climates throughout the Americas, except in the extreme south and the high regions of the Andes. In Argentina, this bird species has a wide distribution from the north to San Juan, San Luis, Córdoba, and Buenos Aires Provinces (Ridgely & Tudor 1997RIDGELY SR & TUDOR G. 1997. The birds of South America, (ed: University of Texas Press, Austin), p. 516.).

P. dumicola, for example, has been described as a bird species with ‘many calls’, however, this species has never been cataloged and alarm call as ‘meowing’ (contact call between both sexes as ‘tripp’ and ‘song’) was only described (Fraga & Salvador 2013FRAGA RM & SALVADOR SA. 2013. Conducta y biología reproductiva de la tacuarita azul (Polioptila dumicola) en un área pampeana y otra del espinal. Argentina. Hist Nat 3: 37-50.).

In the present study, the adult call repertoire from mid-eastern of Argentina was cataloged and each different call was related to its function for the first time. As a social species, a large amplitude in the call repertoire is expected according to the social complexity hypothesis, where groups with complex social systems require more complex communicative systems to regulate interactions and relations among members. So this type of behavior of P. dumicola is positively related to the amplitude of the call repertoire in birds (Dunbar 1998DUNBAR RIM. 1998. The social brain hypothesis. Evol Anthropol 6: 178-190., Freeberg et al. 2012FREEBERG TM, DUNBAR RIM & ORD TJ. 2012. Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity. Phil Trans R Soc B 367: 1785-1801.). In this context, establishing the repertoire of this species and the different functions of calls provides unknown aspects related to its behavioral ecology and systematic.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Study area

The study was performed in riparian areas of the Parana´ River situated on the Mid-eastern of Argentina (31° 39’S; 60° 35’W). The records were carried out on one side of a National Route (NR) (NR 168, Santa Fe Province, Argentina). This area has 7000 ha and includes a complex system of islands, lagoons, ponds, and permanent freshwater marshes, interspersed with riparian woodland and gallery forests (Peltzer & Lajmanovich 2004PELTZER PM & LAJMANOVICH RC. 2004. Anuran tadpole assemblages in riparian areas of the Middle Paraná River, Argentina. Biodiv Conserv 13: 1833-1842.). Different tributary orders, streams and rivulets delimited the riparian areas. The intermediate zones or flooded transitional zones are flat and exposed to periodic floods (Sánchez et al. 2009SÁNCHEZ LC, PELTZER PM & LAJMANOVICH RC. 2009. Structure of wetland-breeding anuran assemblages from the southern section of Paraná river. Argentina. Herpetol J 19: 173-184.). The study area has forest fragments with species such as Albizia inundata, Croton urucurana, Sapium haematospermum, Celtis tala and Vachellia caven, among other trees, shrubs, and grasses. The climate is temperate, with a mean annual temperature of 27.5°C (SD±3) and mean annual precipitation of 995 mm (data provided by Facultad de Ciencias Hídricas de la Universidad Nacional del Litoral, FICH-UNL).

Study design

A total of 50 males of P. dumicola were observed and recorded during the pre-breeding and breeding periods (August – February of 2016 and 2017). The inner area of a forest fragment situated more than 200 m away from the route was selected due to the entrance area easily (Olguín 2016OLGUÍN PF. 2016. Biología reproductiva de especies de aves del río Paraná medio, Argentina. Tesis doctoral. Facultad de Bioquímica y Ciencias Biológicas, Universidad Nacional del Litoral.). The nests with their corresponding couple were identified examining areas near to active females and then enumerated. Mist nets were used to capture and mark individuals with color rings to avoid repeating record of the same individual (Ralph et al. 1996RALPH CJ, GEUPEL GR, PYLE P, MARTIN TE & DE SANTE DF & MILÁ B. 1996. Manual de métodos de campo para el monitoreo de aves terrestres. Albany, CA: Pacific Southwest Research Station, Forest Service, U.S, p. 8-24.) and to observe the vocal behavior of the birds during capture. The focal method was used to observe male´s behaviors (occurrence and duration of all types of behavior patterns) (Altmann 1974ALTMANN J. 1974. Observational Study of Behavior: Sampling Methods. Behaviour 49: 3-4., Martin & Bateson 2007MARTIN P & BATESON P. 2007. Measuring Behaviour. 3rd ed. (New York: Cambridge University Press), 186 p.). The couples were monitored every two days, in periods of up to 2.5 hours at a distance of 15-30 m from the previously identified male.

Recording methods

Vocalizations were recorded in different situations: 1) during nuptial interactions, 2) during the nest’s exploration and 3) prior to the male capture. Each adult male was recorded for three minutes each individual or until 20 calls were registered, two times a week between 07:00 and 12:00 hr. A unidirectional ME - 66 microphone Senheiser with a K6 - C condenser and a Korg MR - 1000 digital recorder (sampling rate: 44.1 kHz, 16 bits) was used. The recording equipment was placed close to the selected individual and only recordings with the best level of signal-to-noise ratio (SNR 20-30 dB) were analyzed. Recordings were achieved in wave format, mono quality, and 16 bits. This instrumental does not compress the sound, which avoids the loss of frequencies that, although are not audible to humans, are components of the songs of the birds (Budney & Grotke 1997BUDNEY GF & GROTKE RW. 1997. Techniques for audio recording vocalizations of tropical birds. Ornithol Monogr 48: 147-163.).

Measurement of acoustic parameters

A call has been defined as a stereotypical vocalization used in intra-group communication (Tobias et al. 2011TOBIAS M, EVANS BJ & KELLEY DB. 2011. Evolution of advertisement calls in African clawded frogs. Behaviour 148: 519-549.), consisting of either a single note given at isolation or repetition, as well as given in a consistent sequence of ordered notes being the note a solid line on a spectrogram (Vargas-Castro et al. 2012VARGAS-CASTRO LE, SANCHEZ NV & BARRANTES G. 2012. Repertoire size and syllable sharing in the songof the clay-colored thrush (Turdus grayi). Wilson J Ornithol 124: 446-453., Sosa-López & Mennill 2014SOSA-LÓPEZ JR & MENNILL DJ. 2014. The vocal behavior of the Brown-throated Wren (Troglodytes brunneicollis): song structure, repertoires, sharing, syntax, and diel variation. J Ornithol 155: 435-446.). Each call was analyzed using the Raven Pro 1.5 program (Bioacoustics Research Program 2014BIOACOUSTICS RESEARCH PROGRAM. 2014. Raven Pro: Interactive Sound Analysis Software (Version 1.5) [Computer software]. Ithaca, NY: The Cornell Lab of Ornithology. Available from http://www.birds.cornell.edu/raven.
http://www.birds.cornell.edu/raven...
) employing the following parameters: type window Hann, window size, and Fourier transform of 512 samples: overlap of 50% (hop size of 256 samples) and spacing 86.1 Hz (Garcia 2016GARCIA NC. 2016. Estudio comparativo filogenético de los patrones de variación vocal y de coloración del plumaje en los “Cardinalinos Azules” (Passeriformes: Cardinalidae). Tesis doctoral, Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires.). The acoustics parameters measured were A) number of notes, B) duration of the call (s), C) maximum frequency (kHz), D) minimum frequency (kHz) and E) peak frequency (kHz, i.e. the frequency for which amplitude is greatest). Frequency measurements were accurate at 0.012 kHz increments, time measurements at 1 ms and the spectrogram figures were cut out at 12.5 kHz to obtain an adequate visualization. Standard parameters were recorded with an onscreen cursor which has been considered to quantify the acoustic structure of calls (Langmore et al. 2003LANGMORE NE, HUNT S & KILNER RM. 2003. Escalation of a coevolutionary arms race through host rejection of brood parasitic young. Nature 422: 157-160., Gloag & Kacelnik 2013GLOAG R & KACELNIK A. 2013. Host manipulation via begging call structure in the brood-parasitic Shiny Cowbird. Anim Behav 86: 101-109.). Average values were calculated for each of the five acoustic variables pooled together by category. T-tests were performed in R (R Studio Team 2020R STUDIO TEAM. 2020. RStudio: Integrated Development for R. RStudio, PBC, Boston, MA URL http://www.rstudio.com/.
http://www.rstudio.com/...
) to compare the acoustic parameters among alarm and contact calls.

Classification and function of calls

Calls were classified by 1. Number and type of notes: number of minimum continuous sound units of a call; 2. Structure observed in the spectrogram (note form): to interpret the form of a note on the spectrogram it was considered that a. high-pitched sounds (with a higher frequency) appear higher on the y-axis, b. a short whistle of the constant pitch will appear as a pure, unmodulated frequency trace on the spectrogram, c. a whistle which starts at a higher frequency and drops to a lower one is said to be frequency modulated and appears on the spectrogram as a slope from left to right. d. If more rapid modulations appear, as in a fast vibrato, e. unclear short sound like click (if several occur close together a buzzing sound is produced), f. when a sound has higher frequencies as multiples of the first or fundamental frequency is called harmonics. 3. Audition of call evaluated by a human observer (identification of clear auditory differences from the playback of recordings). Categories were validated based on two observers who received a call example by category. In order to classify function of calls, preliminary observations were used to identify a set of behaviors and vocal responses that could easily be distinguished. Behavioral responses were inferred during field observations (see above) by assessing the behavior of other individuals in the immediate second following a call, but these responses should be interpreted cautiously in case they were triggered by the same context as their associated call. The potential for subjectivity in these classification methods was recognized, but a simple classification method to outline the minimum number of differences a contextually view associated with clear auditory was selected.

RESULTS

Recorded calls of P. dumicola males (N=50) were classified in 12 different types (Tables I and II). Five calls were categorized as alarm (alert, alert short distance, danger call, distress call, and anguish call), five calls as contact (call to female, territorial call, incubation, nest building, long- distance) and two calls could not be categorized.

Table I
Characteristics of 12 call types of Polioptila dumicola males. Values are expressed as mean ± SD.
Table II
Data were collected from 50 couple of P. dumicola throughout the breeding season. Percentages (%) indicate how often a context or response was associated with a specific call.

Acoustics characteristics of calls

The sonograms and oscillograms analyzed were obtained from N = 664 calls (Range: 50 – 80 for an individual). The maximum frequency of the calls varied between 4.8 and 7.14 kHz and the minimum frequencies between 2.28 and 3.3 kHz. In addition, the highest dominant frequency was 5.5 kHz and the bandwidth of all calls ranged between 0.68 and 2.99 kHz. The longest duration was 2.05 s and the highest number of notes was 11 (Table I). Of the acoustic variables compared, only the number of notes differed among types of calls, had contact calls with higher number of notes ( = 8.40 ± 2.7) than alarm calls ( = 4.75 ±1.8, t-test7.04 = -2.37, P = 0.04).Alarm calls had harmonic and broadband structures, and they were noisy, harsh and abrupt. Contact call had frequency-modulates structures that sounded like a whistle.

Alarm calls

Alert call

It is composed of high-frequency harmonics, short duration, and variation of dominant frequency (Fig. 1a). This sound was associated with threatening situations involving either terrestrial or aerial predators of eggs as Pitangus sulphuratus or Didelphis albiventris in the 66% (N =43) of observed call, whereas the other 34% (N=22) is emitted in presence of people nearby (all located more than 30 m, Table II). It was emitted by males and can play a potential alarm role for the female.

Figure 1
Oscillogram (upper part) and spectrogram (lower part) of alert (a), short-distance alert (b), danger (c), distress (d) and anguish (e) calls of P. dumicola.

Short-distance alert: It was characterized by short clicks (Table I, Fig. 1b). It is audible no more than 20 m by the human ear. It was associated with both the close presence of the couple or juvenile individuals and the territorial displacements during food search in the 35 % (N= 52) of the recorded call. But, in the remaining 65%, this call was related to eye contact (Table II).

Danger call

It is composed of complex modulated frequency notes, and it is given in the presence of a predator (Fig. 1c). In this situation, individuals of the P. dumicola are not easy to visualize and are located in trees very close to the nest. This sound is associated with situations related to predation by snakes such as Philodryas aestiva (Table II).

Distress call

This consists of strong-tone notes characterized by a high frequency and a long duration. This call was associated with extreme stress (e.g. during captures in mist nets or during chick manipulation, Table II). In general, an effect called “mobbing” (behavior to drive away potential predators) occurred simultaneously and it was characterized by the arrival of the couple members and other species at the site of conflict (Fig. 1d). On each of these occasions, other individuals of the same social group quickly approached to the distressed bird, chattering and mobbing the predator. This call appears to function as a signal of immediate danger, and it is only given during actual or attempted predation.

Anguish call

It consists of sounds with jumps of frequency. Emitted by the male before chick or adult predation events (Table II and Fig. 1e). On each of these occasions, the individual emitting the sound was located next to the predated nest.

Contact calls

Call to female

It is emitted by the male, and it has eight notes in the form of “V” (Fig. 2a). It has the function of attracting the female and it was recorded during moments before the emission of alarm or danger calls (Table II).

Figure 2
Oscillogram (upper part) and spectrogram (lower part) of call to female (a) and long distance (b), territorial (c), nest building (d) incubation (e) call of P. dumicola.

Long distance

It is constituted by notes of descending modulated frequency in the form of “inverted V” (Fig. 2b). It was observed when one member of the couple was not present in the territory (Table II). Individuals perch on higher branches about the location of the nest. This call occurs in conditions where there is no visual contact between members of the couple, and the receiver responds approaching to the site where the call comes from.

Territorial call

It is composed of 9 to 12 notes of modulated frequency. It was observed at the beginning of the reproductive season, during the beginning of the nest building and in the presence of another male (Table II). This causes that other individuals of the same sex move away from the nesting site or from the conflict between both males (Table II, Fig. 2c).

Nest building

It is characterized by long calls (10-11 notes) composed of short whistles and ascending modulated frequencies. It was associated with the moments of nest construction such as during searching of elements useful for that purpose (Table II, Fig. 2d).

Incubation

It is a short and ascending call produced by males and females. It is not frequently issued call (Table I) and it was associated with an egg incubation period (90% of observed call). In P. dumicola the care of eggs and chicks is biparental, and usually, the male usually emitted this signal inside the nest and then the female arrives to incubate (Table II, Fig. 2e).

Unassigned calls

Unclassified calls are grouped in phrases. Other calls are short whistles and are preceded by high and acute modulated frequencies (Fig. 3).

Figure 3
Oscillogram (upper part) and spectrogram (lower part) of unassigned call of P. dumicola.

DISCUSSION

The repertoire of bird songs mainly in those Neotropical species is little described. This lack of knowledge is notable even in passerine species distributed in Argentina, such as P. dumicola. Following what was expected from the social behavior, P. dumicola showed great amplitude in its call repertoire. Adult males emitted at least 12 different calls that varied in structure and function. Calls were composed of a series of repeated notes and, in some cases multiple harmonics of dominant frequency (e.g. alarm). This study is the first that describes the complex repertoire of this bird species. Although this study did not cover the complete distribution range of this species, it provides a detailed description of the call repertoire with a large sample size that can be useful in future comparative studies, like those related to call variation and complexity among bird species.

P. dumicola is considered a social species, with (family groups of up to 5-6 individuals, large territories, cooperative-breeding (Fraga & Salvador 2013FRAGA RM & SALVADOR SA. 2013. Conducta y biología reproductiva de la tacuarita azul (Polioptila dumicola) en un área pampeana y otra del espinal. Argentina. Hist Nat 3: 37-50.). Agreeing with this, its call variety was similar to that observed in other groups of social birds that present a wide repertoire such as Pomatostomus ruficeps (Crane et al. 2016CRANE JMS, SAVAGE JL & RUSSELL AF. 2016. Diversity and function of vocalisations in the cooperative chestnut-crowned babbler. Emu 116: 241-253.) and Baeolophus bicolor (Sieving et al. 2010SIEVING KE, HETRICK SA & AVERY ML. 2010. The versatility of graded acoustic measures in classification of predation threats by the tufted titmouse Baeolophus bicolor: exploring a mixed framework for threat communication. Oiko 119: 264-276.). Unlike social species, non-social species such as Upucerthia valdirostris has vocal repertoires in which calls and songs are almost indistinguishable (Areta & Pearman 2013ARETA JI & PEARMAN M. 2013. Species Limits and Clinal Variation in a Widespread High Andean Furnariid: The Buff-breasted Earthcreeper (Upucerthia validirostris). Condor 115(1).).

Thus, our results agreed with the hypothesis of social complexity (McComb & Semple 2005MCCOMB K & SEMPLE S. 2005. Coevolution of vocal communication and sociality in primates. Biol Lett 1(4): 381-385., Le Roux et al. 2009LE ROUX A, CHERRY MI & MANSER MB. 2009. The vocal repertoire in a solitary foraging carnivore, Cynictis penicillata, may reflect facultative sociality. Naturwissenschaften 96: 575-584.), showing that as association with conspecifics increases, communicative complexity and the potential to be used in mutual and more complex behavioral interactions also increases (Bradbury & Vehrencamp 2011BRADBURY JW & VEHRENCAMP SL. 2011. Principles of animal communication, 2nd ed. (Sinauer, Sunderland. MA), 697 p.). This communicative complexity found in P. dumicola suggests that vocal variation may be assigned to sociability differences that occur among different populations and it may be derived from learning deviations that occur over generations (Podos & Warren 2007PODOS J & WARREN PS. 2007. The evolution of geographic variation in birdsong. Adv Study Behav 37: 403-458.).

Alarm calls

Call repertoire was composed of 50% of alarm calls emitted in the presence of possible predators of eggs and chicks - e.g. snakes, rodents, and other birds (Bradbury & Vehrencamp 1998BRADBURY JW & VEHRENCAMP SL. 1998. Principles of Animal Communication (Sinauer, Sunderland, MA). Chap3, 65-112.). These calls are considered referential because they transmit encoded information about different environmental situations. Several studies show that different numbers of notes, harmonics, and bandwidths associated with the intensity of the threat are recorded among alarm calls in birds. Although intensity or frequency of the predator was not evaluated here, the number of notes is similar that those reported for Poecile atricapilla (i.e. 2 and 6 notes) when was stimulated by taxidermic mount of a Falco mexicanus (Baker & Becker 2002BAKER MC & BECKER AM. 2002. Mobbing calls of Black-capped Chickadees: effects of urgency on call production. Wilson J Ornithology 114: 510-516., Templeton et al. 2005TEMPLETON CN, GREENE E & DAVIS K. 2005. Allometry of alarm calls: black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science 308: 1934-1937.). This similarity could imply that P. dumicola could undergo selection pressures similar to P. atricapilla against the potential risk of a specific predator.

Short-distance alert calls were the most recorded. These calls had high maximum and minimum frequencies, even greater than Pomatostomus ruficeps, a species that emits calls of high frequency (Crane et al. 2016CRANE JMS, SAVAGE JL & RUSSELL AF. 2016. Diversity and function of vocalisations in the cooperative chestnut-crowned babbler. Emu 116: 241-253.). Short-distance alert calls were emitted when the male was approximately 15 meters away, that is, the distance at which this sound is perceived. For example, distress calls were longer than warning calls and generally, they were heard when individuals were captured and manipulated in mist nets. Then, this vocal signal likely contains information about the degree of threat that a situation represents. The results of the short distance alert of P. dumicola may be explained in terms of its subtle and sophisticated signaling systems because this species combines both referential and risk-based antipredator vocalization systems (Blumstein 1999BLUMSTEIN DM. 1999. The evolution of functionally referential alarm communication: multiple adaptations; mutiple constraints. Evol Commun 3: 135-147., Seyfarth & Cheney 2003SEYFARTH RM & CHENEY DL. 2003. Signalers and receivers in animal communication. Ann Rev Psychol 54: 145-173.).

Danger calls can be associated with the “warning hypothesis” (Rohwer et al. 1976ROHWER S, FRETWELL SD & TUCKFIELD RC. 1976. Distress Screams as a Measure of Kinship in Birds. Am Midl Nat 96(2).) which states that these types of calls can be altruistic when attempting to warn nearby kin of the presence of a predator. Coinciding with Caro (2005)CARO T. 2005. Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, USA, p. 398-403., this is a key antipredator strategy that has evolved in a wide range of species. Specific calls of individuals of P. dumicola were recorded in the presence of snakes as Philodrya aestiva. This is similar to those pointed out for Cracticus tibicen dorsalis, that emit and which is able to discriminate between alarms calls of different individuals associated with predatory stimuli (Silvestri et al. 2019SILVESTRI A, MORGAN K & RIDLEY AR. 2019. The association between evidence of a predator threat and responsiveness to alarm calls in Western Australian magpies (Cracticus tibicen dorsalis). Peer J 7: e7572.). These calls allow individuals to escape against an imminent danger by either fleeing or undertaking appropriate defensive measures (Griesser 2013GRIESSER M. 2013. Do warning calls boost survival of signal recipients? Evidence from a field experiment in a group-living bird species. Front Zool 10(1): 49., Hollén & Radford 2009HOLLÉN LI & RADFORD AN. 2009. The development of alarm call behaviour in mammals and birds. Anim Behav 78(4): 791-800.) Further research is needed to determine whether P. dumicola discriminates between individual alarm callers.

However, based on their social behavior, it is likely that P. dumicola can also shift their behavioral response according to the accuracy of social information (i.e., calls of different individuals and different human or natural threats) as documented in social species such as babblers (Flower et al. 2014FLOWER TP, GRIBBLE M & RIDLEY AR. 2014. Deception by flexible alarm mimicry in an african bird. Science 344(6183).) and weavers (Baigrie et al. 2014BAIGRIE BD, THOMPSON AM & FLOWER TP. 2014. Interspecific signalling between mutualists: Food-thieving drongos use a cooperative sentinel call to manipulate foraging partners. P Roy S B: Biol Sci 281(1791).).

Anguish calls were directly associated with stress caused by the loss of chicks or couples and they had the primary purpose of alerting the couple, conspecifics, or hetero-specific individuals (Møller et al. 2011MØLLER AP, CHRISTIANSEN SS & MOUSSEAU TA. 2011. Sexual signals, risk of predation and escape behavior. Behav Ecol 22: 800-807., Class & Brommer 2016CLASS B & BROMMER JE. 2016. Senescence of personality in a wild bird. Behav Ecol Sociobol 70: 733-744.). It was a strong and concise sound associated with predation events, such as has been determined in Pomatostomus ruficeps (Krams et al. 2014KRAMS I, VRUBLEVSKA J, KOOSA K, KRAMA T, MIERAUSKAS P, RANTALA MJ & TILGAR V. 2014. Hissing calls improve survival in incubating female great tits (Parus major). Acta Ethol 17: 83.). This type of call has been recorded in other terrestrial vertebrates, such as on common frog Leptodactylus chaquensis, and although it presents different frequency values, they are also short-lived and strident sounds (Dorado Rodrigues et al. 2012DORADO RODRIGUES TF, AZARIAS CAMPOS V, DOS SANTOS MM, PANSONATO A & STRÜSMAN C. 2012. Circumstances and bioacoustics of the distress call of Leptodactylus chaquensis (Anura: Leptodactylidae) during predation by Thamnodynastes chaquensis (Serpentes: Dipsadidae) in the Brazilian Pantanal. Salamandra 48: 237-240.). According to some authors, it is likely that adult males are familiar with their permanent social mates for weeks and months (Krams et al. 2006KRAMS I, KRAMA T & IGAUNE K. 2006. Alarm calls of wintering great tits Parus major: warning of mate, reciprocal altruism or a message to the predator? J Avian Biol 37: 131-136.). Thus, the emission of anguish calls can be an altruistic act helping their fellow against the predator.

Contact calls

Contact calls occur as a response within a group, as it has been shown in dolphins (Janik & Slater 1998JANIK VM & SLATER PJ. 1998. Context-specific use suggests that bottlenose dolphin signature whistles are cohesion calls. Anim Behav 56: 829-838.) and birds (Farnsworth 2005FARNSWORTH A. 2005. Flight calls and their value for future ornithological studies and conservation research. Auk 122: 733-746.). In birds, contact calls are emitted during the feeding of chicks (Balsby & Bradbury 2009BALSBY TJS & BRADBURY JW. 2009. Vocal matching by orange-fronted conures (Aratinga canicularis). Behav Process 82: 133-139.) and coordination activities (Jouventin & Aubin 2002JOUVENTIN P & AUBIN T. 2002. Acoustic systems are adapted to breeding ecologies: individual recognition in nesting penguins. Anim Behav 64: 747-757.). Contact calls are soft sounds easily identifiable, of broadband, and audible only at close range.

Contact calls between members of the P. dumicola couple (contact call to the female) were similar to that found in Forpus passerinus (Berg et al. 2011BERG KS, DELGADO S, OKAWA R, BEISSINGER SR & BRADBURY JW. 2011. Contact calls are used for individual mate recognition in free-ranging green-rumped parrotlets, Forpus passerinus. Anim Behav 81: 241-248.). These are high-frequency calls (5-6 kHz), with short duration and completely distinguishable from the rest of the calls. These contact-call characteristics possibly complicate the location of the sound source to predators (Marler 2004MARLER P. 2004. Bird calls: their potential for behavioral neurobiology. Ann New York Acad Sci 1016: 31-44.). Therefore, similarly to Berg et al. (2011)BERG KS, DELGADO S, OKAWA R, BEISSINGER SR & BRADBURY JW. 2011. Contact calls are used for individual mate recognition in free-ranging green-rumped parrotlets, Forpus passerinus. Anim Behav 81: 241-248. findings, mate recognition via contact calls in P. dumicola may be selectively advantageous during incubation because it reduces the potential costs associated with confusing contact calls of mates with those of non-mates.

Territorial call was recorded when a male of P. dumicola entered territories previously occupied by another conspecific male. Territorial calls were characterized by a high frequency (5.25 kHz) and several repeated notes (between 9 and 12). Although Crotophaga ani presented a lower mean frequency (1 kHz) in territorial calls (Grieves et al. 2015GRIEVES LA, LOGUE DM, QUINN JS. 2015. Vocal repertoire of cooperatively breeding smooth-billed anis. J Field Ornithol 86: 130-143.), they had frequency towards the end of the note and territorial calls were also recorded when a group or alone bird entered another group’s territory. According to Crane et al. (2016)CRANE JMS, SAVAGE JL & RUSSELL AF. 2016. Diversity and function of vocalisations in the cooperative chestnut-crowned babbler. Emu 116: 241-253., territorial calls could be used in dominance interactions in both breeding and roosting behaviors.

Incubation calls emitted by males of P. dumicola during incubation were short whistles of modulated frequency. Studies carried out with Parus major mention that this call can have an effect on predation because the issuing individual has fewer possibilities of becoming prey (Krams et al. 2006KRAMS I, KRAMA T & IGAUNE K. 2006. Alarm calls of wintering great tits Parus major: warning of mate, reciprocal altruism or a message to the predator? J Avian Biol 37: 131-136.). Records on the vocal structure of incubation calls are scarce. It has been documented that Passer domesticus, which also has biparental nest construction, emits this call after placing feathers in the nest construction (García-Lopez de Hierro et al. 2013GARCÍA-LOPEZ DE HIERRO L, MOLEON M & RYAN PG. 2013. Is carrying feathers a sexually selected trait in house sparrows?. Ethology 119: 1e13.). Probably, the importance of this call is associated with its role in reproduction and parental care, including the nest construction by both members of the couple.

Long distance call of P. dumicola had approximately eight notes of modulated frequency, where the beginning of the tone has a lower frequency like in P. ruficeps repertoire (Crane et al. 2016CRANE JMS, SAVAGE JL & RUSSELL AF. 2016. Diversity and function of vocalisations in the cooperative chestnut-crowned babbler. Emu 116: 241-253.). This type of call is common among other vertebrate groups (e.g. frogs, bats, apes, etc.) and can report spatial position (Class & Brommer 2016CLASS B & BROMMER JE. 2016. Senescence of personality in a wild bird. Behav Ecol Sociobol 70: 733-744., Spillman et al. 2017SPILLMANN B, VAN SCHAIK CP, SETIA TM & SADJADI SO. 2017. Who shall I say is calling? Validation of a caller recognition procedure in Bornean flanged male orangutan (Pongo pygmaeuswurmbii) long calls. Bioacoustics 26: 109-120.) or increase feeding possibilities (Cortopassi & Bradbury 2006CORTOPASSI KA & BRADBURY JW. 2006. Contact call diversity in wild Orange-fronted Parakeet pairs, Aratinga canicularis. Anim Behav 71: 1141-1154.). Calls of modulated frequencies are not easily masked by environmental noise or other conspecific signals (Catchpole & Slater 2008CATCHPOLE CK & SLATER JB. 2008. Bird song: biological themes and variations. Ed: Cambridge University Press, USA, 335 p.).

In P. dumicola the long-distance call may be the first step toward recognizing individual identity or kinship as indicated by Crane et al. (2016)CRANE JMS, SAVAGE JL & RUSSELL AF. 2016. Diversity and function of vocalisations in the cooperative chestnut-crowned babbler. Emu 116: 241-253.. Further research must be done to test whether short-distance alert call is usually emitted before the long-distance call or if a territorial call is given before the call to a female. This will, allow extracting novel call sequences in the future, representing a new analogy between animal communication systems and human language (Hurford 2014HURFORD JR. 2014. The Origins of Language: A Slim Guide. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, p. 18-40.).

Cataloging calls of bird species is key for understanding the complexity of bird vocalizations considering that informal or individual descriptions rarely assess vocal behavior correctly. The male’s repertoire of P. dumicola provides important information to understand the link between ecology and behavior of this species. In this sense, the wide variety of calls such as those found in P. dumicola may allow birds to distinguish different environmental and ecological situations. The baseline information reported here is also key for future studies on the acoustic structure of passerines. Thus, these types of studies improve our understanding of adaptations of communication and provide valuable insights intothe ecological factors that drive the variation and evolution of animal signals.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This research was part of Leon E. PhD Universidad Nacional del Litoral and CONICET scholarship. Thank to S. Regner, M. Juani and A. Orbes for assistance in the field samples. The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest.

REFERENCES

  • ALTMANN J. 1974. Observational Study of Behavior: Sampling Methods. Behaviour 49: 3-4.
  • ARETA JI & PEARMAN M. 2013. Species Limits and Clinal Variation in a Widespread High Andean Furnariid: The Buff-breasted Earthcreeper (Upucerthia validirostris). Condor 115(1).
  • BAIGRIE BD, THOMPSON AM & FLOWER TP. 2014. Interspecific signalling between mutualists: Food-thieving drongos use a cooperative sentinel call to manipulate foraging partners. P Roy S B: Biol Sci 281(1791).
  • BAKER MC & BECKER AM. 2002. Mobbing calls of Black-capped Chickadees: effects of urgency on call production. Wilson J Ornithology 114: 510-516.
  • BALSBY TJS & BRADBURY JW. 2009. Vocal matching by orange-fronted conures (Aratinga canicularis). Behav Process 82: 133-139.
  • BERG KS, DELGADO S, OKAWA R, BEISSINGER SR & BRADBURY JW. 2011. Contact calls are used for individual mate recognition in free-ranging green-rumped parrotlets, Forpus passerinus. Anim Behav 81: 241-248.
  • BIOACOUSTICS RESEARCH PROGRAM. 2014. Raven Pro: Interactive Sound Analysis Software (Version 1.5) [Computer software]. Ithaca, NY: The Cornell Lab of Ornithology. Available from http://www.birds.cornell.edu/raven
    » http://www.birds.cornell.edu/raven
  • BLUMSTEIN DM. 1999. The evolution of functionally referential alarm communication: multiple adaptations; mutiple constraints. Evol Commun 3: 135-147.
  • BONCORAGLIO G & SAINO N. 2007. Habitat structure and the evolution of bird song: A meta-analysis of the evidence for the acoustic adaptation hypothesis. Funct Ecol 21: 134-142.
  • BRADBURY JW & VEHRENCAMP SL. 1998. Principles of Animal Communication (Sinauer, Sunderland, MA). Chap3, 65-112.
  • BRADBURY JW & VEHRENCAMP SL. 2011. Principles of animal communication, 2nd ed. (Sinauer, Sunderland. MA), 697 p.
  • BRADBURY RB & ALLEN DS. 2003. Evaluation of the impact of the pilot UK Arable Stewardship Scheme on breeding and wintering birds. Bird Study 50(2): 131-141.
  • BRIEFER E, OSIEJUK TS, RYBAK F & AUBIN T. 2010. Are bird song complexity and song sharing shaped by habitat structure? An information theory and statistical approach. J Theor Biol 262(1): 151-164.
  • BUDNEY GF & GROTKE RW. 1997. Techniques for audio recording vocalizations of tropical birds. Ornithol Monogr 48: 147-163.
  • CARO T. 2005. Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, USA, p. 398-403.
  • CATCHPOLE CK & SLATER JB. 2008. Bird song: biological themes and variations. Ed: Cambridge University Press, USA, 335 p.
  • CLASS B & BROMMER JE. 2016. Senescence of personality in a wild bird. Behav Ecol Sociobol 70: 733-744.
  • CORTOPASSI KA & BRADBURY JW. 2006. Contact call diversity in wild Orange-fronted Parakeet pairs, Aratinga canicularis. Anim Behav 71: 1141-1154.
  • CRANE JMS, SAVAGE JL & RUSSELL AF. 2016. Diversity and function of vocalisations in the cooperative chestnut-crowned babbler. Emu 116: 241-253.
  • DAVIS WE. 1988. Distress calls in birds: an avian enigma. Birds Obs 16(4): 205-211.
  • DORADO RODRIGUES TF, AZARIAS CAMPOS V, DOS SANTOS MM, PANSONATO A & STRÜSMAN C. 2012. Circumstances and bioacoustics of the distress call of Leptodactylus chaquensis (Anura: Leptodactylidae) during predation by Thamnodynastes chaquensis (Serpentes: Dipsadidae) in the Brazilian Pantanal. Salamandra 48: 237-240.
  • DUNBAR RIM. 1998. The social brain hypothesis. Evol Anthropol 6: 178-190.
  • ENDLER JA. 1993. Some general comments on the evolution and design of animal communication systems. Philos Trans Biol Sci 340: 215-225.
  • FARNSWORTH A. 2005. Flight calls and their value for future ornithological studies and conservation research. Auk 122: 733-746.
  • FLOWER TP, GRIBBLE M & RIDLEY AR. 2014. Deception by flexible alarm mimicry in an african bird. Science 344(6183).
  • FRAGA RM & SALVADOR SA. 2013. Conducta y biología reproductiva de la tacuarita azul (Polioptila dumicola) en un área pampeana y otra del espinal. Argentina. Hist Nat 3: 37-50.
  • FREEBERG TM, DUNBAR RIM & ORD TJ. 2012. Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity. Phil Trans R Soc B 367: 1785-1801.
  • GARCIA NC. 2016. Estudio comparativo filogenético de los patrones de variación vocal y de coloración del plumaje en los “Cardinalinos Azules” (Passeriformes: Cardinalidae). Tesis doctoral, Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires.
  • GARCÍA-LOPEZ DE HIERRO L, MOLEON M & RYAN PG. 2013. Is carrying feathers a sexually selected trait in house sparrows?. Ethology 119: 1e13.
  • GLOAG R & KACELNIK A. 2013. Host manipulation via begging call structure in the brood-parasitic Shiny Cowbird. Anim Behav 86: 101-109.
  • GOEDERT D, DIAS RI & MACEDO RH. 2014. Nestling use of alternative acoustic antipredator responses is related to immune condition and social context. Anim Behav 91: 161-169.
  • GRIEG-SMITH IW. 1984. Distress calling by woodland birds: seasonal patterns, individual consistency and the presence of conspecifics. Z Tierpsychol 66: 1-10.
  • GRIESSER M. 2013. Do warning calls boost survival of signal recipients? Evidence from a field experiment in a group-living bird species. Front Zool 10(1): 49.
  • GRIEVES LA, LOGUE DM, QUINN JS. 2015. Vocal repertoire of cooperatively breeding smooth-billed anis. J Field Ornithol 86: 130-143.
  • HAMILTON WJ. 1962. Evidence concerning the Function of Nocturnal Call Notes of Migratory Birds. The Condor 64(5).
  • HOLLÉN LI & RADFORD AN. 2009. The development of alarm call behaviour in mammals and birds. Anim Behav 78(4): 791-800.
  • HURFORD JR. 2014. The Origins of Language: A Slim Guide. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, p. 18-40.
  • IUCN. 2020. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. Version 2020-1. https://www.iucnredlist.org Downloaded on 19 March 2020.
    » https://www.iucnredlist.org
  • JANIK VM & SLATER PJ. 1998. Context-specific use suggests that bottlenose dolphin signature whistles are cohesion calls. Anim Behav 56: 829-838.
  • JOUVENTIN P & AUBIN T. 2002. Acoustic systems are adapted to breeding ecologies: individual recognition in nesting penguins. Anim Behav 64: 747-757.
  • JURISEVIC MA & SANDERSON KJ. 1998. Acoustic discrimination of passerine anti-predator signals by Australian raptors. Aust J Zool 46(4): 369-379.
  • KITZMANN CD & CAINE NG. 2009. Marmoset (Callithrix geoffroyi) food-associated calls are functionally referential. Ethology 115(5): 439-448.
  • KLUMP GM & SHALTER MD. 1984. Acoustic behaviour of birds and mammals in the predator context. I. Factors affecting the structure of alarm signals. II. The functional significance and evolution of alarm signals. Zeitschrift fürTierpsychoogie 66: 189-226.
  • KONDO N & WATANABE S. 2009. Contact calls: Information and social function. Jpn Psychol Res 51: 197-208.
  • KRAMS I, KRAMA T & IGAUNE K. 2006. Alarm calls of wintering great tits Parus major: warning of mate, reciprocal altruism or a message to the predator? J Avian Biol 37: 131-136.
  • KRAMS I, VRUBLEVSKA J, KOOSA K, KRAMA T, MIERAUSKAS P, RANTALA MJ & TILGAR V. 2014. Hissing calls improve survival in incubating female great tits (Parus major). Acta Ethol 17: 83.
  • LANGMORE NE, HUNT S & KILNER RM. 2003. Escalation of a coevolutionary arms race through host rejection of brood parasitic young. Nature 422: 157-160.
  • LEAVESLEY AJ & MAGRATH RD. 2005. Communicating about danger: Urgency alarm calling in a bird. Anim Behav 70(2): 365-373.
  • LE ROUX A, CHERRY MI & MANSER MB. 2009. The vocal repertoire in a solitary foraging carnivore, Cynictis penicillata, may reflect facultative sociality. Naturwissenschaften 96: 575-584.
  • MARLER P. 1955. Characteristics of some animal calls. Nature 176: 6-8.
  • MARLER P. 2004. Bird calls: their potential for behavioral neurobiology. Ann New York Acad Sci 1016: 31-44.
  • MARTIN JP, DOUCET SM, KNOX RC & MENILL DJ. 2011. Body size correlates negatively with frequency of distress calls and songs of Neotropical birds. J Field Ornith 82(3): 259-268.
  • MARTIN P & BATESON P. 2007. Measuring Behaviour. 3rd ed. (New York: Cambridge University Press), 186 p.
  • MATHEVON N, AUBIN T, VIELLIARD J, DA SILVA ML, SEBE F & BOSCOLO D. 2008 Singing in the rain forest: how a tropical bird song transfers information. PLoS ONE 3:e1580.
  • MAYNARD-SMITH J & HARPER D. 2003. Animal signals. Oxford University Press.
  • MCCOMB K & SEMPLE S. 2005. Coevolution of vocal communication and sociality in primates. Biol Lett 1(4): 381-385.
  • MØLLER AP, CHRISTIANSEN SS & MOUSSEAU TA. 2011. Sexual signals, risk of predation and escape behavior. Behav Ecol 22: 800-807.
  • NAGUIB M & RIEBEL K. 2006. Bird song: a key model in animal communication. In: Brown K (Ed). Encyclopaedia of Language and Linguistics, 2nd ed. Elsevier, Oxford, p. 40-53.
  • NEUDORF DL & SEALY SG. 2002. Distress Calls of Birds in a Neotropical Cloud Forest1. Biotropica 34(1): 118-126.
  • NOCERA JJ, TAYLOR PD & RATCLIFFE LM. 2008. Inspection of mob-calls as sources of predator information: Response of migrant and resident birds in the Neotropics. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62(11).
  • OLGUÍN PF. 2016. Biología reproductiva de especies de aves del río Paraná medio, Argentina. Tesis doctoral. Facultad de Bioquímica y Ciencias Biológicas, Universidad Nacional del Litoral.
  • PARTAN S & MARLER P. 2005. Issues in the classification of multimodal communication signals. Am Nat 166: 231-245.
  • PELTZER PM & LAJMANOVICH RC. 2004. Anuran tadpole assemblages in riparian areas of the Middle Paraná River, Argentina. Biodiv Conserv 13: 1833-1842.
  • PODOS J. 2014. Sexual selection and the evolution of vocal mating signals: lessons from neotropical songbirds. In: Macedo RH and Machado G (Eds) Sexual selection: perspectives and models from the neotropics. Elsevier Inc., Amsterdam, p. 341-363.
  • PODOS J & WARREN PS. 2007. The evolution of geographic variation in birdsong. Adv Study Behav 37: 403-458.
  • RALPH CJ, GEUPEL GR, PYLE P, MARTIN TE & DE SANTE DF & MILÁ B. 1996. Manual de métodos de campo para el monitoreo de aves terrestres. Albany, CA: Pacific Southwest Research Station, Forest Service, U.S, p. 8-24.
  • RIDGELY SR & TUDOR G. 1997. The birds of South America, (ed: University of Texas Press, Austin), p. 516.
  • ROHWER S, FRETWELL SD & TUCKFIELD RC. 1976. Distress Screams as a Measure of Kinship in Birds. Am Midl Nat 96(2).
  • R STUDIO TEAM. 2020. RStudio: Integrated Development for R. RStudio, PBC, Boston, MA URL http://www.rstudio.com/
    » http://www.rstudio.com/
  • RYAN MJ, BRENOWITZ EA. 1985. The role of body size, phylogeny, and ambient noise in the evolution of bird song. Am Nat 126: 87-100.
  • SÁNCHEZ LC, PELTZER PM & LAJMANOVICH RC. 2009. Structure of wetland-breeding anuran assemblages from the southern section of Paraná river. Argentina. Herpetol J 19: 173-184.
  • SEYFARTH RM & CHENEY DL. 2003. Signalers and receivers in animal communication. Ann Rev Psychol 54: 145-173.
  • SIEVING KE, HETRICK SA & AVERY ML. 2010. The versatility of graded acoustic measures in classification of predation threats by the tufted titmouse Baeolophus bicolor: exploring a mixed framework for threat communication. Oiko 119: 264-276.
  • SILVESTRI A, MORGAN K & RIDLEY AR. 2019. The association between evidence of a predator threat and responsiveness to alarm calls in Western Australian magpies (Cracticus tibicen dorsalis). Peer J 7: e7572.
  • SOSA-LÓPEZ JR & MENNILL DJ. 2014. The vocal behavior of the Brown-throated Wren (Troglodytes brunneicollis): song structure, repertoires, sharing, syntax, and diel variation. J Ornithol 155: 435-446.
  • SPILLMANN B, VAN SCHAIK CP, SETIA TM & SADJADI SO. 2017. Who shall I say is calling? Validation of a caller recognition procedure in Bornean flanged male orangutan (Pongo pygmaeuswurmbii) long calls. Bioacoustics 26: 109-120.
  • SUZUKI TN. 2011. Parental alarm calls warn nestlings about different predatory threats. Curr Biol 21(1): R15-R16.
  • TEMPLETON CN, GREENE E & DAVIS K. 2005. Allometry of alarm calls: black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science 308: 1934-1937.
  • TIBBETTS EA & DALE J. 2007. Individual recognition: it is good to be different. Trends Ecol Evol 22(10): 529-537.
  • TOBIAS M, EVANS BJ & KELLEY DB. 2011. Evolution of advertisement calls in African clawded frogs. Behaviour 148: 519-549.
  • VARGAS-CASTRO LE, SANCHEZ NV & BARRANTES G. 2012. Repertoire size and syllable sharing in the songof the clay-colored thrush (Turdus grayi). Wilson J Ornithol 124: 446-453.
  • WHEELER BC. 2008. Selfish or altruistic? An analysis of alarm call function in wild capuchin monkeys, Cebusa pellanigritus. Anim Behav 76: 1465-1475.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    25 Nov 2022
  • Date of issue
    2022

History

  • Received
    02 Apr 2020
  • Accepted
    27 Feb 2021
Academia Brasileira de Ciências Rua Anfilófio de Carvalho, 29, 3º andar, 20030-060 Rio de Janeiro RJ Brasil, Tel: +55 21 3907-8100 - Rio de Janeiro - RJ - Brazil
E-mail: aabc@abc.org.br