Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Learning, Portfolio Complexity and Informational Asymmetry in Forecasts of Sell-Side Analysts

ABSTRACT

The aim of this study was to analyze the association of learning and complexity in the target price forecasts and sell-side analysts' recommendations on the BM&FBovespa. The sample comprised forecasts of 195 stocks, 75 brokers and 569 analysts between 2005 and 2013, analyzed by linear models with panel data. Our results suggest that the experience with the stock, with the sector and complexity of the portfolio confirmed the learn by doing, but the overall experience showed contradictions due to information asymmetry. Despite anchoring in their peers, analysts achieved significant returns, but showed forecasts with low accuracy. Therfore, we concluded that more experienced analysts may intentionally contradict themselves in an attempt to bias the market. Finally, we suggest the development of less biased analyst rankings in order to increase the competitiveness and quality in the results of the analyzes.

Keywords:
Financial analysts; Target price forecast; Stock recommendations; Learning; Portfolio complexity

RESUMO

O objetivo desse trabalho foi analisar a associação do aprendizado e a complexidade nas previsões de preços-alvo e recomendações de analistas sell-side na BM&FBovespa. A amostra compreendeu previsões de 195 ações, 75 corretoras e 569 analistas no período entre 2005 e 2013, analisadas através de modelos lineares com dados em painel. Os resultados apresentaram indícios de que a experiência com o ativo, com o setor e a complexidade da carteira confirmaram o lean by doing, porém a experiência em geral apresentou contradições devido à assimetria informacional. Apesar da ancoragem em seus pares, os analistas obtiveram retornos significativos, mas apresentaram previsões com baixa acurácia. Dessa forma, concluímos que analistas mais experientes podem entrar em contradição de forma intencional na tentativa de viesar o mercado. Por fim, sugerimos a formulação de rankings de analistas menos viesados como forma de aumentar a competitividade e a qualidade nos resultados das análises.

Palavras-chaves:
Analistas financeiros; Previsão de preços-alvo; Recomendações de ações; Aprendizado; Complexidade da carteira

1. INTRODUCTION

The financial market analysts regularly conduct target price and profit forecasts in order to support their recommendations to buy and sale assets on the stock market. For such purpose, they use tools and ability to price these assets at a given time horizon. Issued reports aim to provide good analyzes to their customers, contributing to operations that enable maximizing investors returns (CHUNG; JO, 1996CHUNG, K. H.; JO, H. The impact of security analysts' monitoring and marketing functions on the market value of firms. Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis, v. 31, n. 04, p. 493-512, 1996.).

In the exercise of their activities, there are elements that improve or deteriorate the performance of these forecasts. However, few studies have observed these mechanisms in the Brazilian market, which shows the need to verify how these professionals practice, how they observe their mistakes and those by their peers. Based on these elements, we can verify how their forecasts and recommendations can be improved (MARTINEZ, 2007MARTINEZ, A. L. Analysts' optimism and selection bias. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v. 4, n. 2, p. 104-118, 2007.; 2008MARTINEZ, A. L. The effect of earnings projection revisions on stock returns in brazil. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v.5, n.2, p.121-135, 2008.; 2009MARTINEZ, A. L. Determinantes da acurácia das previsões dos analistas do mercado de capitais. Contabilidade, Gestão e Governança, v. 10, n. 2, 2009.).

This study aimed to analyze the association of learning and complexity of the portfolio in sell-side analysts' target price forecasts and recommendations considering the information asymmetry on the BM&FBovespa. The learning analysis in the forecast is a reflection of the evolution of performance with the experience in the industry, the complexity of the hedging portfolio and the informational effect of the revisions in analyzes.

Research by Mikhail, Walther and Willis (1997)MIKHAIL, M. B.; WALTHER, B. R.; WILLIS, R. H. Do security analysts improve their performance with experience? Journal of Accounting Research, v. 35, p. 131-157, 1997. investigated the effects of learning by repetition, based on learn by doing, with the argument that the abilities of each individual are not homogeneous. The metrics used as a proxy for experience, based on the number of forecasts performed repeatedly in a given period, including categorization by asset and sector.

The results by Mikhail, Walther and Willis (1997)MIKHAIL, M. B.; WALTHER, B. R.; WILLIS, R. H. Do security analysts improve their performance with experience? Journal of Accounting Research, v. 35, p. 131-157, 1997., as those by Jacob, Lys and Neale (1999)JACOB, J.; LYS, T. Z.; NEALE, M. A. Expertise in forecasting performance of security analysts. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 28, n. 1, p. 51-82, nov. 1999. and Clement (1999)CLEMENT, M. B. Analyst forecast accuracy: Do ability, resources, and portfolio complexity matter? Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 27, n. 3, p. 285-303, jul. 1999. demonstrate ample evidence that the experience with the firm in the North American market contributes to improvements in accuracy. These surveys focus on observing whether these experience metrics promote improvements in their results, observing recommendations' accuracy and returns. In the analysis of experience, we can observe whether analysts are performing new analysis or just replicating previous reports.

The complexity of the hedging portfolio is another factor that affects the analyst's cognitive ability. Clement (1999)CLEMENT, M. B. Analyst forecast accuracy: Do ability, resources, and portfolio complexity matter? Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 27, n. 3, p. 285-303, jul. 1999. provides evidence that the increased complexity, which is the number of companies and sectors that the analyst covers in their portfolio, reduces accuracy. This effect is especially important for brokers, for it contributes to observe ways in which analysts can organize themselves into teams which enable better results.

Informativity also influences the activity, because the role of analysts is to convey information through their analysis, as well as the ability to use this disclosed information. This measure, from the learning perspective, seeks to observe the ability to absorb and verify their own mistakes and those by their peers in face of previous forecasts, or if analysts are simply copying modifications previously made (BRAV; LEHAVY, 2003BRAV, A.; LEHAVY, R. An empirical analysis of analysts' target prices: Short-term informativeness and long-term dynamics. The Journal of Finance, v. 58, n. 5, p. 1933-1968, 2003.; ASQUITH; MIKHAIL; AU, 2005ASQUITH, P.; MIKHAIL, M. B.; AU, A. S. Information content of equity analyst reports. Journal of Financial Economics, v. 75, n. 2, p. 245-282, fev. 2005.).

Despite the empirical evidence, there are elements in the Brazilian market showing evidence that some of these mechanisms of improvements do not work the same way as in the countries investigated. Studies by Martinez (2007MARTINEZ, A. L. Analysts' optimism and selection bias. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v. 4, n. 2, p. 104-118, 2007., 2008MARTINEZ, A. L. The effect of earnings projection revisions on stock returns in brazil. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v.5, n.2, p.121-135, 2008. and 2009)MARTINEZ, A. L. Determinantes da acurácia das previsões dos analistas do mercado de capitais. Contabilidade, Gestão e Governança, v. 10, n. 2, 2009. and Saito, Villalobos and Benetti (2008)SAITO, R.; VILLALOBOS, S. J. S.; BENETTI, C. Qualidade das projeções dos analistas sell-side: evidência empírica do mercado brasileiro. Revista de Administração da Universidade de São Paulo, v. 43, n. 4, 2008. show evidence that in Brazil, analysts present lower performance than in other markets such as the North American. Furthermore, economic characteristics can also influence this process, as the market instability, smaller amount of traded assets, the differentiated amount of the investor population and, especially by the absence of effective mechanisms that promote competition in the industry.

In Brazil there are few rankings and awards that encourage improvements in the activity. We enumerate the Institutional Investor ranking that works by the voting of managers, the award of the Association of Investment Analysts and Professionals of the Capital Market (APIMEC) that works by vote among peers and the ranking of the State Agency Broadcast which analyzes only registered analysts for the return of their recommendations. Some of these mechanisms are questioned due to biases that their methodologies present (EMERY; LI, 2009EMERY, D. R.; Li, X. Are the wall street analyst rankings popularity contests? Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis, v. 44, n. 2, 2009.).

Another reason for this research is due to the growth in the volume of public offerings in recent years, see Figure 1. The growth in the number of traded assets and increased informational efficiency reinforce the idea that analysts' experience and their portfolios complexity are following the evolution and the increase in transactions (MOBAREK; FIORANTE, 2014).

Figure 1
Volume of IPOs in recent years in Brazil.

Evidence in Brazil found by Martinez (2009)MARTINEZ, A. L. Determinantes da acurácia das previsões dos analistas do mercado de capitais. Contabilidade, Gestão e Governança, v. 10, n. 2, 2009., using earnings forecasts show that there are improvements with the experience, but no effects were observed on the complexity of hedging portfolio. Given the lack of research in the Brazilian market, and specificities in relation to other markets already researched, this gap motivated the need to understand how analysts improve their forecasts and recommendations abilities, especially by analyzing the target price, a variable which is little explored.

The advantage of the target price use are evidence of aspects less conflicting than earnings forecasts. This consideration is based on the following results: (i) target price forecasts seem to be less biased by influences such as conflicts of interest of individuals, and (ii) the effect of informational target price revisions have greater information content than revisions of earnings forecasts. Such evidence indicate attractiveness for the analysis of this variable (ASQUITH, MIKHAIL, AU 2005ASQUITH, P.; MIKHAIL, M. B.; AU, A. S. Information content of equity analyst reports. Journal of Financial Economics, v. 75, n. 2, p. 245-282, fev. 2005.; KERL, 2011KERL, A. G. Target price accuracy. BuR - Business Research, v. 4, n. 1, p. 74-96, mar. 2011.).

This research is distinguished for its approach of the period between 2005 and 2013 in the Brazilian market, as well as the learning effects with regard to target price forecasts and recommendations (buy/sell/hold) by sell-side analysts. We believe that the evidence of improvements in forecasts and recommendations in emerging markets such as Brazil allows us to assume that the relationship between learning, portfolio complexity and accuracy may demonstrate new behaviors, including observation of how the improvement of this process occurs over time.

This work was structured as follows: topic 2 presents a survey of previous empirical evidence; topic 3 describes the performance metrics used; topic 4 lists the hypotheses based on previous studies; topic 5 explains the data sample used; topic 6 details the method used based on the previous items; topic 7 analyzes the results; and topic 8 the final considerations.

2. PREVIOUS STUDIES

Research addressing analysts' forecasts are began over half a century ago, starting with discussions on the role of analysts (e.g. Godfrey, 1953GODFREY, J. P. What is an analyst? Financial Analysts Journal, v. 9, n. 5, p. 103-105, 1953.). Ramnath, Rock and Shane (2008)RAMNATH, S.; ROCK, S.; SHANE, P. The financial analyst forecasting literature: A taxonomy with suggestions for further research. International Journal of Forecasting, v. 24, n. 1, p. 34-75, 2008. and Bradshaw, Brown and Huang (2013)BRADSHAW, M. T.; BROWN, L. D.; HUANG, K. Do sell-side analysts exhibit differential target price forecasting ability? Review of Accounting Studies, v. 18, n. 4, p. 930-955, 2013. summarize that most studies investigate forecasts based on corporate earnings estimates, based on exploring the determinants of good forecasts and the existence of biases of agents in the development of this activity.

According to Bradshaw (2002)BRADSHAW, M. T. The use of target prices to justify sell-side analysts' stock recommendations. Accounting Horizons, v. 16, n. 1, p. 27-41, 2002., target price forecasts are used by analysts as a way to support the recommendations to their investor clients. When the recommendation suggests buying, the prediction is that the company's value is undervalued; when holding is suggested the asset presents an approximately fair value, and when selling is suggested, the expectation is that the company's value is overvalued.

Brav and Lehavy (2003)BRAV, A.; LEHAVY, R. An empirical analysis of analysts' target prices: Short-term informativeness and long-term dynamics. The Journal of Finance, v. 58, n. 5, p. 1933-1968, 2003. and Asquith, Mikhail and Au (2005)ASQUITH, P.; MIKHAIL, M. B.; AU, A. S. Information content of equity analyst reports. Journal of Financial Economics, v. 75, n. 2, p. 245-282, fev. 2005. performed na informational analyzed and observed significant market impact by revisions of the target price forecasts, as well as the revisions of recommendations and earnings forecasts. The key point by Brav and Lehavy (2003)BRAV, A.; LEHAVY, R. An empirical analysis of analysts' target prices: Short-term informativeness and long-term dynamics. The Journal of Finance, v. 58, n. 5, p. 1933-1968, 2003. is that analysts converge the degree of recommendation to convey confidence in their price forecast. Based on this argument, Brav and Lehavy (2003)BRAV, A.; LEHAVY, R. An empirical analysis of analysts' target prices: Short-term informativeness and long-term dynamics. The Journal of Finance, v. 58, n. 5, p. 1933-1968, 2003. confirmed the argument by Bradshaw (2002)BRADSHAW, M. T. The use of target prices to justify sell-side analysts' stock recommendations. Accounting Horizons, v. 16, n. 1, p. 27-41, 2002. that forecasts support recommendations.

However, there is evidence that this relationship between forecast and recommendation presents moments of informational asymmetry, because analysts are better informed than investors. This asymmetry can stimulate changes in interest and modifications in the status of recommendations (BRADSHAW, 2002BRADSHAW, M. T. The use of target prices to justify sell-side analysts' stock recommendations. Accounting Horizons, v. 16, n. 1, p. 27-41, 2002.). In some cases, these incentives provide an imbalance in analyst reports, which in somecases therefore, will prefer not to disclose their analyzes.

his effect of suppressing certain analysis is termed as self-selction bias, which means that analysts prefer not to disclose price forecasts when these do not support their recommendations or when they are uncertain about their estimates (BRADSHAW, 2002BRADSHAW, M. T. The use of target prices to justify sell-side analysts' stock recommendations. Accounting Horizons, v. 16, n. 1, p. 27-41, 2002.). When this occurs more often, recommendations in some cases may present imbalances in the buy, hold and sell ratio. For this reason, Francis and Soffer (1997)FRANCIS, J.; SOFFER, L. The relative informativeness of analysts' stock recommendations and earnings forecast revisions. Journal of Accounting Research, p. 193-211, 1997. argue that the trend in the proportion of reports to buy rather than to hold and sell is intentional.

Considering the imbalance in recommendations, Asquith, Mikhail and Au (2005)ASQUITH, P.; MIKHAIL, M. B.; AU, A. S. Information content of equity analyst reports. Journal of Financial Economics, v. 75, n. 2, p. 245-282, fev. 2005. observed the differentiation between revisions for less (downgrades) and revisions for more (upgrades). An important sign is that investors react more significantly when reports show reviews for less (downgrades). The excess of buying recommendations and information asymmetry can explain the reason for optimism normally observed by research.

The results presented by the imbalance of forecasts and recommendations also allow criticism regarding metrics. In the Italian market, Bonini et al (2010)BONINI, S.; ZANETTI, L.; BIANCHINI, R.; SALVI, A. Target price accuracy in equity research. Journal of Business Finance & Accounting, v. 37, n. 9-10, p. 1177-1217, 2010., in turn, criticize the traditional metric of accuracy, especially by signs of no reversion to the mean and the autocorrelation. These elements make it difficult to analyze the determinants, which affects previous evidence. For this, it is necessary to use models for analysis to correct these aspects. Although investors do not consider the accuracy of target prices as a differential, as they focus on recommendations, the metric is defended by Bradshaw, Brown and Huang (2013)BRADSHAW, M. T.; BROWN, L. D.; HUANG, K. Do sell-side analysts exhibit differential target price forecasting ability? Review of Accounting Studies, v. 18, n. 4, p. 930-955, 2013. as an important measure of the analyst's performance, as it is a way to validate the recommendation.

Kerl (2011)KERL, A. G. Target price accuracy. BuR - Business Research, v. 4, n. 1, p. 74-96, mar. 2011., in the German market, also focused on analyzing the accuracy of target prices, but from another viewpoint. The main results show persistence in accuracy, as well as in buying recommendations, which corroborates the effects previously discussed. In spite of this, Bradshaw, Brown and Huang (2013)BRADSHAW, M. T.; BROWN, L. D.; HUANG, K. Do sell-side analysts exhibit differential target price forecasting ability? Review of Accounting Studies, v. 18, n. 4, p. 930-955, 2013. analyze the determinants and the behavior of errors over time and show that analysts have limited capacity to persistently perform forecasts with accuracy.

In Brazil, few studies were dedicated to the research of analysts covering companies on the BM&FBovespa, we can cite studies by Martinez (2007)MARTINEZ, A. L. Analysts' optimism and selection bias. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v. 4, n. 2, p. 104-118, 2007., investigating the optimism of analysts' forecasts in the Brazilian market; Martinez (2008)MARTINEZ, A. L. The effect of earnings projection revisions on stock returns in brazil. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v.5, n.2, p.121-135, 2008., who analyzes the impact of revisions of projections; and Martinez (2009)MARTINEZ, A. L. Determinantes da acurácia das previsões dos analistas do mercado de capitais. Contabilidade, Gestão e Governança, v. 10, n. 2, 2009., who investigates the determinants of accuracy. The main findings by Martinez (2007)MARTINEZ, A. L. Analysts' optimism and selection bias. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v. 4, n. 2, p. 104-118, 2007. were: the poor accuracy performance and forecasting errors, which are corelated to prior period errors, which is also evidence found in Martinez (2008)MARTINEZ, A. L. The effect of earnings projection revisions on stock returns in brazil. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v.5, n.2, p.121-135, 2008..

Martinez (2009)MARTINEZ, A. L. Determinantes da acurácia das previsões dos analistas do mercado de capitais. Contabilidade, Gestão e Governança, v. 10, n. 2, 2009. shows that analysts of Brazilian companies showed a persistent optimism on average. Despite the optimism, the result by Martinez (2008)MARTINEZ, A. L. The effect of earnings projection revisions on stock returns in brazil. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v.5, n.2, p.121-135, 2008. complements the evidence that there are moments of pessimism in the market, demonstrating different impacts for negative and positive revisions. But the most evident in this latest survey is that the Brazilian market is more sensitive to bad news than developed markets such as the North American.

Recently, the research by Dalmacio et al (2013)DALMACIO, F. Z.; LOPES, A. B.; REZENDE, A. J.; NETO, A. S. An analysis of the relationship between corporate governance and accuracy of the analysts forecasts of the brazilian market. RAM Revista de Administração Mackenzie, v. 14, n. 5, p. 104-139, 2013. analyzed the impact of governance practices in the accuracy of analysts. The research presents strong relationships in improving the accuracy in the level of the firm's corporate governance. On the other hand, the research by Matinez and Dumer (2014)MARTINEZ, A. L.; DUMER, M. Adoption of IFRS and the properties of analysts' forecasts: The brazilian case. Revista de Contabilidade e Organizações, v. 8, n. 20, abr. 2014., which analyzes the effect of the performance on the adoption of International Financial Reporting Standards - IFRS in Brazil, no significant association of improvements with this change were observed.

The studies by Saito, Villalobos by Benetti (2008)SAITO, R.; VILLALOBOS, S. J. S.; BENETTI, C. Qualidade das projeções dos analistas sell-side: evidência empírica do mercado brasileiro. Revista de Administração da Universidade de São Paulo, v. 43, n. 4, 2008., on the other hand, investigate the determinants of the quality of forecasts of market analysts. The research points out the reasons why analysts in Brazil have lower performance, among the limitations: (1) difficulties related to the economic instability of the country; (2) the limitations of analysts' abilities in the Brazilian market, especially in relation to more sophisticated statistical models; and (3) the stability of the economy and the results of companies in other markets are more easily foreseen. In general, it is confirmed in research by Martinez (2007MARTINEZ, A. L. Analysts' optimism and selection bias. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v. 4, n. 2, p. 104-118, 2007., 2008MARTINEZ, A. L. The effect of earnings projection revisions on stock returns in brazil. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v.5, n.2, p.121-135, 2008. and 2009)MARTINEZ, A. L. Determinantes da acurácia das previsões dos analistas do mercado de capitais. Contabilidade, Gestão e Governança, v. 10, n. 2, 2009. and Saito, Villalobos and Benetti (2008) that the Brazilian market for financial analysts need improvements in the quality and performance of the services offered.

3. PERFORMANCE METRICS

Despite recent criticism on the ways of measuring the accuracy and bias by Bonini et al (2010)BONINI, S.; ZANETTI, L.; BIANCHINI, R.; SALVI, A. Target price accuracy in equity research. Journal of Business Finance & Accounting, v. 37, n. 9-10, p. 1177-1217, 2010., in this study we opted for the classic metrics used in previous studies, as the evidence found were not clearly different from those found by the metrics already used. We summarized by using bias, the accuracy, returns and informativity of forecasts and recommendations.

3.1. Bias

This metric attempts to capture the forecasting errors. Equation 1 refers to Percentage Forecast Error from the percentage difference between Forecast Price, which is the expected price months before and the Last Price, closing price months after the forecast. This metric reflects the bias of estimates, considering each asset at time . If the average of forecast errors is negative and significant, then the forecasts were higher than the results, demonstrating optimistic bias. If positive and significant, then there is a pessimistic bias (BRADSHAW; BROWN; HUANG, 2013BRADSHAW, M. T.; BROWN, L. D.; HUANG, K. Do sell-side analysts exhibit differential target price forecasting ability? Review of Accounting Studies, v. 18, n. 4, p. 930-955, 2013.).

(1) PFE ijt = LP ijt FP ijt LP ijt

3.2. Accuracy

Equation 2 tries to capture the absolute error, Percentage Absolute Forecast Error, obtained by the absolute percentage relationship between the Forecast Price, which is the expected price months before and the Last Price, closing price months after this forecast. This metric reflects the accuracy of forecasts, considering each analyst , asset at time . The closer the averages of are to zero, the greater the accuracy (BRADSHAW; BROWN; HUANG, 2013BRADSHAW, M. T.; BROWN, L. D.; HUANG, K. Do sell-side analysts exhibit differential target price forecasting ability? Review of Accounting Studies, v. 18, n. 4, p. 930-955, 2013.).

(2) PAFE ijt = LP ijt FP ijt LP ijt

3.3. Returns for the recommendation

The gain from the buy/hold/sell rating recommendation, termed as is the analyst's recommendation for the period. The recommendation is rated on a continuous scale from 1 to 5, in which 1-sale, 2-weak sale, 3-hold, 4-weak buy, 5-buy. The annual cumulative return Cumulative Returns of Recommendation is calculated by the recommendation of the return for months derived from the difference between the last price negotiated months before and the price negotiated subsequently (BRADSHAW; BROWN; HUANG, 2013BRADSHAW, M. T.; BROWN, L. D.; HUANG, K. Do sell-side analysts exhibit differential target price forecasting ability? Review of Accounting Studies, v. 18, n. 4, p. 930-955, 2013.).

The return calculation on the recommendation is performed by Equation 3, wherein the accumulated return is calculated according to the recommendation , considering each analyst , asset at time . This return is nothing more than the percentage difference of positions to buy and sell assets.

(3) CRR ijt = LP ijt ( t n ) / LP ijt 1 0 LP ijt / LP ij ( t = n ) 1 caso 1 REC ijt < 3 caso REC ijt = 3 ; caso 3 < REC ijt 5 ;

We also used Cumulative Market-Adjusted Return calculated by adjusting the cumulative return to the market in the equation 4. The adjusted return is derived from the mean difference between the return of recommendation and the market return Cumulative Market Return for each time period . The market return is calculated by the return from the variation of the index score that represents the market (BRADSHAW; BROWN; HUANG, 2013BRADSHAW, M. T.; BROWN, L. D.; HUANG, K. Do sell-side analysts exhibit differential target price forecasting ability? Review of Accounting Studies, v. 18, n. 4, p. 930-955, 2013.).

(4) CMAR ijt = CRR ijt CMR t

3.4. Informativity

The informativity metric measures the association between revisions of forecasts of analysts and the abnormal returns on assets . According to Givoly and Lakonishok (1979)GIVOLY, D.; LAKONISHOK, J. The information content of financial analysts' forecasts of earnings: Some evidence on semi-strong inefficiency. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 1, n. 3, p. 165-185, dez. 1979., it is the abnormal relationship between the direction of revisions and the return of recommendations, verifying the market reaction by revealing analysts' revisions. It is worth noting that the revisions are measured by the percentage variation of the forecast.

The informativity coefficient is measured by beta , as it is calculated by regressing the abnormal returns, of the equation 4, revisions of each asset and period , according to equation 5. It is worth noting that this work deals with revisions of price forecasts. The higher the beta, the greater the informational effect will be.

(5) CMAR jt = α + β jt REV jt + ε jt

4. HYPOTHESES

4.1. Learning through experience and the complexity of the portfolio

The starting point for analysts' learning analysis were through research by Mikhail, Walther and Willis (1997)MIKHAIL, M. B.; WALTHER, B. R.; WILLIS, R. H. Do security analysts improve their performance with experience? Journal of Accounting Research, v. 35, p. 131-157, 1997., Jacob, Lys and Neale (1999)JACOB, J.; LYS, T. Z.; NEALE, M. A. Expertise in forecasting performance of security analysts. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 28, n. 1, p. 51-82, nov. 1999. and Clement (1999)CLEMENT, M. B. Analyst forecast accuracy: Do ability, resources, and portfolio complexity matter? Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 27, n. 3, p. 285-303, jul. 1999.,

performing analyzes based on the experience and complexity of the industry. The models build the relationship between performance metrics and determining variables of experience, the complexity of assets portfolio and the absorption of information by individuals.

Jacob, Lys and Neale (1999)JACOB, J.; LYS, T. Z.; NEALE, M. A. Expertise in forecasting performance of security analysts. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 28, n. 1, p. 51-82, nov. 1999., by contrast, argue that the simple and direct association between experience and accuracy is fragile because not all experiences by repetition have significant effect on returns. Therefore, the results by Martinez (2007)MARTINEZ, A. L. Analysts' optimism and selection bias. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v. 4, n. 2, p. 104-118, 2007. present some evidence of these contrapositions in the earnings forecasts of Brazilian firms. As the learning analysis was not part of the central discussion by Martinez (2007)MARTINEZ, A. L. Analysts' optimism and selection bias. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v. 4, n. 2, p. 104-118, 2007., and because of the particularities presented in the Brazilian market we centralized our central hypothesis in the analyst' experience.

In emerging markets it is also possible to observe the experience of associations with accuracy. However, the results by Karamanou (2012)KARAMANOU, I. Value relevance of analyst earnings forecasts in emerging markets. Advances in Accounting, v. 28, n. 1, p. 128-137, jun. 2012. were general, considering several emerging countries in the world. Due to the heterogeneity between countries used in the sample, it was necessary to perform a cross section in Brazil, mainly due to its different economic characteristics to other developing countries that were investigated Chile, Turkey, Thailand, Korea and China.

By using the learn by doing principle used by Mikhail, Walther and Willis (1997)MIKHAIL, M. B.; WALTHER, B. R.; WILLIS, R. H. Do security analysts improve their performance with experience? Journal of Accounting Research, v. 35, p. 131-157, 1997., Jacob, Lys and Neale (1999)JACOB, J.; LYS, T. Z.; NEALE, M. A. Expertise in forecasting performance of security analysts. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 28, n. 1, p. 51-82, nov. 1999. and Clement (1999)CLEMENT, M. B. Analyst forecast accuracy: Do ability, resources, and portfolio complexity matter? Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 27, n. 3, p. 285-303, jul. 1999., the metrics that we take as a basis in hypothesis 1 from equation 6 were measured as follows: EXPGEN we count the number of previous periods that the analyst issued a forecast; EXPSETOR we count the number of previous periods that the analyst issued a forecast for a particular sector; and, EXPASSET we count the number of previous periods that the analyst issued a forecast for a particular asset. Based on the studies, we expect that increasing experience contributes to a reduction of forecast errors.

H1: Experience in the execution of forecasts contributes to improved accuracy

(6) PAFE ijt = β 0 β 1 EXPGEN ijt β 2 EXPSETOR ijt β 3 EXPASSET ijt + β 4 NSETOR ift + β 5 NASSET ij + ε

Regarding the complexity of portfolio, research by Clement (1999)CLEMENT, M. B. Analyst forecast accuracy: Do ability, resources, and portfolio complexity matter? Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 27, n. 3, p. 285-303, jul. 1999., Duru and Reeb (2002)DURU, A.; REEB, D. M. International diversification and analysts' forecast accuracy and bias. The Accounting Review, v. 77, n. 2, p. 415-433, 2002. and Hirst, Hopkins and Wahlen (2004)HIRST, D. E.; HOPKINS, P. E.; WAHLEN, J. M. Fair values, income measurement, and bank analysts' risk and valuation judgments. The Accounting Review, v. 79, n. 2, p. 453-472, 2004. assume that analysts lose quality in their forecasts from the moment the portfolio diversifies. Lobo, Song and Stanford (2012)LOBO, G. J.; SONG, M.; STANFORD, M. Accruals quality and analyst coverage. Journal of Banking & Finance, v. 36, n. 2, p. 497-508, 2012. claim that the increase in expertise in certain companies and sectors helps to improve analysts' forecasts. Hirst, Hopkins and Wahlen (2004)HIRST, D. E.; HOPKINS, P. E.; WAHLEN, J. M. Fair values, income measurement, and bank analysts' risk and valuation judgments. The Accounting Review, v. 79, n. 2, p. 453-472, 2004. obtained evidence that analysts who follow fewer firms than the average make better forecasting decisions.

Nevertheless, the study by Martinez (2009)MARTINEZ, A. L. Determinantes da acurácia das previsões dos analistas do mercado de capitais. Contabilidade, Gestão e Governança, v. 10, n. 2, 2009. failed to observe this effect with respect to earnings forecasts in Brazilian companies. It is worth mentioning that there is no evidence neither for the analysis of this relationship based on target price forecasts. For this reason, we added two metrics of complexity: NSETOR, which refers to the number of sectors that the analyst covered for the period; and NASSET which corresponds to the number of assets that the analyst covered for the period. Based on studies, we expect that the increased amount of sectors and assets that the analyst covers contribute to reducing accuracy and increasing forecasting errors.

In order to find more answers, equation 6 was also analyzed from the perspective of bias. Hypothesis 2, according to equation 7, contributes to verifying that the experience and complexity measures are also associated to the effects of the analyst's pessimistic or optimistic behavior. This association is based on the study by Duru and Reeb (2002)DURU, A.; REEB, D. M. International diversification and analysts' forecast accuracy and bias. The Accounting Review, v. 77, n. 2, p. 415-433, 2002., by observing aspects that increased accuracy is associated with the pessimistic bias, to a conservative behavior of the individual.

H2: Experience in the execution of forecasts is associated with pessimism

(7) PFE ijt = β 0 + β 1 EXPGEN ijt + β 2 EXPSETOR ijt + β 3 EXPASSET ijt β 4 NSETOR ijt β 5 NASSET ijt + ε

We expect based on research by Duru and Reeb (2002)DURU, A.; REEB, D. M. International diversification and analysts' forecast accuracy and bias. The Accounting Review, v. 77, n. 2, p. 415-433, 2002. that the increase of experience is associated with a pessimist behavior. It is assumed that the increase of experience causes the analyst to become more conservative, which contributes to better results. Regarding the complexity of the portfolio, we expect to understand, on an exploratory basis, the effect of diversification.

On the hypothesis 3, equation 8, we insert the discussion on the learning effect on returns achieved from analysts' recommendations. As there is no evidence in Brazil, we used as a basis the research Mikhail, Walther and Willis (2003)MIKHAIL, M. B.; WALTHER, B. R.; WILLIS, R. H. The effect of experience on security analyst underreaction. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 35, n. 1, p. 101-116, 2003. who found evidence that the experience did not show association with the returns of the recommendations. We expect for experience not to present significant association in Brazil in relation to returns.

H3: Experience in performing the forecasts does not contribute to abnormal returns

(8) CMAR ijt = β 0 + β 1 EXPGEN ijt + β 3 EXPASSET ijt β 4 NSETOR ijt β 5 NASSET ijt + ε

Considering the complexity of the portfolio, we are based on the studies by Clement (1999)CLEMENT, M. B. Analyst forecast accuracy: Do ability, resources, and portfolio complexity matter? Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 27, n. 3, p. 285-303, jul. 1999., Duru and Reeb (2002)DURU, A.; REEB, D. M. International diversification and analysts' forecast accuracy and bias. The Accounting Review, v. 77, n. 2, p. 415-433, 2002. and Hirst, Hopkins and Wahlen (2004)HIRST, D. E.; HOPKINS, P. E.; WAHLEN, J. M. Fair values, income measurement, and bank analysts' risk and valuation judgments. The Accounting Review, v. 79, n. 2, p. 453-472, 2004. who found that analysts lose quality in their forecasts from the moment the portfolio diversifies. Thus, in hypothesis 3, we expect that the metrics related to the complexity of portfolios to hinder obtaining abnormal returns.

4.2. Learning through the use of information

The informativity is the degree to which the revisions of forecasts impact the movement of asset prices. In the analysis by Givoly and Lakonishok (1979)GIVOLY, D.; LAKONISHOK, J. The information content of financial analysts' forecasts of earnings: Some evidence on semi-strong inefficiency. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 1, n. 3, p. 165-185, dez. 1979., considering an inefficient market, abnormal returns can be observed months after the revisions of forecasts, and this shows that the reaction is not instantaneous. these effects in emerging markets were also found by Moshirian, Ng and Wu (2009)MOSHIRIAN, F.; NG, D.; WU, E. The value of stock analysts' recommendations: Evidence from emerging markets. International Review of Financial Analysis, v. 18, n. 1-2, p. 74-83, mar. 2009., But these evidence indicate that they have a lower degree of informativity.

Little evidence was found considering the forecast prices, and research by Brav and Lehavy (2003)BRAV, A.; LEHAVY, R. An empirical analysis of analysts' target prices: Short-term informativeness and long-term dynamics. The Journal of Finance, v. 58, n. 5, p. 1933-1968, 2003. and Asquith, Mikhail and Au (2005)ASQUITH, P.; MIKHAIL, M. B.; AU, A. S. Information content of equity analyst reports. Journal of Financial Economics, v. 75, n. 2, p. 245-282, fev. 2005. show strong association between revisions of forecasts and market returns. In particular, Brav and Lehavy (2003)BRAV, A.; LEHAVY, R. An empirical analysis of analysts' target prices: Short-term informativeness and long-term dynamics. The Journal of Finance, v. 58, n. 5, p. 1933-1968, 2003. found effects of persistent informativity up to six months after the revisions. Despite these persistent signs of these abnormal changes, we observe that these disorders tend to disappear in the long term (GIVOLY; LAKONISHOK, 1979GIVOLY, D.; LAKONISHOK, J. The information content of financial analysts' forecasts of earnings: Some evidence on semi-strong inefficiency. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 1, n. 3, p. 165-185, dez. 1979.).

In Brazil, considering the profit projections, Martinez (2008)MARTINEZ, A. L. The effect of earnings projection revisions on stock returns in brazil. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v.5, n.2, p.121-135, 2008. found low informativity evidence, as did Moshirian, Ng and Wu (2009)MOSHIRIAN, F.; NG, D.; WU, E. The value of stock analysts' recommendations: Evidence from emerging markets. International Review of Financial Analysis, v. 18, n. 1-2, p. 74-83, mar. 2009. in emerging markets. Considering that Brazil has increased efficiency, according to Mobarek and Fiorante (2014), it is possible that informativity has changed over time, which motivated us to check their level of information in relation to the target prices in Brazil.

The effect of the revisions on prices seek to verify the analyst's ability to observe changes in previous forecasts. Knowing this, we can see the individual revisions of the actual analyst, as well as the change in consensus forecasts. In contrast, Asquith, Mikhail and Au (2005)ASQUITH, P.; MIKHAIL, M. B.; AU, A. S. Information content of equity analyst reports. Journal of Financial Economics, v. 75, n. 2, p. 245-282, fev. 2005. refute the association of individual reviews, showing that these effects are only reiterations.

The argument used by Asquith, Mikhail and Au (2005)ASQUITH, P.; MIKHAIL, M. B.; AU, A. S. Information content of equity analyst reports. Journal of Financial Economics, v. 75, n. 2, p. 245-282, fev. 2005. starts to make sense when we consider the consensus1 1 The measurement of consensus is the average of all target price forecasts for the last three months from the date of issue. of forecasts. According to Campbell and Sharpe (2009)CAMPBELL, S. D.; SHARPE, S. A. Anchoring bias in consensus forecasts and its effect on market prices. Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis, v. 44, n. 2, p. 369, 2009., analysts are anchored in the consensus of their peers. Williams (2013)WILLIAMS, J. Financial analysts and the false consensus effect. Journal of Accounting Research, v. 51, n. 4, p. 855-907, 2013. explain that the anchoring relationship among peers is an over-estimation that individuals do with others. Then there is the possibility that the consensus forecast revision exhibit greater informational effect than analysts' individual revisions.

Recent evidence by Clement, Hales and Xue (2011)CLEMENT, M. B.; HALES, J.; XUE, Y. Understanding analysts' use of stock returns and other analysts' revisions when forecasting earnings. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v.51,n.3,p.279-299,abr.2011. show significant effects that the increase of the consensus revisions cause an increase of individual revisions. This result proves that analysts observe revisions of other analysts before submitting their own revisions, and this leads to the hypothesis that the consensus of the revision is more associated with abnormal returns than individual revisions. Based on this discussion, hypothesis 4 investigates the informativity from the perspective of the percentage of revision of the forecasts of analysts individually REV and the percentage of revision of the consensus of forecasts CREV.

H4: The consensus revision is more associated with abnormal returns than with analysts' individual revisions

(9) CMAR ijt = β 0 + β 1 REV ijt + β 2 lag REV ijt , 1 + β 3 CREV ijt + β 4 lag CREV ijt , 1 + β 5 REVGRADE ijt + β 6 CREVGRADE ijt + log VOLM + ε

To analyze the informational effect between individual revisions and consensus revisions we will use three reference models. Equation 9 verifies the association between the individual revisions REV and the consensus revisions CREV in abnormal market returns CMAR. Considering the possibility of lag, we also use the same variables REV and CREV with 1 lag.

In the research by Asquith, Mikhail and Au (2005)ASQUITH, P.; MIKHAIL, M. B.; AU, A. S. Information content of equity analyst reports. Journal of Financial Economics, v. 75, n. 2, p. 245-282, fev. 2005., individuals react more intesively when revisions are for less (downgrades), which allows us to assume that these revisions are absorbed differently. Because of this difference between positive reviews (upgrades) and negative revisions (downgrades), we inserted the variables REVGRADE and CREVGRADE, which assume the values of 0 or 1.

REVGRADE is a dummy of analysts' individual revisions, in which positive (upgrade) corresponds to 0 and negatives (downgrade) to 1, and the CREVGRADE, is a dummy for consensus revisions, in which positive (upgrade) corresponds to 0 and negatives (downgrade) to 1. These controls are based on evidence by Asquith, Mikhail and Au (2005)ASQUITH, P.; MIKHAIL, M. B.; AU, A. S. Information content of equity analyst reports. Journal of Financial Economics, v. 75, n. 2, p. 245-282, fev. 2005.. The control log(VOLM), trading volume of the asset, is justified by the efficiency of results by Bonini et al (2010)BONINI, S.; ZANETTI, L.; BIANCHINI, R.; SALVI, A. Target price accuracy in equity research. Journal of Business Finance & Accounting, v. 37, n. 9-10, p. 1177-1217, 2010..

Another variable that we use as a proxy for informational effect is the trading volume. Chae (2005)CHAE, J. Trading volume, information asymmetry, and timing information. The Journal of Finance, v. 60, n. 1, p. 413-442, 2005., Brown, Crocker and Foerster (2009)BROWN, J. H.; CROCKER, D. K.; FOERSTER, S. R. Trading volume and stock investments. Financial analysts journal, p. 67-84, 2009. and Bamber, Barron and Stevens (2011)BAMBER, L. S.; BARRON, O. E.; STEVENS, D. E. Trading volume around earnings announcements and other financial reports: Theory, research design, empirical evidence, and directions for future research. Contemporary Accounting Research, v. 28, n. 2, p. 431-471, jun. 2011. explore the argument that the trading volume of a proxy for the informativity in the market. This argument is used from the impact received by the trading volume from a decision on the market. Therefore, we proposed the informativity analysis by regressing the variable □□□□ by the variation of the revisions of price forecasts previously detailed.

Hypothesis 5, in equation 10, use the trading volume as a proxy for informativity, instead of CMAR. As the evidence discussed in hypothesis 4, we expect that the effects of consensus revisions are also more significant than the individual revisions.

H5: The consensus revision is more associated with traded volume returns than with analysts' individual revisions

(10) log VOLM ijt = β 0 + β 1 REV ijt + β 2 lag REV ijt , 1 + β 3 CREV ijt + β 4 lag CREV ijt , 1 + β 5 REVGRADE ijt + β 6 CREVGRADE ijt + ε

Finally, the third performance metric used to check informational effects was the accuracy. Clement, Hales and Xue (2011)CLEMENT, M. B.; HALES, J.; XUE, Y. Understanding analysts' use of stock returns and other analysts' revisions when forecasting earnings. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v.51,n.3,p.279-299,abr.2011. noted the increased accuracy from the increase in analysts' revisions. Thus, hypothesis 6, through equation 11, uses the argument as a way to verify if revisions affect learning for future forecasts. We expect that revisions in general have a positive association with the accuracy.

H6: The consensus revision is more associated with analysts' accuracy than their own individual revisions

(11) PAFE ijt = β 0 + β 1 REV ijt + β 2 lag REV ijt , 1 + β 3 CREV ijt + β 4 lag CREV ijt , 1 + β 5 REVGRADE ijt + β 6 CREVGRADE ijt + ε

5. DATA

From Bloomberg® we collected data on asset prices, their forecasts and recommendations of companies pertaining to the register of the BM&FBovespa. It is worth mentioning that these price estimates are held in the months prior to their target. The collection window comprised the years between 2005 and 2013, mainly by continuing research by Martinez (2007MARTINEZ, A. L. Analysts' optimism and selection bias. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v. 4, n. 2, p. 104-118, 2007., 2008MARTINEZ, A. L. The effect of earnings projection revisions on stock returns in brazil. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v.5, n.2, p.121-135, 2008. and 2009)MARTINEZ, A. L. Determinantes da acurácia das previsões dos analistas do mercado de capitais. Contabilidade, Gestão e Governança, v. 10, n. 2, 2009. and Saito, Villalobos and Benetti (2008)SAITO, R.; VILLALOBOS, S. J. S.; BENETTI, C. Qualidade das projeções dos analistas sell-side: evidência empírica do mercado brasileiro. Revista de Administração da Universidade de São Paulo, v. 43, n. 4, 2008. and for the availability of observations of forecasts.

The selected companies were all those with assets traded on the BM&FBovespa. The list showed the total of 404 companies and 641 stocks. For the sample, only 195 securities of 176 companies had forecasts in the window used. Thus, out the total of 404 companies registered on the BM&FBovespa, only 44% took part in the research. The database with individual forecasts and recommendations resulted in a total of 62,548 observations. In relation to brokers, 75 of the 80 registered on the BM&FBovespa took part in the research, and 569 of the 1,102 registered analysts in the Association of Investment Analysts and Professionals of the Capital Market - APIMEC. Data collection was performed on a daily basis, though, to optimize the relationships, we used monthly averages.

6. RESEARCH METHOD

The hypotheses were investigated using linear models in panel or longitudinal, according to equation 12. Each dependent variable yit of each individual observed i at time t was evaluated by n determinants Xitβ, considering, also, specific control variables according to each hypothesis tested. The use of panel models contribute to obtaining higher degree of freedom and increased efficiency of parameters estimation, as well as assisting to observe elements in time, termed as ci.

(12) 𝒴 ijt = X it β + c i + μ it

In a cross-examination, without considering this effect, the component ci is within the term error uit, reducing the explanation of the dependent variable (BALTAGI, 2008BALTAGI, B. Econometric analysis of panel data. John Wiley & Sons, 2008.; PETERSEN, 2009PETERSEN, M. A. Estimating standard errors in finance panel data sets: Comparing approaches. Review of financial studies, v. 22, n. 1, p. 435-480, 2009.; WOOLDRIDGE, 2010WOOLDRIDGE, J. M. Econometric analysis of cross section and panel data. MIT press, 2010.). For the analysis of the individual on the panel, we built an indexer from the concatenation between the analyst's identification variable ANALYST and asset identification ASSET, for it is a two-dimensional panel between each i = analyst&company in each time period t.

The unbalanced panel is common in this type of study due to the fact that not all companies have price forecasts at all times. As it is not intended to make comparisons between individuals, but to analyze the determinants of the metrics, the panel balancing is not required, avoiding information losses. Nor did we any model to complete the panel due to the large data gap in the first years, which could lead to unrealistic results. The unbalanced panel is justified to contain all market forecasts in the period analyzed (OBRIEN, 1987O'BRIEN, P. C. Individual forecasting ability. Managerial Finance, v. 13, n. 2, p. 3-9, 1987.; SO, 2013SO, E. C. A new approach to predicting analyst forecast errors: Do investors overweight analyst forecasts? Journal of Financial Economics, v. 108, n. 3, p. 615-640, jun. 2013.).

In using the Ros software, for the most part, the models used presented fixed effect, according to test results of grouped OLS, Breusch-Pagan's Lagrange Multiplier (1980)BREUSCH, T. S.; PAGAN, A. R. The lagrange multiplier test and its applications to model specification in econometrics. The Review of Economic Studies, p. 239-253, 1980. and Hausman's specification test (1978)HAUSMAN, J. A. Specification tests in econometrics. Econometrica: Journal of the Econometric Society, p. 1251-1271, 1978.. Since this is a long panel, the autocorrelation test becomes more rigorous for a more reliable estimate of parameters, therefore the test by Breusch (1978)BREUSCH, T. S. Testing for autocorrelation in dynamic linear models. Australian Economic Papers, v. 17, n. 31, p. 334-355, 1978. and Godfrey (1978)GODFREY, L. G. Testing against general autoregressive and moving average error models when the regressors include lagged dependent variables. Econometrica, v. 46, n. 6, p. 1293-1301, 1978. served as a basis. The test results of all models presented evidence of serial autocorrelation in residues, as well as the presence of heteroscedasticity. All tests are described in Appendix.

The presence of autocorrelation and heteroscedasticity affect the covariance matrix, causing loss of the models reliability. To resolve this problem, some studies have suggested the use of more robust alternatives, as Clatworthy, Peei and Pope (2007)CLATWORTHY, M. A.; PEEL, D. A.; POPE, P. F. Evaluating the properties of analysts' forecasts: A bootstrap approach. The British Accounting Review, v. 39, n. 1, p. 3-13, mar. 2007. and So (2013)SO, E. C. A new approach to predicting analyst forecast errors: Do investors overweight analyst forecasts? Journal of Financial Economics, v. 108, n. 3, p. 615-640, jun. 2013.; thus, a simple solution was to estimate the models using robust standard errors corrected for the autocorrelation and heteroscedasticity, proposed by Arellano (1987)ARELLANO, M. Computing robust standard errors for within-groups estimators. Oxford Bulletin of Economics and Statistics, v. 49, n. 4, p. 431-434, 1987..

7. DISCUSSION OF RESULTS

7.1. Descriptive statistics

For the analysis, each performance metrics was analyzed for its descriptive statistics, according to Table 1. In the first analysis, price forecasts have shown on average, with an optimistic bias, considering an average PFE at -0.70, including a minimum of -117.85 points and a maximum of 0.95.

Table 1
Descriptive Statistics.

This observed optimism exceeds the results by Schipper (1991)SCHIPPER, K. Analysts' forecasts. Accounting Horizons, v. 5, n. 4, p. 105-121, 1991.; Dreman and Berry (1995)DREMAN, D. N.; BERRY, M. A. Analyst forecasting errors and their implications for security analysis. Financial Analysts Journal, p. 30-41, 1995.; Conroy and Harris (1995)CONROY, R. M.; HARRIS, R. S. Analysts' earnings forecasts in japan: Accuracy and sell-side optimism. Pacific-Basin Finance Journal, v. 3, n. 4, p. 393-408, dez. 1995.; Brown (1996)BROWN, L. D. Analyst forecasting errors and their implications for security analysis: An alternative perspective. Financial Analysts Journal, p. 40-47, 1996.; and Beaver (2002)BEAVER, W. H. Perspectives on recent capital market research. The Accounting Review, v. 77, n. 2, p. 453-474, abr. 2002. in the North American market, as well as Martinez (2007)MARTINEZ, A. L. Analysts' optimism and selection bias. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v. 4, n. 2, p. 104-118, 2007. in Brazil, considering earnings forecasts. The individual bias showed more optimism that the results of the consensus bias de -0.41.

The accuracy PAFE was demonstrated around 0.83 points, above the average by Hilary and Hsu (2013)HILARY, G.; HSU, C. Analyst forecast consistency. The Journal of Finance, v. 68, n. 1, p. 271-297, 2013. and Bradshaw, Brown and Huang (2013)BRADSHAW, M. T.; BROWN, L. D.; HUANG, K. Do sell-side analysts exhibit differential target price forecasting ability? Review of Accounting Studies, v. 18, n. 4, p. 930-955, 2013. in the North American market and by Bonini et al. (2010)BONINI, S.; ZANETTI, L.; BIANCHINI, R.; SALVI, A. Target price accuracy in equity research. Journal of Business Finance & Accounting, v. 37, n. 9-10, p. 1177-1217, 2010. and Kerl (2011)KERL, A. G. Target price accuracy. BuR - Business Research, v. 4, n. 1, p. 74-96, mar. 2011. in European markets. Individual errors presented less accuracy than the results of bias consensus 0.53, and this result confirms the argument by Givoly and Lakonishok (1984)GIVOLY, D.; LAKONISHOK, J. Properties of analysts' forecasts of earnings: A review and analysis of the research. Journal of Accounting Literature, v. 3, n. 1, p. 117-52, 1984. that the consensus forecast errors are lesser.

The standard deviation was also greater individually, showing less consistency of forecasts, even in comparison with other markets. The annual returns resulting from the recommendations CRR resulted in a cumulative average of 6% and an average premium of 3% above the market CMAR. The annual consensus returns were also higher, with average CRR at 13% and average CMAR at 6%.

The variable EXPGEN shows that, in general terms, analysts have obtained an average of 2.5 years of experience, compared to 6.5 years in the North American market. Experience with assets resulted in an average of 1.2 years, compared to 3 years in the North American market. The results reflect a less experienced market compared to other more developed ones (YU, 2000).

The average number of sectors in the portfolio of analysts was around 3 segments. The average number of assets in the portfolio analysts was around 6 stocks, compared with latest averages of 14 in the North American market. Thus, the results show evidence that the number of sectors is less diverse than the number of assets. This result shows that analysts in Brazil still cover less assets in the portfolio, possibly due to the lower amount of assets in the market (MCNICHOLS; O'BRIEN, 1997MCNICHOLS, M.; O'BRIEN, P. C. Self-selection and analyst coverage. Journal of Accounting Research, p. 167-199, 1997.; BARTH; KASZNIK; MCNICHOLS, 2001BARTH, M. E.; KASZNIK, R.; MCNICHOLS, M. F. Analyst coverage and intangible assets. Journal of accounting research, p. 1-34, 2001.).

7.2. Evolution in time

To analyze the evolution over the years, it was possible to extract the current means of the variables used in the study, and Table 2 shows this extract. The bias over time changes according to the economic instability in the market, with excesses during the economic crisis in 2008 of -1.68. The explanation for this behavior is that possibly, analysts have not considered the impact of the crisis in the Brazilian market, which creates excessive optimism and consequently greater PAFE forecast errors.

Table 2
Annual Means of Variables.

In the period of crisis, individual and consensus reviews became negative, which shows the attempt to contain errors in the period. The reduction of individual and consensus revisions in the last three years 2010-2011-2012 possibly affected the accuracy, causing the errors to increase over recent years. This initial result shows the weakness in the accuracy of analysts, especially with the argument by Saito, Villalobos and Benetti (2008)SAITO, R.; VILLALOBOS, S. J. S.; BENETTI, C. Qualidade das projeções dos analistas sell-side: evidência empírica do mercado brasileiro. Revista de Administração da Universidade de São Paulo, v. 43, n. 4, 2008. on the low use of more sophisticated statistical techniques in this market.

In addition to accuracy, the cumulative returns of the recommendations were not as significant in the last three years, obtaining returns of 1%, 3% and 4% respectively. Nonetheless, there was a significant improvement in adjusted returns, consequently due to the decline in the market. This shows that returns of recommendations are not always associated with improved accuracy, also observed by Lim (2001)LIM, T. Rationality and analysts' forecast bias. The Journal of Finance, v. 56, n. 1, p. 369-385, 2001..

It was also possible to observe that both the experience and complexity of the portfolio in general, by asset and by sector, increments obtained over the years. The overall experience increased to 3.2 years, with the asset to 1.6 years and 10 years in the industry. This result shows that in Brazil, analysts remain always focused on a particular industry, alternating only the target asset in their portfolio.

The number of reports issued by analysts has grown considerably, as well as the average number of assets in the portfolio, which reached eight stocks. The average number of sectors that the analyst covers was close to four segments. Thus, despite the increase in coverage and issued reports, accuracy has been reduced in recent years, showing possible evidence that growth in the number of reports does not match the improvement in the quality of the results.

7.2. Correlation matrix

As the variables present evidence non-linearity for the development of correlations, we used the Spearman's correlation between the variables. Regarding the result of the matrix, we obtained few strong and significant correlations. They were considered weak below 0.3, medium till 0.6, and strong above 0.6. Based on these criteria, we highlight the main relationships found.

The result of the matrix demonstrates, firstly, an inverse relationship between the bias PFE and the accuracy PAFE of -0.7. The Association presented confirms that the increased pessimism possibly causes a more conservative behavior in the individual, contributing to the increase in accuracy. This conservative effect also presented evidence of a positive association with returns from the recommendations CRR of 0.44. This result explains that analysts who get more returns are more conservative in their forecasts.

Another important highlighted association is that the increase of accuracy is not directly associated with the increase of the recommendations returns. PAFE presented an association of -0.46 for CRR and -0.28 for the CMAR. This result corroborates with Lim (2001)LIM, T. Rationality and analysts' forecast bias. The Journal of Finance, v. 56, n. 1, p. 369-385, 2001., for whom accuracy will not always reflect in positive returns due to conflicts that may occur between estimates and recommendations by the actual analyst. The explanation for this association is that, at times, the analyst changes their recommendation even if not in accordance to their own forecast, due to informational asymmetry.

Finally, other relationships observed showed only minor associations. The independent variables, experience and complexity of the portfolio showed significant relationships with each other, presenting collinearity. However, the models used in panel are not affected because of multicollinearity (BALTAGI, 2008BALTAGI, B. Econometric analysis of panel data. John Wiley & Sons, 2008.).

7.3. Analysis of learning through experience and complexity of the portfolio

Table 4 shows the evaluation results of the analysts' experience and the complexity of the portfolio. The first results are of hypotheses 1, 2 and 3 associated with the learning of the analyst, with both the accuracy as with the bias and the returns of the recommendations. The log(VOLM) control, which is the trading volume of assets in the period, has been previously used by Bonini et al (2010)BONINI, S.; ZANETTI, L.; BIANCHINI, R.; SALVI, A. Target price accuracy in equity research. Journal of Business Finance & Accounting, v. 37, n. 9-10, p. 1177-1217, 2010..

Table 3
Correlation matrix.
Table 4
Experience and Complexity

Based on the study by Asquith, Mikhail and Au (2005)ASQUITH, P.; MIKHAIL, M. B.; AU, A. S. Information content of equity analyst reports. Journal of Financial Economics, v. 75, n. 2, p. 245-282, fev. 2005., the variable REVGRADE was inserted to control the imbalance between revisions for less (downgrades) and revisions for more (upgrades). This control shows that investors react more significantly when reports show revisions for less (downgrades).

Due informational asymmetry control purposes, we calculate the so-called variable CONFLICT; this dummy variable is classified with 1 when there is conflict to the rule according to Bradshaw (2002)BRADSHAW, M. T. The use of target prices to justify sell-side analysts' stock recommendations. Accounting Horizons, v. 16, n. 1, p. 27-41, 2002. between the forecast and the recommendation and 0 when the rule is followed and there is no conflict. Informational asymmetry comes from the imbalance between the indication of the forecast and the analyst's recommendation. Another control variable was the variation of the Bovespa index, and this choice was made due to the change in the market in 2008; probably because of the economic crisis, the IBOV variable tries to control the effects of the economic movement.

Regarding the results observed in Table 4, increased experience with the sector and the increased experience with the asset demonstrated concordance with increased accuracy and are associated with a pessimistic behavior, strengthening the evidence by Mikhail, Walther and Willis (1997)MIKHAIL, M. B.; WALTHER, B. R.; WILLIS, R. H. Do security analysts improve their performance with experience? Journal of Accounting Research, v. 35, p. 131-157, 1997. and Mikhail, Walther and Willis (2003)MIKHAIL, M. B.; WALTHER, B. R.; WILLIS, R. H. The effect of experience on security analyst underreaction. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 35, n. 1, p. 101-116, 2003.. This relationship demonstrates that experience with certain segments and assets contributes over repeated exercise to achieve better target price forecasts.

On the other hand, as according to Martinez (2007)MARTINEZ, A. L. Analysts' optimism and selection bias. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v. 4, n. 2, p. 104-118, 2007., the overall experience showed negative effects with accuracy, refuting Mikhail, Walther and Willis (2003)MIKHAIL, M. B.; WALTHER, B. R.; WILLIS, R. H. The effect of experience on security analyst underreaction. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 35, n. 1, p. 101-116, 2003.. It is possible that in Brazil, for the counting of these issued reports to be biased due to informational asymmetry from the difference between forecast and recommendations. Somehow, a junior analyst with lower reputation, has no discretional power to bias their estimates, unlike a senior analyst who incurs in intentional bias. If the increase of issued reports are confrontational, as shown in Table 2, it can explain the reason for this contradictory relationship to learning through repetition.

In relation to the complexity of the analyst's hedging portfolio, the results presented significant association for both of the variables NASSET and NSETOR. The variable NSETOR confirms the evidence by Jacob, Lys and Neale (1999)JACOB, J.; LYS, T. Z.; NEALE, M. A. Expertise in forecasting performance of security analysts. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 28, n. 1, p. 51-82, nov. 1999., Duru and Reeb (2002)DURU, A.; REEB, D. M. International diversification and analysts' forecast accuracy and bias. The Accounting Review, v. 77, n. 2, p. 415-433, 2002., Hirst, Hopkins and Wahlen (2004)HIRST, D. E.; HOPKINS, P. E.; WAHLEN, J. M. Fair values, income measurement, and bank analysts' risk and valuation judgments. The Accounting Review, v. 79, n. 2, p. 453-472, 2004. and Lobo, Song and Stanford (2012)LOBO, G. J.; SONG, M.; STANFORD, M. Accruals quality and analyst coverage. Journal of Banking & Finance, v. 36, n. 2, p. 497-508, 2012. that the increase in the number of sectors is related to the increase in forecast errors and, consequently, associated with reduced accuracy.

In contrast, the NASSET variable indicated that the increase in the number of assets is linked to improving accuracy and reducing errors, this effect is contradictory to the results by Jacob, Lys and Neale (1999)JACOB, J.; LYS, T. Z.; NEALE, M. A. Expertise in forecasting performance of security analysts. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 28, n. 1, p. 51-82, nov. 1999.. It is possible that, in Brazil, the analysis of assets within the same sector has a beneficial association due to the lower amount of traded assets on the BM&FBovespa and in the portfolios of analysts.

All control variables showed significant results in the analysis of performance metrics. We highlight the variable CONFLICT, which represents part of the informational asymmetry between analysts and investors. The variable was also significant from the interactions between the experience and accuracy metrics and demonstrates that analysts' reports in Brazil need to be less conflictive and have more technical rigor.

Regarding hypothesis 3, there were some significant effects pointing that experience is associated with the recommendations returns, but only when these experiences are disassociated from forecasts. Although small, the EXPGEN showed that increasing the overall experience is positively related to the increase of abnormal returns and that the increase in EXPASSET is negatively associated with increasing returns. This observed relationship refutes the results by Mikhail, Walther and Willis (2003MIKHAIL, M. B.; WALTHER, B. R.; WILLIS, R. H. The effect of experience on security analyst underreaction. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 35, n. 1, p. 101-116, 2003.that experience is not related to returns. In other words, when there is informational asymmetry, experience and complexity of the portfolio distort the effects of the learn by doing assumptions.

7.4. Learning analysis by informativity

The results of Table 5 show the correlations in the hypotheses of informativity. The first model of the hypothesis 4, demonstrates a significant association of analysts' abnormal returns in relation to consensus revision, which was not observed with the individual revisions. The positive relationship confirms the anchoring that individuals place on their peers, reinforcing evidence by Campbell and Sharpe (2009)CAMPBELL, S. D.; SHARPE, S. A. Anchoring bias in consensus forecasts and its effect on market prices. Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis, v. 44, n. 2, p. 369, 2009. and Williams (2013)WILLIAMS, J. Financial analysts and the false consensus effect. Journal of Accounting Research, v. 51, n. 4, p. 855-907, 2013.. Considering the increase in reports and conflicts between forecast and recommendations, it is possible to assume that in some cases analysts are only replicating previous reports of their peers.

Table 5
Informativity

Despite evidence of increased efficiency in the Brazilian market Mobarek and Fiorante (2014), the association between consensus review and abnormal returns persists even for a lag, confirming the argument by Givoly and Lakonishok (1979)GIVOLY, D.; LAKONISHOK, J. The information content of financial analysts' forecasts of earnings: Some evidence on semi-strong inefficiency. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 1, n. 3, p. 165-185, dez. 1979. and Brav and Lehavy (2003)BRAV, A.; LEHAVY, R. An empirical analysis of analysts' target prices: Short-term informativeness and long-term dynamics. The Journal of Finance, v. 58, n. 5, p. 1933-1968, 2003. of persistence of the informational effect. The weak informativity in the market also confirms the research by Martinez (2008)MARTINEZ, A. L. The effect of earnings projection revisions on stock returns in brazil. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v.5, n.2, p.121-135, 2008. and Moshirian, Ng and Wu (2009)MOSHIRIAN, F.; NG, D.; WU, E. The value of stock analysts' recommendations: Evidence from emerging markets. International Review of Financial Analysis, v. 18, n. 1-2, p. 74-83, mar. 2009.. However, based on the informativity of the consensus revisions, it is possible to obtain abnormal earnings observing the movements of these revisions in the Brazilian market.

Regarding the model's weakness of the hypothesis 5, the trading volume did not present a good proxy to analyze the market's informational effect. The result meets the variable defended by Chae (2005)CHAE, J. Trading volume, information asymmetry, and timing information. The Journal of Finance, v. 60, n. 1, p. 413-442, 2005., Brown, Crocker and Foerster (2009)BROWN, J. H.; CROCKER, D. K.; FOERSTER, S. R. Trading volume and stock investments. Financial analysts journal, p. 67-84, 2009. and Bamber, Barron and Stevens (2011)BAMBER, L. S.; BARRON, O. E.; STEVENS, D. E. Trading volume around earnings announcements and other financial reports: Theory, research design, empirical evidence, and directions for future research. Contemporary Accounting Research, v. 28, n. 2, p. 431-471, jun. 2011.. Possibly, revisions and changes in price forecasts alter the shape of companies' value and do not create incentives to increase the trading volume.

In considering hypothesis 6, the effect of informational consensus revisions intensifies the impact on returns when revisions are for less (downgrades) confirming the evidence by Asquith, Mikhail and Au (2005)ASQUITH, P.; MIKHAIL, M. B.; AU, A. S. Information content of equity analyst reports. Journal of Financial Economics, v. 75, n. 2, p. 245-282, fev. 2005.. Hypothesis 6 shows that the analysts' consensus revisions CREVGRADE are undermining target price forecasts and therefore, it explains the adverse relationship in preliminary abnormal returns.

The result of the association of the consensus confirms the hypothesis that analysts point out anchoring moments on their peers and corroborates the argument by Williams (2013)WILLIAMS, J. Financial analysts and the false consensus effect. Journal of Accounting Research, v. 51, n. 4, p. 855-907, 2013. that individuals are anchored on the similarities of their peers to issue their recommendations. However, the negative association with the returns and the positive association with the errors, show that this anchoring is damaging to the activity.

8. FINAL CONSIDERATIONS

In this study, we explored the association between learning, the complexity of the hedging portfolio and the informativity in target price forecasts and recommendations of sell-side analysts considering the informational asymmetry on the BM&FBovespa. The variables demonstrated that the Brazilian market presented optimism and excessive errors compared to other markets such as the North American.

Revisions by individual analysts presented associations with analysts' consensus, which confirmed the argument of anchoring on their peers by Campbell and Sharpe (2009)CAMPBELL, S. D.; SHARPE, S. A. Anchoring bias in consensus forecasts and its effect on market prices. Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis, v. 44, n. 2, p. 369, 2009. and Williams (2013)WILLIAMS, J. Financial analysts and the false consensus effect. Journal of Accounting Research, v. 51, n. 4, p. 855-907, 2013.. The possible explanation for this association is that, the accuracy of consensus forecasts was greater than the individual accuracy. Whereas the average presents better results, analysts seek to observe their peers in order to conduct new analyses. As an example, the award of the returns of consensus recommendations were 6% per annum compared to 3% by analysts individually.

With regard to learning, the overall experience was 2.5 years, much lower than the 6.5 years of the North American market, as well as the experience with each asset being 1.2 years, much lower than 3 years, respectively. These results show less experienced professionals than in more developed markets, which explains the lower accuracy.

The average number of sectors in the portfolio of analysts was around 3 segments, with an average of 6 stocks in the portfolio, compared to the average of 14 stocks in a more recent survey in the North American market. This result shows how analysts in Brazil still cover less stocks in the portfolio, possibly due to the lower amount of assets offered in the market.

The increase in experience with sectors and assets, as well as the complexity of the portfolio, confirmed the learn by doing principle in Brazil. But increasing experience in general presented a hindering effect on accuracy due to the observed informational asymmetry. The result of this distortion are the positive recommendations returns, but defected target price forecasts.

One possible conclusion is that more experienced analysts in the market, with greater discretionary power, prefer to change their recommendations, contradicting their own target price forecasts in an attempt to intentionally bias the market. Thus, less experienced analysts cannot cause such effect, but their lack of abilities and the low use of more sophisticated statistical models reduce its accuracy with assets and sectors, confirming the argument by Saito, Villalobos and Benetti (2008)SAITO, R.; VILLALOBOS, S. J. S.; BENETTI, C. Qualidade das projeções dos analistas sell-side: evidência empírica do mercado brasileiro. Revista de Administração da Universidade de São Paulo, v. 43, n. 4, 2008..

To solve the observed bias, we suggest formulating analysts' classification that are more objective, less biased and who value good analysis, reinforcing the argument by Emery and Li (2009)EMERY, D. R.; Li, X. Are the wall street analyst rankings popularity contests? Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis, v. 44, n. 2, 2009.. Existing rankings in the Brazilian market should encourage the elimination of possible contradictions between forecasts and recommendations, enabling the increase in the quality of analysis results.

  • Note from the Editor: The article was accepted by Bruno Felix.
  • 1
    The measurement of consensus is the average of all target price forecasts for the last three months from the date of issue.

APPENDIX A. Statistical Tests of Models in Panel.

Table A1
Statistical Tests of Panel in Models.

9. REFERENCES

  • ARELLANO, M. Computing robust standard errors for within-groups estimators. Oxford Bulletin of Economics and Statistics, v. 49, n. 4, p. 431-434, 1987.
  • ASQUITH, P.; MIKHAIL, M. B.; AU, A. S. Information content of equity analyst reports. Journal of Financial Economics, v. 75, n. 2, p. 245-282, fev. 2005.
  • BALTAGI, B. Econometric analysis of panel data. John Wiley & Sons, 2008.
  • BAMBER, L. S.; BARRON, O. E.; STEVENS, D. E. Trading volume around earnings announcements and other financial reports: Theory, research design, empirical evidence, and directions for future research. Contemporary Accounting Research, v. 28, n. 2, p. 431-471, jun. 2011.
  • BARTH, M. E.; KASZNIK, R.; MCNICHOLS, M. F. Analyst coverage and intangible assets. Journal of accounting research, p. 1-34, 2001.
  • BEAVER, W. H. Perspectives on recent capital market research. The Accounting Review, v. 77, n. 2, p. 453-474, abr. 2002.
  • BONINI, S.; ZANETTI, L.; BIANCHINI, R.; SALVI, A. Target price accuracy in equity research. Journal of Business Finance & Accounting, v. 37, n. 9-10, p. 1177-1217, 2010.
  • BRADSHAW, M. T. The use of target prices to justify sell-side analysts' stock recommendations. Accounting Horizons, v. 16, n. 1, p. 27-41, 2002.
  • BRADSHAW, M. T.; BROWN, L. D.; HUANG, K. Do sell-side analysts exhibit differential target price forecasting ability? Review of Accounting Studies, v. 18, n. 4, p. 930-955, 2013.
  • BRAV, A.; LEHAVY, R. An empirical analysis of analysts' target prices: Short-term informativeness and long-term dynamics. The Journal of Finance, v. 58, n. 5, p. 1933-1968, 2003.
  • BREUSCH, T. S. Testing for autocorrelation in dynamic linear models. Australian Economic Papers, v. 17, n. 31, p. 334-355, 1978.
  • BREUSCH, T. S.; PAGAN, A. R. The lagrange multiplier test and its applications to model specification in econometrics. The Review of Economic Studies, p. 239-253, 1980.
  • BROWN, L. D. Analyst forecasting errors and their implications for security analysis: An alternative perspective. Financial Analysts Journal, p. 40-47, 1996.
  • BROWN, J. H.; CROCKER, D. K.; FOERSTER, S. R. Trading volume and stock investments. Financial analysts journal, p. 67-84, 2009.
  • CAMPBELL, S. D.; SHARPE, S. A. Anchoring bias in consensus forecasts and its effect on market prices. Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis, v. 44, n. 2, p. 369, 2009.
  • CHAE, J. Trading volume, information asymmetry, and timing information. The Journal of Finance, v. 60, n. 1, p. 413-442, 2005.
  • CHUNG, K. H.; JO, H. The impact of security analysts' monitoring and marketing functions on the market value of firms. Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis, v. 31, n. 04, p. 493-512, 1996.
  • CLATWORTHY, M. A.; PEEL, D. A.; POPE, P. F. Evaluating the properties of analysts' forecasts: A bootstrap approach. The British Accounting Review, v. 39, n. 1, p. 3-13, mar. 2007.
  • CLEMENT, M. B. Analyst forecast accuracy: Do ability, resources, and portfolio complexity matter? Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 27, n. 3, p. 285-303, jul. 1999.
  • CLEMENT, M. B.; HALES, J.; XUE, Y. Understanding analysts' use of stock returns and other analysts' revisions when forecasting earnings. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v.51,n.3,p.279-299,abr.2011.
  • CONROY, R. M.; HARRIS, R. S. Analysts' earnings forecasts in japan: Accuracy and sell-side optimism. Pacific-Basin Finance Journal, v. 3, n. 4, p. 393-408, dez. 1995.
  • DALMACIO, F. Z.; LOPES, A. B.; REZENDE, A. J.; NETO, A. S. An analysis of the relationship between corporate governance and accuracy of the analysts forecasts of the brazilian market. RAM Revista de Administração Mackenzie, v. 14, n. 5, p. 104-139, 2013.
  • DREMAN, D. N.; BERRY, M. A. Analyst forecasting errors and their implications for security analysis. Financial Analysts Journal, p. 30-41, 1995.
  • DURU, A.; REEB, D. M. International diversification and analysts' forecast accuracy and bias. The Accounting Review, v. 77, n. 2, p. 415-433, 2002.
  • EMERY, D. R.; Li, X. Are the wall street analyst rankings popularity contests? Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis, v. 44, n. 2, 2009.
  • FRANCIS, J.; SOFFER, L. The relative informativeness of analysts' stock recommendations and earnings forecast revisions. Journal of Accounting Research, p. 193-211, 1997.
  • GIVOLY, D.; LAKONISHOK, J. The information content of financial analysts' forecasts of earnings: Some evidence on semi-strong inefficiency. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 1, n. 3, p. 165-185, dez. 1979.
  • GIVOLY, D.; LAKONISHOK, J. Properties of analysts' forecasts of earnings: A review and analysis of the research. Journal of Accounting Literature, v. 3, n. 1, p. 117-52, 1984.
  • GLEASON, C. A.; LEE, C. M. Analyst forecast revisions and market price discovery. The Accounting Review, v. 78, n. 1, p. 193-225, 2003.
  • GODFREY, J. P. What is an analyst? Financial Analysts Journal, v. 9, n. 5, p. 103-105, 1953.
  • GODFREY, L. G. Testing against general autoregressive and moving average error models when the regressors include lagged dependent variables. Econometrica, v. 46, n. 6, p. 1293-1301, 1978.
  • HAUSMAN, J. A. Specification tests in econometrics. Econometrica: Journal of the Econometric Society, p. 1251-1271, 1978.
  • HILARY, G.; HSU, C. Analyst forecast consistency. The Journal of Finance, v. 68, n. 1, p. 271-297, 2013.
  • HIRST, D. E.; HOPKINS, P. E.; WAHLEN, J. M. Fair values, income measurement, and bank analysts' risk and valuation judgments. The Accounting Review, v. 79, n. 2, p. 453-472, 2004.
  • JACKSON, A. R. Trade generation, reputation, and sell-side analysts. The Journal of Finance, v. 60, n. 2, p. 673-717, 2005.
  • JACOB, J.; LYS, T. Z.; NEALE, M. A. Expertise in forecasting performance of security analysts. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 28, n. 1, p. 51-82, nov. 1999.
  • KARAMANOU, I. Value relevance of analyst earnings forecasts in emerging markets. Advances in Accounting, v. 28, n. 1, p. 128-137, jun. 2012.
  • KERL, A. G. Target price accuracy. BuR - Business Research, v. 4, n. 1, p. 74-96, mar. 2011.
  • LIM, T. Rationality and analysts' forecast bias. The Journal of Finance, v. 56, n. 1, p. 369-385, 2001.
  • LOBO, G. J.; SONG, M.; STANFORD, M. Accruals quality and analyst coverage. Journal of Banking & Finance, v. 36, n. 2, p. 497-508, 2012.
  • MARTINEZ, A. L. Analysts' optimism and selection bias. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v. 4, n. 2, p. 104-118, 2007.
  • MARTINEZ, A. L. The effect of earnings projection revisions on stock returns in brazil. BBR-Brazilian Business Review, v.5, n.2, p.121-135, 2008.
  • MARTINEZ, A. L. Determinantes da acurácia das previsões dos analistas do mercado de capitais. Contabilidade, Gestão e Governança, v. 10, n. 2, 2009.
  • MARTINEZ, A. L.; DUMER, M. Adoption of IFRS and the properties of analysts' forecasts: The brazilian case. Revista de Contabilidade e Organizações, v. 8, n. 20, abr. 2014.
  • MCNICHOLS, M.; O'BRIEN, P. C. Self-selection and analyst coverage. Journal of Accounting Research, p. 167-199, 1997.
  • MIKHAIL, M. B.; WALTHER, B. R.; WILLIS, R. H. Do security analysts improve their performance with experience? Journal of Accounting Research, v. 35, p. 131-157, 1997.
  • MIKHAIL, M. B.; WALTHER, B. R.; WILLIS, R. H. The effect of experience on security analyst underreaction. Journal of Accounting and Economics, v. 35, n. 1, p. 101-116, 2003.
  • MOSHIRIAN, F.; NG, D.; WU, E. The value of stock analysts' recommendations: Evidence from emerging markets. International Review of Financial Analysis, v. 18, n. 1-2, p. 74-83, mar. 2009.
  • O'BRIEN, P. C. Individual forecasting ability. Managerial Finance, v. 13, n. 2, p. 3-9, 1987.
  • PETERSEN, M. A. Estimating standard errors in finance panel data sets: Comparing approaches. Review of financial studies, v. 22, n. 1, p. 435-480, 2009.
  • POMPIAN, M. Behavioral finance and wealth management: how to build optimal portfolios that account for investor biases. John Wiley & Sons, 2011.
  • RAMNATH, S.; ROCK, S.; SHANE, P. The financial analyst forecasting literature: A taxonomy with suggestions for further research. International Journal of Forecasting, v. 24, n. 1, p. 34-75, 2008.
  • SAITO, R.; VILLALOBOS, S. J. S.; BENETTI, C. Qualidade das projeções dos analistas sell-side: evidência empírica do mercado brasileiro. Revista de Administração da Universidade de São Paulo, v. 43, n. 4, 2008.
  • SCHIPPER, K. Analysts' forecasts. Accounting Horizons, v. 5, n. 4, p. 105-121, 1991.
  • SO, E. C. A new approach to predicting analyst forecast errors: Do investors overweight analyst forecasts? Journal of Financial Economics, v. 108, n. 3, p. 615-640, jun. 2013.
  • WEINSTEIN, N. D. Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of personality and social psychology, v. 39, n. 5, p. 806, 1980.
  • WILLIAMS, J. Financial analysts and the false consensus effect. Journal of Accounting Research, v. 51, n. 4, p. 855-907, 2013.
  • WOOLDRIDGE, J. M. Econometric analysis of cross section and panel data. MIT press, 2010.
  • YU, F. F. Analyst coverage and earnings management. Journal of Financial Economics, v. 88, n. 2, p. 245-271, 2008.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Mar-Apr 2017

History

  • Received
    17 Apr 2015
  • Reviewed
    04 Aug 2015
  • Accepted
    09 Nov 2015
Fucape Business School Av. Fernando Ferrari, 1358, Boa Vista, 29075-505, Vitória, Espírito Santo, Brasil, (27) 4009-4423 - Vitória - ES - Brazil
E-mail: bbronline@bbronline.com.br